Next Article in Journal
Parental Perceived Usefulness on a School-Integrated App to Prevent Bullying and Eating Disorders
Previous Article in Journal
Social–Emotional Management to Promote Quality in Education: A Training Program for Teachers
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Systematic Review

A Systematic Literature Review of the Impact of Cognitive Stimulation Programs on Reading Skills in Children Aged between 6 and 12 Years Old

by
Claudia Reina-Reina
1,
Eneko Antón
1,2 and
Jon Andoni Duñabeitia
1,3,*
1
Centro de Investigación Nebrija en Cognición (CINC), Universidad Nebrija, 28043 Madrid, Spain
2
Faculty of Humanities and Education Sciences, Mondragon University, 20500 Arrasate, Spain
3
Department of Languages and Culture, UiT the Arctic University of Norway, 9019 Tromsø, Norway
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Educ. Sci. 2024, 14(3), 229; https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14030229
Submission received: 12 December 2023 / Revised: 20 February 2024 / Accepted: 21 February 2024 / Published: 22 February 2024
(This article belongs to the Section Language and Literacy Education)

Abstract

:
The scientific evidence regarding the possibility of transferring benefits derived from cognitive training focused on working memory and inhibitory control to reading skills in children aged 6 to 12 is inconclusive. This study carries out a systematic review of recent published studies on this topic with the aim of analysing the specific role of various cognitive stimulation programs in the growth of executive functions and reading performance in children from ages 6 to 12. Here, we present the main results reported in the most recent literature, where the impact of intervention programs on working memory and inhibitory control in children with typical development are analysed. Even though the effectiveness of executive function training programs in terms of close transfer is conspicuous, there is still a lack of convergence in recently published articles, especially regarding the effects of far transfer in reading comprehension after cognitive stimulation programs are applied.

1. Introduction

In recent years, there has been growing interest in modulating different cognitive skills, such as executive functions, based on training programs in populations with diverse qualities and characteristics. However, a lack of consensus regarding the effectiveness of the applied programs can be observed, as outcomes vary based on the type of population, stimulated abilities, and intervention duration, among other factors. For this reason, this review performs an exhaustive analysis of the impact of different executive function training programs in populations without any type of neurodevelopmental difficulty or disorder.

1.1. Definition of Executive Functions

Executive functions are defined as a sequence of higher cognitive processes, such as working memory, inhibitory control, or cognitive flexibility [1,2]. Their neural correlates primarily take place in the prefrontal cortex [1,3,4] and, to a lesser extent, in the basal ganglia and cerebellum [1,5,6,7].
Working memory has been conceptualized as a cognitive process that involves the storage of a limited amount of information, while the same or other additional information is managed and/or manipulated [8,9,10,11]. According to Baddeley [9,10], this executive function is composed of three levels: a central component responsible for processing manipulated information, and two other components, one verbal and one visuospatial, derived from the central component [1,12,13].
Inhibitory control is defined as the ability to control and suppress inappropriate, distracting, or irrelevant responses to certain stimuli, as well as thoughts and emotions. Thus, inhibitory control contributes to the development of other complex processes, such as reasoning, attentional capacity, planning, or problem-solving [12,14].
Cognitive flexibility refers to the ability to adapt easily to changing mental demands, goals, thoughts, and perspectives, and to change between activities that are being implemented. This executive function allows the confronting of new challenges when they arise unexpectedly, which in turn permits learning from mistakes [15,16,17,18,19,20].
These executive functions are crucial for performing simultaneous tasks aimed at achieving a specific goal, autonomously and independently [21]. Therefore, they can act as strong predictors of academic skills [9,22,23,24,25,26] and reading abilities [15,27,28,29,30,31,32,33]. However, it is worth noting that some authors claim that the relationship between cognitive flexibility and reading abilities might be less consistent than the associations between working memory, inhibitory control, and reading performance [16,17,18,34].
Consequently, the idea that executive functions, such as working memory and inhibitory control, can be trained by different cognitive stimulation programs has gained interest in recent years [22,35,36,37,38,39,40], especially because the impact of cognitive training of working memory and inhibitory control extends beyond executive functions, benefiting complex tasks and activities in the educational context, where executive functions play a crucial role [41,42,43,44,45]. Similarly, deficits in working memory [31,45,46,47,48,49,50,51] and inhibitory control [50,52] are associated with low academic performance. Therefore, difficulties in working memory and inhibitory control, as well as the underlying learning difficulties, could potentially be compensated for through cognitive training [53].

1.2. Definition of Reading Skills

Reading involves several sets of skills that must be mastered to achieve a successful level in said ability.
Grapheme-to-phoneme conversion, also known as decoding, is the root of reading and the defining ability of a reader’s phonological skills. To master it, prereaders must assimilate, understand, and manipulate the correspondence between a given letter and the phoneme it represents [54,55], thus allowing for visual recognition of words [56]. Consequently, phonological awareness and letter identification in the early stages are considered one of the first predictors of reading skills development [24,54,55,57]. Specifically, the speed and accuracy of letter naming and phonological awareness shown by 5-year-olds can function as strong predictors of word reading at 6–7 years of age [57]. Authors such as De la Calle [58] find that, while word naming speed has a greater influence on the reading speed of 6-year-old children, phonological awareness is more involved in the accuracy demonstrated by these participants.
Reading fluency is defined as the ability to recognise words appropriately, with expression, intonation, and pacing appropriate to the text being read [59]. This skill unequivocally contributes to reading comprehension in children and adolescents [60,61]. However, reading fluency is not considered a strong predictor of reading comprehension [60,62,63]. This is due to the influence of numerous additional factors involved in reading comprehension, such as the content and characteristics of the text, or the syntactic, semantic, and pragmatic context in which the words or sentences are located [64].
Reading comprehension involves different coordinated and coherent complex cognitive processes that integrate the information received [56,65,66,67,68,69]. On the one hand, the reader must recognise the letters that compose each word of the text and the syntactic structure to which it belongs, and then extract the meaning from their mental lexicon [54,56,60,65,66,67,69,70]. On the other hand, readers have a requirement to construct a mental representation of the information they are reading, incorporate this information into their prior knowledge, and thus make inferences about the content of the text [54,56,65,66,67,68,69,71].
To achieve this, two main routes or pathways of word recognition and reading can be distinguished [55,56,72]. The initial route for prereaders and beginner readers is known as the sub-lexical pathway, and it is linked to the grapheme–phoneme conversion rules [55]. Once the sub-lexical pathway is developed and automatized, the lexical pathway is responsible for connecting the global spelling of known words with the reader’s internal representation and meaning of that word, accelerating the processes of reading words and sentences [55,56,72]. However, even if the reader has reached a high level of automatized word recognition via the lexical pathway during reading, the sub-lexical pathway is still essential for the development of any reading activity, complementing the lexical pathway [55]. Therefore, when the reader is confronted with an unfamiliar or unusual word, or a pseudo-word, they require the use of the sub-lexical pathway to decompose the letters that compose the word [55,56,72].
From the developmental perspective, the first stages of reading acquisition are conditioned by the cognitive and psychological maturation of children [54,73]. However, it is important to note that, in languages based on the Latin alphabet, the knowledge and identification of letters are usually developed between the ages of 3 and 6 [54,57]. This period demands high levels of focused spatial attention, making the progressive decoding of letters a cognitively challenging task [72]. Likewise, phonological awareness and decoding are usually consolidated at around 8 and 9 years of age, a period in which children can apply the mechanisms of grapheme–phoneme conversion automatically, so that they no longer need complex cognitive strategies or highly demanding attention requirements to recognise the letters that make up the word [60,61,66,67]. From this period onwards, children move into a new period of development focused on the acquisition of richer vocabulary, while speed, accuracy, reading fluency, and ultimately comprehension are also trained. Therefore, some authors describe this period as the transition from learning to read to learning to comprehend [60], since reading words accurately and automatically allows for greater cognitive involvement in the processes of constructing meaning [61].

1.3. Role of Executive Functions in Reading

Working memory abilities, both central [29,74,75], verbal [76,77], and visuospatial [48,78,79], are significantly related to the multiple processes of literacy and text comprehension that children acquire and develop. For instance, reading speed may be relevant for visual word processing, to pronounce syllables and phonemes while temporarily storing new words from the text, and to be able to combine this information with the context and thus achieve text comprehension [24,32,80,81].
Similarly, the components of inhibitory control and attention are responsible for focusing on the letters appearing in the text, inhibiting irrelevant information from the text and from the reader’s immediate environment, so that proficient reading accuracy and final text comprehension can be achieved [14,81,82,83,84,85,86].
Consequently, and considering the central role that reading plays in academic development, it is not surprising that inhibitory skills and working memory exhibit a positive relationship with academic performance [12,22,26,32,47,51,77,87,88]. Additionally, a relationship has been observed between poor performance on tasks requiring executive functions management and academic and learning difficulties [89,90], and vice versa [91].

1.4. Training of Executive Functions and Effects on Reading Skills

Given the above reported relationships between working memory, inhibitory control, and different types of reading skills, such as phonological processes [92,93], lexical processes [94,95], morpho-syntactic processes [93], reading fluency [60], or comprehension of written texts [17,60], numerous studies have recently explored whether the potential benefits of executive function training can also be transferred to other untrained skills of interest, such as academic skills and reading [22,35,36].
Authors such as Melby-Lervag et al. [35,36], Sala and Gobet [37], Shipstead et al. [96], and Simons et al. [39], following Mayer [97], categorize the effects derived from cognitive stimulation programs into two types of transfer. On the one hand, they define near transfer as the effects found in untrained cognitive processes that are underlying and closely related to trained skills. On the other hand, far transfer refers to post-training effects found in tasks tapping into skills that were not directly trained.
As an example of the latter, some studies find benefits in reading skills in typically developing children after receiving training in working memory or inhibitory control [15,31,98,99,100,101]. However, there is a discrepancy in the findings, as authors such as Astle et al. [102], Dunning et al. [42], Hardy et al. [103], and Roberts et al. [104] claim that executive function stimulation interventions do not show far transfer effects on reading comprehension or on other skills necessary to achieve this comprehension effectively, such as word decoding [37] or reading accuracy [48,81] and reading fluency [33,81,103].

1.5. The Present Study

Multiple reviews and meta-analyses have been released in the past few years, reporting the associations between executive functions and reading abilities, as well as the impact of various training programs on these skills. Specifically, these reviews have focused on the relationships between executive functions and reading skills in typically developing children [16,32,49,87,90,105], as well as the associations between executive functions and reading abilities in children with learning difficulties [17,89] and/or neuropsychological disorders [17,106]. Similarly, various review studies address the impact of working memory training on the reading performance of children and adolescents [18,22,35,36,37,38,107], the effects of reading skills training on executive functions [108], and the outcomes of mixed interventions targeting both working memory and reading skills on these abilities [109].
However, to the best of our knowledge, no recent study has examined the impact of working memory and inhibitory control training on reading skills in children without any type of difficulty and/or neurodevelopmental disorder. Thus, the main objective of this review is to examine and analyse the scientific literature published in the last 6 years regarding the impact of cognitive stimulation programs on the development of working memory and/or inhibitory control, and the potential transfers that may be observed to reading skills in children aged 6 to 12 years, globally and comprehensively.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sample

This review study was conducted following the criteria described by Moher et al. [110] in Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA, cited by Aksayli [22]; Conesa et al. [111]; Melby-Lervag et al. [35,36]; Sala and Gobet [37,38]. Of the 3567 studies published between 2017 and 2022 in Web of Science [112], Scopus [113], and ERIC [114], a total of 16 were selected, all of them indexed in Web of Science [112], because of redundancy with other databases (see Figure 1 for a detailed description following a PRISMA flowchart).

2.2. Procedure and Analysis

Different key concepts were collected for the search in different thesauri, resulting in the search strategy with the following terms: (read* OR comprehension) AND (“execut* function*” OR “execut* control” OR “cogniti* skill*” OR “cogniti* function*” OR “cogniti* abilit*” OR “cogniti* control” OR “inhibit* control” OR inhibit* OR “self-regulat*” OR “working memory” OR “operat* memory”) AND (child* OR “school age*” OR “early ages” OR adolescent* OR teen*) AND (“training” OR “intervention”).
With this search strategy, between November and December 2022, a total of 3567 English-language papers published in the last 6 years were found: 1333 studies indexed in the Web of Science database [112], 894 in Scopus [113], and 1340 in ERIC [114]. These studies were incorporated into a shared folder from the Google Drive platform, so that all authors were able to review and manage the incorporated research papers.
Subsequently, the inclusion principles were elaborated by all authors following the PICO model described by Booth [115] for population, intervention, comparison, and outcomes. Participants in this study had to be children aged 6 to 12 years attending elementary (primary) school with typical development. This age range was chosen because, although executive functions develop throughout life [28,116], during this period there is an observable cognitive growth and frontal lobe activity in children [117,118], which gradually decreases later [82,119,120]. Similarly, it is also the approximate age at which children have developed the cognitive skills necessary to make sense of grapho-phonetic decoding and manage it automatically, so it is recommended to start the reading acquisition process in this period, always considering the maturational and neuropsychological development of children [108,121]. In addition, as inclusion criteria, we only considered peer-reviewed and empirical studies that addressed an active intervention in working memory and/or inhibitory control, and collected specific measures focused on the evaluation related to children’s reading skills.
Following removal of duplicates (n = 138), in the next step of the review, titles, abstracts, keywords, and the full text of the found studies were thoroughly and independently read by two authors (CR-R and EA), excluding those that did not meet the selected inclusion and exclusion criteria (n = 3413). Discrepancies regarding the inclusion or exclusion of the selected studies were discussed with a third author (JAD), who confirmed the final selection of the selected studies. Additionally, the bibliographic references included in the selected works were reviewed for the possibility of including new studies, although all those that met the inclusion criteria were previously selected. Therefore, the final compilation of articles for this review consisted of 16 experimental or quasi-experimental quantitative studies indexed in Web of Science [112].
Finally, the selected studies were categorized and coded in data sheets using Microsoft Excel [122] and IBM SPSS version 22 [123]. The data were organized according to the following categories: methodological variables, linked to the sample sizes of the selected studies and the instruments used for data collection; temporal and demographic variables, addressing the publication year of each article and the country in which each study was conducted; and categorical variables, related to the main findings found, which were coded based on the type of intervention and training tasks performed by the participants, as well as the type of transfer found after the interventions.

3. Results

Following the method described in the previous section, a total of 16 articles were selected, six of which implemented training in verbal and visuospatial working memory [44,53,124,125,126,127], one focused on verbal working memory interventions [33], two specifically targeted visuospatial working memory development [34,128], and two stimulated central working memory or did not specify the specific working memory skill they targeted [70,129]. It is noteworthy that two studies conducted combined training of working memory, inhibitory control, and cognitive flexibility [130] or working memory and inhibitory control [19], and only one of the selected studies addressed inhibitory control training individually [34]. Similarly, two of the selected studies carried out combined training of executive functions and reading processes simultaneously [62,71], while only one combined training in working memory with mathematical skills [101]. Finally, 13 of the selected studies opted for a quasi-experimental design, while only three studies followed an experimental design, which can ensure greater internal validity [131].

3.1. Measurement Instruments

3.1.1. Measurement of Executive Functions

Measurement of working memory: Different assessment instruments for working memory were identified in the selected studies, such as the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) [132,133,134,135] or the Automated Working Memory Assessment (AWMA) developed by Alloway [136].
The WISC scale, designed for children aged 6 to 16 [137,138], was used in four of the 12 selected studies that assessed working memory [53,101,126,129]. It has been adapted to different formats and translated into different languages [139,140,141,142,143]. This scale includes 10 primary subtests and seven secondary subtests in its latest edition, with a reliability index ranging from 0.86 to 0.95 and a validity coefficient ranging from 0.65 to 0.89 [137,144]. The most commonly used WISC tasks in recent years for working memory assessment are Digit Span [53,101,126,129], a test that requires the repetition of number sequences in the same order (short-term memory) or in reverse order (working memory), with a test–retest reliability coefficient of 0.83 in its fourth edition [129]; Visuospatial Span [53], where a sequence of blocks is presented, and children are asked to repeat the sequence in the same order (short-term memory) or in reverse order (working memory), with a test–retest reliability of 0.87 [103,145]; and Letter-Number Sequencing [53,129], where a sequence of numbers and letters is presented, and participants must first recall the numbers in ascending order and then the letters in alphabetical order, with a test–retest reliability of 0.83 [129].
Measurement of inhibitory control: Regarding the instruments used in the five studies assessing inhibitory control, the Stroop colour-word interference test [146] or similar tests [34,53,62] was one of the most commonly employed. In this test, colour names are presented under congruent conditions (the written word matches the colour of the ink, such as the word “red” in red ink) and incongruent conditions (words appear written in a different colour, for example, the word “blue” written in red), requiring the participant either to read the written word or to name the colour in which the word is written, ignoring what is written. The difference in reaction time in both conditions is considered an index of inhibitory skills [52,147,148,149,150]. The Go/NoGo, which is widely used [34,101], involves participants pressing a button when presented with a specific target stimulus among a set of 10 stimuli, while refraining from pressing the button when presented with a non-target stimulus [101]. A variant of this test is the Stop Signal task, which requires pressing a key to the right or left in response to a green circle appearing on the right or left side of the screen and refraining from pressing any key when the circle appears in red [53].

3.1.2. Measurement of Reading Skills

Significant heterogeneity was identified in the assessment batteries and scales employed by the authors of the selected studies. One of the two articles that investigated reading accuracy assessment [62,101] used the Test of Word Reading Efficiency (TOWRE) [62], developed by Torgesen et al. [151], and intended for participants aged 6 to 18 years with a Cronbach’s alpha above 0.70. This test provides reliability ranging from 0.89 to 0.97 depending on the age of the participants evaluated [125,152]. The remaining study used the Revised Battery for the Assessment of Reading Processes (Batería de Evaluación de los Procesos Lectores, Revisada, PROLEC-R, in Spanish) [101], designed by Cuetos et al. [56], applicable to children aged 6 to 12 years for the measurement of children’s reading processes, yielding a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.79.
In addition, of the 16 articles composing this review, 6 of them measured reading speed [19,34] and reading fluency [19,33,62,125,128]. Although no battery for speed and/or fluency reading stands out in this selection, Ralph et al. [125] employed the reading fluency subtask of the TOWRE test, mentioned above. The said test was also used in one of the studies [62] to assess phonological awareness, a skill evaluated in 3 of the 16 studies in this review [33,62,101]. Specifically, the PROLEC-R battery [101], the TOWRE test [62], or tasks designed specifically by the authors for the study [33] were used.
Finally, reading comprehension was the most notable variable in this sample, as it was assessed in 13 of the 16 selected articles [19,34,44,53,62,70,71,124,125,126,127,129,130]. Although in this case no specific battery stands out in terms of frequency of use within the selected articles, it is noteworthy for its high internal consistency the use of several tests. On the one hand, the Wechsler Individual Achievement Test (WIAT-II), employed by Jones et al. [44] in this review and designed by Wechsler [153], allows the assessment of academic performance in individuals aged 4 to 85 years with a test–retest reliability ranging between 0.85 and 0.98, depending on the age of the participants evaluated. On the other hand, the Test of Reading Comprehension for Students in 1st to 6th grade (Ein Leseverständnistest für Erst-bis Sechstklässler ELFE 1–6) [154], used by Johann and Karbach [34], reports a total Cronbach’s alpha between 0.91 and 0.97 based on the sub-scales of word comprehension, sentence comprehension, and text comprehension, in addition to reading speed. Finally, the Progressive Linguistic Complexity Test (CLP) by Alliende et al. [64] was implemented in the study by Reina-Reina et al. [130] for measuring reading comprehension, reporting a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.97.

3.2. Transfer of Working Memory Intervention

Eleven publications focused on the cognitive stimulation of working memory, as described in Table 1, and two were conducted in Argentina, while the rest were from other countries, including: Germany (n = 1); Australia (n = 1); Brazil (n = 1); China (n = 1); Denmark (n = 1); United States (n = 1); Italy (n = 1); United Kingdom (n = 1); and Sweden (n = 1). Concerning sample sizes, only three of these articles exceeded 100 participants, four had between 50 and 99 participants, and the rest had samples ranging from 7 to 47 children and adolescents. Moreover, eight of these studies implemented training in classrooms or educational centres, two carried out stimulation programs in the children’s homes, and one conducted intervention in psychological laboratories or clinics. Table 1 presents in detail the characteristics of the sample of participants in each of the studies, as well as the research design, assessment instruments used, intervention location, duration, frequency, and conditions of training, as well as the results obtained by the authors, fidelity indicators, and effect size of the interventions.
These interventions varied in their findings: authors such as Artuso et al. [70], Johann and Karbach [31], Jones et al. [44], Lopez and Aran [53], Studer-Luethi et al. [128], and Vernucci et al. [127] found near-transfer benefits of computer-based working memory training on tasks assessing verbal and visuospatial working memory both immediately after training, with moderate (d = 0.52–0.714) [126,127] and large effect sizes (d = 0.80–1.21) [31,70], and 3 months [34,44] or 6 months after training [127]. In fact, some authors who explored the effect size of the results they obtained after training detailed how the benefits of training turn from moderate (d = 0.714) to large (d = 0.819) in the follow-up measure [127]. The opposite is the case in the study by Studer-Luethi et al. [128], as the results gradually decreased with the different training measures applied. On the other hand, Sondergaard and de Lopez [126] implemented a non-computerized, face-to-face intervention aimed at developing verbal and visuospatial working memory capacities, finding effects only in visuospatial working memory measures right after training, with a moderate effect size (g = 0.52), which did not persist 12 months after the intervention (d = 0.15). Siu et al. [33] implemented a more specific intervention, focusing only on verbal working memory, obtaining benefits in these abilities. However, studies such as those conducted by Hitchcock and Westwell [129] or Novaes et al. [124] did not find any effect on working memory compared to the control group or the placebo program, despite implementing interventions of similar duration to the previous studies.
When analysing the 11 selected studies implementing interventions focused on working memory stimulation, only one explored far-transfer effects on inhibitory control [53]. However, these authors did not find post-training transfer effects on this variable.
Similarly, of the four articles reviewing reading speed and fluency, only Siu et al. [33] reported gains after the intervention. In contrast, no significant differences were found after training in reading fluency or speed in three articles [34,124,125,128].
However, some authors did not calculate the concrete effect size of the training implemented, so the measures obtained were based exclusively on the psychometric evaluations applied [33,44,53,124,129].
Finally, it is worth noting that a considerable number of studies investigated the transfer to reading comprehension post-training, as this variable was considered in nine of the selected articles. Indeed, the transfer of working memory training to reading comprehension has been found in some articles [70], with a moderate effect size (d = 0.60), but numerous studies fail to find significant post-training effect on this variable [34,44,53,124,125,126,127,129].

3.3. Transfer of Inhibitory Control Intervention

Of the studies that conducted a cognitive stimulation program aimed at inhibitory control development, only one of them did so exclusively [34], as detailed in Table 2. This study was conducted in Germany, with 152 children who participated from home. Table 2 presents in detail the characteristics of the sample of participants in each of the studies, as well as the research design, assessment instruments used, intervention location, duration, frequency, and conditions of training, as well as the results obtained by the authors, fidelity indicators and effect size of the interventions.
In this research, the impact of a game-based inhibitory control training program was compared with that of a standard intervention in typically developing children. Both interventions show a significant impact on both trained and untrained measures of inhibitory control, with a large effect size (d = 1.09–2.47). However, only the children who participated in the game-based training showed differences compared to the control group in reading speed after training (d = 0.58), as well as in reading speed (d = 1.09) and sentence comprehension (d = 0.63) 3 months after training. Notably, the reading speed performance of children who participated in the game-based inhibitory control training significantly increased 3 months after the intervention, with large effect size benefits [34].

3.4. Transfer of Combined Working Memory and Inhibitory Control Intervention

Of the retrieved articles, only two focused on the combined training of working memory and inhibitory control, as indicated in Table 3. These studies were conducted in Brazil (n = 1) and Spain (n = 1), both with a sample size exceeding 100 participants, who performed the intervention in their classrooms and educational centres. Table 3 presents in detail the characteristics of the sample of participants in each of the studies, the research design, assessment instruments used, intervention location, duration, frequency, and conditions of training, as well as the results obtained by the authors, fidelity indicators, and effect size of the interventions.
As a result of the implementation of these mixed training programs, positive results in near-transfer effects can be observed, reporting medium and large benefits after training in verbal working memory (d = 0.53) and inhibitory control (d = 0.99) in children from disadvantaged socioeconomic contexts. However, the impact of this type of training does not transfer to visuospatial working memory abilities [19].
Far transfer can also be observed in these variables, as after-training improvements can be observed in reading speed and fluency in children from low socioeconomic contexts, with moderate effect sizes (d = 0.61), but not in children from high socioeconomic contexts [19]. Benefits in reading comprehension are also found [130], with moderate effect sizes (ηp2 = 0.071), after a controlled intervention with a validated commercial computerized cognitive training platform (CogniFit Inc., San Francisco, CA, USA). However, Weissheimer et al. [19] did not find a significant impact of the training program on reading comprehension in children after training, regardless of the socioeconomic context of the participants.
In addition, Reina-Reina et al. [130] explored the inter-individual differences that participants showed after completing the computerized cognitive training. The only covariate that modulated the effect of intervention on executive functions was the age of the participants, indicating that the effects of training were more significant in the younger population.

3.5. Transfer of Combined Intervention in Executive Functions and Academic Skills

Finally, among the 16 empirical studies included in this review, three focused on combined training, stimulating executive functions, and either reading or mathematical skills, as described in Table 4. These studies were conducted in American (n = 1), Italian (n = 1), and Spanish (n = 1) populations. Additionally, two of these studies were implemented in the educational centres of the participants, and the remaining one conducted the training program in their homes. Table 4 presents in detail the characteristics of the sample of participants in each of the studies, as well as the research design, assessment instruments used, intervention location, duration, frequency, and conditions of training, as well as the results obtained by the authors, fidelity indicators, and effect size of the interventions.
Carretti et al. [71] and Horowitz-Kraus et al. [62] explored the near-transfer effects post-training of a program aimed at developing working memory and reading comprehension, as well as a similar program combined with phonological-based training. After the implementation of these interventions, near-transfer effects are observed in both variables after the intervention [62,71], as well as 2 months after this implementation [71].
Similarly, children who participated in a training program for working memory and mathematical skills in the study by Sanchez-Perez et al. [101] showed an increase in inhibitory control scores obtained after training.
Regarding transfer to untrained skills, interventions focused on working memory and reading comprehension showed a positive impact on reading accuracy and fluency after training [62]. Additionally, Carretti et al. [71] confirmed the results obtained using Cohen’s d [121]. Thus, the experimental group showed a medium effect (d = 0.76) on reading comprehension performance after training, which became large (d = 1.75) two months after the intervention. However, the active control group only showed a small effect size for these measures. On the other hand, although Horowitz-Kraus et al. [62] did not calculate effect sizes in their intervention results, they supported the measurements of cognitive assessments with neuroimaging evaluations. Thus, they reported that, although the three training groups—children with reading difficulties, participants with ADHD and reading difficulties, and the typically developing population—showed an increased activation of certain brain areas after training, the observed changes in the connectivity between neural circuits were different for each group. On the one hand, training had a direct positive effect on functional connectivity related to memory—para-hippocampal gyrus—and attention—right and left insula. On the other hand, children with reading difficulties only showed a post-training change in the neural circuits of the right and left insula, where attentional skills partially take place, although they did not show increased functional connectivity after training in brain areas related to executive functions, such as the right and left superior frontal lobe. Finally, participants with ADHD and reading difficulties demonstrated a post-training change in executive function related areas—right and left superior frontal gyrus—and in attention related neural circuits—right and left insula—that were more functionally connected [62].
Finally, it is noteworthy that children who collaborated in the study by Sanchez-Perez et al. [101], who participated in an intervention based on executive functions and mathematical skills, showed benefits in reading speed and accuracy after training. Although these authors did not calculate the effect size of the intervention on untrained skills, they applied several multiple linear regression analyses to test for specific effects of the training activities. In this way, they reported a significant contribution of the working memory component of the intervention, as opposed to the mathematical tasks in the training, to benefits in reading skills and inhibitory control in typically developing children who participated in the intervention [101].

4. Discussion

The main objective of this study was to examine and analyse the recent findings regarding the impact of working memory and/or inhibitory control stimulation in typically developing children aged 6 to 12 years, as well as the transfer of this type of training to untrained skills, such as reading abilities.

4.1. Working Memory Interventions

Recent studies evidence a positive impact of working memory training on trained cognitive skills [19,33,34,44,53,70,126,127,128,129]. These results are consistent with previous findings, in which near-transfer effects were observed after training in children between 6 and 12 years of age [31,48,99,100,102,104] and preschool-aged participants [42,197].
The transfer from working memory training to inhibitory control was not as evident. Recent studies have not reported such transfer in typically developing children [19,34,53], aligning with previous results in children with ADHD [100,197].
In fact, authors such as Butterfuss and Kendeou [17], Chang [61], Kieffer et al. [198], Klingberg et al. [199,200], Miyake et al. [2,20], and Roughan and Hadwin [201] justify that inhibitory control and working memory are reciprocally related. In other words, in working memory tasks, inhibitory control is necessary to filter out irrelevant information. Similarly, to maintain attention in a task, the working memory is necessary to retain the required information for task completion.
More specifically, the selected studies revealed gains from working memory training in phonological skills [33], reading accuracy or speed [19,33,124], and reading comprehension [70,124,125] in typically developing children. These findings are consistent with previous studies that have reported similar results [31,98,99], asserting the close relationship between working memory and reading [29,32,48,76,77].
Nevertheless, it is possible that there was a sleeper effect on reading skills [46,99,202], with benefits reported only in the long term, but not immediately after training. It is worth mentioning, however, that in the study by Ralph et al. [125] participants in the control group dropped out of the experiment after post-test evaluations. Given that reading exhibits a trajectory of gradual improvement, it is possible that the control groups in these studies would have also demonstrated these improvements in the follow-up phase [125].
Despite these findings, other recent intervention studies have reported that improvements are only observed in trained working memory tasks, but not in tasks that would not have been part of the intervention, such as reading skills [34,104,125] or reading comprehension [19,34,44,53,126,127,129]. These studies are in line with previous research conducted by Astle et al. [102], Dunning et al. [42], and Roberts et al. [101] (see also meta-analyses [35,36,37,38]).
The lack of convergence in the transfer of working memory training on reading performance is justified in some studies by the influence of the compensation effect [19,34,53,70,71,124,127,129], in which greater development of cognitive skills and reading performance is observed in children who presented poor performance before participating in the intervention [18,100,203,204,205,206,207]. This factor could also explain the absence of results in some meta-analyses that analysed participants as a group, making it difficult to observe the compensation effect, as seen in studies conducted by Melby-Lervag et al. [35,36], or Sala and Gobet [37,38].
Furthermore, the effectiveness of cognitive stimulation interventions on reading skills may be related to the use of linguistic materials [33]. Non-linguistic tools, such as digits, shapes, or images, used in some interventions here [19,44], do not promote an increase in post-training reading performance. Therefore, further research is needed to thoroughly examine the role of linguistic components in cognitive stimulation programs, as the presence of linguistic materials does not always predict a positive impact on verbal working memory or reading performance in children [35,126].
Another possible factor contributing to this lack of convergence may be attributed to the use of different reading comprehension assessment batteries. Measurement tools that incorporate images to guide responses may impose low cognitive demands, which might not allow researchers to observe the possible benefits of training [126]. In this regard, it has been argued that reading comprehension measurement tasks that require complex inferences about the text might also require cognitive capacities related to working memory [16,189,208].
Similarly, the relevance of participants’ training times should be highlighted. Authors such as Jones et al. [44] and Sondergaard and de Lopez [126] report that participants’ limited adherence to the intervention hampers the far transfer to skills related to inhibitory control and children’s reading performance, in line with Hardy et al. [103], Klingberg et al. [199], Loosli et al. [31], or Thorell et al. [197]. These results also coincide with the established minimum threshold of at least 10 training sessions over a period of 5 to 8 weeks [103].

4.2. Inhibitory Control Interventions

Among the studies analysing the impact of inhibitory control stimulation programs in typically developing children, Johann and Karbach [34] compared the effects of gamified executive function training versus standard training. These authors concluded that both types of training impact the performance in trained and untrained inhibitory control tasks similarly, consistent with previous evidence [197,209].
However, no transfer of inhibitory control intervention to tasks related to working memory was found [34], in line with previous interventions [197]. Possibly, this lack of benefits may be due to the fact that gamified interventions necessarily include different tasks in each session, which may prevent transfer from one domain of executive function to another [210].
Regarding the far transfer of this type of stimulation program to reading skills, Johann and Karbach [34] found benefits of computerized inhibitory control training on reading speed in the short and long term. These results support findings showing the relationship between inhibitory control and phonological skills and decoding [16,211,212], reading fluency or accuracy [50,213], or reading comprehension [50,61,198].
In the case of Johann and Karbach’s study [34], it is possible that there was also a sleeper effect on reading comprehension demonstrated by children after gamified training, since gains were only observed 3 months after the intervention, and not immediately [46,99,202].
Similarly, Johann and Karbach [34] observed a possible compensatory effect [18,19,71,100,129,203,204,205,206,207], as the analysed population with lower performance in pre-test measures demonstrated greater gains post-intervention.
Although the benefits of computerized inhibitory control stimulation can be observed in reading skills, these results have not been found in recent studies implementing standard cognitive interventions [34]. These findings are consistent with similar research in which executive functions were trained in isolation, limiting the transfer to other complex cognitive tasks such as reading [214]. In this sense, some authors argue that the use of gamified cognitive stimulation programs may increase the necessary cognitive demand and, therefore, facilitate distant transfer to academic skills [34].
Along the same lines, authors such as Diamond [215] and Roughan and Hadwin [201] report that there are some factors supporting the improvement of executive functions in cognitive interventions, such as perceived joy, feelings of social belonging, support, and self-efficacy experienced during training. These factors are related to increased adaptability, discipline, and concentration, which significantly increase in gamified stimulation compared to standard interventions. This could explain why only the game-based training group demonstrated better long-term reading speed performance compared to the standard training group or the control group.

4.3. Combined Interventions in Working Memory and Inhibitory Control

Regarding far transfer effects, children showed benefits in speed, fluency [19], and reading comprehension [130] after the intervention. These results align with prior studies that underscore the fundamental role of inhibitory control [50,213], attention [216], and working memory [31,207] in children’s reading processes. In contrast to the results reported by Reina-Reina et al. [130] and Weissheimer et al. [19], no benefits were found for inhibitory control and reading comprehension after the applied intervention.
However, it is important to consider that participants from a more disadvantaged socioeconomic background demonstrate a greater impact of cognitive stimulation programs on executive functions and reading skills [19,130]. Interestingly, these results are in line with previous research conducted with children and adolescents, which explored the effects observed in participants of different socioeconomic status [217,218]. Together, these findings suggest that cognitive stimulation protocols may be effective in partially counteracting these social differences [101,219].

4.4. Combined Interventions in Executive Functions and Academic Skills

Several studies have also examined the impact of combined programs focused on the development of working memory and reading comprehension. Near transfers are present in trained skills in typically developing children [71], supporting previous contributions under similar conditions [192].
Similarly, these mixed working memory and reading comprehension interventions also seem to produce far transfer in short and long-term measures, in reading speed and accuracy, as well as in inhibitory control, in typically developing children, children with reading difficulties, or participants with ADHD and reading difficulties [62].
Carretti et al. [71] and Horowitz-Kraus et al. [62] also report the relevant role that the compensation effect can play in the analysed children, where participants who initially showed poor performance obtained greater benefits in the short and long term [18,19,100,129,203,204,205,206,207].
Interventions stimulating working memory and mathematical skills have also been implemented in recent years [101], showing near benefits in central working memory and far transfers to reading skills and inhibitory control in typically developing children. Interestingly, Sanchez-Perez et al. [101] found that post-training reading benefits were significantly related to performance in working memory rather than mathematical tasks, in line with prior results such as those reported by Cain et al. [29], Engle et al. [76], Holmes et al. [48], Peng et al. [32], Shiran and Breznitz [79], and Swanson and Howell [77].
However, in Carretti et al. [71], Horowitz-Kraus et al. [62], and Sanchez-Perez et al. [101], the combined training activities were not independent, so it was difficult to determine which of the trained skills had a greater impact. In line with these authors, a recent study implemented an intervention focused on working memory and phonological skills independently [81].

4.5. Limitations and Future Research

Some limitations encountered during the systematic review process are worth mentioning. First, our work relied exclusively on studies that analysed the performance of typically developing children, as seen in some recent reviews [17,22,35,37,38]. Therefore, it would be interesting for future review studies to consider the impact of cognitive stimulation protocols in diverse clinical populations, such as in the work of Church et al. [89], Farah et al. [106], Horowitz-Kraus [109], Melby-Lervag et al. [36], Peng et al. [49], Swanson and Orosco [105], and Titz and Karbach [18], to determine how much training influence children with different neuropsychological characteristics.
Second, the conclusions presented here are limited by the intrinsic characteristics of systematic reviews. To delve further into the explored topics, a meta-analysis could be very informative in the future, as the statistical component would allow for a quantitative perspective to synthesise and generalise the evidence reported here—in the form, for example, of a summary effect size.
Finally, and related to the previous point, only 7 out of the 16 selected studies (i.e., less than half) reported the size of the concrete effect on the dependent variables [19,34,70,71,126,127,130]. For this reason and given that the results were measured only through psychometric assessments, it was difficult to robustly determine the impact of cognitive stimulation on reading skills in half of the sample [33,44,53,62,101,124,125,128,129]. Therefore, including a larger sample of studies in future reviews could be a potential way to obtain a more reliable assessment of the impact of executive function training programmes on different reading skills. However, despite the absence of such statistical analyses, some of the selected studies compared their results with active control groups [19,44,71,125,126,127,128,129], or supported their findings with neuroimaging data [62]. This type of research design allows the detection of possible confounding variables that could influence the results obtained, which greatly increases the consistency and degree of internal validity of the reported findings.
In addition,, and taking into account the limitations of the articles selected for this review, future neuroimaging studies exploring the possible far transfer of executive function stimulation programs would be very informative, and helpful in determining the specific role these trainings play in increasing cognitive capacities [62,102,220,221,222,223].
As the main limitation of the studies selected for this review, it is worth mentioning that the sample sizes in Artuso et al. [70], Jones et al. [44], Pasqualotto et al. [81], Siu et al. [33] were relatively small compared to some previous studies [104]. However, moderate changes from the Cogmed program can be detected with 12 participants per group [145].
Moreover, some studies selected report limitations, such as having only a passive control group [33,70,101] or a placebo or active control group with non-adaptive training [126]. Interventions in which participants are assigned to three independent groups allow testing of the data obtained under different experimental conditions. In line with this, some contributions, such as those made by Carretti et al. [71] or Hitchcock and Westwell [129], enable, on the one hand, verification of whether certain factors can act as extraneous variables, such as attention directed towards the experimenter or expectations reached in training [224]. On the other hand, they allow checking for any placebo effect in the applied assessment measures. For example, scores from questionnaires directed to teachers and families may be influenced by their perception of children’s development [103].
It is worth mentioning that, due to the possible sleeper effect that may manifest in the development of reading skills after cognitive stimulation interventions [46,99,202], some studies incorporated in this review emphasize the importance of conducting longitudinal studies with short- and long-term assessments to confirm if these effects may appear months after training [19,70,101].
Finally, sometimes difficulties may arise in distinguishing between the specific effects of cognitive stimulation interventions combined with some other type of academic training. Therefore, studies implementing combined training might want to consider applying the interventions independently, at least for some period during the research, as seen in Pasqualotto and Venuti [81].
For all these reasons, we strongly believe that future research delving into cognitive stimulation programs in school populations would benefit from considering the limitations reported in the literature published to date. This way, the intrusion of extraneous variables that may occur in empirical research could potentially be largely avoided, obtaining the highest possible consistency and internal and external validity.

5. Conclusions

The objective of this research was to review and analyse the recently published findings regarding the impact of working memory and/or inhibitory control stimulation in children aged 6 to 12, as well as the transfer of this type of training to untrained skills such as reading abilities.
Although, at present, there is still some lack of convergence in the impact of executive function training on reading skills, half of the selected articles report that there has been a compensatory effect in the results obtained. Thus, it seems that there is an evident positive impact of such interventions on the reading skills of children showing some difficulty related to executive functions and/or reading abilities.
Interestingly, in some circumstances, a sleeper effect occurs, and these benefits are not visible until a few months after the training. Therefore, longitudinal analyses exploring possible long-term gains from these trainings would be interesting, although this is uncommon.
It is worth noting that, in the studies reporting far benefits of executive function training in reading skills of children without any difficulty, either linguistic components are included in materials, or a compensatory effect has occurred. Similarly, the use of combined training programs of executive functions and academic skills is advised, so that the trained tasks are more numerous, and the potential effects of far transfer are favoured. Integrated interventions in the school routine combined with a phonological, mathematical, or reading focus alongside executive function training seem to be significant for typically developing children.
Finally, it would be convenient to address some of the common limitations found in recent studies. This would minimize the intrusion of extraneous variables that may influence the reported results, and thus obtain the highest possible consistency and internal and external validity.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.R.-R., E.A. and J.A.D.; methodology, C.R.-R.; investigation, C.R.-R.; data curation, C.R.-R.; writing—original draft preparation, C.R.-R.; writing—review and editing, C.R.-R. and E.A.; supervision, J.A.D. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was partially funded by PID2021-126884NB-I00 by the MCIN/AEI/10.13039/501100011033.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

All the articles analysed in this systematic review are indexed in the main scientific databases Web of Sciences and Scopus and are available in their corresponding scientific journals.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Diamond, A. Why assessing and improving executive functions early in life is critical. In Executive Function in Preschool Age Children: Integrating Measurement, Neurodevelopment, and Translational Research; Griffin, J.A., McCardle, P., Freund, L.S., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Worcester, MA, USA, 2016; pp. 11–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.; Emerson, M.; Witzki, A.; Howerter, A.; Wager, T.D. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latest variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 2000, 41, 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Hanakawa, T. Rostral premotor cortex as a gateway between motor and cognitive networks. Neurosci. Res. 2011, 70, 144–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Swaab, D. Somos Nuestro Cerebro; Plataforma Editorial: Barcelona, Spain, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  5. McNab, F.; Klingberg, T. Prefrontal cortex and basal ganglia control access to working memory. Nat. Neurosci. 2008, 11, 103–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Postle, B.R. Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain. Neuroscience 2006, 139, 23–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Tranel, D.; Grabowski, T.J.; Lyon, J.; Damasio, H. Naming the same entities from visual or from auditory stimulation engages similar regions of left inferotemporal cortices. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2005, 17, 1293–1305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Alloway, T.P. How does working memory work in the classroom? Educ. Res. Rev. 2006, 1, 134–139. [Google Scholar]
  9. Baddeley, A. Working Memory. Science 1992, 255, 556–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Baddeley, A. The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends Cogn. Sci. 2000, 4, 417–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Swanson, H.L.; Berninger, V. The Role of Working Memory in Skilled and Less Skilled Readers’ Comprehension. Intelligence 1995, 21, 83–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Diamond, A. Executive Functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Garcia-Madruga, J.A.; Vila, J.O.; Gomez-Veiga, I.; Duque, G.; Elosua, M.R. Executive processes, reading comprehension and academic achievement in 3rd grade primary students. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2014, 35, 41–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Kamza, A. Developmental patterns of relationships between inhibitory control and reading skill in early-school children. L1 Educ. Stud. Lang. Lit. 2017, 17, 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Johann, V.; Koenen, T.; Karbach, J. The unique contribution of working memory, inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and intelligence to reading comprehension and reading speed. Child Neuropsychol. 2018, 26, 324–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Best, J.R.; Miller, P.H.; Naglieri, J.A. Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2011, 21, 327–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Butterfuss, R.; Kendeou, P. The Role of Executive Functions in Reading Comprehension. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2018, 30, 801–826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Titz, C.; Karbach, J. Working memory and executive functions: Effects of training on academic achievement. Psychol. Res. 2014, 78, 852–868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Weissheimer, J.; Fujii, R.C.; Marques de Souza, J.G. The effects of cognitive training on executive functions and reading in typically developing children with varied socioeconomic status in Brazil. Ilha Desterro A J. Engl. Lang. Lit. Engl. Cult. Stud. 2019, 72, 85–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.P. The Nature and Organization of Individual Differences in Executive Functions: Four General Conclusions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 21, 8–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Ramos-Galarza, C.; Villegas, C.; Ortiz, D.; Garcia, A.; Bolanos, M.; Acosta, P.; Lepe, N.; Del Valle, M.; Ramos, V. Evaluación de las Habilidades de la Corteza Prefrontal: La Escala Efecto II-VC y II-VR. Rev. Ecuat. Neurol. 2018, 27, 36–42. [Google Scholar]
  22. Aksayli, N.D.; Sala, G.; Gobet, F. The cognitive and academic benefits of Cogmed: A meta-analysis. Educ. Res. Rev. 2019, 27, 229–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Alloway, T.P. Working memory, but not IQ, predicts subsequent learning in children with learning difficulties. Eur. J. Psychol. Assess. 2009, 25, 92–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Baddeley, A. Working memory and language: An overview. J. Commun. Disord. 2003, 36, 189–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Fuhs, M.W.; Farran, D.C.; Nesbitt, K.T. Prekindergarten children’s executive functioning skills and achievement gains: The utility of direct assessments and teacher ratings. J. Educ. Psychol. 2015, 107, 207–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Fuhs, M.W.; Nesbitt, K.T.; Farran, D.C.; Dong, N. Longitudinal Associations Between Executive Functioning and Academic Skills Across Content Areas. Dev. Psychol. 2014, 50, 1698–1709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Alloway, T.P.; Alloway, R.G. Investigating the predictive roles of working memory and IQ in academic attainment. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2010, 106, 20–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Altemeier, L.E.; Abbott, R.D.; Berninger, V.W. Executive functions for reading and writing in typical literacy development and dyslexia. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2008, 30, 588–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Cain, K.; Oakhill, J.; Bryant, P. Children’s reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. J. Educ. Psychol. 2004, 96, 31–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Gathercole, S.E.; Pickering, S.J. Assessment of working memory in six- and seven-year-old children. J. Educ. Psychol. 2000, 92, 377–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Loosli, S.V.; Buschkuehl, M.; Perrig, W.J.; Jaeggi, S.M. Working memory training improves reading processes in typically developing children. Child Neuropsychol. 2012, 18, 62–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Peng, P.; Fuchs, D.; Fuchs, L.S.; Elleman, A.M.; Kearns, D.M.; Gilbert, J.K.; Compton, D.L.; Cho, E.; Patton, S., III. A Longitudinal Analysis of the Trajectories and Predictors of Word Reading and Reading Comprehension Development Among At-Risk Readers. J. Learn. Disabil. 2018, 52, 195–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Siu, T.C.; McBride, C.; Tse, C.; Tong, X.; Maurer, U. Evaluating the Effects of Metalinguistic and Working Memory Training on Reading Fluency in Chinese and English: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 2510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Johann, V.E.; Karbach, J. Effects of game-based and standard executive control training on cognitive and academic abilities in elementary school children. Dev. Sci. 2020, 23, e12866. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Melby-Lervag, M.; Hulme, C. Is Working Memory Training Effective? A Meta-Analytic Review. Dev. Psychol. 2013, 49, 270–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Melby-Lervag, M.; Redick, T.S.; Hulme, C. Working Memory Training Does Not Improve Performance on Measures of Intelligence or Other Measures of “Far Transfer”: Evidence From a Meta-Analytic Review. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2016, 11, 512–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Sala, G.; Gobet, F. Working Memory Training in Typically Developing Children: A Meta-Analysis of the Available Evidence. Dev. Psychol. 2017, 53, 671–685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Sala, G.; Gobet, F. Working memory training in typically developing children: A multilevel meta-analysis. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2020, 27, 423–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Simons, D.J.; Boot, W.R.; Charness, N.; Gathercole, S.E.; Chabris, C.F.; Hambrick, D.Z.; Stine-Morrow, E.A.L. Do “Brain-Training” Programs Work? Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 2016, 17, 103–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Strobach, T.; Karbach, J. Cognitive Training: An Overview of Features and Applications; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  41. Blakey, E.; Carroll, D.J. A short executive function training program improves preschoolers’ working memory. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1827. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Dunning, D.L.; Holmes, J.; Gathercole, S.E. Does working memory training lead to generalized improvements in children with low working memory? A randomized controlled trial. Dev. Sci. 2013, 16, 915–925. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Holmes, J.; Gathercole, S.E. Taking working memory training from the laboratory into schools. Educ. Psychol. 2014, 34, 440–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Jones, J.S.; Milton, F.; Mostazir, M.; Adlam, A.R. The academic outcomes of working memory and metacognitive strategy training in children: A double-blind randomized controlled trial. Dev. Sci. 2020, 23, e12870. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Swanson, H.L.; Alloway, T.P. Working memory, learning, and academic achievement. In APA Educational Psychology Handbook; Harris, K.R., Graham, S., Urdan, T., McCormick, C.B., Sinatra, G.M., Sweller, J., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Worcester, MA, USA, 2012; Volume 1, pp. 327–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Avtzon, S.A. Effect of Neuroscience-based Cognitive skill Training on Growth of Cognitive Deficits Associated with Learning Disabilities in children Grades 2–4. Learn. Disabil. A Multidiscip. J. 2012, 18, 111–121. [Google Scholar]
  47. Fried, R.; Chan, J.; Feinberg, L.; Pope, A.; Woodworth, K.Y.; Faraone, S.V.; Biederman, J. Clinical correlates of working memory deficits in youth with and without ADHD: A controlled study. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2016, 38, 487–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Holmes, J.; Gathercole, S.E.; Dunning, D.L. Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children. Dev. Sci. 2009, 12, F9–F15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Peng, P.; Barnes, M.; Wang, C.; Wang, W.; Li, S.; Swanson, H.L.; Dardick, W.; Tao, S. A Meta-Analysis on the Relation Between Reading and Working Memory. Psychol. Bull. 2018, 144, 48–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Savage, R.; Cornish, K.; Manly, T.; Hollis, C. Cognitive processes in children’s reading and attention: The role of working memory, divided attention, and response inhibition. Br. J. Psychol. 2006, 97, 368–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Sjowall, D.; Bohlin, G.; Rydell, A.; Thorell, L.B. Neuropsychological deficits in preschool as predictors of ADHD symptoms and academic achievement in late adolescence. Child Neuropsychol. 2017, 23, 111–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Borella, E.; Carretti, B.; Pelgrina, S. The specific role of inhibition in reading comprehension in good and poor comprehenders. J. Learn. Disabil. 2010, 43, 541–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Lopez, M.; Aran, V. Transfer effects of working memory training on language and mathematics performance in school-aged children. Cuad. Neuropsicol.-Panam. J. Neuropsychol. 2021, 15, 97–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Gutierrez-Fresneda, R. El aprendizaje de la lectura, ¿cuál es el mejor momento para iniciar su enseñanza? In Lectura y Dificultades Lectoras en el Siglo XXI; Díez-Mediavilla, A., Gutierrez-Fresneda, R., Coords, Eds.; Octaedro: Quito, Ecuador, 2020; pp. 77–88. [Google Scholar]
  55. Martinez-Cubelos, J. Relationship between the Executive Functions, Phonological Awareness and Early Reading Performance in 1st Elementary Students. Educ. Y Futuro Digit. 2014, 10, 65–80. Available online: https://dialnet.unirioja.es/servlet/articulo?codigo=4998798 (accessed on 10 November 2023).
  56. Cuetos, F.; Rodriguez, B.; Ruano, E.; Arribas, D. PROLEC-R. Batería de Evaluación de Los Procesos Lectores, Revisada; TEA Ediciones S.A.: Madrid, Spain, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  57. De la Calle, A.M.; Guzman-Simon, F.; Garcia-Jimenez, E. Letter Knowledge and Learning Sequence of Graphemes in Spanish: Precursors of Early Reading. Rev. Psicodidáctica 2018, 23, 128–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. De la Calle, A.M. The prediction of early reading performance: A comparative perspective in Spanish and Chilean children. Rev. Complut. Educ. 2019, 30, 935–950. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Kuhn, M.; Schwanenflugel, P.; Meisinger, E.; Levy, B.; Rasinski, T. Aligning theory and assessment of reading fluency: Automaticity, prosody, and definitions of fluency. Read. Res. Q. 2010, 45, 230–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Gonzalez-Hernandez, K.; Otero, L.; Castro, A.M. Comprensión lectora, memoria de trabajo, fluidez y vocabulario en escolares cubanos. Actual. Investig. Educ. 2016, 16, 162–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Chang, I. Influences of executive function, language comprehension, and fluency on reading comprehension. J. Early Child. Res. 2019, 18, 44–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Horowitz-Kraus, T.; Hershey, A.; Kay, B.; DiFrancesco, M. Differential effect of reading training on functional connectivity in children with reading difficulties with and without ADHD comorbidity. J. Neurolinguist. 2019, 49, 93–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Amiama-Espaillat, C.; Mayor-Ruiz, C. Analysing the relationship between Reading fluency and Reading competence in Secondary Education. Ocnos. Rev. Estud. Sobre Lect. 2018, 17, 21–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Alliende, F.; Condemarin, M.; Milicic, N. Prueba CLP. Formas Paralelas. Manual Para la Aplicación de la Prueba de Comprensión Lectora de Complejidad Lingüística Progresiva: 8 Niveles de Lectura; CEPE: Auderghem, Belgium, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  65. Abusamra, V.; Cartoceti, R.; Raiter, A.; Ferreres, A. Una perspectiva cognitiva en el estudio de la comprensión de textos. Psico 2008, 39, 352–361. [Google Scholar]
  66. Canet-Juric, L.; Burin, D.; Andres, M.L.; Urquijo, S. Perfil cognitivo de niños con rendimientos bajos en comprensión lectora. An. Psicol. 2013, 29, 996–1005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Canet-Juric, L.; Urquijo, S.; Richards, M.M.; Burin, D. Predictores Cognitivos de Niveles de Comprensión Lectora Mediante Análisis Discriminante. Int. J. Psychol. Res. 2009, 2, 99–111. Available online: https://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=299023513003 (accessed on 10 November 2023). [CrossRef]
  68. Cartoceti, R.; Abusamra, V. El rol del mecanismo de actualización en la comprensión de textos. Rev. Neuropsicol. Latinoam. 2013, 5, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Gomez-Veiga, I.; Vila, J.O.; Garcia-Madruga, J.A.; Contreras, A.; Elosua, M.J. Reading comprehension and working memory’s executive processes. Psicol. Educ. 2013, 19, 103–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Artuso, C.; Carretti, B.; Palladino, P. Short-term training on working memory updating and metacognition in primary school: The effect on reading comprehension. Sch. Psychol. Int. 2019, 40, 641–657. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Carretti, B.; Borella, E.; Elosua, M.R.; Gomez-Veiga, I.; Garcia-Madruga, J.A. Improvements in Reading Comprehension Performance After a Training Program Focusing on Executive Processes of Working Memory. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 2017, 1, 268–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Grainger, J.; Lete, B.; Bertand, D.; Dufau, S.; Ziegler, J.C. Evidence for multiple routes in learning to read. Cognition 2012, 123, 280–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Fernandez-Herrera, N.; Bausela-Herreras, E. Relationship between Executive Functions and Processes of Decoding Grapheme-phoneme in Elementary School Children. Rev. Mex. Neurocienc. 2016, 17, 51–63. Available online: https://previous.revmexneurociencia.com/articulo/relacion-entre-las-funciones-ejecutivas-los-procesos-de-decodificacion-grafema-fonema-en-educacion-primaria/ (accessed on 10 November 2023).
  74. Ramus, F.; Rosen, S.; Dakin, S.C.; Day, B.L.; Castellote, J.M.; White, S.; Frith, U. Theories of developmental dyslexia: Insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain 2003, 126, 841–865. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Shankweiller, D.; Lundquist, E.; Katz, L.; Stuebing, K.K.; Fretcher, J.M.; Brady, S.; Fowler, A.; Dreyer, L.G.; Marchione, K.E.; Shaywitz, S.E.; et al. Comprehension and Decoding: Patterns of Association in Children with Reading Difficulties. Sci. Stud. Read. 2009, 3, 69–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Engle, R.W.; Cantor, J.; Carullo, J.J. Individual-Differences in Working Memory and Comprehension: A Test of 4 Hypotheses. J. Exp. Psychol.-Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1992, 18, 972–992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Swanson, H.L.; Howell, M. Working memory, short-term memory, and speech rate as predictors of children’s reading performance at different ages. J. Educ. Psychol. 2001, 93, 720–734. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Gioyagnoli, G.; Vicari, S.; Tomassetti, S.; Menghini, D. The Role of Visual-Spatial Abilities in Dyslexia: Age Differences in Children’s Reading? Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Shiran, A.; Breznitz, Z. The effect of cognitive training on recall range and speed of information processing in the working memory of dyslexic and skilled readers. J. Neurolinguist. 2011, 24, 524–537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Gathercole, S.E.; Willis, C.S.; Baddeley, A.D.; Emslie, H. The children’s test of nonword repetition. A test of phonological working memory. Memory 1994, 2, 103–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Pasqualotto, A.; Venuti, P. A Multifactorial Model of Dyslexia: Evidence from Executive Functions and Phonological-based Treatments. Learn. Disabil. Res. Pract. 2020, 35, 150–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Conners, F.A. Attentional control and the Simple View of reading. Read. Writ. 2009, 22, 591–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Juhasz, B.J.; Starr, M.S.; Inhoff, A.W.; Placke, L. The effects of morphology on the processing of compound words: Evidence from naming, lexical decisions and eye fixations. Br. J. Psychol. 2003, 94, 223–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Pylkkänen, L.; Feintuch, S.; Hopkins, E.; Marantz, A. Neural correlates of the effects of morphological family frequency and family size: An MEG study. Cognition 2004, 91, B35–B45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Shalev, L.; Tsal, Y.; Mevorach, C. Computerized Progressive Attentional Training (CPAT) Program: Effective Direct Intervention for Children with ADHD. Child Neuropsychol. 2007, 13, 382–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Shaywitz, S.E.; Shaywitz, B.A. Paying attention to reading: The neurobiology of reading and dyslexia. Dev. Psychopathol. 2008, 20, 1329–1349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Peng, P.; Namkung, J.; Barnes, M.; Sun, C. A Meta-Analysis of Mathematics and Working Memory: Moderating Effects of Working Memory Domain, Type of Mathematics Skill, and Sample Characteristics. J. Educ. Psychol. 2016, 108, 455–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Shaul, S.; Schwartz, M. The role of the executive functions in school readiness among preschool-age children. Read. Writ. 2014, 27, 749–768. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Church, J.A.; Cirino, P.L.; Miciak, J.; Juranek, J.; Vaughn, S.; Fletcher, J.M. Cognitive, Intervention, and Neuroimaging Perspectives on Executive Function in Children With Reading Disabilities. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 2019, 2019, 25–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Spiegel, J.A.; Goodrich, J.M.; Morris, B.M.; Osborne, C.M.; Lonigan, C.J. Relations Between Executive Functions and Academic Outcomes in Elementary School Children: A Meta-Analysis. Psychol. Bulettin 2021, 147, 329–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  91. Rodriguez, G.L.M.; Muntana, M.C.; Pros, R.C. Relationship of Executive Functioning and Metacognitive Processes with the Academic Performance in Primary Children. Rev. Complut. Educ. 2018, 29, 1059–1073. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Beneventi, H.; Tonnessen, F.E.; Ersland, L.; Hugdahl, K. Executive working memory processes in dyslexia: Behavioral and fMRI evidence. Scand. J. Psychol. 2010, 51, 192–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  93. Perez-Pereira, M.; Martinez-Lopez, Z.; Maneiro, L. Longitudinal Relationships Between Reading Abilities, Phonological Awareness, Language Abilities and Executive Functions: Comparison of Low Risk Preterm and Full-term Children. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  94. Currie, N.K.; Cain, K. Children’s inference generation. The role of vocabulary and working memory. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2015, 137, 57–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Florit, E.; Cain, K.; Mason, L. Going beyond children’s single-text comprehension: The role of fundamental and higher-level skills in 4th graders’ multiple-document comprehension. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2019, 90, 449–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Shipstead, Z.; Hicks, K.L.; Engle, R.M. Cogmed working memory training: Does the evidence support the claims? J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn. 2012, 1, 185–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Mayer, R.E. Information processing variables in learning to solve problems. Rev. Educ. Res. 1975, 45, 525–541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Alloway, T.P.; Alloway, R.G. The efficacy of working memory training in improving crystallized intelligence. Nat. Preced. 2009, 1, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Henry, L.A.; Messer, D.J.; Nash, G. Testing for Near and Far Transfer Effects with a Short, Face-to-Face Adaptive Working Memory Training Intervention in Typical Children. Infant Child Dev. 2013, 23, 84–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Karbach, J.; Strobach, T.; Schubert, T. Adaptive working-memory training benefits reading, but not mathematics in middle childhood. Child Neuropsychol. 2015, 21, 285–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Sanchez-Perez, N.; Castillo, A.; Lopez-Lopez, J.A.; Pina, V.; Puga, J.L.; Campoy, G.; Gonzalez-Salinas, C.; Fuentes, L.J. Computer-Based Training in Math and Working Memory Improves Cognitive Skills and Academic Achievement in Primary School Children: Behavioral Results. Front. Psychol. 2018, 8, 2327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Astle, D.E.; Barnes, J.J.; Baker, K.; Colclough, G.L.; Woolrich, M.W. Cognitive Training Enhances Intrinsic Brain Connectivity in Childhood. J. Neurosci. 2015, 35, 6277–6283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Hardy, S.J.; Bills, S.E.; Meier, E.R.; Schatz, J.C.; Keridan, K.J.; Wise, S.; Hardy, K.K. A Randomized Controlled Trial of Working Memory Training in Pediatric Sickle Cell Disease. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 2021, 46, 1001–1014. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Roberts, G.; Quach, J.; Spencer-Smith, M.; Anderson, P.J.; Gathercole, S.; Gold, L.; Sia, K.; Mensah, F.; Rickards, F.; Ainley, J.; et al. Academic Outcomes 2 Years After Working Memory Training for Children With Low Working Memory A Randomized Clinical Trial. JAMA Pediatr. 2016, 170, e154568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Swanson, H.L.; Orosco, M. Predictive validity of dynamic testing and working memory as it relates to reading growth in children with reading disabilities. In Assessment and Intervention (Advances in Learning and Behavioral Disabilities); Scruggs, T.E., Mastropieri, M.A., Eds.; Emerald Group Publishing Limited: Bingley, UK, 2011; Volume 24, pp. 1–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Farah, R.; Ionta, S.; Horowitz-Kraus, T. Neuro-Behavioral Correlates of Executive Dysfunctions in Dyslexia Over Development From Childhood to Adulthood. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 708863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Redick, T.S.; Shipstead, Z.; Wiemers, E.A.; Melby-Lervag, M.; Hulme, C. What’s Working in Working Memory Training? An Educational Perspective. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2015, 27, 617–633. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Peng, P.; Goodrich, J.M. The Cognitive Element Model of Reading Instruction. Read. Res. Q. 2020, 55, S77–S88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Horowitz-Kraus, T. The role of executive functions in the reading process. In Reading Fluency: Literacy Studies; Khateb, A., Bar-Kochva, I., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2016; Volume 12, pp. 51–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Moher, D.; Liberati, A.; Tetzlaff, J.; Altman, D.G.; PRISMA Grp. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. BMJ Br. Med. J. 2009, 339, b2535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Conesa, P.J.; Onandia-Hinchado, I.; Duñabeitia, J.A.; Moreno, M.A. Basic psychological needs in the classroom: A literature review in elementary and middle school students. Learn. Motiv. 2022, 79, 101819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Clarivate. Web of Science. Clarivate. 2016. Available online: https://clarivate.com/products/web-of-science/ (accessed on 10 November 2023).
  113. Elsevier. Scopus. 2021. Available online: www.scopus.com (accessed on 10 November 2023).
  114. Institute of Education Sciences. ERIC (EBSCO). 1993. Available online: https://eric.ed.gov/ (accessed on 10 November 2023).
  115. Booth, A. Using evidence in practice. Health Inf. Libr. J. 2011, 28, 87–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Diamond, A. Development of the Ability to Use Recall to Guide Action, as Indicated by Infants’ Performance on AB. Child Dev. 1985, 56, 868–883. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Moriguchi, Y.; Hiraki, K. Prefrontal cortex and executive function in young children: A review of NIRS studies. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 867. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Volckaert, A.M.S.; Noel, M. Training executive function in preschoolers reduce externalizing behaviors. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 2015, 4, 37–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Roebers, C.M.; Roethlisberger, M.; Neuenschwander, R.; Cimeli, P.; Michel, E.; Jaeger, K. The relation between cognitive and motor performance and their relevance for children’s transition to school: A latent variable approach. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2014, 33, 284–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Rodriguez, C.; Jimenez, J.E.; Diaz, A.; Garcia, E.; Martin, R.; Hernandez, S. Datos normativos para el Test de los Cinco Dígitos: Desarrollo evolutivo de la flexibilidad en Educación Primaria. Eur. J. Educ. Psychol. 2012, 5, 27–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Cohen, R.; Castillo, C.J. Aprendizaje Precoz de la Lectura, ¿a los 6 Años ya es Demasiado Tarde? Cincel: Ciudad de México, Mexico, 1982. [Google Scholar]
  122. Microsoft Corporation. Microsoft Excel; Version 365; Microsoft Corporation: Redmond, WA, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  123. IBM Company. Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS); Version 22; IBM Statistics: Armonk, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  124. Novaes, C.B.; Zuanetti, P.A.; Fukuda, M.T.H. Effects of working memory intervention on students with Reading comprehension difficulties. Rev. CEFAC Speech Lang. Hear. Sci. Educ. J. 2018, 21, 17918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Ralph, K.J.; Gibson, B.S.; Gondoli, D.M.; Sztybel, P.; Pauszek, J.R.; Miller, R.W.; Litzow, E. Targeting the Three Stages of Retrieval from Secondary Memory in a Double-Blinded, Placebo-Controlled, Randomized Working Memory Training Study. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 2017, 1, 455–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Sondergaard, H.B.; Lopez, K.M.J. Face-to-face working memory training does not enhance children’s reading comprehension-a pilot study with Danish children. Nord. Psychol. 2021, 73, 211–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Vernucci, S.; Canet-Juric, L.; Richard’s, M.M. Effects of working memory training on cognitive and academic abilities in typically developing school-age children. Psychol. Res. 2023, 87, 308–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  128. Studer-Luethi, B.; Toermaenen, M.; Margelisch, K.; Hogrefe, A.B.; Perrig, W.J. Effects of Working Memory Training on Children’s Memory and Academic Performance: The Role of Training Task Features and Trainee’s Characteristics. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 2022, 6, 340–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Hitchcock, C.; Westwell, M.S. A cluster-randomised, controlled trial of the impact of Cogmed Working Memory Training on both academic performance and regulation of social, emotional and behavioural challenges. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2017, 58, 140–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  130. Reina-Reina, C.; Conesa, P.J.; Duñabeitia, J.A. Impact of a cognitive stimulation program on the reading comprehension of children in primary education. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 985790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  131. Castejon, J.L. Introducción a los Métodos y Técnicas de Investigación y Obtención de Datos en Psicología; Ediciones Club Universitario: Alicante, Spain, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  132. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 3rd ed.; Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  133. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 4th ed.; Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  134. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 5th ed.; Pearson Assessment: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  135. Wechsler, D.; Naglieri, J.A. Wechsler Non-Verbal Scale of Ability; Harcourt Assessment: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  136. Alloway, T.P. Automated Working Memory Assessment (AWMA); Harcourt Assessment: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  137. Canivez, G.L.; Watkins, M.W.; Dombrowski, S.C. Structural validity of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Fifth Edition: Confirmatory factor analyses with the 16 primary and secondary subtests. Psychol. Assess. 2017, 29, 458–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  138. Hildebrand, D.K.; Ledbetter, M.F. Assessing Children’s Intelligence and Memory: The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition and the Children’s Memory Scale. In Handbook of Psychoeducational Assessment. Ability, Achievement, and Behavior in Children; Andrews, J.J.W., Janzen, H.L., Saklofske, D.H., Eds.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2001; pp. 13–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  139. Corral, S.; Arribas, D.; Santamaria, P.; Sueiro, M.J.; Perena, J. Escala de Inteligencia de Wechsler Para Niños (WISC-IV); TEA Ediciones S.A.: Madrid, Spain, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  140. Georgas, J.; van de Vijver, F.J.R.; Weiss, L.G.; Saklofske, D.H. A Cross-Cultural Analysis of the WISC-III. In Culture and Children’s Intelligence; Georgas, J., Weiss, L.G., van de Vijver, F.J.R., Eds.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2003; pp. 277–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  141. Golay, P.; Lecerf, T. Orthogonal Higher Order Structure and Confirmatory Factor Analysis of the French Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS-III). Psychol. Assess. 2011, 23, 143–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  142. Gregoire, J.; Georgas, J.; Saklofske, D.H.; van de Vijver, F.; Wierzbicki, C.; Weiss, L.G.; Zhu, J. Cultural issues in clinical use of the WISC-IV. In WISC-IV Clinical Assessment and Intervention, 2nd ed.; Prifitera, A., Saklofske, D.H., Weiss, L.G., Eds.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2008; pp. 517–544. [Google Scholar]
  143. Liu, J.; Lynn, R. Factor structure and sex differences on the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence in China, Japan and United States. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2011, 50, 1222–1226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  144. Montero-Linares, J.; Navarro-Guzman, J.I.; Aguilar-Vallagran, M. Procesos de automatización cognitiva en alumnado con altas capacidades intelectuales. An. Psicol. 2013, 29, 454–461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  145. Dentz, A.; Guay, M.; Gauthier, B.; Romo, L.; Parent, V. Is the Cogmed program effective for youths with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder under pharmacological treatment? Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2020, 34, 577–589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  146. Stroop, J.R. Studies of interference in serial verbal reaction. J. Exp. Psychol. 1935, 18, 643–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  147. Dellis, D.C.; Kaplan, E.; Kramer, J.H. Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System; Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  148. Golden, C.J. STROOP. Test de Colores y Palabras—Edición Revisada; Ruiz-Fernandez, B., Luque y, T., Sanchez-Sanchez, F., adaptadores, Eds.; TEA Ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  149. Golden, Z.L.; Golden, C.J. Patterns of performance on the Stroop Color and Word Test in children with learning, attentional, and psychiatric disabilities. Psychol. Sch. 2002, 39, 489–495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  150. Homack, S.; Riccio, C.A. A meta-analysis of the sensitivity and specificity of the Stroop color and word test with children. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2004, 19, 725–743. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  151. Torgesen, J.K.; Wagner, R.K.; Rashotte, C.A. Test of Word Reading Efficiency; AGS Publishing: Crowley, TX, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  152. Jimenez, J.E.; Siegel, L.S.; O’Shanahan, I.; Mazabel, S. Analyzing Cognitive and Reading Skills in Spanish-speaking English-language Learners and English-speaking Canadian Learners. Rev. Educ. 2012, 358, 310–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  153. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Individual Achievement Test (WIAT-II UK). WIAT-II UK; Pearson Assessment: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  154. Lenhard, W.; Schneider, W. Ein Leseverständnistest für Erst-Bis Sechstklässler (ELFE 1–6); Hogrefe: Göttingen, Germany, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  155. Morris, N.; Jones, D.M. Memory updating in working memory: The role of the central executive. Br. J. Psychol. 1990, 81, 111–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  156. Cornoldi, C.; Colpo, G.; Carretti, B. Prove MT-Kit Scuola Primaria [The Assessment of Reading in Primary School]; Giunti EDU: Firenze, Italy, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  157. Stephanou, A. The Progressive Achievement Tests in Reading (PAT-R), 4th ed.; Australian Council for Educational Research (ACER): Brisbane, Australia, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  158. Pearson Education. Cogmed, version; Pearson Education: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2016; Available online: https://www.cogmed.com/ (accessed on 10 November 2023).
  159. Kuhn, J.-T.; Holling, H. Number sense or working memory? The effect of two computer-based trainings on mathematical skills in elementary school. Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 2014, 10, 59–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  160. Bergman-Nutley, S.; Klingberg, T. Effect of working memory training on working memory, arithmetic and following instructions. Psychol. Res. 2014, 78, 869–877. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  161. Synaptikon GmbH. NeuroNation; Synaptikon GmbH: Berlin, Germany, 2020; Available online: https://www.neuronation.com/ (accessed on 10 November 2023).
  162. Hoskinson, P.; Toomim, J. Brain Workshop—A Dual N-Back Game; Version 4.8.1; The Brain Workshop: Dubai, United Arab Emirates, 2010; Available online: https://brainworkshop.sourceforge.net/ (accessed on 10 November 2023).
  163. Reitan, R.M.; Wolfson, D. Neuropsychological Evaluation of Older Children; Neuropsychology Press: London, UK, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  164. Archibald, S.J.; Kerns, K. Identification and description of new test of executive functioning in children. Child Neuropsychol. 1999, 5, 115–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  165. Capovilla, F.C.; Seabra, A.G. Teste Constrativo de Compreensão Auditiva e de Leitura. In Availiação Neuropsicológica Cognitiva: Leitura, Escrita e Aritmética; Seabra, A.G., Dias, N.M., Capovilla, F.C., Eds.; Memnom: Birmingham, UK, 2012; pp. 29–44. [Google Scholar]
  166. Stein, L.M. Teste de Desempenho Escolar: Manual para Aplicação e Interpretação; Casa do Psicólogo: São Paulo, Brazil, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  167. Northwest Evaluation Association. RIT Scale Norms: For Use with Measures of Academic Progress (MAP) for Primary Grades; Northwest Evaluation Association: Portland, OR, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  168. Northwest Evaluation Association. Technical Manual: For Use with Measured of Academic Progress (MAP) and MAP for Primary Grades; Northwest Evaluation Association: Portland, OR, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  169. Ho, C.S.-H.; Chan, D.W.-O.; Lee, S.-H.; Tsang, S.-M.; Luan, V.H. Cognitive profiling and preliminary subtyping in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Cognition 2004, 91, 43–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  170. Ho, C.S.-H.; Chan, D.W.-O.; Tsang, S.-M.; Lee, S.-H. The cognitive profile and multiple-deficit hypothesis in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Dev. Psychol. 2002, 38, 543–553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  171. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, WISC-IV, Danish Version; Pearson Assessment: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  172. Gaulin, C.A.; Campbell, T.F. Procedure for assessing verbal working memory in normal school-age children: Some preliminary data. Percept. Mot. Ski. 1994, 79, 55–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  173. Sundahl, L.; de Lopez, K.M. SLI er mere end blot forsinket udvikling af sproglige faerdigheder. Psyke Logos 2010, 31, 83–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  174. Henry, L.A. How does the severity of a learning disability affect working memory performance? Memory 2001, 9, 233–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  175. Moller, L.; Juul, H. Sætningslæseprøve 2 & Vejledning; Hogrefe: Göttingen, Germany, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  176. Roebers, C.M.; Kauer, M. Motor and cognitive control in a normative sample of 7-year-olds. Dev. Sci. 2009, 12, 175–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  177. Walter, J. LDL: Lernfortschrittsdiagnostik Lesen: Ein Curriculumbasiertes Verfahren; Hogrefe: Göttingen, Germany, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  178. Canet-Juric, L.; Stelzer, F.; Andres, M.L.; Vernucci, S.; Introzzi, I.; Burin, D.I. Evidencias de validez de una tarea computarizada de memoria de trabajo verbal y viso-espacial para niños. Rev. Interam. Psicol./Interam. J. Psychol. 2018, 52, 112–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  179. Injoque-Ricle, I.; Calero, A.D.; Alloway, T.P.; Burin, D.I. Assessing working memory in Spanish-speaking children: Automated Working Memory Assessment battery adaptation. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2011, 21, 78–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  180. Abusamra, V.; Ferreres, A.; Raiter, A.; De Beni, R.; Cornoldi, C. Test Leer para Comprender TLC: Evaluación de la Comprensión de Textos; Paidós: Barcelona, Spain, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  181. Brocki, K.C.; Bohlin, G. Executive functions in children aged 6 to 13: A dimensional and developmental study. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2004, 26, 571–593. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  182. McDermott, J.M.; Perez-Edgar, K.; Fox, N.A. Variations of the flanker paradigm: Assessing selective attention in young children. Behav. Res. Methods 2007, 39, 62–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  183. Gonzalez-Trujillo Calet, N.; Defior, S.; Gutierrez-Palma, N. Scale of reading fluency in Spanish: Measuring the components of fluency. Study Psychol. 2014, 35, 104–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  184. Boletín Oficial del Estado (BOE). Ley Orgánica 8/2013. Para la Mejora de la Calidad Educativa. 9 de Diciembre de 2013 (España). B.O.E. No. 295. Available online: https://www.boe.es/eli/es/lo/2013/12/09/8/con (accessed on 10 November 2023).
  185. Breznitz, S. CogniFit, version; 4.0; CogniFit Inc.: San Francisco, CA, USA, 2021; Available online: https://www.cognifit.com/es (accessed on 10 November 2023).
  186. Brickenkamp, R. Test d2. The d2 Test of Attention, 9th ed.; Hogrefe: Göttingen, Germany, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  187. Saraiva, R.A.; Moojen, S.M.P.; Munarski, R. Avaliação da Compreensão Leitora de Textos Expositivos, 2nd ed.; Casa do Psicólogo: São Paulo, Brazil, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  188. Salles, J. Habilidades e Dificuldades de Leitura e Escrita em Crianças de 2ª Série: Abordagem Neuropsicológica Cognitiva. Ph.D. Thesis, Programa de Pós-graduação em Psicologia do Desenvolvimento, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Brazil, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  189. Palladino, P.; Cornoldi, C.; De Beni, R.; Pazzaglia, F. Working memory and updating processes in reading comprehension. Mem. Cogn. 2001, 29, 344–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  190. August, D.; Francis, D.J.; Hsu, H.A.; Snow, C.E. Assessing reading comprehension in bilinguals. Elem. Sch. J. 2006, 107, 221–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  191. Cornoldi, C.; Colpo, G. Prove di Lettura MT-2 per la Scuola Primaria [Test for Reading Assessment in the Primary School]; Giunti OS: Firenze, Italy, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  192. Garcia-Madruga, J.A.; Elosua, M.; Gil, L.; Gomez-Veiga, I.; Vila, J.; Orjales, I.; Contreras, A.; Rodriguez, R.; Melero, M.; Duque, G. Reading Comprehension and Working Memory’s Executive Processes: An Intervention Study in Primary School Students. Read. Res. Q. 2013, 48, 155–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  193. Breznitz, Z.; Shaul, S.; Horowitz-Kraus, T.; Sela, I.; Nevat, M.; Karni, A. Enhanced reading by training with imposed time constraint in typical and dyslexic adults. Nat. Commun. 2013, 4, 1486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  194. Wiederholt, J.L.; Bryant, B.R. Gray Oral Reading Test, 3rd ed.; Pro-Ed: Austin, TX, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  195. Davidson, M.C.; Amso, D.; Anderson, L.C.; Diamond, A. Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia 2006, 44, 2037–2078. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  196. Durston, S.; Thomas, K.M.; Yang, Y.; Ulug, A.M.; Zimmerman, R.D.; Casey, B.J. A neural basis for the development of inhibitory control. Dev. Sci. 2002, 5, F9–F16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  197. Thorell, L.B.; Lindqvist, S.; Nutley, S.B.; Bohlin, G.; Klingberg, T. Training and transfer effects of executive functions in preschool children. Dev. Sci. 2009, 12, 106–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  198. Kieffer, M.J.; Vukovic, R.K.; Berry, D. Roles of attention shifting and inhibitory control in fourth-grade reading comprehension. Read. Res. Q. 2013, 48, 333–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  199. Klingberg, T.; Fernell, E.; Olesen, P.J.; Johnson, M.; Gustafsson, P.; Dahlstrom, K.; Gillberg, C.G.; Forssberg, H.; Westerberg, H. Computerized Training of Working Memory in Children With ADHD—A randomized, Controlled Trial. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2005, 44, 177–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  200. Klingberg, T. Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2010, 14, 317–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  201. Roughan, L.; Hadwin, J.A. The impact of working memory training in young people with social, emotional and behavioural difficulties. Learn. Individ. Differ. 2011, 21, 759–764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  202. Barnett, W.S. Effectiveness of Early Educational Intervention. Science 2011, 333, 975–978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  203. Cornoldi, C.; Carretti, B.; Drusi, S.; Tencati, C. Improving problem solving in primary school students: The effect of a training programme focusing on metacognition and working memory. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2015, 85, 424–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  204. Diamond, A.; Lee, K. Interventions Shown to Aid Executive Function Development in Children 4 to 12 Years Old. Science 2011, 333, 959–964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  205. Karbach, J.; Koenen, T.; Spengler, M. Who Benefits the Most? Individual Differences in the Transfer of Executive Control Training Across the Lifespan. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 2017, 1, 394–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  206. Kray, J.; Karbach, J.; Haenig, S.; Freitag, C. Can task-switching training enhance executive control functioning in children with attention deficit/-hyperactivity disorder? Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012, 5, 180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  207. Luo, Y.; Wang, J.; Wu, H.; Zhu, D.; Zhang, Y. Working-memory training improves developmental dyslexia in Chinese children. Neural Regen. Res. 2013, 8, 452–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  208. Gathercole, S.E.; Alloway, T.P.; Willis, C.; Adams, A.M. Working memory in children with reading disabilities. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2006, 93, 265–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  209. Prins, P.J.M.; Dovis, S.; Ponsioen, A.; ten Brink, E.; van der Oord, S. Does Computerized Working Memory Training with Game Elements Enhance Motivation and Training Efficacy in Children with ADHD? Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw. 2011, 14, 115–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  210. Karbach, J.; Kray, J. How useful is executive control training? Age differences in near and far transfer of task-switching training. Dev. Sci. 2009, 12, 976–990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  211. Blair, C.; Razza, R.P. Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in Kindergarten. Child Dev. 2007, 78, 647–663. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  212. Protopapas, A.; Archonti, A.; Skaloumbakas, C. Reading ability in negatively related to stroop interference. Cogn. Psychol. 2007, 54, 251–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  213. Liew, J.; McTigue, E.M.; Barrois, L.; Hugues, J.N. Adaptive and effortful control and academic self-efficacy beliefs on achievement: A longitudinal study of 1st through 3rd graders. Early Child. Res. Q. 2008, 23, 515–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  214. Schwaighofer, M.; Fischer, F.; Buehner, M. Does Working Memory Training Transfer? A Meta-Analysis Including Training Conditions as Moderators. Educ. Psychol. 2015, 50, 138–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  215. Diamond, A. Want to Optimize Executive Functions and Academic Outcomes? Simple, Just Nourish the Human Spirit. Dev. Cogn. Control. Process. Mech. Implic. Interv. 2014, 37, 205–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  216. Kerns, K.A.; MacSween, J.; Wekken, S.V.; Gruppuso, V. Investigating the efficacy of an attention training programme in children with foetal alcohol spectrum disorder. Dev. Neurorehabilit. 2010, 13, 413–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  217. Katz, B.; Shah, P. The role of child socioeconomic status in cognitive training outcomes. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2017, 53, 139–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  218. Noble, K.G.; Norman, M.F.; Farah, M.J. Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Dev. Sci. 2005, 8, 74–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  219. Goldin, A.P.; Hermida, M.J.; Shalom, D.E.; Elias-Costa, M.; Lopez-Rosenfeld, M.; Segretin, M.S.; Fernandez-Slezak, D.; Lipina, S.J.; Sigman, M. Far transfer to language and math of a short software-based gaming intervention. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2014, 111, 443–6448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  220. Horowitz-Kraus, T. Improvement in non-linguistic executive functions following reading acceleration training in children with reading difficulties. An ERP study. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 2015, 4, 77–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  221. Horowitz-Kraus, T.; DiFrancesco, M.; Kay, B.; Wang, Y.; Holland, S.K. Increased resting-state functional connectivity of visual- and cognitive-control brain networks after training in children with reading difficulties. Neuroimage Clin. 2015, 8, 619–630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  222. Horowitz-Kraus, T.; Holland, S.K. Greater functional connectivity between reading and error-detection regions following training with the reading acceleration program in children with reading difficulties. Ann. Dyslexia 2015, 65, 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  223. Horowitz-Kraus, T.; Vannest, J.J.; Kadis, D.; Cicchino, N.; Wang, Y.Y.; Holland, S.K. Reading acceleration training changes brain circuitry in children with reading difficulties. Brain Behav. 2014, 4, 886–902. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  224. Lotfi, S.; Rostami, R.; Shokoohi-Yekta, M.; Ward, R.T.; Motamed-Yeganeh, N.; Mathew, A.S.; Lee, H. Effects of computerized cognitive training for children with dyslexia: An ERP study. J. Neurolinguist. 2020, 55, 100904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Flowchart for systematic reviews and meta-analysis following the PRISMA guidelines.
Figure 1. Flowchart for systematic reviews and meta-analysis following the PRISMA guidelines.
Education 14 00229 g001
Table 1. Studies illustrating the impact on near and far transfer of working memory training.
Table 1. Studies illustrating the impact on near and far transfer of working memory training.
Authors, YearParticipantsDesignAssessment InstrumentsTraining EFIntervention LocationTraining ConditionsRepresentative Findings
EvidenceLittle Evidence
Artuso et al. [70]62 Italian children, AR = 9–10 years; M = 9.5 years.CE
PRE–I–POST
Adaptation of RMT [155]; Tasks designed by the authors to measure working memory; BIRT [156].Training focused on (UWM) was designed by the authors.School20 sessions of 50 min each, for 4 weeks.Benefits of WMT on UWM + RC (F (2,69) = 4.14, p = 0.02, ηp2 = 0.11, d = 0.80) and RC (F (2,69) = 6.31, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.16, d = 0.60) in POST, compared with PG.There were no benefits of WMT on UWM in POST, compared with PG.
Hitchcock and Westwell [129]148 Australian children, AR = 10.58–13.50 years; M = 12.25 years.CE
PRE–I–POST–FU
WISC-IV [132]; PAT-R [157].CogMed [158].School45-min sessions, 5 times per week, for 5 weeks.-There were no benefits of WMT on VWM or RC in POST, or at 3 months FU, compared with PG and CG.
Johann and Karbach [34]152 German children, AR = 8–11 years; M = 9.56 years.E
PRE–I–POST– FU
N-back task [159]; Spatial Span task [160]; Complex Span task [100]; ELFE 1–6 [154].NeuroNation [161].Home21 sessions of 30 min, 3–5 times per week, for 4 weeks.Benefits of game-based WMT on VWM (F (1, 62) = 6.58, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.10, d = 0.87) and VSWM (F(1, 64) = 6.91, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.10, d = 1.06) in POST, and on VSWM (F(1, 60) = 6.31, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.10, d = 0.95) at 3 months FU, compared with CG.There were no benefits of game-based WMT on RS or RC in POST, or on VWM, RS or RC at 3 months FU, compared with CG.
Benefits of standard WMT on VWM (F (1, 62) = 4.79, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.07, d = 0.92) in POST, and on VWM (F (1, 59) = 5.12, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.08, d = 1.21) and VSWM (F (1, 60) = 6.31, p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.10, d = 1.04) at 3 months FU, compared CG.There were no benefits of standard WMT on VSWM, RS or RC in POST, and on RS or RC at 3 months FU, compared with CG.
Jones et al. [44]95 English children, AR = 9–14 years; M = 12.51 years.CE
PRE–I–POST– FU
AWMA [136]; WIAT [153].CogMed [158].School20–25 sessions of 45 min each, 5 times a week, for 6–7 weeks.Benefits of WMT on WM (p < 0.001) in POST, and WM (p = 0.04) at 3 months FU, compared with PG.There were no benefits of WMT on RC in POST, or at 3 months FU, compared with PG.
Lopez and Aran [53]20 Argentine children, AR = 6 years; M = 6.20 years.CE
PRE–I–POST
WISC-IV [133]; n-back task [162]; TMT [163]; Stroop task [164].CogMed [158].School20–25 sessions, 5 times a week, for 3 months.Benefits of WMT on VWM (p = 0.001), VSWM (p = 0.001), and UWM (p = 0.00) in POST, compared with CG.There were no benefits of WMT on IC or RC in POST, compared with CG.
Novaes et al. [124]43 Brazilian children with typical development or reading comprehension difficulties, AR = 8–10 years; M = n/s.E
PRE–I–POST
TCCAL–Let [165]; TDE [166].Training focused on WM was designed by the authors.School15 sessions of 1 h each, twice a week, for 2 months. There were no benefits of WMT on RC in POST in typically developing children.
Ralph et al. [125]101 American children, AR = 10–13 years; M= n/s. E
PRE–I–POST– FU
MAP [167,168]; TOWRE [151].CogMed [158].Home25 sessions of 30–45 min each, 5 times a week, for 5 weeks.-There were no benefits of WMT on RF or RC in POST, or at 6 months FU, compared with PG.
Siu et al. [33]37 Chinese children, AR = 7–8 years; M = 7.5 years.E
PRE–I–POST
Tasks were designed by the authors to measure RF; Tasks used in previous studies to measure VWM [169,170].Training focused on WMT was designed by the authors.Home/University laboratory.Sessions of 30 min, 4 times per week at home + one session of 60 min each, once a week in the laboratory, for 8 weeks.Benefits of WMT on Chinese VWM (t (9) = −2.3, p < 0.05) English VWM (t (12) = −2.91, p < 0.05), and Chinese RF (t (9) = −7.58, p < 0.001; t (9) = −4.05, p < 0.001) in POST, compared with CG.There were no benefits of WMT on Chinese and English PS, or English RF in POST compared with CG.
Sondergaard and de Lopez [126]38 Danish children, AR = 8.3–10.7 years; M = 9.4 years.E
PRE–I–POST– FU
WISC-IV [171]; Danish adaptation of CLPT [172,173]; Odd-One-Out task [174,175]; Sentence Reading Test (Sætningslæseprøve 2) [175].Training focused on WMT was designed by the authors.School12 sessions of 10 min each, 3 times a week, for 4 weeks.Benefits of WMT on VSWM (g = 0.52) in POST, compared with PG. There were no benefits of WMT on VWM or RC in POST, or on VWM, VSWM or RC at 12 months FU, compared with PG.
Studer-Luethi et al. [128]86 Swedish children, AR = 8–12 years; M = 10.1 years.CE
PRE–I–POST– FU
Backward Colour Recall task [176]; RLPD [177].Training focused on VSWMT was designed by the authors.School17–18 sessions of 15 min each, 3 times a week, for 6 weeks.Benefits of VSWMT on VSWM (Δ = 0.82, p = 0.038) in POST, compared with PG.There were no benefits of VSWMT on RF in POST, compared with PG.
Vernucci et al. [127]89 Argentine children, AR = 9–10 years; M = 9.52 years.CE
PRE–I–POST– FU
CSRT [178]; adaptation of AWMA [179]; Reading to Comprehend [180].Training focused on WMT was designed by the authors. School10–13 sessions of 20 min each, twice a week, for 7 weeks.Benefits of WMT on VWM (p < 0.001, d = 0.714) in POST, and on VWM (p < 0.001, d = 1.17) at 6 months FU, compared with PG.There were no benefits of WMT on VSWM or RC in POST, or at 6 months FU, compared with PG.
Note: AR = Age Range; ARCCT = Auditory and Reading Comprehension Contrastive Test; AWMA = Automatized Working Memory Assessment; BIRT = Battery of Italian reading tests; CE = Quasi-experimental; CG = Control Group; CLPT = The Competing Language Processing Task; CSRT = Cognitive Self-regulation Tasks; E = Experimental; EF = Executive Functions; ELFE 1–6 = Reading Comprehension Test for First through Sixth Grades (Ein Leseverständnistest für Erst—bis Sechstklässler); FU = Follow-up; IC = Inhibitory Control; I = Intervention; M = Mean age; MAP = Measure of Academic Progress; n/s = Not specified; PAT-R = The Progressive Achievement Test in Reading); PG = Placebo Group; POST = Post-test; PRE = Pre-test; PS = Phonological Skills; RA = Reading Accuracy; RC = Reading Comprehension; RF = Reading Fluency; RLPD = Reading Learning Progress Diagnostics: A Curriculum-based Procedure (Lernfortschrittsdiagnostik Lesen: ein curriculumbasiertes Verfahren); RMT = Running Memory Task; RS = Reading Speed; RSCT = Reading Speed and Comprehension Task; SPT = School Performance Test; SST = Stop Signal Task; TMT = Trail Making Test; TOWRE = Test of Word Reading Efficiency; UWM = Updating Working Memory; VSWMT = Visuospatial Working Memory Training; VSWM = Visuospatial Working Memory; VWM = Verbal Working Memory; VWMT = Verbal Working Memory Training; WIAT-II = Wechsler Individual Achievement Test, 2sd Edition; WISC = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children; WM = Working Memory; WMT = Working Memory Training; WPT = Words and Pseudowords Task.
Table 2. Studies illustrating the impact on near and far transfer of inhibitory control.
Table 2. Studies illustrating the impact on near and far transfer of inhibitory control.
Authors, YearParticipantsDesignAssessment InstrumentsTraining EFIntervention LocationTraining ConditionsRepresentative Findings
EvidenceLittle Evidence
Johann and Karbach [34]152 German children, AR = 8–11 years; M = 9.56 years.E
PRE–I–POST–FU
Go/noGo task [181]; Flanker task [182]; Stroop-like task [52]; ELFE 1–6 [154].NeuroNation [161].Home21 sessions of 30 min each, 3–5 times per week, for 4 weeks.Benefits of game-based ICT on IC (F(1, 58) = 7.01; p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.12, d = 2.04; F(1, 56) = 13.75; p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.20, d = 2.19; (F(1, 143) = 35.10; p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.20, d = 1.83) and RF (F(1, 137) = 5.35; p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.04, d = 0.58) in POST, and on IC (F(1, 56) = 7.66; p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.12, d = 2.47; F(1, 55) = 8.37; p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.13, d = 1.91; (F(1, 135) = 12.39; p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.08, d = 1.77), RF (F(1, 128) = 4.21; p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.03, d = 1.09), and on RC (F(1, 138) = 4.56; p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.03, d = 0.63) at 3 months FU, compared with CG.There were no benefits of game-based ICT on RC in POST, compared with CG.
Benefits of standard ICT in IC (F(1, 58) = 13.64; p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.20, d = 1.77; (F(1, 143) = 13.20; p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.09, d = 1.68) in POST, and on IC (F(1,56) = 5.21; p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.09, d = 1.78; F(1, 135) = 5.21; p < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.04, d = 1.80) at 3 months FU, compared with CG.There were no benefits of standard ICT on RF or RC, in POST and at 3 months FU, compared with CG.
Note: AR = Age Range; E = Experimental; EF = Executive functions; ELFE 1–6 = Reading comprehension test for first through sixth grades (Prueba de comprensión lectora para estudiantes de 1° a 6° grado—Ein Leseverständnistest für Erstbis Sechstklässler); FU = Follow-up; I = Intervention; IC = Inhibitory Control; ICT = Inhibitory Control Training; M = Mean age; PG = Placebo Group; POST = post-test; PRE = pre-test; RC = Reading Comprehension; RF = Reading Fluency.
Table 3. Studies illustrating the impact on near and far transfer of combined training of working memory and inhibitory control.
Table 3. Studies illustrating the impact on near and far transfer of combined training of working memory and inhibitory control.
Authors, YearParticipantsDesignAssessment InstrumentsTraining EFIntervention LocationTraining ConditionsRepresentative Findings
EvidenceLittle Evidence
Reina-Reina et al. [130]196 Spanish children, AR = 9–12 years; M = 9.9 years.CE
I–POST
CLP [64,183]; Spanish academic curriculum [184].CogniFit Inc. [185].SchoolSessions of 15–20 min each, 3–4 times per week, for 8 weeks.Benefits of EFE on CL (F (1, 190) = 14.61, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.071) in POST.-
Weissheimer et al. [19]121 Brazilian children, AR = 8–10 years; M = n/s.CE
PRE–I–POST–FU
Adaptation of the n-back task (adaptation of Spatial Span task; Dogs and Monkeys task; Test d2 [186]; RSCT [187]; WPT [188].Training focused on EFT was designed by the authors.School10 sessions of 20–25 min each, for 5–7 weeks.Benefits of EFT on VWM (F (1, 55) = 6.865, p = 0.011, d = 0.53), IC (F (2, 210) = 5.374, p = 0.024, d = 0.99), RS (F (1, 120) = 6.022, p = 0.029, d = 0.61), in POST, and at 3 months FU, in children from low socioeconomic backgrounds compared with PG.There were no benefits of EFT on VSWM, RF, or RC in POST, or at 3 months FU, in children from low socioeconomic backgrounds compared with PG.
There were no benefits of EFT on VWM, VSWM, IC, RS, RF, or RC in POST, or at 3 months FU, in children from high socioeconomic backgrounds compared with PG.
Note: AR = Age Range; CE = Quasi-experimental; CLP = Progressive Linguistic Complexity Test (Test de Complejidad Lingüística Progresiva); EF = Executive Functions; EFT = Executive Functions Training; I = Intervention; M = Mean age; POST = post-test; PRE = pre-test; RC = Reading Comprehension; RF = Reading Fluency.
Table 4. Studies illustrating the impact on near and far transfer of combined training of executive functions and academic skills training.
Table 4. Studies illustrating the impact on near and far transfer of combined training of executive functions and academic skills training.
Authors, YearParticipantsDesignAssessment InstrumentsTraining FEIntervention LocationTraining ConditionsRepresentative Findings
EvidenceLittle Evidence
Carretti et al. [71]48 Italian children, AR = 8–9 years; M = 8.54 years.CE
PRE–I–POST–FU
Adaptation of UFRC [189]; DARC [190]; MTRA [191].Italian adaptation of Garcia-Madruga et al. [192].School10 sessions of 40 min each, twice a week, for 5 weeks.Benefits of WMT + RC on UWM (p = 0.001, d = 1.14) and RC (d = 0.76) in POST, and on WM (p = 0.002, d = 1.49) and RC (d = 1.75) at 2 months FU, compared to CG and PG.-
Horowitz-Kraus et al. [62]54 American children with reading difficulties, ADHD and reading difficulties, and typical development, AR = n/s; M = 9.76 years.E
PRE–I–POST
TOWRE-2 [151]; RAP [193]; GORT-III [194]; D-KEFS Stroop Task [147].RAP [193].Home20 sessions of 15–20 min each, 5 times per week, for 4 weeks.Benefits of EFT + RC on PP (t (18) = −0.721, p = 0.480), RA (t (18) = −6.727, p < 0.001; t (18) = −5.776, p < 0.001), and RF (t (18) = −4.574, p < 0.001) in POST.There were no benefits of EFT + RC on IC or RC in POST.
Sanchez-Perez et al. [101]104 Spanish children, AR = 7–12 years; M = 9.17 years.CE
PRE–I–POST
WISC-IV [133]; Dot Task [195]; Go/noGo task [196]; PROLEC-R [56].Training focused on EFT + MT was designed by the authors.School30-min sessions each, twice a week, for 13 weeks.Benefits of WMT + MT on IC (F (1,81) = 12.80, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.14, d = 0.61), and RAS (F (1,86) = 9.76, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.10, d = 0.61) in POST, compared to GC. There were no benefits of WMT + MT on VWM in POST, compared to GC.
Note: ADHD = Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder; AR = Age Range; CE = Quasi-experimental; CG = Control Group; DARC = Diagnostic Assessment of Reading Comprehension; D-KEFS = Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System; E = experimental; EF = Executive Functions; EFT = Executive Functions Training; FU = Follow-Up; GORT-III = Gray Oral Reading Test; I = Intervention; IC = Inhibitory Control; M = Mean age; MT = Mathematical Skills Training; MTRA = MT Test for reading assessment in the school; n/s = Not specified; PD = Phonological decoding; POST = post-test; PP = Phonological Processing; PRE = pre-test; PROLEC-R = Reading Process Assessment Battery—Revised (Batería de Evaluación de los Procesos LectoresR); RA = Reading Accuracy; RAS = Reading skills; RAP = Reading Acceleration Program; RC = Reading Comprehension; RF = Reading Fluency; RS = Reading Speed; TOWRE = Test of Word Reading Efficiency; UFRC = Updating Following a Relevant Criterion; UWM = Updating Working Memory; VWM = Verbal Working Memory; WISC = Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children; WM = Working Memory; WMT = Working Memory Training.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Reina-Reina, C.; Antón, E.; Duñabeitia, J.A. A Systematic Literature Review of the Impact of Cognitive Stimulation Programs on Reading Skills in Children Aged between 6 and 12 Years Old. Educ. Sci. 2024, 14, 229. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14030229

AMA Style

Reina-Reina C, Antón E, Duñabeitia JA. A Systematic Literature Review of the Impact of Cognitive Stimulation Programs on Reading Skills in Children Aged between 6 and 12 Years Old. Education Sciences. 2024; 14(3):229. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14030229

Chicago/Turabian Style

Reina-Reina, Claudia, Eneko Antón, and Jon Andoni Duñabeitia. 2024. "A Systematic Literature Review of the Impact of Cognitive Stimulation Programs on Reading Skills in Children Aged between 6 and 12 Years Old" Education Sciences 14, no. 3: 229. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14030229

APA Style

Reina-Reina, C., Antón, E., & Duñabeitia, J. A. (2024). A Systematic Literature Review of the Impact of Cognitive Stimulation Programs on Reading Skills in Children Aged between 6 and 12 Years Old. Education Sciences, 14(3), 229. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14030229

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop