The Effect of Self-Monitoring on Mental Effort and Problem-Solving Performance: A Mixed-Methods Study
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
3. Results
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
- What is the genotype of a hereditary quality such as hair color?
- What is the phenotype of a hereditary quality such as eye color?
- What does it mean if a person is homozygous for a hereditary quality such as hair color?
- What does it mean if a person is heterozygous for a hereditary quality such as eye color?
- What does it mean if an allele in a genotype is dominant?
- What does it mean if an allele in a genotype is recessive?
- Which feature will be visible if the same recessive alleles are present in the genotype?
- Which feature will be visible if two different alleles are present in the genotype?
- What do you have to do to find the genotypes of children if you know the genotypes of the two parents?
Appendix B
Keyword Grading Scheme for Pretest
Question | Answer | Keywords |
1 | Genotype refers to the two alleles a person/organism has inherited for a particular gene | 1. Alleles 2. Gene(s) |
2 | Phenotype is how this genetic information (genotype) is reflected in physical characteristics | 1. Genetic information/genotype 2. Physical |
3 | Homozygous means that both alleles are the same | 1. Alleles 2. both are the same/other words that describe this |
4 | Heterozygous means that there is dominant and a recessive allele | 1. Alleles 2. Different |
5 | A dominant allele produces the dominant phenotype. Even with one dominant allele they will have the dominant phenotype | 1. Dominant phenotype 2. one copy (3. Uppercase) |
6 | A recessive allele produces the recessive phenotype. To produce this phenotype the genotype must have two copies of the recessive allele. | 1. Recessive phenotype 2. Two copies needed (3. Lowercase) |
7 | The recessive phenotype | 1. Recessive |
8 | The dominant phenotype | 2. Dominant |
9 | Put the genotypes of the parents in a cross table so you can form the possible combinations of genotypes for the child. | 1. Cross table or Punnett Square |
Appendix C
Learning Phase Material
- A genotype (such as for hair shape) is the information that lies in the genes and consists of two alleles (e.g., BB, Bb, or bb).
- 2.
- A phenotype is how this information is reflected in physical characteristics (e.g., curly or straight hair).
- 3.
- A dominant allele (represented by a capital/uppercase letter) produces the dominant phenotype. If individuals have even only one dominant allele in their genotype, they will have the dominant phenotype. This dominant allele can come from one parent or both.
- 4.
- For a recessive allele (represented by a lowercase letter), to produce a recessive phenotype, the individual’s genotype must have two copies of the recessive allele, which means they received one recessive allele from each parent.
- 5.
- For hair shape, the dominant allele for curly hair is (H) and the recessive allele for straight hair is (h).
- 6.
- Each genotype is composed of two alleles (i.e., HH, Hh, or hh). A dominant phenotype (i.e., curly hair) appears when an individual’s genotype has at least one copy of the dominant allele (i.e., HH or Hh).
- 7.
- A recessive phenotype (i.e., straight hair) only appears when an individual’s genotype has two recessive alleles (i.e., hh).
- 8.
- Homozygous means that both alleles are the same. This could mean there are two uppercase letters or two lowercase letters (i.e., HH or hh).
- 9.
- Heterozygous means that there is a dominant and a recessive allele in the genotype (i.e., Hh).
- 10.
- Deductive reasoning means you reason from the parents down to the child.
- 11.
- Inductive reasoning means you reason from a child and one parent up to the other parent.
- 12.
- To use a cross table/Punnett square, put the two alleles from the genotype of the first parent on the left-hand side of the cross table so that one allele is in each row (e.g., see image below). For the second parent, put one allele on top of the first column and the second allele in the next column. In this example, each parent is heterozygous for a trait. Combine each of the alleles, one from the row and one from the column, in the appropriate boxes to form all the possible genotypes of their child. It is then possible that their child could have the genotypes of RR, Rr, OR rr for this trait.
Appendix D
Appendix D.1. Level 1 Complexity Hereditary Example Problem
Appendix D.2. Level 3 Complexity Hereditary Example Problem
Appendix D.3. Level 5 Complexity Hereditary Example Problem
Appendix E
Mental Effort Rating Scale Adapted from Paas [32]
Appendix F
Appendix F.1. Complexity Levels 1–3 Self-Assessment Measure
Appendix F.2. Complexity Level 4 Self-Assessment Measure
Appendix F.3. Complexity Level 5 Self-Assessment Measure
Appendix G
Appendix G.1. Instructions for the Problem-Solving Tasks for the Control Group
Appendix G.2. Instructions for the Problem-Solving Tasks for the Experimental Group
Appendix H
Coding Scheme for Qualitative Analysis (Based on Azevedo et al. [45]) | ||
Variable | Description | Example |
A—Forethought, Planning, and Activation | ||
A1—Planning | Includes listing the actions needed to solve the problem before executing them. | “I need to do the table first” |
A2—Sub-Goals | Participant states actions that are intended to help solve the problem while solving the problem. | “And then so we know that so we can do deductive again” |
A3—Prior Knowledge Activation | Participant recalls relevant prior knowledge before a task or during. | “Homozygous means they are both the same gene” |
A4—Recycle Goal in Working Memory | Participant repeats the goal verbally to refresh it in their working memory. | “Both parents have brown eyes…both parents have brown eyes” |
B—Monitoring | ||
B1—Negative Judgment of Learning (NJOL) | Participant becomes aware that they do not understand or know the material during problem-solving. | “Still not sure” “The child, wait hold on, this is so confusing, what am I doing?” |
B2—Positive Judgment of Learning (PJOL) | Participant becomes aware that they do understand or know the material during problem solving. | “Which means yeah that makes sense” “So parent one is big B, big B. Right? Yeah” |
B3—Self-assessment | Participate gives a positive or negative self-assessment of their performance (when prompted to during self-assessment rating question) | “I think I scored all of them, probably” “Guessing four points hopefully” |
B4—Feeling of Knowing (FOK) | Participant states that they remember the material vaguely and understand some of it, but not well. | “There was another way to do this, but I forgot” “And it can either be both recessive or large, I think that’s possible? Yeah” |
B5—Content Evaluation | Participant monitors content relative to goals. | “I don’t think the child’s eye colour was mentioned even” “So the parent has a big B, otherwise they would not had brown eyes” |
B6—Identify Adequacy of Information and Task Instructions | Participant determines the usefulness or adequacy of the material and task instructions. | “So I am just going to put information parent two as… I don’t know how to put that in there, honestly” “I think we are talking about the child here, because that is the only one that is kinda missing and I would technically say it is both…oh for the first set of the cross tables, okay okay that makes a lot of sense now.” |
C—Strategy Use | ||
C1—Goal-Free Search | Participant states that they will look around the information without stating a particular goal or plan. | [Did not appear in any the transcripts] |
C2—Summarization | Participant summarizes what they read. | “The partner has curly hair and is heterozygotic, so they have the same traits as the parents of the child” “Okay, so one parent is homozygous and dominant and the other one is heterozygous” |
C3—Rereading | Participants reread or revisit earlier material. | “And then the partner had heterozygotic set, right? Curly hair, yes. And is heterozygotic.” |
C4—Inferences | Participant makes inferences based on what they read or saw in the environment. | “So both have brown eyes and are homozygous so big B big B.” “Deductive reasoning” “Which means the parent can be big little and then little. So then this one” |
C5—Hypothesizing | Participant asks questions that go beyond what they read in the material. | “But these are the options that the child definitely does not have, if it is two small h’s it would not work. But it is probabilities so who knows.” “So we have a 50/50 chance that the child will have homozygous brown eyes or homozygous… heterozygous brown eyes.” |
C6—Knowledge Elaboration | Participant elaborates what they read in the material with prior knowledge. | “Ok so h h. And to have a child that has one big H that means that they have to have at least one big h.” “Again this is deductive, because we are looking at the parents and then trying to guess the child one” |
D—Task Difficulty, Complexity, and Demands | ||
D1—Time and Effort Planning | Participant deliberately controls their behavior due to limited energy or other physical demands. | “Just going to say small instead of lowercase or uppercase” “Since question is going to ask I’m just going to fill the first one because I can’t” |
D2—Help-Seeking Behavior | Participant seeks assistance from the experimenter regarding the task. | [Any dialogues with the experimenters about the task] |
D3—Task Difficulty | Participant indicates that the task is easy or difficult. | “It’s a brawl of kk, this is easy” “Okay, this is a bit more complex” |
D4—Mental Effort | Participant gives an indication of their mental effort invested in the task. | “I guess same mental effort or a bit more maybe” “Say low, probably. But I did think about it” |
E—Miscellaneous Actions | ||
E1—Stating Answer | Participant states an answer to a final step in the problem-solving task. | “All the possibilities” |
E2—Copying information | Participant copies information they wrote again or found in the environment or verbalizes information they input into a blank or cross table. | “Parent one is big big, parent two is little little” “So big big little big big and big little” |
E3—Reading Question | Participant reads questions or information (close to) verbatim on screen out loud. | “Two parents are having a child.” |
Note. All italicized examples are direct quotes from the transcripts. All unitalicized examples in brackets are explanations |
- Simplified or adjusted definition for this context and changed example to relevant example from the transcripts
- ○
- A1—Planning
- ○
- A3—Prior Knowledge Activation
- ○
- A4—Recycle Goal in Working Memory
- ○
- B4—Feeling of Knowing (FOK)
- ○
- C2—Summarization
- ○
- C3—Re-reading
- ○
- C4—Inferences
- ○
- C5—Hypothesizing
- ○
- C6—Knowledge Elaboration
- ○
- D1—Time and Effort Planning
- ○
- D2—Help-Seeking Behavior
- ○
- D3—Task Difficulty
- Heavily adjusted definition for this context, moved it to a completely new code group, and put relevant example taken from the transcripts
- ○
- E2—Copying information
- Simplified definition for this context and gave a hypothetical example not from the transcripts
- ○
- A2—Sub-Goals
- ○
- C1—Goal-Free Search
- Definition kept the same but relevant example from the transcripts replaced original example
- ○
- B5—Content Evaluation
- Added this code due to frequent occurrences in the text and/or relevancy to research
- ○
- E3—Reading Question
- ○
- D4—Mental Effort
- ○
- E1—Stating Answer
- Originally one code of called judgments of learning, this code was split up into two different codes, opposites of each other, for more in depth analysis of kinds of judgments of learning the participants verbalized. Definitions were adjusted and relevant examples from the transcripts were given.
- ○
- B1—Negative Judgment of Learning (NJOL)
- ○
- B2—Positive Judgment of Learning (PJOL)
- This code was added to the monitoring code group to be able to separate out when participants monitored spontaneously (such as a JOL) and when participants monitored their performance when prompted in the task environment when doing the self-assessment rating scale.
- ○
- B3—Self-assessment
- Originally the two codes of “Identify Adequacy of Information” and “Expectation of Adequacy of Information”, these codes were combined together to form this code, which included judgments based on the usefulness and adequacy of task instructions and material presented, which fit better with this context. Definition was adjusted to fit the new code and relevant examples from the transcripts were added.
- ○
- B6—Identify Adequacy of Information and Task Instructions
- These codes were removed due to irrelevance or redundancy.
- ○
- Self-Questioning
- ○
- Selecting a New Informational Source
- ○
- Control of Context
- Code group called Task Difficulty and Demands was renamed to Task Difficulty, Complexity, and Demands
- Added code group called Miscellaneous Actions due to these codes not fitting in with other code groups well but being frequent types of verbalizations
Appendix I
Appendix J
Pairwise Comparisons for Average Performance per Complexity Level
95% CI for Difference | ||||||
Complexity (I) | Complexity (J) | Mean Difference (I–J) | Standard Error | Sig. | Lower Bound | Upper Bound |
1 | 2 | 1.310 | 2.299 | 1.000 | −5.380 | 7.999 |
3 | 7.530 | 2.572 | 0.047 | 0.047 | 15.012 | |
4 | 9.292 | 2.757 | 0.013 | 1.272 | 17.311 | |
5 | 8.562 | 2.520 | 0.012 | 1.230 | 15.893 | |
2 | 1 | −1.310 | 2.299 | 1.000 | −7.999 | 5.380 |
3 | 6.220 | 1.919 | 0.019 | 0.637 | 11.803 | |
4 | 7.982 | 2.264 | 0.008 | 1.396 | 14.568 | |
5 | 7.252 | 2.106 | 0.010 | 1.126 | 13.378 | |
3 | 1 | −7.530 | 2.572 | 0.047 | −15.012 | −0.047 |
2 | −6.220 | 1.919 | 0.019 | −11.803 | −0.637 | |
4 | 1.762 | 2.120 | 1.000 | −4.407 | 7.930 | |
5 | 1.032 | 1.983 | 1.000 | −4.739 | 6.802 | |
4 | 1 | −9.292 | 2.757 | 0.013 | −17.311 | −1.272 |
2 | −7.982 | 2.264 | 0.008 | −14.568 | −1.396 | |
3 | −1.762 | 2.120 | 1.000 | −7.930 | 4.407 | |
5 | −0.730 | 1.994 | 1.000 | −6.531 | 5.071 | |
5 | 1 | −8.562 | 2.520 | 0.012 | −15.893 | −1.230 |
2 | −7.252 | 2.106 | 0.010 | −13.378 | −1.126 | |
3 | −1.032 | 1.983 | 1.000 | −6.802 | 4.739 | |
4 | 0.730 | 1.994 | 1.000 | −5.071 | 6.531 | |
Note: For the performance measure, complexity levels one and two are significantly different from levels three through five, but they are similar to each other. Meanwhile, complexity levels three through five also are statistically similar to each other. Therefore, for the greater analysis, we decided to average together the performances on levels one and two for a simple performance mean and then averaged levels three through five together to obtain a complex performance mean. This was similarly done with mental effort to obtain a simple and complex mental effort mean and absolute SA accuracy to obtain a simple and complex absolute SA accuracy mean. |
References
- Bjork, R.A.; Dunlosky, J.; Kornell, N. Self-regulated learning: Beliefs, techniques, and illusions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 417–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zimmerman, B.J. Becoming a self-regulated learner: Which are the key subprocesses? Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 1986, 11, 307–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stefanou, C.; Stolk, J.D.; Prince, M.; Chen, J.C.; Lord, S.M. Self-regulation and autonomy in problem- and project-based learning environments. Act. Learn. High. Educ. 2013, 14, 109–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lai, C.; Hwang, G. A self-regulated flipped classroom approach to improving students’ learning performance in a mathematics course. Comput. Educ. 2016, 100, 126–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Littlejohn, A.; Hood, N.; Milligan, C.; Mustain, P. Learning in MOOCs: Motivations and self-regulated learning in MOOCs. Internet High. Educ. 2016, 29, 40–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Winne, P.H. A metacognitive view of individual differences in self-regulated learning. Learn. Individ. Differ. 1996, 8, 327–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Azevedo, R.; Cromley, J. Does training on self-regulated learning facilitate students’ learning with hypermedia? J. Educ. Psychol. 2004, 96, 523–535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baars, M.; Visser, S.; Van Gog, T.; de Bruin, A.; Paas, F. Completion of partially worked-out examples as a generation strategy for improving monitoring accuracy. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2013, 38, 395–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baars, M.; Van Gog, T.; de Bruin, A.; Paas, F. Effects of problem solving after worked example study on primary school children’s monitoring accuracy. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2014, 28, 382–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baars, M.; Vink, S.; Van Gog, T.; De Bruin, A.; Paas, F. Effects of training self-assessment and using assessment standards on retrospective and prospective monitoring of problem solving. Learn. Instr. 2014, 33, 92–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Gog, T.; Kester, L.; Paas, F. Effects of concurrent monitoring on cognitive load and performance as a function of task complexity. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2011, 25, 584–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Bruin, A.B.; Roelle, J.; Carpenter, S.K.; Baars, M.; EFG-MRE. Synthesizing cognitive load and self-regulation theory: A theoretical framework and research agenda. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2020, 32, 903–915. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sweller, J.; Van Merrienboer, J.J.G.; Paas, F.G.W.C. Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 1998, 10, 251–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sweller, J.; Van Merrienboer, J.J.G.; Paas, F.G.W.C. Cognitive architecture and instructional design: 20 years later. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2019, 31, 261–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baddeley, A. Working memory. Science 1992, 255, 556–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paas, F.G.W.C.; Van Merrienboer, J.J.G. Instructional-control of cognitive load in the training of complex cognitive tasks. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 1994, 6, 351–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brünken, R.; Plass, J.L.; Leutner, D. Direct measurement of cognitive load in multimedia learning. Educ. Psychol. 2003, 38, 53–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paas, F.; Van Gog, T.; Sweller, J. Cognitive load theory: New conceptualizations, specifications, and integrated research perspectives. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2010, 22, 115–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Radović, S.; Seidel, N.; Haake, J.M.; Kasakowskij, R. Analyzing students’ self-assessment practice in a distance education environment: Student behavior, accuracy, and task-related characteristics. J. Comput. Assist. Learn. 2023, 40, 654–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pintrich, P.R. The role of goal orientation in self-regulated learning. In Handbook of Self-Regulation; Boekaerts, M., Pintrich, P.R., Zeidner, M., Eds.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2000; pp. 451–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Winne, P.H.; Hadwin, A.F. Studying as self-regulated learning. In Metacognition in Educational Theory and Practice, 2nd ed.; Hacker, D., Dunlosky, J., Graesser, A., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1998; pp. 277–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Panadero, E. A review of self-regulated learning: Six models and four directions for research. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Winne, P.H. Cognition and metacognition within self-regulated learning. In Handbook of Self-Regulation of Learning and Performance, 2nd ed.; Schunk, D.H., Greene, J.A., Eds.; Routledge: London, UK, 2017; pp. 36–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ku, K.Y.L.; Ho, I.T. Metacognitive strategies that enhance critical thinking. Metacogn. Learn. 2010, 5, 251–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mirandola, C.; Ciriello, A.; Gigli, M.; Cornoldi, C. Metacognitive monitoring of text comprehension: An investigation on postdictive judgments in typically developing children and children with reading comprehension difficulties. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 2253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Barenberg, J.; Dutke, S. Testing and metacognition: Retrieval practise effects on metacognitive monitoring in learning from text. Memory 2019, 27, 269–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dunlosky, J.; Rawson, K.A. Overconfidence produces underachievement: Inaccurate self evaluations undermine students’ learning and retention. Learn. Instr. 2012, 22, 271–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pieschl, S. Metacognitive calibration—An extended conceptualization and potential applications. Metacogn. Learn. 2009, 4, 3–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peng, Y.; Tullis, J.G. Dividing attention impairs metacognitive control more than monitoring. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2021, 28, 2064–2074. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tuysuzoglu, B.B.; Greene, J.A. An investigation of the role of contingent metacognitive behavior in self-regulated learning. Metacogn. Learn. 2015, 10, 77–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Seufert, T. The interplay between self-regulation in learning and cognitive load. Educ. Res. Rev. 2018, 24, 116–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paas, F.G.W.C. Training strategies for attaining transfer of problem-solving skill in statistics: A cognitive-load approach. J. Educ. Psychol. 1992, 84, 429–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Winne, P.H. Inherent details in self-regulated learning. Educ. Psychol. 1995, 30, 173–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vangsness, L.; Young, M.E. More isn’t always better: When metacognitive prompts are misleading. Metacogn. Learn. 2021, 16, 135–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liefooghe, B.; Barrouillet, P.; Vandierendonck, A.; Camos, V. Working memory costs of task switching. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2008, 34, 478–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Patterson, M.C. A naturalistic investigation of media multitasking while studying and the effects on exam performance. Teach. Psychol. 2017, 44, 51–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Finley, J.R.; Benjamin, A.S.; McCarley, J.S. Metacognition of multitasking: How well do we predict the costs of divided attention. J. Exp. Psychol. Appl. 2014, 20, 158–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trabasso, T.; Magliano, J.P. Conscious understanding during comprehension. Discourse Process. 1996, 21, 255–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meyers, J.; Lytle, S.; Palladino, D.; Devenpeck, G.; Green, M. Think-aloud protocol analysis: An investigation of reading comprehension strategies in fourth- and fifth-grade students. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 1990, 8, 112–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schellings, G.L.M.; Van Hout-Wolters, B.H.A.M.; Veenman, M.V.J.; Meijer, J. Assessing metacognitive activities: The in-depth comparison of a task-specific questionnaire with think-aloud protocols. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 2013, 28, 963–990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zachariou, A.; Whitebread, D. Developmental differences in young children’s self-regulation. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 2019, 62, 282–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jordano, M.L.; Touron, D.R. How often are thoughts metacognitive? Findings from research on self-regulated learning, think-aloud protocols, and mind-wandering. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2018, 25, 1269–1286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Qualtrics XM—Experience Management Software. Available online: https://www.qualtrics.com/ (accessed on 5 May 2023).
- Moos, D.C.; Bonde, C. Flipping the classroom: Embedding self-regulated learning prompts in videos. Technol. Knowl. Learn. 2016, 21, 225–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Azevedo, R.; Guthrie, J.T.; Seibert, D. The role of self-regulated learning in fostering students’ conceptual understanding of complex systems with hypermedia. J. Educ. Comput. Res. 2004, 30, 87–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnstone, C.J.; Bottsford-Miller, N.A.; Thompson, S.J.; Council of Chief State School Officers (CCSSO); National Association of State Directors of Special Education (NASDSE). Using the Think Aloud Method (Cognitive Labs) to Evaluate Test Design for Students with Disabilities and English Language Learners; University of Minnesota, National Center on Educational Outcomes: Minneapolis, MN, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- ATLAS.ti Scientific Software Development GmbH. ATLAS.ti Windows (Version 24.0.0) [Qualitative Data Analysis Software]. 2024. Available online: https://atlasti.com (accessed on 1 January 2024).
- IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, version 29.0; Computer software; IBM Corp.: Armonk, NY, USA, 2023. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Leppink, J.; Paas, F.; Van der Vleuten, C.P.M.; Van Gog, T.; Van Merriënboer, J.J.G. Development of an instrument for measuring different types of cognitive load. Behav. Res. Methods 2013, 45, 1058–1072. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hoch, E.; Sidi, Y.; Ackerman, R.; Hoogerheide, V.; Scheiter, K. Comparing mental effort, difficulty, and confidence appraisals in problem-solving: A metacognitive perspective. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2023, 35, 61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Albulescu, P.; Macsinga, I.; Rusu, A.; Sulea, C.; Bodnaru, A.; Tulbure, B.T. “Give me a break!” A systematic review and meta-analysis on the efficacy of micro-breaks for increasing well-being and performance. PLoS ONE 2022, 17, e0272460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tyler, J.M.; Burns, K.C. After depletion: The replenishment of the self’s regulatory resources. Self Identity 2008, 7, 305–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steinborn, M.B.; Huestegge, L. A walk down the lane gives wings to your brain. Restorative benefits of rest breaks on cognition and self-control. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2016, 30, 795–805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nietfeld, J.L.; Cao, L.; Osborne, J.W. Metacognitive monitoring accuracy and student performance in the postsecondary classroom. J. Exp. Educ. 2005, 74, 7–28. [Google Scholar]
Control (n = 35) | Experimental (n = 30) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
M | SD | M | SD | ||
Mental Effort | 3.96 | 2.08 | 3.83 | 1.57 | |
Simple | Performance | 91.79 | 14.60 | 86.25 | 19.03 |
SA Accuracy | 13.33 | 16.39 | |||
Mental Effort | 5.51 | 1.68 | 5.55 | 1.39 | |
Complex | Performance | 86.29 | 19.79 | 76.13 | 24.05 |
SA Accuracy | 18.30 | 12.08 |
Codes | Raw Frequency | Percentage |
---|---|---|
A—Forethought, Planning, and Activation | 51 | 3.32 |
A1—Planning | 7 | 0.46 |
A2—Sub-Goals | 19 | 1.24 |
A3—Prior Knowledge Activation | 12 | 0.78 |
A4—Recycle Goal in Working Memory | 13 | 0.85 |
B—Monitoring | 351 | 22.85 |
B1—Negative Judgment of Learning (NJOL) | 41 | 2.67 |
B2—Positive Judgment of Learning (PJOL) | 71 | 4.62 |
B3—Self-assessment | 26 | 1.69 |
B4—Feeling of Knowing (FOK) | 38 | 2.47 |
B5—Content Evaluation | 115 | 7.49 |
B6—Identify Adequacy of Information and Task Instructions | 60 | 3.91 |
C—Strategy Use | 480 | 31.25 |
C1—Goal-Free Search | 0 | 0 |
C2—Summarization | 9 | 0.59 |
C3—Rereading | 18 | 1.17 |
C4—Inferences | 311 | 20.25 |
C5—Hypothesizing | 25 | 1.63 |
C6—Knowledge Elaboration | 117 | 7.62 |
D—Task Difficulty, Complexity, and Demands | 43 | 2.80 |
D1—Time and Effort Planning | 9 | 0.59 |
D2—Help-Seeking Behavior | 1 | 0.07 |
D3—Task Difficulty | 9 | 0.59 |
D4—Mental Effort | 24 | 1.56 |
E—Miscellaneous Actions | 611 | 39.78 |
E1—Stating Answer | 72 | 4.69 |
E2—Copying Information | 233 | 15.17 |
E3—Reading Question | 306 | 19.92 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Graham, M.; Ilic, M.; Baars, M.; Ouwehand, K.; Paas, F. The Effect of Self-Monitoring on Mental Effort and Problem-Solving Performance: A Mixed-Methods Study. Educ. Sci. 2024, 14, 1167. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14111167
Graham M, Ilic M, Baars M, Ouwehand K, Paas F. The Effect of Self-Monitoring on Mental Effort and Problem-Solving Performance: A Mixed-Methods Study. Education Sciences. 2024; 14(11):1167. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14111167
Chicago/Turabian StyleGraham, Madison, Marinela Ilic, Martine Baars, Kim Ouwehand, and Fred Paas. 2024. "The Effect of Self-Monitoring on Mental Effort and Problem-Solving Performance: A Mixed-Methods Study" Education Sciences 14, no. 11: 1167. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14111167
APA StyleGraham, M., Ilic, M., Baars, M., Ouwehand, K., & Paas, F. (2024). The Effect of Self-Monitoring on Mental Effort and Problem-Solving Performance: A Mixed-Methods Study. Education Sciences, 14(11), 1167. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci14111167