Neighbourhood Density in Spoken Word Recognition: An Eye-Tracking Study
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Word Access and Neighbourhood Density
1.2. Lexical Acquisition and Neighbourhood Density
1.3. The Current Study
- Does the neighbourhood size of target words affect the accuracy and speed of retrieval in monolingual and bilingual children in the picture-naming task? Is there a difference between monolingual and bilingual children?
- Does the neighbourhood size of the target words affect the activation of same-language competitors in monolingual and bilingual adults and children in the eye-tracking task?
- Does the neighbourhood size of target words affect the activation of cross-language competitors in bilingual adults and children in the eye-tracking task? With regard to these questions, the following outcomes are expected:
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Picture Naming Task
Picture Naming Participants and Procedure
2.2. Eye-Tracking Task
Eye-Tracking Task Participants and Procedure
3. Results
3.1. Picture -Naming Task
3.2. Eye-Tracking Task
4. Discussion
5. Limitations of the Study
6. Conclusions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Item | Word Length (Phonemes) | Freq (Per-Mill) | Phonological Neighbours, Same-Language | Phonological Neighbours, Cross-Language | Total Neighbours | Age of Acquisition (AoA) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bell | 3 | 39.33 | 41 | 17 | 58 | 2.8 |
Tail | 3 | 23.9 | 49 | 4 | 53 | 3 |
Bowl | 3 | 21.5 | 45 | 5 | 50 | 3.1 |
Bone | 3 | 26.1 | 44 | 6 | 50 | 3.1 |
Corn | 3 | 14.2 | 42 | 8 | 50 | 2.8 |
Duck | 3 | 24.7 | 42 | 5 | 47 | 2.4 |
Bike | 3 | 25.9 | 32 | 14 | 46 | 2.6 |
Ham | 3 | 11.5 | 43 | 2 | 45 | 3.6 |
Rack | 3 | 7.8 | 44 | 0 | 44 | 4 |
Road | 3 | 111.9 | 42 | 0 | 42 | 2.8 |
Hoop | 3 | 2.7 | 25 | 15 | 40 | 3.5 |
Bird | 3 | 45.5 | 39 | 0 | 39 | 2.3 |
Cake | 3 | 45.1 | 35 | 3 | 38 | 2.6 |
Towel | 3 | 14.2 | 25 | 13 | 38 | |
Map | 3 | 31.8 | 35 | 2 | 37 | 3.4 |
Cook | 3 | 45.6 | 23 | 9 | 32 | 3.1 |
Tear | 3 | 27 | 30 | 1 | 31 | 3 |
Doll | 3 | 24.7 | 30 | 0 | 30 | 2.4 |
Coin | 3 | 9.7 | 22 | 8 | 30 | 3.1 |
Witch | 3 | 27.6 | 28 | 1 | 29 | 3 |
Goat | 3 | 10.5 | 23 | 3 | 26 | 2.6 |
Dog | 3 | 192.8 | 25 | 0 | 25 | 2.2 |
Vase | 3 | 3.8 | 20 | 5 | 25 | 3.7 |
Plane | 4 | 95.5 | 22 | 1 | 23 | 3.2 |
Average | 3.0 | 36.8 | 33.6 | 5.1 | 38.7 | 3.0 |
Item | Word Length (Phonemes) | Freq (Per-Mill) | Phonological Neighbours, Same-Language | Phonological Neighbours, Cross-Language | Total Neighbours | Age of Acquisition (AoA) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Low Density | ||||||
Tree | 3 | 65 | 22 | 1 | 23 | 2.5 |
Swing | 4 | 26 | 19 | 1 | 20 | 2.8 |
Hood | 3 | 15.4 | 19 | 2 | 20 | 3.6 |
Tailor | 4 | 4.2 | 19 | 3 | 20 | |
Horse | 3 | 92.9 | 18 | 4 | 19 | |
Fawn | 3 | 0.7 | 18 | 11 | 19 | 4.8 |
Belt | 4 | 24.4 | 18 | 14 | 19 | 3.3 |
Mower | 3 | 1.5 | 15 | 3 | 16 | |
Tooth | 3 | 13.6 | 13 | 6 | 14 | 2.3 |
Cloud | 4 | 11.7 | 12 | 5 | 13 | 2.8 |
Knife | 3 | 46.8 | 12 | 7 | 13 | 3 |
Table | 4 | 105.6 | 12 | 0 | 13 | |
Saddle | 4 | 7.8 | 10 | 0 | 11 | |
Stairs | 4 | 23.8 | 10 | 0 | 11 | 3.2 |
Bench | 4 | 9.67 | 9 | 1 | 10 | 3.4 |
Snail | 4 | 1.8 | 9 | 0 | 10 | 3.1 |
bottle | 4 | 50.7 | 9 | 0 | 10 | |
Bridge | 4 | 45.7 | 9 | 0 | 10 | 3.2 |
Dress | 4 | 87.2 | 9 | 1 | 10 | 2.6 |
Kitten | 5 | 4.7 | 8 | 15 | 9 | |
Floss | 4 | 2 | 6 | 5 | 7 | 3.6 |
Glove | 4 | 10 | 6 | 0 | 7 | 3.2 |
Church | 3 | 69.7 | 6 | 0 | 7 | 2.7 |
Ankle | 4 | 8 | 3 | 0 | 4 | |
Average | 3.7 | 30.4 | 12.1 | 3.3 | 13.1 | 3.1 |
Appendix B
High Density | ||||
Target Item | Competitor | Distractor 1 | Distractor 2 | Total Neighbours |
Bell | belt | onion | ghost | 58 |
Tail | tailor | biscuit | soap | 53 |
Bike | book | parachute | frog | 46 |
Road | rope | eye | moon | 42 |
Towel | tower | magnet | squirrel | 38 |
Map | man | banana | crib | 37 |
Tear | deer | peas | mattress | 31 |
Doll | dog | carrot | ladybird | 30 |
Low Density | ||||
Target Item | Competitor | Distractor 1 | Distractor 2 | Total Neighbours |
Tree | tray | wing | milk | 23 |
Swing | sling | egg | ball | 20 |
Hood | hook | feather | elephant | 20 |
Cloud | clown | glove | bucket | 13 |
Bench | beach | hoof | palm | 10 |
Snail | snake | bag | present | 10 |
bottle | beetle | fork | lion | 10 |
Bridge | brick | hamster | candle | 10 |
High Density | ||||
Target Item | Competitor | Distractor 1 | Distractor 2 | Total Neighbours |
Bone | Bein (leg) | pig | mushroom | 50 |
Corn | Korb (basket) | needle | goat | 50 |
Duck | Decke (blanket) | foot | strawberry | 47 |
Ham | Hemd (shirt) | stairs | pot | 45 |
Rack | Rock (skirt) | hut | crocodile | 44 |
Hoop | Hupe (horn) | leaf | toe | 40 |
Cook | Krug (jug) | ruler | dress | 32 |
Coin | Kinn (chin) | wolf | saddle | 30 |
Low Density | ||||
Target Item | Competitor | Distractor 1 | Distractor 2 | Total Neighbours |
Tailor | Teller (plate) | bear | corn | 20 |
Horse | Hose (trousers) | bride | comb | 19 |
Fawn | Foen (hairdryer) | tent | star | 19 |
Belt | Bett (bed) | sledge | globe | 19 |
Mower | Mauer (wall) | cherries | owl | 16 |
Kitten | Kissen (pillow) | nose | lamp | 9 |
Floss | Floss (raft) | beetle | mushroom | 7 |
Ankle | Angel (rod) | chair | pencil | 4 |
References
- Barac, Raluca, Ellen Bialystok, Dina C. Castro, and Marta Sanchez. 2014. The cognitive development of young dual language learners: A critical review. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 29: 699–714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bialystok, Ellen. 2010. Global-local and trail-making tasks by monolingual and bilingual children: Beyond inhibition. Developmental Psychology 46: 93–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bialystok, Ellen. 2018. Bilingualism and executive function. In Bilingual Cognition and Language. Edited by Fatih Bayram, David Miller, Jason Rothman and Ludovica Serratrice. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 283–306. [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok, Ellen, and Raluca Barac. 2012. Emerging bilingualism: Dissociating advantages for metalinguistic awareness and executive control. Cognition 122: 67–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bialystok, Ellen, Gigi Luk, Kathleen F. Peets, and Y. A. N. G. Sujin. 2010. Receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 13: 525–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blumenfeld, Henrike K., and Viorica Marian. 2007. Constraints on parallel activation in bilingual spoken language processing: Examining proficiency and lexical status using eye-tracking. Language and Cognitive Processes 22: 633–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, Roger. 1973. A First Language. Oxford: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Brysbaert, Marc, Paweł Mandera, and Emmanuel Keuleers. 2018. The word frequency effect in word processing: An updated review. Current Directions in Psychological Science: A Journal of the American Psychological Society 27: 45–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bylund, Emanuel, Niclas Abrahamsson, Kenneth Hyltenstam, and Gunnar Norrman. 2019. Revisiting the bilingual lexical deficit: The impact of age of acquisition. Cognition 182: 45–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Calafato, Raees. 2022. Fidelity to participants when researching multilingual language teachers: A systematic review. Review of Education 10: e3344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caramazza, Alfonso, Albert Costa, Michele Miozzo, and Yanchao Bi. 2001. The specific-word frequency effect: Implications for the representation of homophones in speech production. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition 27: 1430–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coady, Jeffry A., and Richard N. Aslin. 2003. Phonological neighbourhoods in the developing lexicon. Journal of Child Language 30: 441–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cortese, Michael J., and Maya M. Khanna. 2008. Age of acquisition ratings for 3,000 monosyllabic words. Behavior Research Methods 40: 791–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Costa, Albert, and Alfonso Caramazza. 2000. The cognate faciliation effect: Implications for models of lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 26: 1283–96. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Desroches, Amy S., Randy Lynn Newman, and Marc F. Joanisse. 2009. Investigating the time course of spoken word recognition: Electrophysiological evidence for the influences of phonological similarity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 21: 1893–906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Diependaele, Kevin, Kristin Lemhöfer, and Marc Brysbaert. 2013. The word frequency effect in first- and second-language word recognition: A lexical entrenchment account. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 66: 843–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dufour, Sophie, and Ronald Peereman. 2003. Inhibitory priming effects in auditory word recognition: When The target’s competitors conflict with the prime word. Cognition 88: B33–B44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fenson, Larry, Philip S. Dale, J. Steven Reznick, Elizabeth Bates, Donna J. Thal, Stephen J. Pethick, Michael Tomasello, Carolyn B. Mervis, and Joan Stiles. 1994. Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 59: 1–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Freedman, Skott E., and Jessica A. Barlow. 2012. Using whole-word production measures to determine the Influence of phonotactic probability and neighborhood density on bilingual speech production. The International Journal of Bilingualism: Cross-disciplinary, Cross-linguistic studies of Language Behavior 16: 369–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garlock, Victoria M., Amanda C. Walley, and Jamie L. Metsala. 2001. Age-of-Acquisition, word frequency, and neighborhood density effects on spoken word recognition by children and adults. Journal of Memory and Language 45: 468–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gaskell, M. Gareth, and William D. Marslen-Wilson. 2002. Representation and competition in the perception of spoken words. Cognitive Psychology 45: 220–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Genesee, Fred. 2003. Rethinking bilingual acquisition. In Bilingualism: Beyond Basic Principles: Festschrift in Honour of Hugo Baetens-Beardsmore. Edited by Jean-Marc Dewaele, Alex Housen and Li Wei. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters, pp. 58–182. [Google Scholar]
- Genesee, Fred. 2022. The monolingual bias: A critical analysis. Journal of Immersion and Content-Based Language Education 10: 153–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goh, Winston D., Lidia Suárez, Melvin J. Yap, and Seok Hui Tan. 2009. Distributional analyses in auditory lexical decision: Neighborhood density and word-frequency effects. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 16: 882–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hansen, Pernille. 2017. What makes a word easy to acquire? The effects of word class, frequency, imageability and phonological neighbourhood density on lexical development. First Language 37: 205–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoff, Erika, and Krystal M. Ribot. 2017. Language growth in English monolingual and Spanish-English bilingual children from 2.5 to 5 Years. The Journal of Pediatrics 190: 241–45.e1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Huettig, Falk, Joost Rommers, and Antje S. Meyer. 2011. Using the visual world paradigm to study language processing: A review and critical evaluation. Acta Psychologica 137: 151–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ju, Min, and Paul A. Luce. 2004. Falling on sensitive ears: Constraints on bilingual lexical activation. Psychological Science 15: 314–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Juhasz, Barbara J. 2005. Age-of-acquisition effects in word and picture identification. Psychological Bulletin 131: 684–712. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Klee, Thomas, and Claire Harrison. 2001. CDI Words and Sentences: Validity and Preliminary Norms for British English. Paper presented at the Child Language Seminar, University of Hertfordshire, Hertfordshire, UK, July 9–11. [Google Scholar]
- Luce, Paul A., and David B. Pisoni. 1998. Recognizing spoken words: The neighborhood activation model. Ear and Hearing 19: 1–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Magnuson, James S., James A. Dixon, Michael K. Tanenhaus, and Richard N. Aslin. 2007. The Dynamics of lexical competition during spoken word recognition. Cognitive Science 31: 133–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marchman, Virginia A., and Anne Fernald. 2008. Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and language outcomes in later childhood. Developmental Science 11: F9–F16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marchman, Virginia A., Anne Fernald, and Nereyda Hurtado. 2010. How vocabulary size in two languages relates to efficiency in spoken word recognition by young Spanish–English bilinguals. Journal of Child Language 37: 817–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marian, Viorica, and Henrike Blumenfeld. 2006. Phonological neighbourhood density guides: Lexical access in native and non-native language production. Journal of Social and Ecological Boundaries 2: 3–35. [Google Scholar]
- Marian, Viorica, and Michael Spivey. 2003. Bilingual and monolingual processing of competing lexical items. Applied Psycholinguistics 24: 173–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marian, Viorica, Michael Spivey, and Joy Hirsch. 2003. Shared and separate systems in bilingual language processing: Converging evidence from eyetracking and brain imaging. Brain and Language 86: 70–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Marian, Viorica, Henrike K. Blumenfeld, and Olga V. Boukrina. 2008. Sensitivity to phonological similarity within and across languages. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 37: 141–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marian, Viorica, James Bartolotti, Sarah Chabal, and Anthony Shook. 2012. CLEARPOND: Cross-linguistic easy-access resource for phonological and orthographic neighborhood densities. PLoS ONE 7: e43230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Marslen-Wilson, William D. 1987. Functional parallelism in spoken word recognition. Cognition 25: 71–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Metsala, Jamie L. 1997. An examination of word frequency and neighborhood density in the development of spoken-word recognition. Memory and Cognition 25: 47–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meyer, David E., and Roger W. Schvanefeldt. 1971. Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology 90: 227–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morrison, Catriona M., Andrew W. Ellis, and Philip T. Quinlan. 1992. Age of acquisition, not word frequency, affects object naming, not object recognition. Memory and Cognition 20: 705–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Munson, Benjamin. 1999. Relationships between expressive vocabulary size and spoken word recognition in children. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 106: 2245–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pearson, Barbara Zurer, Sylvia C. Fernández, and D. Kimbrough Oller. 1993. Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: Comparison to monolingual norms. Language Learning 43: 93–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Poarch, Gregory J., and Janet G. van Hell. 2012. Cross-language activation in children’s speech production: Evidence from second language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 111: 419–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schelletter, Christina. 2002. The effect of form similarity on bilingual children’s lexical development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 5: 93–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schelletter, Christina. 2016. Lexikalische Entwicklung von bilingualen Vorschulkindern. In Wortschatzlernen in bilingualen Schulen und Kindertagesstaetten. Edited by A. Steinlen and T. Piske. Forum Angewandte Linguistik, 57. Frankfurt: Peter Lang, pp. 65–82. [Google Scholar]
- Schröder, A., C. Kauschke, and R. Bleser. 2004. Messungen des Erwerbsalters für konkrete Nomina. Neurolinguistik 18: 107–38. [Google Scholar]
- Snodgrass, Joan G., and Mary Vanderwart. 1980. A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Learning and Memory 6: 174–215. [Google Scholar]
- Stokes, Stephanie F. 2010. Neighborhood density and word frequency in toddlers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 53: 670–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Storkel, Holly L. 2004. Do children acquire dense neighborhoods? An investigation of similarity neighborhoods in lexical acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics 25: 201–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sullivan, Margot D., Gregory J. Poarch, and Ellen Bialystok. 2018. Why is Lexical Retrieval Slower for Bilinguals? Evidence from Picture Naming. Bilingualism (Cambridge, England) 21: 479–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Szekely, Anna, Thomas Jacobsen, Simona D’Amico, Antonella Devescovi, Elena Andonova, Daniel Herron, Ching Ching Lu, Thomas Pechmann, Csaba Pléh, Nicole Wicha, and et al. 2004. A new on-line resource for psycholinguistic studies. Journal of Memory and Language 51: 247–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vitevitch, Michael S. 2002. The influence of phonological similarity neighborhoods on speech production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 28: 735–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Von Holzen, Katie, and Nivedita Mani. 2012. Language nonselective lexical access in bilingual toddlers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 113: 569–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Weber, Andrea, and Anne Cutler. 2004. Lexical competition in non-native spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language 50: 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wobbrock, Jacob O., Leah Findlater, Darren Gergle, and James J. Higgins. 2011. The Aligned Rank Transform for Nonparametric Factorial Analyses Using Only ANOVA Procedures. Paper presented at the ACM Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI ‘11), Vancouver, BA, USA, May 7–12; New York: ACM Press, pp. 143–46. [Google Scholar]
Measures/Group | High Density | Low Density | ANOVA |
---|---|---|---|
Accuracy (SD) | |||
Monolinguals (n = 12) | 0.83 (0.07) | 0.73 (0.05) | |
Bilinguals (n = 12) | 0.65 (0.18) | 0.60 (0.16) | |
All (n = 24) | 0.74 (0.16) | 0.67 (0.13) | F = 9.6 ** |
Reaction Times [ms] (SD) | |||
Monolinguals (n = 12) | 2280 (327) | 2197 (303) | |
Bilinguals (n = 12) | 2600 (581) | 2798 (392) | |
All (n = 24) | 2451 (502) | 2480 (437) | F = 0.86 |
Neighbours | Frequency | Age of Acquisition (AoA) | Monolingual RT | Bilingual RT | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Neighbours | 1 | 0.176 | −0.158 | 0.039 | −0.142 |
Frequency | 0.176 | 1 | −0.537 ** | −0.405 * | −0.271 |
AoA | −0.158 | −0.537 ** | 1 | 0.461 ** | 0.259 |
Monolingual RT | 0.039 | −0.405 * | 0.461 ** | 1 | 0.628 ** |
Bilingual RT | −0.142 | −0.271 | 0.259 | 0.628 ** | 1 |
Measures/Group | High Density | Low Density | ANOVA |
---|---|---|---|
Accuracy (SD) | |||
Adults (n = 33) ** | |||
Monolingual adults (n = 21) | 1 (0) | 0.99 (0.04) | |
Bilingual adults (n = 12) | 0.9 (0.12) | 0.92 (0.11) | |
Children (n = 23) | |||
Monolingual children (n = 12) | 0.97 (0.08) | 0.97 (0.08) | |
Bilingual children (n = 11) | 0.92 (0.12) | 0.92 (0.10) | |
F = 0.29 | |||
Reaction Times [ms] (SD) | |||
Adults (n = 33) | |||
Monolingual adults (n = 21) | 1316 (195) | 1365 (296) | |
Bilingual adults (n = 12) | 1501 (351) | 1548 (344) | |
Children (n = 23) ** | |||
Monolingual children (n = 12) | 1632 (389) | 1848 (277) | |
Bilingual children (n = 11) | 2275 (791) | 2306 (572) | |
F = 6.43 * |
Measures/Group | High Density | Low Density | ANOVA |
---|---|---|---|
Accuracy (SD) | |||
Adults (n = 33) ** | |||
Monolingual adults (n = 21) | 0.96 (0.06) | 1 (0) | |
Bilingual adults (n = 12) | 0.83 (0.11) | 0.8 (0.11) | |
Children (n = 23) ** | |||
Monolingual children (n = 12) | 0.99 (0.04) | 0.96 (0.06) | |
Bilingual children (n = 11) | 0.97 (0.12) | 0.8 (0.2) | |
F = 7.9 ** | |||
Reaction Times [ms] (SD) | |||
Adults (n = 33) | |||
Monolingual adults (n = 21) | 1459 (222) | 1483 (212) | |
Bilingual adults (n = 12) | 1551 (265) | 1523 (277) | |
Children (n = 23) ** | |||
Monolingual children (n = 12) | 1842 (302) | 2222 (516) | |
Bilingual children (n = 11) | 2146 (288) | 2458 (546) | |
F = 6.8 * |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Schelletter, C. Neighbourhood Density in Spoken Word Recognition: An Eye-Tracking Study. Languages 2024, 9, 329. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9100329
Schelletter C. Neighbourhood Density in Spoken Word Recognition: An Eye-Tracking Study. Languages. 2024; 9(10):329. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9100329
Chicago/Turabian StyleSchelletter, Christina. 2024. "Neighbourhood Density in Spoken Word Recognition: An Eye-Tracking Study" Languages 9, no. 10: 329. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9100329
APA StyleSchelletter, C. (2024). Neighbourhood Density in Spoken Word Recognition: An Eye-Tracking Study. Languages, 9(10), 329. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9100329