Main Concepts in the Spoken Discourse of Persons with Aphasia: Analysis on a Propositional and Linguistic Level
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Informativeness of Discourse in PwA
1.2. Verbal Productivity and Grammaticality of Discourse in PwA
2. Aims of the Study
3. Methods
3.1. Participants
3.2. Materials
3.3. Procedure
3.3.1. Participant Recruitment and Testing Procedure
3.3.2. Transcription Methodology
- All transcripts were checked in such a way that only utterances related to the content of the picture were included. All personal comments of the PwA and HS (e.g., I don’t know what else to say, I don’t know what it is) were excluded from further analyses.
- The ‘cleaned transcripts’ were then reviewed to identify and list the content units, i.e., relevant statements that contain only one verb and convey information about the picture (Nicholas and Brookshire 1995; Hameister and Nickels 2018). Each of the three independent raters (all three authors) analysed one-third of the transcripts and segmented the utterances into relevant concepts. From each rater, one-third of the transcripts were randomly selected and assigned to another rater for evaluation. In other words, three independent raters randomly exchanged one-third of their transcripts to check for agreement between raters. The results show that the 38 HS produced 363 relevant concepts, and the agreement between the raters was 96%, whereas the 38 PwA produced 264 relevant concepts, and inter-rater agreement was 92%. All disagreements were resolved by consensus.
- Using relevant concepts produced by HS, three independent raters (all three authors) identified the phrases that expressed the same meaning despite being worded differently. For example, The cat was catching a fish from the aquarium and The cat grabs the fish both stand for the concept Cat wants to catch a fish from the aquarium. After all the concepts of the HS were identified, all phrases that made up one concept were combined. Based on this procedure, 29 concepts were identified from the discourse production of HS (see Table S2 in Supplementary Materials).
3.3.3. MC Coding and Variables
- Using the criterion mentioned in Richardson and Dalton (2016), the main concept (MC) could be considered essential if it was mentioned by at least 30% of participants. All concepts that were produced by HS at this percentage served as the baseline for analysing the ideas expressed by PwA. In this step, only nine out of twenty-nine MCs satisfied this criterion (see Table S2 in Supplementary Materials).
- MCs are structures that consist of two or more essential elements—minimally, a verb and its constituent nouns or/and prepositional phrases or other clauses that operate on the main verb (Dalton and Richardson 2019). Using the established MC lists, stories produced by all speakers were scored for the presence or absence of MCs, as well as for the accuracy and completeness of the MCs present. Coding procedures proposed by Nicholas and Brookshire (1995) and later refined by Dalton and Richardson (2019) were utilised:
- (a)
- Missing MCs were coded as absent (AB).
- (b)
- MCs that were present (P) could receive one of four codes based on their accuracy and completeness: the AC code was assigned if all essential elements were accurate (A) and complete (C); the AI code was assigned if all essential information that was produced was accurate (A), but one or more of them were missing, i.e., incomplete (I); the IC code was assigned if all essential elements were present and complete (C), but some essential elements were inaccurate (I) based on the control speakers’ productions; and finally, the II code was assigned if one or more essential elements were incomplete (I), and one or more of the essential elements that were produced were inaccurate (I) (see Table S3 in the Supplementary Materials; Richardson and Dalton 2016). In the present study, the inter-rater agreement in assigning codes was overall 89% for all groups combined. All disagreements were resolved by consensus.
3.3.4. Specific Language Measures—Coding and Variables
- (1)
- Number of words—words had to be intelligible in the context, but did not have to be accurate, relevant, or informative relative to the eliciting stimulus (Nicholas and Brookshire 1993);
- (2)
- Number of verbs—all verbs were counted separately. For example, similar to English, in Croatian, all modal verbs require the main verb in the infinitive form (on želi jesti—he wants to eat), and these two verbs in modal constructions were treated as two separated verbs;
- (3)
- Number of correct information units (CIU)—words had to be accurate, relevant, and informative relative to the eliciting stimulus; they did not have to be used in a grammatically accurate manner to be counted as ClUs. Each CIU consisted of a single word, and only words that were included in the word count could be counted as CIUs (Nicholas and Brookshire 1993);
- (4)
- Ratio of verbs per words—since main concepts consist of a verb, it was important to determine the share of verbs in the total lexical production;
- (5)
- Ratio of CIU per words—this ratio provides an insight into how many, out of the total words produced, are directly related to the informative content of discourse production;
- (6)
- Number of clauses—a string of words that includes a subject and a predicate;
- (7)
- Number of T-units (terminable unit)—the shortest grammatically allowable sentence that consists of one main clause and any attached subordinate clauses (Hunt 1970; Nippold et al. 2008);
- (8)
- Clausal density—the only measure that was calculated as the average number of clauses produced per T-unit.
4. Results
4.1. Production of Main Concepts
4.1.1. Presence and Omissions of Main Concepts
4.1.2. Accuracy and Completeness of the Produced Main Concepts
4.2. Productivity, Informativeness, and Grammaticality in a Spoken Discourse
4.3. Correlations between Main Concepts and Language Measures of Productivity, Informativeness, and Grammaticality
5. Discussion
5.1. Production of Main Concepts
- (1)
- MC1, MC2, and MC7—these three MCs are the ones with the lowest percentage of marking in both groups (5.3–23.7% in PwA and a little above 31% in HS for all three). This indicates that out of the nine extracted MCs, these three seem to be the least important, because they are related to the description of the space and setting up the context. The only significant difference between PwA and HS was observed in Code II for two concepts, MC1 and MC7. This indicates that when PwA decided to mark MC1 and MC7, these concepts were more often inaccurate and incomplete compared to the marking by HS. Due to the extremely low percentage of marking in both groups, no difference was observed for MC2 for any code.
- (2)
- MC3, MC4, and MC9—we found that MC4 and MC9 are moderately represented in HS and barely in PwA, whereas MC3 is represented to a greater extent in both groups. For these three concepts, significant differences were found for two codes: Code AC, which states that the HS group is significantly more successful, as they mark these concepts entirely accurately and more completely than PwA; and Code AI, for which PwA produced accurate but incomplete concepts more often than HS. This means that PwA omitted some language elements while forming the MC. For example, they said: She wakes up (object missing—the father); The cat knocks down (object missing—books); and so on.
- (3)
- MC5, MC6, and MC8—in HS, MC6 and MC8 are the most frequently expressed concepts, whereas it is MC8 in the PwA group. MC6 is moderately frequent in both groups. There was a significant difference between the two groups in all four codes: in Code AC for all three MCs, for which HS marked these MCs more accurately and completely than PwA; Code AI was significant only for MC8, implying that the PwA were more likely to leave out something when marking this concept, because the MC was not completed. For example, The cat catches (missing object—fish in an aquarium). The significant difference in the Code IC appeared in MC5 and MC6, for which PwA had more problems with the correct lexical marking of the concept. For example, The girl calls him by the hand, instead of pulls him. Code II was significantly different between the groups for all three concepts, which means that the PwA often marked these concepts inaccurately and incompletely. For example, The fox is hunting. She wants to take this jelasa* (a neologism).
5.2. Trade-Offs in Discourse between MC and Linguistic Encoding
5.3. Clinical Implications
5.4. Study Limitations and Future Perspectives
6. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Andreetta, Sara, Anna Cantagallo, and Andrea Marini. 2012. Narrative discourse in anomic aphasia. Neuropsychologia 50: 1787–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Armstrong, Elisabeth. 2000. Aphasic discourse analysis: The story so far. Aphasiology 14: 875–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Armstrong, Linda, Marion Brady, Catherine Mackenzie, and John Norrie. 2007. Transcription-less analysis of aphasic discourse: A clinician’s dream or a possibility? Aphasiology 21: 355–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bastiaanse, Roelien, and Roel Jonkers. 1998. Verb retrieval in action naming and spontaneous speech in agrammatic and anomic aphasia. Aphasiology 12: 951–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bastiaanse, Roelien, Elif Bamyaci, Chien-Ju Hsu, Jiyeon Lee, Tuba Yarbay Duman, and Cynthia K. Thompson. 2011. Time reference in agrammatic aphasia: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Neurolinguistics 24: 652–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bastiaanse, Roelien, Judith Rispens, Esther Ruigendijk, Onésimo Juncos Rabadaán, and Cynthia K. Thompson. 2002. Verbs: Some properties and their consequences for agrammatic Broca’s aphasia. Journal of Neurolinguistics 15: 239–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bastiaanse, Roelien, Susan Edwards, and Katalin Kiss. 1996. Fluent aphasia in three languages: Aspects of spontaneous speech. Aphasiology 10: 561–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Black, Maria, and Shula Chiat. 2000. Putting thoughts into verbs—Developmental and acquired impairments. In Semantic Processing: Theory and Practice. Edited by Wendy Best, Karen L. Bryan and Jane Maxim. London: Whurr Publishers, pp. 52–79. [Google Scholar]
- Bryant, Lucy, Alison Ferguson, and Elisabeth Spencer. 2016. Linguistic analysis of discourse in aphasia: A review of the literature. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics 30: 489–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chafe, Wallace. 1990. Some things that narratives tell us about the mind. In Narrative Thought and Narrative Language. Edited by Bruce K. Britton and Anthony D. Pellegrini. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., pp. 79–98. [Google Scholar]
- Cruice, Madeline, Madeleine Pritchard, and Lucy Dipper. 2014. Verb use in aphasic and non-aphasic personal discourse: What is normal? Journal of Neurolinguistics 28: 31–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dalton, Sarah Grace, and Jessica D. Richardson. 2015. Core-lexicon and main-concept production during picture-sequence description in adults without brain damage and adults with aphasia. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 24: 923–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dalton, Sarah Grace H., and Jessica D. Richardson. 2019. A Large-Scale Comparison of Main Concept Production Between Persons With Aphasia and Persons Without Brain Injury. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 28: 293–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dean, Michael, and Maria Black. 2005. Exploring event processing and description in people with aphasia. Aphasiology 19: 521–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dipper, Lucy T., Maria Black, and Karen L. Bryan. 2005. Thinking for speaking and thinking for listening: The interaction of thought and language in typical and non-fluent comprehension and production. Language and Cognitive Processes 20: 417–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dipper, Lucy, Jane Marshall, Mary Boyle, Deborah Hersh, Nicola Botting, and Madeline Cruice. 2021. Creating a Theoretical Framework to Underpin Discourse Assessment and Intervention in Aphasia. Brain Sciences 11: 183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fergadiotis, Gerasimos, and Heather Harris Wright. 2011. Lexical diversity for adults with and without aphasia across discourse elicitation tasks. Aphasiology 25: 1414–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Friedmann, Naama, and Yosef Grodzinsky. 1997. Tense and agreement in agrammatic production: Pruning the syntactic tree. Brain and Language 56: 397–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hameister, Inga, and Lyndsey Nickels. 2018. The cat in the tree—Using picture descriptions to inform our understanding of conceptualisation in aphasia. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 33: 1296–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hegde, Mahabalagiri N. 2018. A Coursebook on Aphasia and Other Neurogenic Language Disorders, 4th ed. San Diego: Plural Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Helm-Estabrooks, Nancy, and Gail Ramsberger. 1986. Treatment of agrammatism in long-term Broca’s aphasia. British Journal of Disorders of Communication 21: 39–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hunt, Kellogg W. 1970. Syntactic maturity in school children and adults. In Monograph of the Society for Research in Child Development, Serial N. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, vol. 134, p. 35. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, Lisa, Alexandra Basilakos, Grigori Yourganov, Bo Cai, Leonardo Bonilha, Chris Rorden, and Julius Fridriksson. 2019. Progression of aphasia severity in the chronic stages of stroke. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 28: 639–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, Lisa, Samaneh Nemati, Leonardo Bonilha, Chris Rorden, Natalie Busby, Alexandra Basilakos, Roger Newman-Norlund, Argye E. Hillis, Gregory Hickok, and Julius Fridriksson. 2022. Predictors beyond the lesion: Health and demographic factors associated with aphasia severity. Cortex 154: 375–89. [Google Scholar]
- Jonkers, Roel, and Annelies de Bruin. 2009. Tense processing in Broca’s and Wernicke’s aphasia. Aphasiology 23: 1252–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jonkers, Roel, and Roelien M. Bastiaanse. 1996. The influence of instrumentality and transitivity on action naming in Broca’s and anomic aphasia. Brain and Language 1996: 37–39. [Google Scholar]
- Kolk, Herman. 1995. A time-based approach to agrammatic production. Brain and Language 50: 282–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kuvač Kraljević, Jelena, Ana Matić, and Karolina Lice. 2019. Putting the CAT-HR out: Key properties and specificities. Aphasiology 34: 820–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Larfeuil, Céline, and Guylaine L. Dorze. 1997. An analysis of the word-finding difficulties and of the content of the content of the discourse of recent and chronic aphasic speakers. Aphasiology 11: 783–811. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Laska, Ann Charlotte, Anders Hellblom, Veronica Murray, Thomas Kahan, and Magnus Von Arbin. 2001. Aphasia in acute stroke and relation to outcome. Journal of Internal Medicine 249: 413–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Levelt, Willem J. M., and Herbert Schriffers. 1987. Stages of Lexical Access. In Natural Language Generation: Recent Advances in Artificial Intelligence Psychology and Linguistics. Edited by Gerard Kempen. Dodrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers, pp. 395–404. [Google Scholar]
- Linnik, Anastasia, Roelien Bastiaanse, and Barbara Höhle. 2016. Discourse production in aphasia: A current review of theoretical and methodological challenges. Aphasiology 30: 765–800. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marini, Andea, Sara Andreetta, Silvana Del Tin, and Sergio Carlomagno. 2011. A multi-level approach to the analysis of narrative language in aphasia. Aphasiology 25: 1372–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martini, Karen, Adriana Belletti, Santi Centorrino, and Maria Garraffa. 2020. Syntactic complexity in the presence of an intervener: The case of an Italian speaker with anomia. Aphasiology 34: 1016–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McNeil, Malcolm R., Patrick J. Doyle, Tepanta R. D. Fossett, Grace H. Park, and Amy J. Goda. 2001. Reliability and concurrent validity of the information unit scoring metric for the story retelling procedure. Aphasiology 15: 991–1006. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nicholas, Linda E., and Robert H. Brookshire. 1993. A system for quantifying the informativeness and efficiency of the connected speech of adults with aphasia. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 36: 338–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nicholas, Linda E., and Robert H. Brookshire. 1995. Presence, completeness, and accuracy of main concepts in the connected speech of non-brain-damaged adults and adults with aphasia. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 38: 145–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nippold, Marilyn A., Linda J. Hesketh, Jill K. Duthie, and Tracy C. Mansfield. 2005. Conversational versus expository discourse: A study of syntactic development in children, adolescents, and adults. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 48: 1048–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nippold, Marilyn A., Megan W. Frantz-Kaspar, Paige M. Cramond, Cevilia Kirk, Christine Hayward-Mayhew, and Melanie MacKinnon. 2014. Conversational and Narrative Speaking in Adolescents: Examining the Use of Complex Syntax. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 57: 876–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nippold, Marilyn A., Tracy C. Mansfield, Jesse L. Billow, and J. Bruce Tomblin. 2008. Expository discourse in adolescents with language impairments: Examining syntactic development. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 17: 356–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Osa García, Alberto, Simona M. Brambati, Amélie Brisebois, Marianne Désilets-Barnabé, Bérengère Houzé, Christophe Bedetti, Elizabeth Rochon, Carol Leonard, Alex Desautels, and Karine Marcotte. 2020. Predicting early post-stroke aphasia outcome from initial aphasia severity. Frontiers in Neurology 11: 120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pedersen, Palle M., Kirsten Vinter, and Tom S. Olsen. 2004. Aphasia after stroke: Type, severity and prognosis. Cerebrovascular Diseases 17: 35–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Plowman, Emily, Brecken Hentz, and Charles Ellis. 2012. Post-stroke aphasia prognosis: A review of patient-related and stroke-related factors. Journal of Evaluation in Clinical Practice 18: 689–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Richardson, Jessica D., and Sarah G. Dalton. 2016. Main concepts for three different discourse tasks in a large non-clinical sample. Aphasiology 30: 45–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rossi, Eleonora, and Roelien Bastiaanse. 2008. Spontaneous speech in Italian agrammatic aphasia: A focus on verb production. Aphasiology 22: 347–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Saber-Moghadam, Reihaneh, Afsaneh Zeinalzadeh, Mehrzad Momenzadeh, Mohammad Taghi Farzadfar, Hamide Ghaemi, and Davood Sobhani-Rad. 2022. The Relationship Between Memory, Type, and Severity of Aphasia With Confrontation Naming in Post-stroke Patients With Chronic Aphasia. Iranian Rehabilitation Journal 20: 561–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Soroli, Eva, Halima Sahraoui, and Carol Sacchett. 2012. Linguistic encoding of motion events in English and French: Typological constraints on second language acquisition and agrammatic aphasia. Language, Interaction & Acquisition 3: 261–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Swinburn, Kate, Gillian Porter, David Howard, Jelena Kuvač Kraljević, Karolina Lice, and Ana Matić. 2020. Comprehensive Aphasia Test (CAT-HR). Jastrebarsko: Naklada Slap. [Google Scholar]
- Thompson, Cynthia K., Kenneth L. Lange, Sandra L. Schneider, and Lewis P. Shapiro. 1997. Agrammatic and non-brain-damaged subjects’ verb and verb argument structure production. Aphasiology 11: 473–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tulving, Endel. 2002. Episodic Memory: From Mind to Brain. Annual Review of Psychology 53: 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- WMA (World Medical Association). 2001. World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. Bulletin of the World Health Organization 79: 373–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wright, Heather H., and Gilson J. Capilouto. 2012. Considering a multi-level approach to understanding maintenance of global coherence in adults with aphasia. Aphasiology 26: 656–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yorkston, Kathryn M., and David R. Beukelman. 1980. An analysis of connected speech samples of aphasic and normal speakers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 45: 27–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zingeser, Louise B., and Rita S. Berndt. 1990. Retrieval of nouns and verbs in agrammatism and anomia. Brain and Language 39: 14–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Acute (N = 19) | Chronic (N = 19) | PwA (N = 38) | HS (N = 38) | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Gender | ||||
Male | 13 (68.42%) | 13 (68.42%) | 26 (68.42%) | 26 (68.42%) |
Female | 6 (31.58%) | 6 (31.58%) | 12 (31.58%) | 12 (31.58%) |
Age | ||||
M (SD) | 54.74 (16.19) | 58.34 (15.64) | 56.55 (15.81) | 56.91 (15.27) |
Range | 23–88 | 32–86 | 23–88 | 23–84 |
Education level | ||||
8 years | 1 (5.26%) | 0 (0%) | 1 (2.63%) | 1 (2.63%) |
9–12 years | 12 (63.16%) | 11 (57.89%) | 23 (60.53%) | 23 (60.53%) |
>12 years | 6 (31.58%) | 8 (42.11%) | 14 (36.84%) | 14 (36.84%) |
Time post-stroke (in months) | 0–3 | 5–79 | 0–79 | |
CAT-HR M (SD) | 102.24 (7.5) | 99.5 (7.33) | 102.79 (8.04) | |
Mild—M (SD) | 16 (3.94) range 101–114.88 | 8 (4.76) range 100.75–113.25 | 24 (4.31) range 100.75–114.88 | |
Moderate—M (SD) | 3 (3.88) range 87.25–94.75 | 11 (4.27) range 88.13–99.38 | 14 (4.25) range 87.25–99.38 |
Variable | Group | N | M | SD | Min | Max |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N of main concepts | HS | 38 | 4.71 | 1.49 | 2.00 | 8.00 |
PwA whole | 38 | 2.87 | 1.34 | 1.00 | 6.00 | |
PwA acute | 19 | 3.26 | 1.15 | 1.00 | 6.00 | |
PwA chronic | 19 | 2.58 | 1.43 | 1.00 | 5.00 |
Main Concept | Group | Code AB (%) | Code P (%) |
---|---|---|---|
MC 1 | HS | 65.8 | 34.2 |
PwA whole | 76.3 | 23.7 | |
PwA acute | 73.7 | 26.3 | |
PwA chronic | 78.9 | 21.1 | |
MC 2 | HS | 68.4 | 31.6 |
PwA whole | 89.5 | 10.5 | |
PwA acute | 89.5 | 10.5 | |
PwA chronic | 89.5 | 10.5 | |
MC 3 | HS | 36.8 | 63.2 |
PwA whole | 42.1 | 57.9 | |
PwA acute | 36.8 | 63.2 | |
PwA chronic | 47.4 | 52.6 | |
MC 4 | HS | 60.5 | 39.5 |
PwA whole | 81.6 | 18.4 | |
PwA acute | 73.7 | 26.3 | |
PwA chronic | 89.5 | 10.5 | |
MC 5 | HS | 29 | 71 |
PwA whole | 71 | 29 | |
PwA acute | 63.2 | 36.8 | |
PwA chronic | 78.9 | 21.1 | |
MC 6 | HS | 39.5 | 60.5 |
PwA whole | 52.6 | 47.4 | |
PwA acute | 42.1 | 57.9 | |
PwA chronic | 63.2 | 36.8 | |
MC 7 | HS | 68.4 | 31.6 |
PwA whole | 74.7 | 5.3 | |
PwA acute | 89.5 | 10.5 | |
PwA chronic | 100 | 0 | |
MC 8 | HS | 10.5 | 89.5 |
PwA whole | 21.1 | 78.9 | |
PwA acute | 21.1 | 78.9 | |
PwA chronic | 21.1 | 78.9 | |
MC 9 | HS | 50 | 50 |
PwA whole | 79 | 21 | |
PwA acute | 84.2 | 15.8 | |
PwA chronic | 73.7 | 26.3 |
Main Concepts | Group | Code II (%) | t-Test | Code IC (%) | t-Test | Code AI (%) | t-Test | CODE AC (%) | t-Test |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
MC 1 | PwA | 33.33 | t = 2.24; p = 0.03 * | 0 | t = −1.23; p = 0.22 ns | 66.67 | t = −0.95; p = 0.34 ns | 0 | t = 1.93; p = 0.054 ns |
HS | 0 | 15.39 | 46.15 | 30.77 | |||||
MC 2 | PwA | 25 | t = 1.79; p = 0.07 ns | 25 | t = 0.87; p = 0.38 ns | 25 | t = 1.15; p = 0.25 ns | 25 | t = 0.31; p = 0.77 ns |
HS | 0 | 8.33 | 58.33 | 33.33 | |||||
MC 3 | PwA | 0 | - | 13.04 | t = −1.83; p = 0.07 ns | 26.09 | t = −2.68; p = 0.01 ** | 60.87 | t =−3.41; p = 0.001 ** |
HS | 0 | 0 | 0 | 100 | |||||
MC 4 | PwA | 14.29 | t = 1.55; p = 0.12 ns | 14.29 | t = 0.58; p = 0.56 ns | 71.43 | t = −1.99; p = 0.04 * | 0 | t = −2.93; p = 0.002 ** |
HS | 0 | 6.67 | 26.67 | 66.67 | |||||
MC 5 | PwA | 27.27 | t = 2.15; p = 0.03 * | 36.36 | t = 3.31; p = 0.00 ** | 27.27 | t = 1.24; p = 0.21 ns | 9.09 | t = 4.41; p < 0.000 ** |
HS | 3.7 | 0 | 11.11 | 85.19 | |||||
MC 6 | PwA | 16.67 | t = 2.08; p = 0.04 * | 33.33 | t = −1.98; p = 0.04 * | 5.56 | t = 0.18; p = 0.86 ns | 44.44 | t = −2.90; p = 0.004 ** |
HS | 0 | 8.69 | 4.35 | 86.96 | |||||
MC 7 | PwA | 50 | t = 2.54; p = 0.01 ** | 0 | t = −0.62; p = 0.54 ns | 0 | t = −0.62; p = 0.54 ns | 50 | t = −0.46; p = 0.65 ns |
HS | 0 | 16.67 | 16.67 | 66.67 | |||||
MC 8 | PwA | 20 | t = 2.74; p = 0.006 ** | 0 | t = −1.35; p = 0.18 ns | 70 | t = −3.01; p = 0.003 ** | 10 | t = 4.27; p < 0.000 ** |
HS | 0 | 5.88 | 32.35 | 61.76 | |||||
MC 9 | PwA | 12.5 | t = 1.57; p = 0.12 ns | 12.5 | t = 1.57; p = 0.12 ns | 25 | t = 2.27; p = 0.02 * | 50 | t = −3.34; p = 0.000 ** |
HS | 0 | 0 | 0 | 100 |
Main Concepts | Group | Code II (%) | t-Test | Code IC (%) | t-Test | Code AI (%) | t-Test | Code AC (%) | t-Test |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
MC 1 | PwA acute | 0 | t = −1.805; p = 0.07 ns | 0 | - | 26.3 | t = 1.775; p = 0.077 ns | 0 | - |
PwA chronic | 15.8 | 0 | 15.8 | 0 | |||||
MC 2 | PwA acute | 0 | t = −1.017; p = 0.308 ns | 5.3 | t = −1.017; p = 0.308 ns | 0 | t = −1.017; p = 0.308 ns | 5.3 | t = −1.017; p = 0.308 ns |
PwA chronic | 5.3 | 0 | 5.3 | 0 | |||||
MC 3 | PwA acute | 0 | - | 5.3 | t = −2.946; p = 0.003 ** | 21.1 | t = 1.439; p = 0.149 ns | 36.8 | - |
PwA chronic | 0 | 47.4 | 5.3 | 36.8 | |||||
MC 4 | PwA acute | 0 | t = −1.017; p = 0.308 ns | 5.3 | t = −1.017; p = 0.308 ns | 21.1 | t = 1.439; p = 0.149 ns | 0 | - |
PwA chronic | 5.3 | 0 | 5.3 | 0 | |||||
MC 5 | PwA acute | 10.5 | t = 0.594; p = 0.555 ns | 15.8 | t = 1.054; p = 0.294 ns | 5.3 | t = 0.594; p = 0.555 ns | 5.3 | t = −1.017; p = 0.308 ns |
PwA chronic | 5.3 | 5.3 | 10.5 | 0 | |||||
MC 6 | PwA acute | 0 | t = −1.805; p = 0.07 ns | 21.1 | t = 0.896; p = 0.368 ns | 0 | t = −1.017; p = 0.308 ns | 36.8 | t = 2.382; p = 0.017 * |
PwA chronic | 15.8 | 10.5 | 5.3 | 5.3 | |||||
MC 7 | PwA acute | 5.3 | t = −1.017; p = 0.308 ns | 0 | - | 0 | - | 5.3 | t = −1.017; p = 0.308 ns |
PwA chronic | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | |||||
MC 8 | PwA acute | 15.8 | - | 0 | - | 63.2 | t = 0.979; p = 0.327 ns | 0 | t = −1.805; p = 0.07 ns |
PwA chronic | 15.8 | 0 | 47.4 | 15.8 | |||||
MC 9 | PwA acute | 0 | t = −1.017; p = 0.308 ns | 0 | t = −1.017; p = 0.308 ns | 0 | t = −1.451; p = 0.147 ns | 15.8 | t = 1.054; p = 0.294 ns |
PwA chronic | 5.3 | 5.3 | 10.5 | 5.3 |
Variable | Group | N | M | SD | Min | Max | Between-Group Differences |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N of words | PwA | 38 | 30.55 | 16.37 | 8.00 | 78.00 | U = 424.5; z = −3.257; p = 0.002 ** |
HS | 38 | 44.39 | 21.63 | 17.00 | 107.00 | ||
N of verbs | PwA | 38 | 9.18 | 6.57 | 2.00 | 30.00 | U = 436; z = −2.981; p = 0.003 ** |
HS | 38 | 12.89 | 6.64 | 5.00 | 33.00 | ||
N of CIU | PwA | 38 | 19.38 | 9.75 | 6.00 | 50.00 | U = 182; z = −5.614; p = 0.000 ** |
HS | 38 | 42.16 | 20.78 | 17.00 | 99.00 | ||
Verbs/words | PwA | 38 | 0.30 | 0.09 | 0.10 | 0.51 | U = 690; z = −0.333; p = 0.739 |
HS | 38 | 0.29 | 0.06 | 0.18 | 0.43 | ||
CIU/words | PwA | 38 | 0.69 | 0.21 | 0.24 | 1.00 | U = 156; z = −5.944; p = 0.000 ** |
HS | 38 | 0.95 | 0.08 | 0.57 | 1.00 | ||
N of clauses | PwA | 38 | 6.29 | 3.23 | 1.00 | 16.00 | U = 337.5; z = −4.026; p = 0.000 ** |
HS | 38 | 9.79 | 3.95 | 4.00 | 19.00 | ||
N of T-units | PwA | 38 | 5.47 | 3.20 | 1.00 | 16.00 | U = 484.5; z = −2.492; p = 0.013 ** |
HS | 38 | 7.16 | 3.60 | 2.00 | 16.00 | ||
Clausal density | PwA | 38 | 1.22 | 0.35 | 1.00 | 2.33 | U = 404; z = −3.257; p = 0.001 ** |
HS | 38 | 1.48 | 0.45 | 1.00 | 2.66 |
Variable | Group | N | M | SD | Min | Max | Between-Group Differences |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N of words | PwA acute | 19 | 26.42 | 11.56 | 8.00 | 53.00 | U = 220; z = 1.154; p = 0.258 |
PwA chronic | 19 | 34.68 | 19.52 | 11.00 | 78.00 | ||
N of verbs | PwA acute | 19 | 8.42 | 5.55 | 2.00 | 27.00 | U = 194; z = 0.396; p = 0.708 |
PwA chronic | 19 | 9.95 | 7.53 | 2.00 | 30.00 | ||
N of CIU | PwA acute | 19 | 17.74 | 7.05 | 7.00 | 31.00 | U = 207.5; z = 0.789; p = 0.435 |
PwA chronic | 19 | 21.42 | 11.86 | 6.00 | 50.00 | ||
Verbs/words | PwA acute | 19 | 0.31 | 0.09 | 0.10 | 0.51 | U = 138.5; z = −1.228; p = 0.223 |
PwA chronic | 19 | 0.28 | 0.91 | 0.13 | 0.45 | ||
CIU/words | PwA acute | 19 | 0.73 | 0.23 | 0.24 | 1.00 | U = 135; z = −1.329; p = 0.191 |
PwA chronic | 19 | 0.66 | 0.18 | 0.27 | 0.95 | ||
N of clauses | PwA acute | 19 | 6.05 | 2.44 | 2.00 | 11.00 | U = 183.5; z = 0.089; p = 0.931 |
PwA chronic | 19 | 6.53 | 3.94 | 1.00 | 16.00 | ||
N of T-units | PwA acute | 19 | 5.21 | 2.35 | 2.00 | 10.00 | U = 184; z = 0.104; p = 0.931 |
PwA chronic | 19 | 6.53 | 3.94 | 1.00 | 16.00 | ||
Clausal density | PwA acute | 19 | 1.21 | 0.38 | 1.00 | 2.33 | U = 179; z = 0.269; p = 0.822 |
PwA chronic | 19 | 1.23 | 0.33 | 1.00 | 2.00 |
Group | HS | PwA Whole | PwA Acute | PwA Chronic | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Measure | N of MC | |||||||
r | p | r | p | r | p | r | p | |
N of words | 0.531 | 0.001 ** | 0.154 | 0.357 | 0.068 | 0.782 | 0.261 | 0.280 |
N of verbs | 0.479 | 0.002 ** | 0.262 | 0.113 | 0.183 | 0.454 | 0.382 | 0.106 |
N of CIU | 0.537 | 0.001 ** | 0.476 | 0.002 ** | 0.538 | 0.018 * | 0.469 | 0.043 * |
Verbs/words | −0.023 | 0.892 | 0.109 | 0.515 | −0.057 | 0.816 | 0.188 | 0.440 |
CIU/words | −0.168 | 0.315 | 0.384 | 0.017 * | 0.443 | 0.058 | 0.238 | 0.326 |
N of clauses | 0.501 | 0.001 ** | 0.343 | 0.035 * | 0.300 | 0.212 | 0.436 | 0.062 |
N of T-units | 0.356 | 0.028 * | 0.136 | 0.414 | 0.020 | 0.934 | 0.238 | 0.326 |
Clausal density | 0.068 | 0.683 | 0.214 | 0.204 | 0.438 | 0.061 | 0.097 | 0.702 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Kuvač Kraljević, J.; Matić Škorić, A.; Lice, K. Main Concepts in the Spoken Discourse of Persons with Aphasia: Analysis on a Propositional and Linguistic Level. Languages 2023, 8, 120. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8020120
Kuvač Kraljević J, Matić Škorić A, Lice K. Main Concepts in the Spoken Discourse of Persons with Aphasia: Analysis on a Propositional and Linguistic Level. Languages. 2023; 8(2):120. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8020120
Chicago/Turabian StyleKuvač Kraljević, Jelena, Ana Matić Škorić, and Karolina Lice. 2023. "Main Concepts in the Spoken Discourse of Persons with Aphasia: Analysis on a Propositional and Linguistic Level" Languages 8, no. 2: 120. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8020120
APA StyleKuvač Kraljević, J., Matić Škorić, A., & Lice, K. (2023). Main Concepts in the Spoken Discourse of Persons with Aphasia: Analysis on a Propositional and Linguistic Level. Languages, 8(2), 120. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8020120