Perception and Production of Sentence Types by Inuktitut-English Bilinguals
Abstract
:1. Introduction
(1) Statements (S) | Peter bought a piano. |
(2) Absolute questions (AQ) | Did Peter buy a piano? |
(3) Declarative questions (DQ) | Peter bought a piano? |
(4) taku-vutit | (Dorais 2010, p. 283) |
See Intr–2 SG Declarative | |
You see (something) | |
(5) taku-jutit | (Dorais 2010, p. 284) |
See Intr–2 SG Indicative | |
You see (something) | |
(6) taku-viit | (Dorais 2010, p. 284) |
See Intr–2 SG Interrogative | |
Do you see (something)? | |
(7) taku-git | (Smith 1977, p. 15) |
See Intr–2 SG IMPERATIVE | |
Look! | |
(8) qai–mmarik–tuq | (Spalding 1979, p. 97) |
Come–really–3 SG Indicative | |
He really comes |
2. The Prosody of Sentence Types in Bilinguals
2.1. Cross-Linguistic Influence and Sentence Types
2.2. Role of Task Type in Modulating CLI in Contact Situations
3. Research Questions and Predictions
Is there evidence of CLI in the bilinguals’ perception and production of sentence types?
Is CLI modulated by task type?
4. Methods
4.1. Participants
4.2. Materials
(9) C task | ||
Context (S): | ||
Mary is on vacation in Toronto and really wants to see a racoon. One of her friends knows of a place with a bunch of trees where racoons live and takes Mary there to see if she can finally see one. Soon after they arrive, a racoon shows up and Mary’s friend says, “Look…” | ||
(a) This is a racoon. | (b) This is a racoon? | (c) Is this a racoon? |
Context (DQ): | ||
Before coming to Toronto from Australia, Mary heard about raccoons, looked at some pictures and thought they were cute little things. One evening, she was eating outside with friends and saw a mid-sized animal crossing the street and thought it was a dog. Her friends commented that it was a raccoon, and she asked… | ||
(a) This is a racoon. | (b) This is a racoon? | (c) Is this a racoon? |
Context (AQ): | ||
Mary is from Australia, and she has never seen a raccoon in her life. When she got to Toronto, she spent hours in the evening trying to spot one. One evening, she is sitting outside with a bunch of friends and she sees something that she believes may be a raccoon. She points at the animal and asks… | ||
(a) This is a racoon. | (b) This is a racoon? | (c) Is this a racoon? |
4.3. Procedure and Data Analysis
5. Results
5.1. Perception
5.2. Production
5.2.1. Accuracy
5.2.2. Phonetic Realization of Pitch Accents and Nuclear Contours
5.3. Summary of Results
6. Discussion
6.1. Research Questions and Hypothesis Evaluation
6.2. Perception and Production
6.3. Individual Variability
7. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
1 | Inuktitut is spoken in Nunavut and in Labrador and Quebec. The degree of bilingualism varies in these areas. |
2 | In addition to English, Inuktitut has historically been in contact with several languages. For example, in the 17th century, an Arctic pidgin developed in Labrador because of the commercial contact with Basque, Breton and French speakers (for more details, see Dorais 2010, chp. 8). |
3 | Given the extensive contact, we can also hypothesize that English has influenced Inuktitut prosody, as was the case with the lexicon and some aspects of the morphology (Dorais 2010). As stated, our goal is to understand whether the absence of tonal variation can be transferred into an intonational language. For a discussion on which aspects of sentence prosody could be subject to bidirectional influence, we refer the reader to Colantoni and Sánchez (2021). |
4 | We are aware that there is a complex interaction between language attitudes, language maintenance and actual linguistic skills (see Miller 2017; Pérez Leroux et al. 2011 for a discussion). |
5 | No distractors were included in this task, since contexts, as opposed to sentence repetitions, introduced variability into the task. Additionally, the inclusion of contexts made the task longer, and, since in addition to the tasks described, we tested the perception and production of other structures, we tried to limit the testing time. |
6 | Although pitch excursions in nuclear contours are rather large, they were not considered unnatural by the authors, since they reflect this speaker’s regular intonational patterns. Moreover, these sentences were listened to by two native English speakers who found them to sound natural for Canadian English. |
7 | Models with only random effects (AIC = 320.2) and with a two-way interaction (AIC = 317) were also tested but we report here the results of the best model according to the AIC criterion (AIC = 315). |
8 | The model selected had a lower AIC value (AIC = 5959) than the base model (AIC = 6146) and a model with the three independent variables (AIC = 6024). |
9 | The model with the three-way interaction had a lower AIC value (AIC = 7676) than either the base model (AIC = 7889) or the model with the three independent variables (AIC = 7760). |
10 | The larger pitch excursions in the initial accent in Ss in the stimuli (Table 4) may also contribute to explain our participants’ behaviour. |
References
- Allen, Shanley. 2007. The future of Inuktitut in the face of majority languages: Bilingualism or language shift? Applied Psycholinguistics 28: 515–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Alvord, Scott. 2007. Spanish Intonation in Contact: The Case of Miami Cuban Bilinguals (Florida). Ph.D. dissertation, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Arnhold, Anja, Emily Elfner, and Richard Compton. Forthcoming. Inuktitut and the concept of word-level prominence. In Word Prominence in Languages with Complex Morphology. Edited by Ksenia Bogomolets and Harry van der Hulst. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- Arnhold, Anja. 2014. Prosodic structure and focus realization in West Greenlandic. In Prosodic Typology II. Edited by Sun-Ah Jun. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 216–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aylward, Lynn. 2010. The role of Inuit languages in Nunavut schooling: Nunavut teachers talk about bilingual education. Canadian Journal of Education 33: 295–328. [Google Scholar]
- Barnes, Hilary, and Jim Michnowicz. 2015. Broad focus declaratives in Veneto-Spanish bilinguals: Peak alignment and language contact. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics 8: 35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bartels, Christine. 1999. The Intonation of English Statements and Questions: A Compositional Interpretation. New York: Garland. [Google Scholar]
- Beckman, Mary, and Gayle Ayers Elam. 1997. Guidelines for ToBI labeling. Available online: https://www.ling.ohio-state.edu/research/phonetics/E_ToBI/ (accessed on 22 February 2022).
- Best, Catherine. 1995. A direct realist view of cross-language speech perception. In Speech Perception and Linguistic Experience: Issues in Cross-Language Research. Edited by Winifred Strange. Baltimore: York Press, pp. 107–26. [Google Scholar]
- Best, Catherine, and Michael Tyler. 2007. Nonnative and second-language speech perception. In Language Experience in Second Language Learning: In Honor of James Emil Flege. Edited by Ocke-Schwen Bohn and Murray Munro. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 13–44. [Google Scholar]
- Boersma, Paul, and David Weenink. 2017. Praat: Doing Phonetics by Computer [Computer Program]. Version 6.0.30. Available online: http://www.praat.org/ (accessed on 22 July 2017).
- Cerrón Palomino, Rodolfo. 1988. Lingüística Quechua. Cusco: Centro de Estudios Rurales Andinos Bartolomé de Las Casas. [Google Scholar]
- Colantoni, Laua, and Jorge Gurlekian. 2004. Convergence and intonation: Historical evidence from Buenos Aires Spanish. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 7: 107–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Colantoni, Laura, and Liliana Sánchez. 2021. The Role of Prosody and Morphology in the Mapping of Information Structure onto Syntax. Languages 6: 207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Colantoni, Laura, Natalia Mazzaro, and Alejandro Cuza. 2016. Task related effects in the prosody of Spanish heritage speakers. In Intonational Grammar in Ibero-Romance: Approaches across Linguistic Sub-Fields. Edited by Meghan Amstrong, Nicholas Henrikssen and María del Mar Vanrell. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 3–23. [Google Scholar]
- Cruz-Ferreira, Madalena. 1983. Non-Native Comprehension of Intonation Patterns in Portuguese and in English. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis. Available online: https://www.academia.edu/922883/Non_native_Comprehension_of_Intonation_Patterns_in_Portuguese_and_in_English (accessed on 22 February 2022).
- Dehé, Nicole. 2018. The intonation of polar questions in north American (“Heritage”) Icelandic. Journal of Germanic Linguistics 30: 213–59. [Google Scholar]
- Delais-Roussarie, Elizabeth, Mathieu Avanzi, and Sophie Herment, eds. 2015. Prosody and Languages in Contact: L2 Acquisition, Attrition, Languages in Multilingual Situations. Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- D’Imperio, Mariapaola, Rossana Cavone, and Caterina Petrone. 2014. Phonetic and phonological imitation of intonation in two varieties of Italian. Frontiers in Psychology 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dorais, Louis-Jacques. 2010. The Language of the Inuit: Syntax, Semantics and Society in the Arctic. Montreal: McGill University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Face, Timothy. 2007. The Role of Intonational Cues in the Perception of Declaratives and Absolute Interrogatives in Castilian Spanish. Estudios de Fonética Experimental 16: 185–225. [Google Scholar]
- Flege, James. 1995. Second-language speech learning: Theory, findings, and problems. In Speech Perception and Linguistic Experience: Issue in Cross-Language Research. Edited by Winifred Strange. Timonium: York Press, pp. 229–73. [Google Scholar]
- Flege, James, and Ocke-Schwen Bohn. 2021. The Revised Speech Learning Model (SLM-r). In Second Language Speech Learning: Theoretical and Empirical Progress. Edited by Ratree Wayland. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 3–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fortescue, Michael. 1983. Intonation contours across Inuit dialects. Inuit Studies 7: 113–24. [Google Scholar]
- Fortescue, Michael. 1984. West Greenlandic. New York: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Fortescue, Michael. 2017. The Eskimo-Aleut Language family. In The Cambridge Handbook of Linguistic Typology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 683–706. [Google Scholar]
- Grabe, Esther, Burton Rosner, José Garcia-Albea, and Xiaolin Zhou. 2003. Perception of English intonation by English, Spanish, and Chinese listeners. Language and Speech 46: 375–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gunlogson, Christine. 2002. Declarative questions. In Proceedings of Semantics and Linguistic Theory XII. Edited by Brendan Jackson. Ithaca: Cornell University. [Google Scholar]
- Gussenhoven, Carlos. 2004. The Phonology of Tone and Intonation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Gut, Ulrike. 2005. Nigerian English prosody. English World-Wide 26: 153–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hedberg, Nancy, and Juan Manuel Sosa. 2002. The Prosody of Questions in Natural Discourse. Paper presented at Speech Prosody 2002, Aix-en-Provence, France, April 11–13; pp. 375–78. [Google Scholar]
- Hedberg, Nancy, Juan Manuel Sosa, and Emrah Görgülü. 2017. The Meaning of Intonation in Yes-no Questions in American English: A Corpus Study. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory 13: 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Inuit Language Protection Act. 2008. The Legislative Assembly of Nunavut. Available online: https://www.clo-ocol.gc.ca/en/timeline-event/the-legislative-assembly-of-nunavut-adopts-the-official-languages-act-and-the-inuit (accessed on 22 February 2022).
- Johns, Alana. 2010. Eskimo-Aleut languages. Language and Linguistics Compass 4: 1041–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jun, Sun-Ah, ed. 2014. Prosodic Typology II: The Phonology of Intonation and Phrasing. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kiser, Michele. 2014. Analyzing Navajo Discourse: Investigating form and Function of Intonational Units in Referential Discourse. Ph.D. dissertation, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Lai, Li-Fang. 2018. Intonation in Contact: Prosodic Transfer and Innovation among Yami-Mandarin Bilinguals. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Liu, Chang, and Amanda Rodríguez. 2012. Categorical Perception of Intonation Contrasts: Effects of Listeners’ Language Background. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 131: 427–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Lleó, Conxita, Martin Rakow, and Margaret Kehoe. 2004. Acquisition of language-specific pitch accent by Spanish and German monolingual and bilingual children. In Laboratory Approaches to Spanish Phonology. Edited by Tim Face. Amsterdam: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 3–27. [Google Scholar]
- Marasco, Olivia. 2020. Are You Asking Me or Telling Me? Production and Perception of Yes/No Questions and Statements in L2 Spanish. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada. [Google Scholar]
- Massenet, Jean Marie. 1980. Notes sur l’intonation finale en inuktitut, in Inuktitut et Langues amérindiennes du Québec. Cahier de Linguistique 8: 195–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mennen, Ineke. 2004. Bi-directional interference in the intonation of Dutch speakers of Greek. Journal of Phonetics 32: 543–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miller, Lauren. 2017. The relationship between language proficiency and language attitudes: Evidence from young Spanish-English bilinguals. Spanish in Context 14: 99–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Muntendam, Antje, and Francisco Torreira. 2016. Focus and prosody in Spanish and Quechua: Insights from an interactive task. In Intonational Grammar in Ibero-Romance: Approaches across Linguistic Subfields. Edited by Meghan Armstrong, Nicholas Henriksen and María del Mar Vanrell. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- Official Languages Act. 1988. Government of Canada. Available online: https://laws-lois.justice.gc.ca/eng/acts/o-3.01/fulltext.html (accessed on 22 February 2022).
- Ortega-Llebaria, Marta, and Laura Colantoni. 2014. L2 English intonation: Relations between form-meaning associations, access to meaning, and L1 transfer. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 36: 331–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O’Rourke, Erin. 2009. Phonetics and phonology of Cuzco Quechua declarative intonation: An instrumental analysis. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 39: 291–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O’Rourke, Erin. 2012. The realization of contrastive focus in Peruvian Spanish intonation. Lingua 122: 494–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Patience, Matthew, Olivia Marasco, Laura Colantoni, Gabrielle Klassen, Malina Radu, and Olga Tararova. 2018. Initial pitch cues in English sentence types. Paper presented at Speech Prosody 9, Poznań, Poland, June 13–16. [Google Scholar]
- Patience, Matthew, Laura Colantoni, Gabrielle Klassen, Malina Radu, and Olga Tararova. 2020. The perception and comprehension of L2 English sentence types: Cross-linguistic influence and task effects. Gradus 5: 71–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pérez Leroux, Ana Teresa, Alejandro Cuza, and Danielle Thomas. 2011. From parental attitudes to input conditions: Spanish-English bilingual development in Toronto. In Bilingual Youth: Spanish in English Speaking Societies. Edited by Kim Potowski and Jason Rothman. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 149–76. [Google Scholar]
- Petrone, Caterina, and Oliver Niebuhr. 2014. On the Intonation of German Intonation Questions: The Role of the Prenuclear Region. Language and Speech 57: 108–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Peterson, Tyler. 2016. Mirativity as Surprise: Evidentiality, Information, and Deixis. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 45: 1327–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pierrehumbert, Janet. 1980. The Phonology and Phonetics of English Intonation. Ph.D. dissertation, MIT, Wayne, PA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Queen, Robin. 2001. Bilingual intonation patterns: Evidence of language change from Turkish-German bilingual children. Language in Society 30: 55–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Queen, Robin. 2012. Turkish-German bilinguals and their intonation: Triangulating evidence about contact-induced language change. Language 88: 791–816. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Core Team. 2013. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Available online: http://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 12 September 2021).
- Radu, Malina, Gabrielle Klassen, Laura Colantoni, Matthew Patience, Ana Pérez Leroux, and Olga Tararova. 2018. The perception and interpretation of intonational contours among L2 English speakers. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rakow, Martin, and Conxita Lleó. 2011. Comparing Cues of Phrasing in German and Spanish Child Monolingual and Bilingual Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Saindon, Mathieu, Laura Cirelli, Glenn Schellenberg, Pascal van Lieshout, and Sandra Trehub. 2017a. Children’s and adults’ perception of questions and statements from terminal fundamental frequency contours. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 141: 3123–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Saindon, Mathieu, Sandra Trehub, Glenn Schellenberg, and Pascal van Lieshout. 2017b. When is a question not a question for children and adults? Language Learning and Development 13: 274–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sánchez, Liliana. 2008. Literacy and the expression of social identity in a dominant language: A description of mi familia by Quechua-Spanish bilingual children. In Bilingualism and Identity. Edited by Mercedes Niño-Murcia and Jason Rothman. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins, pp. 109–26. [Google Scholar]
- Santiago, Fabián, and Elisabeth Delais-Roussarie. 2012. Acquiring phrasing and intonation in French as a second Language: The case of Yes-No questions produced by Mexican Spanish Learners. Paper presented at Speech Prosody, Shanghai, China, May 22–25. [Google Scholar]
- Shokeir, Vanessa. 2009. Intonation in Inuktitut. Toronto: Ms. University of Toronto. [Google Scholar]
- Sicoli, Mark, Stivers Tanya, N. Enfield, and Stephen Levinson. 2015. Marked Initial Pitch in Questions Signals Marked Communicative Function. Language and Speech 58: 204–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Simonet, Miquel. 2011. Intonational convergence in language contact: Utterance-final F0 contours in Catalan-Spanish early bilinguals. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 41: 157–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Smith, Lawrence. 1977. Some Aspects of Labrador Inuttut (Eskimo): A Survey of the Inflectional Paradigms of Nouns and Verbs. Ottawa: University of Ottawa Press. [Google Scholar]
- Spalding, Alex. 1979. Learning to Speak Inuktitut: A Grammar of North Baffin Dialects. London: Centre for Research and Teaching of Canadian Native Languages, London: University of Western Ontario. [Google Scholar]
- Statistics Canada. 2019. Evolution of the Language Situation in Nunavut: 2001–2016. Release date: 9 July 2019. Available online: https://www150.statcan.gc.ca/n1/pub/89-657-x/89-657-x2019010-eng.htm (accessed on 13 January 2021).
- Thalbitzer, William. 1904. A Phonetical Study of the Eskimo Language. Copenhagen: MoG. København, vol. 31. [Google Scholar]
- Truckenbrodt, Hubert. 2011. On rises and falls in interrogatives. Paper presented at IDP 2009, Paris, France, Septembre 9–11; pp. 9–17. [Google Scholar]
- Tsimpli, Ianthi. 2014. Aspects of meaning in bilingual minds. Paper presented at the Jackman Humanities Institute Lecture Series, Toronto, ON, Canada, February 7. [Google Scholar]
- Zárate-Sández, Germán. 2015. Perception and Production of Intonation among English-Spanish Bilingual Speakers at Different Proficiency Levels. Ph.D. dissertation, Georgetown University, Washington, DC, USA. [Google Scholar]
Participant | Gender | Age | Education | AoA | English Use | Self-Rating |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
I01 | F | 20 | College | 6 | 25 | A |
I02 | M | 35 | College | 3 | 37.5 | A |
I03 | F | 26 | College | 0 | 50 | NN |
I04 | F | 25 | College | 0 | 55 | A |
I05 | F | 19 | College | 0 | 58.3 | NN |
I06 | F | 22 | College | 8 | 46.6 | A |
I07 | F | 71 | University | 6 | 87.5 | A |
I08 | M | 23 | College | 11 | 62.5 | A |
I09 | F | 55 | College | 6 | 65 | A |
I10 | F | 57 | College | 9 | 53.8 | A |
I11 | M | 47 | Secondary | 6 | 97.5 | A |
I12 | F | 25 | Secondary | 6 | 80 | A |
I13 | F | 28 | College | 6 | 73.3 | NN |
Tasks | Task-Type | Conditions | Number of Trials | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Perception | Intonation only (IO) | Forced-choice identification (low-pass filtered stimuli) (question/statement/exclamation) | Sentence type (S/AQ/DQ) | 10 items × 3 types = (30) + 25 distracters |
Intonation and lexical information (SI) | Forced-choice identification (question/statement/exclamation) | Sentence type (S/AQ/DQ) | 10 items × 3 types = (30) + 25 distracters | |
Context (C) | Sentence preference (choice of statement/non-inverted question/inverted question) | Sentence type (S/AQ/DQ) | 6 items × 3 types = 18 | |
Production | Sentence Imitation | Sentence Repetition | Sentence type (S/AQ/DQ) | 10 items × 3 types = (30) + 25 distracters |
Context | Completion task | Sentence type (S/AQ/DQ) | 6 items × 3 types = 18 |
Sentence Type | Pitch Accent (Max F0) | Nuclear Contour (Max F0) | F0 Excursion (Pitch Accent) | F0 Excursion (Nuclear Contour) |
---|---|---|---|---|
AQ | 97 | 101 | 6 | 16 |
DQ | 98 | 104 | 9 | 20 |
S | 98 | 95 | 6 | 9 |
Sentence Type | Pitch Accent (Max F0) | Nuclear Contour (Max F0) | F0 Excursion (Pitch Accent) | F0 Excursion (Nuclear Contour) |
---|---|---|---|---|
AQ | 93 | 101 | 2 | 14 |
DQ | 93 | 103 | 1 | 18 |
S | 97 | 92 | 4 | 4 |
Task/Sentence Type | IO | SI | C | Mean Accuracy | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
S | AQ | DQ | S | AQ | DQ | S | AQ | DQ | ||
English | 83 | 84 | 86 | 93 | 97 | 83 | 87 | 93 | 84 | 88 |
Inuktitut | 76 | 75 | 73 | 80 | 91 | 76 | 67 | 84 | 33 | 73 |
Fixed Effects | Estimate | SE | z Value | Pr (>|z|) |
---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | −2.98 | 0.51 | −5.85 | <0.001 *** |
Language (I) | 1.01 | 0.62 | 1.63 | <0.001 *** |
Task (IO) | 1.12 | 0.53 | 2.11 | 0.030 * |
Task (SI) | −0.98 | 0.69 | −1.41 | 0.150 |
Sentence (DQ) | 1.00 | 0.57 | 1.72 | 0.080 |
Sentence (S) | 0.79 | 0.58 | 1.34 | 0.170 |
Language (I)*Task (IO) | −0.44 | 0.65 | −0.68 | 0.490 |
Language (I)*Task (SI) | 0.30 | 0.82 | 0.36 | 0.710 |
Language (I)*Sentence (DQ) | 1.81 | 0.67 | 2.67 | 0.007 ** |
Language (I)*Sentence (S) | 0.37 | 0.68 | 0.55 | 0.580 |
Task (IO)*Sentence (DQ) | −1.15 | 0.69 | −1.67 | 0.090 |
Task (SI)*Sentence (DQ) | 1.11 | 0.82 | 1.85 | 0.170 |
Task (IO)*Sentence (S) | −0.79 | 0.69 | −1.14 | 0.250 |
Task (SI)*Sentence (S) | 0.30 | 0.85 | 0.35 | 0.720 |
Language (I)*Task (IO)*Sentence (DQ) | −1.49 | 0.85 | −1.17 | 0.080 |
Language (I)*Task (SI)*Sentence (DQ) | −2.59 | 0.99 | −2.62 | 0.008 ** |
Language (I)*Task (IO)*Sentence (S) | −0.36 | 0.83 | −0.43 | 0.660 |
Language (I)*Task (SI)*Sentence (S) | −0.42 | 1.00 | −0.42 | 0.67 |
Sentence Type | AQ | DQ | S | Mean Accuracy |
---|---|---|---|---|
English | 94 | 83 | 97 | 91 |
Inuktitut | 87 | 64 | 85 | 79 |
Fixed Effects | Estimate | SE | z Value | Pr (>|z|) |
---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | −3.95 | 0.70 | −5.62 | <0.0001 *** |
Language (I) | 0.88 | 0.72 | 1.21 | 0.22 |
Sentence (DQ) | 1.70 | 0.59 | 2.85 | 0.004 ** |
Sentence (S) | 0.04 | 0.63 | 0.07 | 0.94 |
Fixed Effects | Estimate | SE | df | t Value | Pr(>|t|) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 1.23 | 0.44 | 103.31 | 2.80 | 0.006 ** |
Language (I) | −0.54 | 0.59 | 100.51 | −0.91 | 0.360 |
Sentence (DQ) | 1.84 | 0.50 | 135.90 | 3.63 | 0.0004 *** |
Sentence (S) | −0.37 | 0.50 | 110.77 | −0.74 | 0.460 |
Task (SI) | 3.55 | 0.36 | 1154.20 | 9.63 | <0.001 *** |
Language (I)*Task (SI) | −2.66 | 0.54 | 1157.55 | −4.92 | <0.001 *** |
Language (I)*Sentence(DQ) | −1.87 | 0.69 | 1166.44 | −2.68 | 0.007 ** |
Language (I)*Sentence (S) | 0.15 | 0.63 | 1162.57 | 0.24 | 0.800 |
Sentence (DQ)*Task (SI) | −1.89 | 0.53 | 1161.08 | −3.55 | <0.001 *** |
Sentence (S)*Task (SI) | −2.52 | 0.53 | 1174.89 | −4.75 | <0.001 *** |
Language (I)*Sentence (DQ)*Task (SI) | 1.74 | 0.83 | 1163.46 | 2.10 | 0.030 * |
Language (I)*Sentence (S)*Task (SI) | 2.29 | 0.79 | 1160.42 | 2.87 | 0.004 ** |
Fixed Effects | Estimate | SE | df | t Value | Pr(>|t|) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
(Intercept) | 6.82 | 0.79 | 132.84 | 8.55 | <0.001 *** |
Language (I) | −0.55 | 1.14 | 164.58 | −0.48 | 0.620 |
Sentence (DQ) | 1.66 | 0.96 | 203.67 | 1.72 | 0.080 |
Sentence (S) | −13.41 | 0.98 | 203.62 | −13.55 | <0.001 *** |
Task (SI) | 5.54 | 0.78 | 1074.10 | 7.08 | <0.001 *** |
Language (I)*Task (SI) | −0.46 | 1.16 | 1144.69 | −0.40 | 0.680 |
Language (I)*Sentence (DQ) | −1.27 | 1.49 | 1158.93 | −0.85 | 0.390 |
Language (I)*Sentence (S) | 3.45 | 1.39 | 1157.00 | 2.48 | 0.010 * |
Sentence (DQ)*Task (SI) | −1.36 | 1.12 | 1087.04 | −1.21 | 0.220 |
Sentence (S)*Task (SI) | −7.24 | 1.16 | 1133.28 | −6.23 | <0.001 *** |
Language (I)*Sentence (DQ)*Task (SI) | 1.42 | 1.77 | 1154.12 | 0.80 | 0.420 |
Language (I)*Sentence (S)*Task (SI) | −0.77 | 1.73 | 1152.74 | −0.44 | 0.650 |
Experiment | Parameter | Group | Task | Sentence Type |
---|---|---|---|---|
Perception | Accuracy | E > I | IO < SI, C | SI (both groups): DQ < AQ, S C (I only): DQ < AQ, S |
Production | Accuracy | n.s | -- | DQ < AQ, S |
Pitch accent | E > I | SI > C | E: DQ>AQ>S (C task); DQ, AQ > S (SI task) I: n.s. | |
Nuclear contour | I < E (Ss only) | SI > C | Questions > Ss |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Colantoni, L.; Klassen, G.; Patience, M.; Radu, M.; Tararova, O. Perception and Production of Sentence Types by Inuktitut-English Bilinguals. Languages 2022, 7, 193. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7030193
Colantoni L, Klassen G, Patience M, Radu M, Tararova O. Perception and Production of Sentence Types by Inuktitut-English Bilinguals. Languages. 2022; 7(3):193. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7030193
Chicago/Turabian StyleColantoni, Laura, Gabrielle Klassen, Matthew Patience, Malina Radu, and Olga Tararova. 2022. "Perception and Production of Sentence Types by Inuktitut-English Bilinguals" Languages 7, no. 3: 193. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7030193
APA StyleColantoni, L., Klassen, G., Patience, M., Radu, M., & Tararova, O. (2022). Perception and Production of Sentence Types by Inuktitut-English Bilinguals. Languages, 7(3), 193. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7030193