Conventionalization of Iconic Handshape Preferences in Family Homesign Systems
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Iconic Handshape Preferences in Sign Languages
1.2. How Do Homesigners Compare to Communication Partners and Signers?
1.3. Why Look at Homesign to Understand Sign Language Emergence?
1.4. Is Conventionalization Possible in Homesign Systems?
1.5. The Current Study
- (1a)
- Do homesigners and communication partners tend to express iconicity by using a Handling handshape or an Object handshape?
- (1b)
- Do members of families with homesigners share this preference with each other?
- (1c)
- Does a participant’s age at the time of test, the age at which they begin using the homesign system, or the number of years they have been using the system affect handshape type and its conventionalization?
- (2)
- Do some stimulus items elicit higher conventionality in iconic handshape preferences? Which factors are or are not associated with greater conventionality?
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Materials and Procedure
2.3. Transcription and Response Types
3. Results
3.1. Study 1: Participant Characteristics
3.1.1. Do Homesigners and Communication Partners Tend to Express Iconicity by Using a Handling Handshape or an Object Handshape?
3.1.2. Do Members of Families with Homesigners Share This Preference with Each Other?
3.1.3. Does a Participant’s Age at the Time of Test, Age at Which They Begin Using a Homesign System, or the Number of Years They Have Used the System Affect Handshape Type and Conventionalization?
3.2. Study 2: Item Characteristics
3.2.1. Do Some Stimulus Items Elicit Higher Conventionality in Iconic Handshape Preferences?
3.2.2. Which Factors Are or Are Not Associated with Greater Conventionality?
4. Discussion
4.1. Variation in Iconic Handshape Preference and Conventionalization
4.2. Age and Experience as Factors in Conventionalization
4.3. Item-Specific Biases and Proxy Measures
4.4. More Questions and Future Directions
5. Conclusions
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Group | Age (Years) | Handling Responses | Object Responses | Handling+Object Responses | |
Child HS 1 | 9 | 82% * | 14% | 5% | |
Sister | 14 | 67% | 33% | 0% | |
Mother | 26 | 36% | 60% | 4% | |
Grandmother | 50 | 54% | 46% | 0% | |
Mean | 64% | 28% | 8% | ||
Child HS 2 | 12 | 64% | 28% | 8% | |
Father | 31 | 75% * | 13% | 13% | |
Mother | 31 | 52% | 44% | 4% | |
Mean | 64% | 28% | 8% | ||
Child HS 3 | 12 | 36% | 59% | 5% | |
Brother | 9 | 6% | 89% * | 6% | |
Friend | 12 | 74% | 26% | 0% | |
Father | 37 | 61% | 35% | 4% | |
Mean | 47% | 50% | 3% | ||
Child HS 4 | 12 | 76% * | 24% | 0% | |
Child HS 5 | 13 | 80% * | 13% | 7% | |
Brother | 13 | 55% | 45% | 0% | |
Mother | 33 | 17% | 83% * | 0% | |
Father | 52 | 63% | 31% | 6% | |
Mean | 52% | 45% | 3% | ||
Child HS 6 | 13 | 81% * | 15% | 4% | |
Brother | 14 | 75% * | 10% | 15% | |
Mean | 78% | 13% | 9% | ||
Child HS 7 | 14 | 53% | 47% | 0% | |
Overall mean, Child HS | 67% | 29% | 4% | ||
Overall mean, CPs of Child HS | 53% | 43% | 4% |
Group | Age (Years) | Handling Responses | Object Responses | Handling+Object Responses | |
Adult HS 1 | 26 | 69% * | 23% | 8% | |
Niece | 9 | 77% * | 23% | 0% | |
Girlfriend | 19 | 62% | 35% | 4% | |
Brother | 28 | 73% * | 23% | 4% | |
Mother | 54 | 58% | 35% | 8% | |
Father | 64 | 50% | 42% | 8% | |
Mean | 64% | 29% | 7% | ||
Adult HS 2 | 30 | 36% | 40% | 24% | |
Sister | 23 | 73% * | 19% | 8% | |
Brother | 25 | 38% | 29% | 33% | |
Mother | 46 | 38% | 38% | 23% | |
Mean | 47% | 32% | 22% | ||
Adult HS 3 | 34 | 56% | 20% | 24% | |
Brother | 19 | 48% | 44% | 8% | |
Sister | 29 | 59% | 41% | 0% | |
Mean | 54% | 35% | 11% | ||
Adult HS 4 | 35 | 26% | 22% | 52% | |
Brother | 44 | 64% | 28% | 8% | |
Mother | 61 | 27% | 50% | 23% | |
Mean | 38% | 33% | 28% | ||
Overall mean, Adult HS | 47% | 26% | 27% | ||
Overall mean, CPs of Adult HS | 56% | 34% | 11% |
Group | Age (Years) | Handling Responses | Object Responses | Handling+Object Responses |
Hearing Related 1 | 23 | 36% | 60% | 4% |
Hearing Related 2 | 26 | 62% | 35% | 4% |
Hearing Related 3 | 48 | 31% | 54% | 15% |
Hearing Related 4 | 52 | 74% * | 19% | 7% |
Mean | 51% | 42% | 8% | |
Hearing Unrelated 1 | 20 | 56% | 32% | 12% |
Hearing Unrelated 2 | 22 | 56% | 20% | 24% |
Hearing Unrelated 3 | 23 | 65% | 35% | 0% |
Hearing Unrelated 4 | 34 | 38% | 63% | 0% |
Mean | 54% | 37% | 9% |
References
- Benedicto, Elena, and Diane Brentari. 2004. Where did all the arguments go?: Argument-changing properties of classifiers in ASL. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 22: 743–810. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Braithwaite, Ben. 2020. Ideologies of linguistic research on small sign languages in the global South: A Caribbean perspective. Language and Communication 74: 182–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brentari, Diane, Alessio Di Renzo, Jonathan Keane, and Virginia Volterra. 2015. Cognitive, cultural, and linguistic sources of a handshape distinction expressing agentivity. Topics in Cognitive Science 7: 95–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brentari, Diane, and Marie Coppola. 2013. What sign language creation teaches us about language. WIREs Cognitive Science 4: 201–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brentari, Diane, Marie Coppola, Laura Mazzoni, and Susan Goldin-Meadow. 2012. When does a system become phonological? Handshape production in gesturers, signers, and homesigners. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 30: 1–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brentari, Diane, Marie Coppola, Pyeong Whan Cho, and Ann Senghas. 2017. Handshape complexity as a precursor to phonology: Variation, emergence, and acquisition. Language Acquisition 24: 283–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brentari, Diane, Laura Horton, and Susan Goldin-Meadow. 2020. Crosslinguistic similarity and variation in the simultaneous morphology of sign languages. The Linguistic Review 37: 571–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carrigan, Emily M., and Marie Coppola. 2017. Successful communication does not drive language development: Evidence from adult homesign. Cognition 158: 10–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Coppola, Marie. 2002. The Emergence of Grammatical Categories in Home Sign: Evidence from Family-Based Gesture Systems in Nicaragua. Doctoral dissertation, University of Rochester, Rochester, NY, USA. unpublished. [Google Scholar]
- Coppola, Marie. 2020a. Sociolinguistic sketch: Nicaraguan Sign Language and homesign systems in Nicaragua. In Emerging Sign Languages of the Americas. Edited by Olivier Le Guen, Josefina Safar and Marie Coppola. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton, pp. 439–50. [Google Scholar]
- Coppola, Marie. 2020b. Gestures, homesign, sign language: Cultural and social factors driving lexical conventionalization. In Emerging Sign Languages of the Americas. Edited by Olivier Le Guen, Josefina Safar and Marie Coppola. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton, pp. 349–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coppola, Marie, and Diane Brentari. 2014. From iconic handshapes to grammatical contrasts: Longitudinal evidence from a child homesigner. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 830. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Coppola, Marie, and Elissa L. Newport. 2005. Grammatical subjects in home sign: Abstract linguistic structure in adult primary gesture systems without linguistic input. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 102: 19249–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Coppola, Marie, Elisabeth Spaepen, and Susan Goldin-Meadow. 2013. Communicating about quantity without a language model: Number devices in homesign grammar. Cognitive Psychology 67: 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Davies, Mark. 2015. Corpus del Español. Available online: https://www.corpusdelespanol.org/ (accessed on 11 October 2021).
- Eccarius, Petra Nichole. 2008. A Constraint-Based Account of Handshape Contrast in Sign Languages. Order No. 3330229. Linguistics and Language Behavior Abstracts (LLBA); ProQuest Dissertations and Theses Global. 304501247. Available online: https://www.proquest.com/dissertations-theses/constraint-based-account-handshape-contrast-sign/docview/304501247/se-2?accountid=30699 (accessed on 11 October 2021).
- Emmorey, Karen, and David Corina. 1990. Lexical recognition in sign language: Effects of phonetic structure and morphology. Perceptual and Motor Skills 71: 1227–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gagne, Deanna L. 2017. With a Little Help from My Friends: The Contributions of a Peer Language Network on the Conventionalization of Space in an Emerging Language. Doctoral dissertation, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT, USA. unpublished. Available online: https://opencommons.uconn.edu/dissertations/1493/ (accessed on 11 October 2021).
- Goldin-Meadow, Susan, Carolyn Mylander, Jill de Villiers, Elizabeth Bates, and Virginia Volterra. 1984. Gestural communication in deaf children: The effects and noneffects of parental input on early language development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 49: 1–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldin-Meadow, Susan, Diane Brentari, Marie Coppola, Laura Horton, and Ann Senghas. 2015. Watching language grow in the manual modality: Nominals, predicates, and handshapes. Cognition 136: 381–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Horton, Laura A. 2018. Conventionalization of Shared Homesign Systems in Guatemala: Social, Lexical, and Morphophonological Dimensions. Doctoral dissertation, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA. unpublished. Available online: https://knowledge.uchicago.edu/record/1321 (accessed on 11 October 2021).
- Horton, Laura A. 2020. Representation strategies in shared homesign systems from Nebaj Guatemala. In Emerging Sign Languages of the Americas. Edited by Olivier Le Guen, Josefina Safar and Marie Coppola. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton, pp. 97–154. [Google Scholar]
- Horton, Laura, Susan Goldin-Meadow, Marie Coppola, Ann Senghas, and Diane Brentari. 2015. Forging a morphological system out of two dimensions: Agentivity and number. Open Linguistics 1: 596–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hou, Lynn. 2018. Iconic Patterns in San Juan Quiahije Chatino Sign Language. Sign Language Studies 18: 570–611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hunsicker, Dea, and Susan Goldin-Meadow. 2013. How handshape type can distinguish between nouns and verbs in homesign. Gesture 13: 354–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hwang, So-One, Nozomi Tomita, Hope Morgan, Rabia Ergin, Deniz Ilkbaşaran, Sharon Seegers, Ryan Lepic, and Carol Padden. 2017. Of the body and the hands: Patterned iconicity for semantic categories. Language and Cognition 9: 573–602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Le Guen, Olivier, Marie Coppola, and Josefina Safar. 2020. Introduction: How emerging sign languages in the Americas contributes to the study of linguistics and (emerging) sign languages. In Emerging Sign Languages of the Americas. Edited by Olivier Le Guen, Josefina Safar and Marie Coppola. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton, pp. 1–32. [Google Scholar]
- Liddell, Scott K., and Robert E. Johnson. 1986. American Sign Language compound formation processes, lexicalization, and phonological remnants. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 4: 445–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin, Amber, Ann Senghas, and Carol Padden. Forthcoming. The Gestural Origins of Nouns in Nicaraguan Sign Language.
- Mayberry, Rachel I., and Robert Kluender. 2018. Rethinking the critical period for language: New insights into an old question from American Sign Language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 21: 886–905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mayberry, Rachel I., and Susan D. Fischer. 1989. Looking through phonological shape to lexical meaning: The bottleneck of non-native sign language processing. Memory & Cognition 17: 740–54. [Google Scholar]
- Meir, Irit, Mark Aronoff, Wendy Sandler, and Carol Padden. 2010. Sign languages and compounding. In Cross-Disciplinary Issues in Compounding. Edited by Sergio Scalise and Irene Vogel. Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing, pp. 301–22. [Google Scholar]
- Newport, Elissa L. 1990. Maturational constraints on language learning. Cognitive Science 14: 11–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Newport, Elissa L., Daphne Bavelier, and Helen J. Neville. 2001. Critical thinking about critical periods: Perspectives on a critical period for language acquisition. In Language, Brain and Cognitive Development: Essays in Honor of Jacques Mehler. Cambridge: MIT Press, pp. 481–502. [Google Scholar]
- Padden, Carol, Irit Meir, So-One Hwang, Ryan Lepic, Sharon Seegers, and Tory Sampson. 2013. Patterned iconicity in sign language lexicons. Gesture 13: 287–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Padden, Carol, So-One Hwang, Ryan Lepic, and Sharon Seegers. 2015. Tools for language: Patterned iconicity in sign language nouns and verbs. Topics in Cognitive Science 7: 81–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Richie, Russell, Charles Yang, and Marie Coppola. 2014. Modeling the emergence of lexicons in homesign systems. Topics in Cognitive Science 6: 183–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Safar, Josefina, and Rodrigo Petatillo Chan. 2020. Strategies of noun-verb distinction in Yucatec Maya Sign Languages. In Emerging Sign Languages of the Americas. Edited by Olivier Le Guen, Josefina Safar and Marie Coppola. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton, pp. 155–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Senghas, Ann. 2019. How language learns: Linking universals to acquisition. In Proceedings of the 43rd Boston University Conference on Language Development. Edited by Megan M. Brown and Brady Dailey. Somerville: Cascadilla Press, pp. 1–10. [Google Scholar]
- Senghas, Ann, and Marie Coppola. 2001. Children creating language: How Nicaraguan Sign Language acquired a spatial grammar. Psychological Science 12: 323–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Senghas, Ann, Asli Ozyurek, and Sotaro Kita. 2005. Response to comment on “Children creating core properties of language: Evidence from an emerging sign language in Nicaragua”. Science 309: 56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Singleton, Jenny L., Jill P. Morford, and Susan Goldin-Meadow. 1993. Once is not enough: Standards of well-formedness in manual communication created over three different timespans. Language 69: 683–715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wittenburg, Peter, Hennie Brugman, Albert Russel, Alex Klassmann, and Han Sloetjes. 2006. ELAN: A professional framework for multimodality research. Paper presented at the 5th International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC 2006), Genoa, Italy, May 22–28; pp. 1556–59. [Google Scholar]
- Zheng, Mingyu, and Susan Goldin-Meadow. 2002. Thought before language: How deaf and hearing children express motion events across cultures. Cognition 85: 145–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Type of Specific Bias | Possible Sources of Handshape Preference | What We Will Look at |
---|---|---|
Participant (Study 1) | Using a Homesign System with Others | homesigning families vs. hearing non-signing family |
Communicative Familiarity with Others | families vs. unrelated group | |
Relationship to Homesign System | primary user vs. communication partner vs. none | |
Age | child/adolescent vs. adult; age at which first started using system; chronological age | |
Item (Study 2) | Lexical Frequency | English, Spanish and ASL word frequencies as proxies for homesign |
Type of Instrument | e.g., traditional tool vs. makeup vs. non-tool |
N | Age (Years) (Mean, SD) | Gender (% Women) | |
---|---|---|---|
Homesigners | 11 | 19;1 (9.97) | 36% (4) |
Communication Partners | 24 | 31;6 (17.1) | 50% (12) |
Hearing Non-Signers related (4); unrelated (4) | 8 | 31;0 (12.5) | 50% (4) |
All Participants | 43 | 28;3 (15.4) | 47% (20) |
Responses Consisting of Single Gesture/Sign (Mean, SD) | Responses Containing Multiple Gestures/Signs (Mean, SD) | |
---|---|---|
Adult Homesigners | 32% (0.34) | 68% (0.34) |
Child/Adolescent Homesigners | 92% (0.05) | 8% (0.05) |
Communication Partners | 80% (0.26) | 20% (0.26) |
Hearing Non-Signers | 93% (0.14) | 7% (0.14) |
Participant Type | Handling | Object | H+O |
---|---|---|---|
Child Homesigners | 67% | 29% | 4% |
CPs of Child Homesigners | 53% | 43% | 4% |
Adult Homesigners | 47% | 26% | 27% |
CPs of Adult Homesigners | 56% | 34% | 11% |
Hearing Family | 51% | 42% | 8% |
Hearing Unrelated | 54% | 37% | 9% |
Group | Conventionalization (Mean, SD) |
---|---|
Child Homesigner 1 | 61% (0.12) |
Child Homesigner 2 | 71% (0.10) |
Child Homesigner 3 | 54% (0.14) |
Child Homesigner 5 | 48% (0.15) |
Child Homesigner 6 | 65% (0) |
Adult Homesigner 1 | 70% (0.10) |
Adult Homesigner 2 | 66% (0.03) |
Adult Homesigner 3 | 69% (0.11) |
Adult Homesigner 4 | 60% (0.40) |
Hearing Related | 59% (0.08) |
Hearing Unrelated | 70% (0.17) |
Handling Bias (12) | Object Bias (4) | Dependent on Chance (11) |
---|---|---|
Pants | Scissors | Nail File |
Hammer | Knife | Cell Phone |
Spoon | Handsaw | Hat |
Sock | Paintbrush | Jacket |
Mop | Mascara | |
Broom | Nail Polish | |
Hairbrush | Toothbrush | |
Screwdriver | Lipstick | |
Vacuum Cleaner | Rake | |
Hair Dryer | Glove | |
Fork | Shoe | |
Comb |
Type of Specific Bias | Possible Sources of Handshape Preference | What We Found |
---|---|---|
Participant (Study 1) | Using a Homesign System with Others | Homesigning families varied just as much as the hearing family |
Communicative Familiarity with Others | Families varied just as much as the unrelated group of hearing people | |
Relationship to Homesign System | No differences found between homesigners, CPs, and non-signers | |
Age | Chronological age and years of experience predicted conventionalization; adult homesigners produced more Handling+Object responses | |
Item (Study 2) | Lexical Frequency | Proxy measures not related to conventionalization |
Type of Instrument | Traditional tools often more highly conventionalized than other items (e.g., makeup) |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Quam, M.; Brentari, D.; Coppola, M. Conventionalization of Iconic Handshape Preferences in Family Homesign Systems. Languages 2022, 7, 156. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7030156
Quam M, Brentari D, Coppola M. Conventionalization of Iconic Handshape Preferences in Family Homesign Systems. Languages. 2022; 7(3):156. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7030156
Chicago/Turabian StyleQuam, Madeline, Diane Brentari, and Marie Coppola. 2022. "Conventionalization of Iconic Handshape Preferences in Family Homesign Systems" Languages 7, no. 3: 156. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7030156
APA StyleQuam, M., Brentari, D., & Coppola, M. (2022). Conventionalization of Iconic Handshape Preferences in Family Homesign Systems. Languages, 7(3), 156. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7030156