5.1. Accuracy of Responses
Table 7 outlines the descriptive statistics reflecting accuracy improvements from pretest to posttest, revealing significant advancements for Groups One (referential-only) and Three (referential + affective), as opposed to the minimal changes observed in Group Two (affective-only) and Group Four (control). Initial similarities in group performance were confirmed by a one-way ANOVA on pretest accuracy data (see
Table 8), showing no significant pre-existing differences among groups [F(3, 33) = 0.443,
p = 0.723]. The Shapiro–Wilk test confirmed normality of the accuracy data for all groups (
p > 0.05), ensuring the appropriateness of parametric tests (see
Table 9).
Statistical Analysis
Post-intervention, a repeated-measures ANOVA was conducted with factors for instruction type and time. As shown in
Table 10, significant differences were found both among groups [F(3, 33) = 7.013,
p < 0.001] and from pretest to posttest [F(3, 33) = 6.931,
p = 0.009].
Post hoc analyses using the Scheffe test revealed statistically significant distinctions among the groups, indicating the efficacy of specific instructional methods on accuracy:
Group One vs. Group Two: p = 0.032
Group One vs. Group Three: p = 0.146
Group One vs. Group Four: p < 0.001
Group Two vs. Group Three: p = 0.027
Group Two vs. Group Four: p = 0.034
Group Three vs. Group Four: p = 0.015
Accuracy Improvements
Both Groups One and Three showed over 50% improvement in processing English past tense markers accurately, while Group Two exhibited a modest 10% improvement. Group Four showed no significant change. The overall pattern of performance suggests that combined instructional strategies (referential + affective) equaled the effectiveness of referential strategies alone, both surpassing the affective-only and control groups.
5.2. Analysis of Response Time Data
Pretest Consistency Across Groups
As shown in
Table 11 and
Table 12, initial response times across the four instructional groups were statistically analyzed using a one-way ANOVA, revealing no significant differences at the outset of the study [F(3, 33) = 0.738,
p = 0.159]. The Shapiro–Wilk test (see
Table 13) confirmed normality for response time data (
p > 0.05). This uniformity ensures that any posttest differences in response times can be attributed to the instructional treatments rather than baseline discrepancies among the groups.
Posttest Response Time Variations
Subsequent analyses compared response times from pretest to posttest. A repeated-measures ANOVA (see
Table 14) with factors for instructional group and time revealed significant differences [F(3, 33) = 5.122,
p = 0.003 for groups; F(3, 33) = 6.688,
p = 0.001 for time].
Post hoc Scheffe tests indicated significant interactions:
Groups One and Three: No significant difference (p = 0.735), suggesting similar efficacy in reducing response times.
Groups Two and Four: No significant difference (p = 0.539), indicating unaffected response times by the treatments.
Group One vs. Group Four: Significant reduction in Group One (p = 0.023).
Group Three vs. Group Four: Significant reduction in Group Three (p = 0.016).
Detailed Findings
Significant reductions in response times were noted for Groups One (referential-only) and Three (referential + affective), with Group One showing a reduction of at least 1400 milliseconds, and Group Three showing an even more substantial reduction of 2200 milliseconds from pretest to posttest. Conversely, participants in Group Two (affective-only) showed no significant change, aligning closely with the control group’s results.
The data demonstrate that referential and combined referential + affective treatments significantly enhance the speed of sentence interpretation concerning English past tense markers, suggesting a more efficient processing and understanding of grammatical structures due to these instructional interventions. The results underscore the potential of targeted structured input activities to markedly improve linguistic processing efficiency in L2 learners.
5.4. Discussion
This experimental study examined the impact of structured input activities on L2 learners’ online processing of English past tense markers. A self-paced reading test measured accuracy and response times across three instructional groups, addressing three critical research questions regarding the efficacy of referential, affective, and combined instructional treatments (see
Section 1 for SPR methodology). The greater effectiveness of referential and combined treatments aligns with VanPatten’s Input Processing Theory: referential tasks compel learners to attend directly to grammatical cues to derive meaning, fostering stronger form–meaning connections. The lack of additional advantage for combined tasks in the immediate posttest may reflect the short time window and the reduced opportunity for affective tasks to exert influence without rich communicative context. This aligns with
Henshaw (
2012) and
Robayna (
2020), who found that combined treatments often show their greatest benefits in retention or spontaneous production tasks.
Comparison with Previous Research
The superior performance of the referential and combined groups in accuracy and response time aligns with prior studies.
Benati (
2005) and
Benati et al. (
2008) found that processing instruction (PI), particularly with structured input, outperformed traditional instruction for English past tense acquisition among Chinese and Korean EFL learners. Similarly, this study’s results indicate that referential tasks, by forcing attention to verb morphology (e.g., “-ed”), enhance learners’ ability to process grammatical forms, supporting VanPatten’s Primacy of Meaning Principle. The modest improvement in the affective-only group (10% accuracy increase) mirrors findings by
Henshaw (
2012), where affective activities alone were less effective for immediate grammatical processing but contributed to retention when combined with referential tasks. Unlike
Robayna (
2020), who found the combined approach most effective for Spanish OVS and past tense, this study found no significant difference between referential-only and combined groups, possibly due to the shorter instructional period (one week vs. longer interventions in Robayna’s study) or the specific challenges of English past tense for Chinese EFL learners, whose L1 lacks tense inflection.
The increased reaction times (RTs) at the target verb and V + 1 positions for the referential and combined groups suggest deeper processing, consistent with
Jegerski (
2013), who noted that longer RTs indicate heightened attention to linguistic forms. This contrasts with the affective-only and control groups, where RTs remained unchanged, indicating minimal impact on processing strategies. The absence of RT changes at V + 2 aligns with the spill-over effect described by
Chan (
2012), where processing effects may manifest one or two words after the target, but not further. This suggests that the instructional interventions specifically enhanced attention to the verb and its immediate context, rather than broader sentence processing.
Reasons for Similarities and Differences
The similarities with
Benati (
2005) and
Benati et al. (
2008) likely stem from the shared focus on structured input and the use of tasks that prioritize form–meaning connections, particularly for learners whose L1 lacks inflectional morphology. The differences, such as the lack of superiority of the combined approach over referential-only, may be attributed to methodological factors. First, the short duration of the intervention (two sessions within one week) may have limited the affective tasks’ ability to foster deeper communicative engagement, as affective activities often require extended exposure to yield significant effects (
Henshaw, 2012). Second, the Chinese L1 participants’ reliance on lexical cues (e.g., temporal adverbs) over grammatical markers, as predicted by VanPatten’s Preference for Non-redundancy Principle, likely amplified the effectiveness of referential tasks, which explicitly target verb morphology. The SPR methodology, which minimizes explicit knowledge interference, may have further highlighted the strengths of referential tasks by capturing real-time processing improvements.
The lack of significant improvement in the affective-only group could be due to the reduced emphasis on form-focused processing, as affective tasks prioritize personal engagement over grammatical accuracy. This aligns with
VanPatten (
2004), who argues that learners prioritize meaning over form unless explicitly directed otherwise. The control group’s lack of improvement is expected, as they received no targeted instruction, consistent with prior PI studies (e.g.,
Benati & Lee, 2010).
Critical Analysis and Implications
The findings suggest that referential tasks are particularly effective for initial acquisition of English past tense markers among Chinese EFL learners, likely because they counteract the tendency to overlook verb morphology in favor of lexical cues. However, the lack of additional benefit from combining referential and affective tasks in the immediate posttest raises questions about the optimal balance of these activities. Future research could explore longer intervention periods to determine if affective tasks enhance retention when paired with referential tasks over time. Additionally, the increased RTs in the combined group at the target verb and V + 1 positions suggest that combining task types may lead to more deliberate processing, potentially benefiting long-term acquisition, as suggested by
Robayna (
2020).
The study’s reliance on SPR provides a methodological advantage over offline tests used in previous studies (e.g.,
Benati, 2004a), as it captures real-time processing dynamics. However, the ecological validity of SPR is limited, as it isolates word-level processing and may not fully reflect naturalistic language use. This could explain why affective tasks, which rely on communicative context, underperformed compared to expectations from
Henshaw (
2012). Future studies could integrate SPR with tasks that incorporate richer communicative contexts, such as interactive dialogues, to better assess affective activities’ contributions.
Responses to Research Questions
The referential and combined referential + affective groups performed significantly better than affective-only and control groups in interpreting correct past tense sentences. The results were supported by significant improvements from pretest to posttest in these groups, affirming the effectiveness of referential and combined instructional approaches over affective-only instruction. This affirms the effectiveness of referential and combined approaches, aligning with
Benati (
2005) and
VanPatten (
2002).
The response times decreased significantly in the referential-only and combined groups from pretest to posttest, suggesting that these treatments enhance processing speed. In contrast, the affective-only and control groups showed no significant change in response time, underscoring the limited impact of affective activities alone on processing efficiency. This supports the superiority of form-focused tasks for immediate processing gains (
VanPatten, 2015).
Significant RT increases at the V + 1 position for referential and combined groups suggest enhanced attention to grammatical forms, consistent with
Jegerski (
2013). The lack of RT changes at V + 2 indicates a focused effect on the verb and its immediate context, supporting the spill-over effect (
Chan, 2012).