Lost and Found? Shifts in Heritage Speakers’ Processing of Mood Morphology over the Course of a Semester Abroad
Abstract
1. Introduction
1.1. Heritage Language Development in Study Abroad Contexts
1.2. Lexically Selected Subjunctive Mood and Its Acquisition by Heritage Speakers
(1) | Sara | quiere | que | sus hijos | reciban/*reciben | una educación | bilingüe |
Sara | want-3PS | that | her kids | receive-SUBJ/*IND | a bilingual | education | |
Sara wants her kids to get a bilingual education | |||||||
(2) | Alex | pide | que | sus hijos | le digan/*dicen la | historia | en francés |
Alex | ask -3PS | that | his kids | him tell-SUBJ/*IND | the story | in French | |
Alex asks his kids to tell him the story in French |
1.3. Research Questions and Hypotheses
- RQ1: Are Spanish-dominant bilinguals (SDBs) sensitive to mood violations in Spanish? If so, will their sensitivity be affected by morphological regularity?
- RQ2: Are adult HSs of Spanish sensitive to mood violations with regular and irregular verbs in Spanish?
- RQ3: Finally, does HSs’ sensitivity to mood violations change over the course of a semester abroad in Spain?
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Procedure
2.3. Self-Paced Reading Task
2.4. Data Trimming and Statistical Analyses
3. Results
3.1. Spanish-Dominant Bilinguals
3.2. Heritage Speakers
4. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Abbreviations
HL | heritage language |
HS | heritage speaker |
INDIC | indicative mood |
OPI | oral proficiency interview |
RT | reading/response time |
SA | study abroad |
SDB | Spanish-dominant bilingual |
SPR | Self-paced reading |
SUBJ | subjunctive mood |
Appendix A
Lexeme | Indicative Form | Subjunctive Form |
---|---|---|
caer (‘fall’) | ca-en | cai-gan |
decir (‘say’) | di-cen | di-gan |
poner (‘put’) | po-nen | pon-gan |
salir (‘leave’) | sa-len | sal-gan |
tener (‘have’) | tie-nen | ten-gan |
traer (‘bring’) | tra-en | trai-gan |
valer (‘to be worth’) | va-len | val-gan |
venir (‘to come’) | vie-nen | ven-gan |
abrir (‘to open’) | a-bren | a-bran |
beber (‘to drink’) | be-ben | be-ban |
comer (‘to eat’) | co-men | co-man |
correr (‘to run’) | co-rren | co-rran |
cumplir (‘to follow’) | cum-plen | cum-plan |
leer (‘to read’) | le-en | le-an |
subir (‘to go up’) | su-ben | su-ban |
vivir (‘to live’) | vi-ven | vi-van |
1 | See Perez-Cortes (2022) for evidence of regularity effects in a study of contextually selected subjunctive mood in Spanish. |
2 | For studies that use self-paced reading to assess other types of subjunctive mood, see Cameron (2017) and Villegas et al. (2013). |
3 | As discussed by Shively (2016), many US-born/raised HSs study abroad in Spain, which continues to be a very popular destination for American university students (George & Hoffman-González, 2019). Some HSs studying abroad in Spain face criticism from locals for their dialectal background and/or way of speaking (Peace, 2023), an experience which might affect their linguistic development. Nonetheless, not all HSs have this experience (Peace, 2023). Furthermore, because HSs hear linguistic criticism even when traveling to their “ancestral country” (Riegelhaupt & Carrasco, 2000), there is no clear way to eliminate potential effects of these negative experiences on HSs’ linguistic development abroad. |
4 | An anonymous reviewer suggested that explicit grammar instruction on the subjunctive mood in class could cause changes in participants’ sensitivity to mood over time. Nonetheless, we think it is very unlikely that this would be enough to affect participants’ on-line processing of mood. The L2 Spanish learners in Fernández Cuenca and Jegerski (2023), for example, were not sensitive to mood on regular verbs, even though they had many years of classroom Spanish experience and in some cases even worked as Spanish teachers. Furthermore, as described above, the participants in this study were enrolled primarily in “content” courses that focused on literature, culture, film, and medicine, among other topics. Though the professors of these courses may have discussed subjunctive mood at some point in class, they did not focus on this topic, making it especially implausible that instruction would cause major changes in participants’ processing of mood over time. |
5 | Other researchers have categorized the regularity of subjunctive verbs in Spanish differently. Gudmestad (2012), for example, divides Spanish verbs into three regularity categories: (i) regular (e.g., come/coma: ‘eat’), (ii) irregular (e.g., duerme/duerma: ‘sleep’), and (iii) form-specific irregular (e.g., es/sea: ‘be’). Although Perez-Cortes (2022) and López-Beltrán and Dussias (2024) use a binary classification of regularity, as in the present study, their irregular verb category was more expansive than ours, specifically, by including inflections without the -g- consonant that appears in all of our irregular verbs. |
6 | Since the SDBs participated in one testing session, half completed Version A of the SPR task, and half completed Version B. |
7 | As noted by Luke (2017), p-values in mixed effects models are less certain than in simpler models. Furthermore, many researchers have begun to eschew dichotomous interpretations of statistical significance (e.g., Amrhein et al., 2019). |
8 | It is possible, as suggested by an anonymous reviewer, that participants’ faster RTs at Session 2 are caused (at least in part) by improvements in their reading comprehension skills and vocabulary and not necessarily by gains in morphosyntactic sensitivity. |
9 | Unfortunately, it will be very difficult to recruit participants for these comparisons. In the case of comparison (a), it will be difficult to find HSs who are not studying abroad and yet are enrolled in the same number of Spanish classes as HSs who are studying abroad. (Typically, HSs who study abroad take 3–4 Spanish classes). In the case of comparison (b), it will be challenging to find a group of HSs who are residing in an immersive study abroad context for a semester’s length of time but not taking Spanish classes. Most HSs who study abroad in a Spanish-speaking country take at least some courses in Spanish. |
10 |
References
- Amrhein, V., Greenland, S., & McShane, B. (2019). Scientists rise up against statistical significance. Nature, 567(7748), 305–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baayen, H., & Milin, P. (2010). Analyzing reaction times. International Journal of Psychological Research, 3(2), 12–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barr, D. J. (2013). Random effects structure for testing interactions in linear mixed-effects models. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67, 328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bosque, I. (2012). Mood: Indicative vs. subjunctive. In J. Hualde, A. Olarrea, & E. O’Rourke (Eds.), The handbook of Hispanic linguistics (pp. 371–394). Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
- Bowles, M. (2018). Outcomes of classroom Spanish heritage language instruction. In K. Potowski (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of Spanish as a heritage language (pp. 331–344). Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Bowles, M. (2022). Outcomes of university Spanish heritage language instruction in the United States. Georgetown University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Bybee, J., Perkins, R., & Pagliuca, W. (1994). The evolution of grammar. University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Cameron, R. (2017). Lexical preference and online processing of the Spanish subjunctive. Linguistics Unlimited, 2, 1–24. [Google Scholar]
- Cuetos, F., Glez-Nosti, M., Barbón, A., & Brysbaert, M. (2011). SUBTLEX-ESP: Spanish word frequencies based on film subtitles. Psicológica, 32, 133–143. [Google Scholar]
- Davidson, D., & Lekic, M. (2013). The heritage and non-heritage learner in the overseas immersion context: Comparing learning outcomes and target-language utilization in the Russian flagship. Heritage Language Journal, 10(2), 226–252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davies, M. (2016). Corpus del español: Web/dialects. Available online: https://www.corpusdelespanol.org/web-dial/ (accessed on 20 August 2021).
- Dracos, M., & Requena, P. (2023). Child heritage speakers’ acquisition of the Spanish subjunctive in volitional and adverbial clauses. Language Acquisition, 30(1), 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dracos, M., Requena, P., & Miller, K. (2019). Acquisition of mood selection in Spanish-speaking children. Language Acquisition, 26(1), 106–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Escalante, C., Viera, C., & Patiño-Vega, M. (2021). Linguistic development of Spanish heritage learners in study abroad: Considerations, implications, and future directions. In R. Pozzi, T. Quan, & C. Escalante (Eds.), Heritage speakers of Spanish and study aborad (pp. 181–198). Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Fernández Cuenca, S., & Jegerski, J. (2023). A role for verb regularity in the L2 processing of the Spanish subjunctive mood: Evidence from eye-tracking. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 45(2), 318–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fernández Cuenca, S., & Jegerski, J. (2025, June 9–13). Morphological regularity effects in the processing of the Spanish subjunctive by heritage speakers: An eye-tracking study [Paper presentation]. International Symposium on Bilingualism, San Sebastian, Spain. [Google Scholar]
- Foote, R. (2011). Integrated knowledge of agreement in early and late English-Spanish bilinguals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 187–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- George, A., & Hoffman-González, A. (2019). Dialect and identity: US heritage language learners of Spanish abroad. Study Abroad Research in Second Language Acquisition and International Education, 4(2), 252–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Giancaspro, D. (2017). Heritage speakers’ production and comprehension of lexically and contextually selected subjunctive mood morphology [Doctoral dissertation, Rutgers University]. [Google Scholar]
- Giancaspro, D., Perez-Cortes, S., & Higdon, J. (2022). (Ir)regular mood swings: Lexical variability in heritage speakers’ oral production of subjunctive mood. Language Learning, 72(2), 456–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grey, S. (2018). Quantitative approaches for study abroad research. In C. Sanz, & A. Morales-Front (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of study abroad research and practice (pp. 48–57). Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Gudmestad, A. (2012). Toward an understanding of the relationship between mood use and form regularity: Evidence of variation across tasks, lexical items, and participant groups. In K. Geeslin, & M. Díaz-Campos (Eds.), Selected proceedings of the 14th Hispanic linguistics symposium (pp. 214–227). Cascadilla Proceedings Project. [Google Scholar]
- Jegerski, J. (2013). Self-paced reading. In J. Jegerski, & B. VanPatten (Eds.), Research methods in second language psycholinguistics (pp. 20–49). Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Jegerski, J. (2018). Psycholinguistic perspectives on heritage Spanish. In K. Potowski (Ed.), The Routledge handbook on Spanish as a heritage language (pp. 221–234). Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Jegerski, J., & Keating, G. (2023). Using self-paced reading in research with heritage speakers: A role for reading skill in the on-line processing of Spanish verb argument specifications. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1056561. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Keating, G. (2022). The effect of age of onset of bilingualism on gender agreement processing in Spanish as a heritage language. Language Learning, 72(4), 1170–1208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keating, G., & Jegerski, J. (2015). Experimental designs in sentence processing research: A methodological review and user’s guide. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37(1), 1–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuznetsova, A., Brockoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2014). lmerTest: Tests for random and fixed effects for linear mixed effect models (lmer objects of lme4 package). Available online: http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lmerTest/index.html (accessed on 1 May 2025).
- Larson-Hall, J. (2015). A guide to doing statistics in second language research using SPSS and R. Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Lenth, R., Singmann, H., Love, J., Buerkner, P., & Herve, M. (2018). Emmeans: Estimated marginal means, aka least square means. Available online: http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/emmeans/index.html (accessed on 1 May 2025).
- López-Beltrán, P., & Dussias, P. E. (2024). Heritage speakers’ processing of the Spanish subjunctive: A pupillometric study. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 14(6), 809–855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luke, S. G. (2017). Evaluating significance in linear mixed-effects models in R. Behavior Research Methods, 49(4), 1494–1502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marqués-Pascual, L. (2021). The impact of study abroad on Spanish heritage learners’ writing development. In R. Pozzi, T. Quan, & C. Escalante (Eds.), Heritage speakers of Spanish and study abroad (pp. 199–216). Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Marqués-Pascual, L., & Checa-García, I. (2023). Lexical development of Spanish heritage and L2 learners in a study abroad setting. Study Abroad Research in Second Language Acquisition and International Education, 8(1), 115–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marsden, E., Thompson, S., & Plonsky, L. (2018). A methodological synthesis of self-paced reading in second language research. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39(5), 861–904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paradigm. (2016). Experiment builder software [self-paced feature]. Available online: http://www.paradigmexperiments.com/index.html (accessed on 20 August 2021).
- Peace, M. (2023). ‘A mí no me gusta España mucho’ vs. ‘Yo quiero ir a toda España, me encanta España.’ Heritage speakers and their receptiveness to Spain and Peninsular Spanish. Study Abroad Research in Second Language Acquisition and International Education, 8(2), 197–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perez-Cortes, S. (2022). Lexical frequency and morphological regularity as sources of heritage speaker variability in the acquisition of mood. Second Language Research, 38(1), 149–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perez-Cortes, S. (2023). Re-examining the role of mood selection type in Spanish heritage speakers’ subjunctive production. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 13(2), 238–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perez-Cortes, S. (2025). Obviating the mood, but mostly under control: Spanish heritage speakers’ acquisition of the binding constraints of desiderative complements. Language Acquisition, 32(1), 76–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perez-Cortes, S., & Giancaspro, D. (2022). (In)frequently asked questions: On types of frequency and their role(s) in heritage language variability. Frontiers in Psychology, 13, 1002978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Portes, A., & Rumbaut, R. (2014). Immigrant America: A portrait. University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
- Pozzi, R., & Reznicek-Parrado, L. (2021). Problematizing heritage language identities: Heritage speakers of Mexican descent studying abroad in Argentina. Study Abroad Research in Second Language Acquisition and International Education, 6(2), 189–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Putnam, M., & Sánchez, L. (2013). What’s so incomplete about incomplete acquisition? A prolegomenon to modeling heritage language grammars. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 3(4), 478–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Core Team. (2024). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available online: https://www.r-project.org/ (accessed on 1 May 2025).
- Riegelhaupt, F., & Carrasco, R. L. (2000). Mexico host family reactions to a bilingual Chicana teacher in Mexico: A case study of language and culture clash. Bilingual Research Journal, 24, 405–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sedivy, J. (2021). Memory speaks: On losing and reclaiming language and self. Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Shively, R. (2016). Heritage language learning in study abroad: Motivations, identity work, and language development. In D. Pascual y Cabo (Ed.), Advances in Spanish as a heritage language (pp. 259–280). John Benjamins. [Google Scholar]
- Sotomayor, S. (2013). My beloved world. Knopf. [Google Scholar]
- Thane, P. D. (2025). Acquring morphology through adolescence in Spanish as a heritage language: The case of subjunctive mood. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 28, 373–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thane, P. D., Austin, J., Rodríguez, S., & Goldin, M. (2025). The acquisition of the Spanish subjunctive by child heritage and L2 learners: Evidence from a dual language program. Studies in Hispanic and Lusophone Linguistics, 18(1), 193–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Villegas, Á., Demestre, J., & Dussias, P. E. (2013). Processing relative/sentence complement clauses in immersed Spanish-English speakers. In J. Cabrelli Amaro, G. Lord, & A. de Prada Pérez (Eds.), Selected proceedings of the 16th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium (pp. 70–79). Cascadilla Proceedings Project. [Google Scholar]
- Viner, K. (2016). Second-generation NYC bilinguals’ use of the Spanish subjunctive in obligatory contexts. Spanish in Context, 13(3), 343–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Trigger Verb | Translation | Subjunctive Selection # |
---|---|---|
querer | ‘want’ | 100% |
ordenar | ‘order’ | 100% |
esperar | ‘hope’ | 98% |
necesitar | ‘need’ | 100% |
desear | ‘wish’ | 100% |
pedir | ‘ask’ | 100% |
hacer | ‘make’ | 97% |
dejar | ‘allow’ | 100% |
Condition | Properties Targeted | Grammaticality | # of Items |
---|---|---|---|
Experimental | Mood Morphology | Grammatical | 16 |
Mood Morphology | Ungrammatical | 16 | |
Distractors | Subject-Verb Agree | Grammatical | 16 |
Subject-Verb Agree | Ungrammatical | 16 | |
Fillers | Miscellaneous | Grammatical | 32 |
Miscellaneous | Ungrammatical | 32 |
Region | Factor | Estimate | SE | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Critical Verb | Intercept | 6.34 | 0.08 | 75.13 | 0.00 |
Grammaticality | 0.05 | 0.03 | 1.40 | 0.16 | |
Verb Regularity | −0.06 | 0.03 | −1.77 | 0.07 | |
Frequency | −0.03 | 0.02 | −1.47 | 0.14 | |
Grammaticality x Verb Regularity | 0.03 | 0.04 | 0.66 | 0.50 | |
Post-Critical Verb | Intercept | 6.31 | 0.08 | 78.92 | 0.00 |
Grammaticality | 0.09 | 0.03 | 2.77 | 0.00 | |
Verb Regularity | −0.02 | 0.03 | −0.46 | 0.65 | |
Frequency | −0.01 | 0.02 | −0.67 | 0.50 | |
Grammaticality x Verb Regularity | 0.01 | 0.04 | 0.27 | 0.79 | |
Post-Critical Verb + 1 | Intercept | 6.40 | 0.09 | 67.82 | 0.00 |
Grammaticality | 0.00 | 0.03 | 0.04 | 0.96 | |
Verb Regularity | −0.01 | 0.03 | −0.47 | 0.63 | |
Frequency | 0.01 | 0.02 | 0.52 | 0.60 | |
Grammaticality x Verb Regularity | 0.02 | 0.05 | 0.42 | 0.66 |
Factor | Estimate | SE | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 6.20 | 0.08 | 69.74 | 0.00 |
Grammaticality | −0.03 | 0.04 | −0.83 | 0.40 |
Session | 0.16 | 0.04 | 3.94 | 0.00 |
Verb Regularity | −0.08 | 0.04 | −1.94 | 0.05 |
Frequency | −0.00 | 0.02 | −0.33 | 0.73 |
Grammaticality x Session | −0.04 | 0.05 | −0.71 | 0.47 |
Grammaticality x Verb Regularity | 0.04 | 0.06 | 0.81 | 0.41 |
Session x Verb Regularity | 0.00 | 0.05 | 0.01 | 0.98 |
Grammaticality x Verb Regularity x Session | −0.02 | 0.08 | −0.34 | 0.73 |
Factor | Estimate | SE | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 6.49 | 0.09 | 65.35 | 0.00 |
Grammaticality | 0.02 | 0.05 | 0.56 | 0.57 |
Session | 0.13 | 0.05 | 2.69 | 0.00 |
Verb Regularity | −0.09 | 0.05 | −1.84 | 0.06 |
Frequency | −0.08 | 0.02 | −3.41 | 0.00 |
Grammaticality x Session | 0.07 | 0.07 | 1.03 | 0.30 |
Grammaticality x Verb Regularity | 0.01 | 0.07 | 0.26 | 0.79 |
Session x Verb Regularity | 0.07 | 0.07 | 0.97 | 0.33 |
Grammaticality x Verb Regularity x Session | −0.17 | 0.10 | −1.66 | 0.09 |
Factor | Estimate | SE | t | p |
---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 6.40 | 0.10 | 60.23 | 0.00 |
Grammaticality | 0.01 | 0.05 | 0.35 | 0.72 |
Session | 0.15 | 0.05 | 2.69 | 0.00 |
Frequency | −0.03 | 0.05 | −0.63 | 0.06 |
Verb Regularity | −0.03 | 0.02 | −1.14 | 0.25 |
Grammaticality x Session | 0.02 | 0.07 | 0.25 | 0.79 |
Grammaticality x Verb Regularity | 0.03 | 0.07 | 0.38 | 0.70 |
Session x Verb Regularity | 0.10 | 0.07 | 1.32 | 0.18 |
Grammaticality x Verb Regularity x Session | −0.14 | 0.11 | −1.66 | 0.18 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Giancaspro, D.; Fernández Cuenca, S. Lost and Found? Shifts in Heritage Speakers’ Processing of Mood Morphology over the Course of a Semester Abroad. Languages 2025, 10, 163. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages10070163
Giancaspro D, Fernández Cuenca S. Lost and Found? Shifts in Heritage Speakers’ Processing of Mood Morphology over the Course of a Semester Abroad. Languages. 2025; 10(7):163. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages10070163
Chicago/Turabian StyleGiancaspro, David, and Sara Fernández Cuenca. 2025. "Lost and Found? Shifts in Heritage Speakers’ Processing of Mood Morphology over the Course of a Semester Abroad" Languages 10, no. 7: 163. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages10070163
APA StyleGiancaspro, D., & Fernández Cuenca, S. (2025). Lost and Found? Shifts in Heritage Speakers’ Processing of Mood Morphology over the Course of a Semester Abroad. Languages, 10(7), 163. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages10070163