Intelligence and Moral Development: A Critical Historical Review and Future Directions
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. Intelligence from a “Starter Kit” to Mindware
2.1. A Controversy
2.2. A Narrower Conception of Intelligence: The “Starter Kit”
We propose a model—the Parietal-Frontal Integration Theory, or PFIT—that elucidates the critical interaction between association cortices within parietal and frontal brain regions which, when effectively linked by white matter structures, …underpins individual differences in reasoning competence in humans, and perhaps in other mammalian species as well.
We human beings have created not just physical machines … but also mental machines; mechanisms of thought embodied in our nervous system … These distinctively human cognitive mechanisms include causal understanding, episodic memory, imitation, mindreading, normative thinking, and many more. They are ‘gadgets’ rather than ‘instincts’ … because, like many physical devices, they are products of cultural rather than genetic evolution (pp. 1–2).[Please note the reference to normative thinking in her description.]
The genetically inherited differences between our minds and those of our ancestors are small but very important. They enable the development of Big Special cognitive mechanisms in three ways. First, genetically inherited changes in temperament helped to make us remarkably social primates… Second, genetically inherited attentional biases ensure that the attention of human infants is locked-on to other agents from birth. We are driven from early infancy to look at biological motion and faces, to listen to human voices… Finally, humans have uniquely powerful central processors: mechanisms of learning, memory, and control that extract, filter, store and use information.(p. 53)
Each of these processors is domain-general, crunching data from all input domains using the same set of computations, and taxon-general, present in a wide range of animal species. However, humans genetically inherit central processors with unprecedented speed and capacity. Shaped and fed throughout development by the torrents of information flowing in from other agents, domain-general processors not only capture this information but use it to build new, domain-specific cognitive mechanisms—the Big Special mechanisms that make human such peculiar animals. Thus, the Big Special mechanisms are designed by cultural evolution, but they are built in the course of development by souped-up genetically inherited mechanisms of learning and memory, using raw materials that are, from birth, channeled into infant minds by genetically inherited temperamental and attentional biases.(pp. 53–54)
Moral principles, like causal principles and mathematical principles, must be learned … Our moral imagination is initially characterized by parochiality and self-interest and may remain so if not for moral instruction … we will explore the development of three principled distinctions in moral imagination: [1] that bad outcomes are not always caused by wrongdoing, [2] that the way things are is not always the way they ought to be, and [3] that an equal distribution of resources is not always an equitable distribution. These distinctions are a far cry from the higher-order principles that define modern ethics … but they are prerequisites for learning such principles.(pp. 147–48)
2.3. A Broader Conception of Intelligence: Mindware
Model-based judgment [corresponding to Type 2 systems thinking] is based on an explicit representation of cause-effect relationships between actions and outcomes, plus values attached to outcomes. Model-free judgment [corresponding to Type 1 systems thinking] depends, instead, on accessing values attached directly to actions based on prior reinforcement.(p. 35)
Mac explained that the bag contained something very special and irreplaceable—photographs of his grandmother—and that he was not going to let anyone take the photos from him. Mac’s peers understood his point of view but saw it as one-sided: Mac thought only of safeguarding his photos, without considering for a moment the staff member’s perspective or the facility’s necessary rules… [The staff member] was only carrying out institutional policy concerning possible contraband. Nor did Mac consider that she was not abusive and that he had no reason to assume that the photos would be confiscated. Generating the anger and the overt behavior identified as an Authority Problem, then, were “Self-Centered” and “Assuming the Worst” thinking errors.(p. 203)
3. Intelligence and Moral Development: Multiple Perspectives and Classic Research
3.1. Perspectives on Moral Development
3.2. A Psychological Perspective
3.3. A Social Perspective
3.4. A Neuroscience Perspective
children whose prefrontal cortex had been damaged before the age of 16 months. The damage left them unable to acquire social conventions and moral rules, throughout life…Although normal in language and intelligence [emphasis added], these patients exhibit behaviour perceived as antisocial, such as shoplifting, sexually aggressive behaviour and non-responsiveness to punishment.(p. 295)
[a]t the age of three, synaptic density reaches its lifetime peak and is 50% greater than in the adult brain. Brain areas develop at different rates and peak periods are established at different times. For example, the prefrontal cortex, vital for moral functioning, accelerates at 8 months of age and reaches maximal density at the age of two, at which point cortical development plateaus until early adolescence and is not complete till nearly the age of 30.(p. 301)
It is perplexing because the way that morality has been defined, operationalized, and measured in most neuroscience experiments is wildly discrepant from the extensive development and psychological research on how individuals use moral criteria, evaluate morally charged issues, and make moral judgments. In most neuroscience studies, for example, morality is not explicitly defined; it is taken for granted in the nature of the task.(pp. 241–42)
A central lesson is that moral cognition is not localized to one brain area … Instead, our capacity for moral judgments involves a spatially distributed network of areas with domain-general psychological functions that are also relevant to moral evaluation, such as understanding the consequences of an agent’s actions, the agent’s mental state, how the action was causally brought about, and the social norms it violates.(p. 25)
3.5. Some Recurring Issues
3.6. Noteworthy Research Studies from an Earlier Era Until Recent Times
[o]ne could reasonably expect more intelligent children to be less concrete and more flexible in their thinking, possess a higher level of conceptual ability, and have greater insight into the social processes than less intelligent children, and that these factors should have a significant impact on their perceptions and evaluations of moral issues.(p. 28)
- reading and expressing emotion
- taking the perspective of others
- caring by connecting to others
- working with interpersonal and group differences
- preventing social bias
- generating interpretations and options
- identifying the consequences of actions and options (Narvaez 2001, p. 7).
- Their technique used more realistic moral scenarios, as opposed to those used in the Moral Judgment Interview and the DIT; thus, responses may not truly measure one’s moral development/morality. Note, the use of non-realistic dilemmas has been criticized in the past. See, for example, Slavin’s Educational Psychology: Theory and Practice, 12th ed. (Slavin 2018).
- Many previous studies involved gifted students, but how these students were identified as gifted and what determined their giftedness (e.g., cognitive ability) are unknown.
- Their method was similar to that used in many studies on prosocial behavior that found, for example, no relationship between intelligence and working memory and sharing behavior, which is part of prosocial behavior, which is considered part of moral development (See for example, Paulus et al. 2015).
- using participants who are below the average range of intelligence (e.g., Beißert and Hasselhorn 2016). (Note that this brings up again the issue of how intelligence is measured),
- including preschool children in one’s research (e.g., Beißert and Hasselhorn 2016) (Note that this is suggested previously in this paper), and
- conducting cross-cultural studies.
4. Different Take on Morality
4.1. Gert’s Depiction of Common Morality 1: Thoughts on Good and Evil
the view of a human history developed so far in this book suggests that humans behave as if they are concerned with their own genetic interests, and they are also masters at deceiving others. I suggest that the separateness of our individual self-interests, and the conflicts among us that derive from this separateness, have created a social milieu in which, paradoxically, the only way we can actually maximize our own self-interest and deceive successfully is by continually denying—at least in certain social areas—that we are doing such things … The result, I believe, is that in our social scenario-building we have evolved to deceive even ourselves about our true motives.
4.2. Gert’s Depiction of Common Morality 2: The Moral Rules
- Do not kill.
- Do not cause pain.
- Do not disable.
- Do not deprive of freedom or opportunity.
- Do not deprive of pleasure. (p. 86)
- Do not deceive.
- Keep your promise.
- Do not cheat.
- Obey the law.
- Do your duty. (p. 125)
This chapter spells out the wisdom that ideas about karma, the sacred self, the sacred world, and feudal ethics encode in their metaphors. It applies one of the central assumptions of cultural psychology: indigenous or folk theories (our own and others) should be taken seriously as cognitive objects and as potential sources of social scientific and practical knowledge.(p. 120)
4.3. Gert’s Depiction of Common Morality 3: Moral Ideals
5. Intelligence and Morality in Action: Wisdom and Wisdom-Building
5.1. Sternberg’s Work on Wisdom
the application of one’s world knowledge and skills toward (1) attaining a common good; by (2) balancing one’s own, others’, and larger interests; over the (3) long-term as well as the short-term, through (4) the use of positive ethical values, in order to (5) adapt to, shape, or select environments(pp. 1–2).
5.2. Pioneering Work on Rethinking Practical Wisdom
The agent’s own ethical aims and aspirations, her understanding of what it takes to live and act well, and her need to live up to the standards that shape and are shaped by her understanding and experience of what matters. This amounts to what we call a blueprint of flourishing.
[A]n individual integrates different virtue relevant considerations, via a process of checks and balances, especially in circumstances where different ethically salient, or different kinds of virtues or values, appear to be in conflict and agents need to negotiate a dilemmatic space.(pp. 44–45)
Practical wisdom demands more than the skill to be perceptive about others. It also demands the capacity to perceive oneself—to assess what our own motives are, to admit our failure, to figure what has worked or not and why.(pp. 24–25)
5.3. For Wisdom-Building, Having a Socrates Figure as a Guide Is Nice, but Not Necessary
- a focusing exercise to create a relaxed, meditative frame of mind
- a brief link with the previous week’s discussion
- then came the stimulus for discussion—usually reading a story from Thinking Through Philosophy
- next the students work in pairs discussing the story and reflecting on some open-ended questions suggested by the story
- this was followed by dialogue in larger groups where the teacher has encouraged the students to form a community of inquiry by
- (a)
- communicating their views in response to the questions at hand,
- (b)
- supporting their views with reasons,
- (c)
- listening respectfully to the views of others,
- (d)
- indicating whether they agree or disagree with those views,
- (e)
- providing alternative viewpoints, and
- (f)
- gradually developing a process of dialogue.
- the teacher brings closure by encouraging the students to reflect on the discussion and how their thinking might have progressed by providing a “thought for the week” that highlights an idea to serve as a basis for “homework” to be reflected on in order to relate the idea to situations outside the original stimulus (Fair et al. 2015b, pp. 7–8).
- Staff development time needed is minimal. One day of in-service sufficed.
- The materials are inexpensive. For example, at the beginning of 2013 a downloadable pdf copy of Thinking Through Philosophy, Book 4, can be purchased from Educational Printing Services in the UK for £17.50 (about $25.00 USD).
- The P4C lessons require only one hour per week of instructional time.
- The extent of time required for the program to have an effect is reasonable—25 weeks or so (Fair et al. 2015a, p. 35).
- I learned how to show facts and find evidence and how to use it during disputes.
- It helped me understand that close mindedness is not a good thing.
- It helps you think about choices you make and view the world differently.
- I learned that to put your opinion out there you have to make sure you know what you are saying.
- I learned that you should give people a chance to talk because they might change your mind (Fair et al. 2015a, p. 29).
My approach to the course has been one of highlighting individual moral development, so I encourage the students toward introspection and discussion with one another. Students responded well to hypothetical ethical dilemmas, and I asked them to analyze the rationale behind the moral decisions of literary characters. I was impressed by the level of camaraderie that the students showed by the end of the semester, and their willingness to hear multiple viewpoints.
A community of truth is created when people are genuinely interested in seeing and exploring together. They do not try to manipulate each other. They do not immediately judge, say “That’s stupid” or “That’s right.” Instead, they pause to consider what the meaning of the statement is to the person who just uttered it.(p. 264)
6. Conclusions: So Where Do We Go from Here?
6.1. Suggestions for Understanding Intelligence
6.2. Suggestions for Understanding Moral Development
- Using participants who are below the average range of intelligence (e.g., Beißert and Hasselhorn 2016) and noting that this brings up the issue that must be addressed of how best to measure intelligence,
- Including preschool children in one’s research (e.g., Beißert and Hasselhorn 2016), and
- Conducting cross-cultural studies.
6.3. Suggestions About Nurturing Moral Intelligence
Author Contributions
Funding
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Alexander, Richard. 1987. The Biology of Moral Systems. Boston: De Gruyter. First published 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Ambrose, Don, and Tracy Cross, eds. 2009. Morality, Ethics and Gifted Minds. New York: Springer Science. [Google Scholar]
- Anderson, Steven W., Antoine Bechara, Hanna Damasio, Daniel Tranel, and Antonio R. Damasio. 1999. Impairment of social and moral behavior related to early damage in human prefrontal cortex. Nature Neuroscience 2: 1032–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bandura, Albert. 1977. Social Learning Theories. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall. [Google Scholar]
- Barlow, Horace B. 1983. Intelligence, guesswork, language. Nature 304: 207–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bebeau, Muriel. 2002. The defining issues test and the four component model: Contributions to professional education. Journal of Moral Education 31: 271–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Beißert, Hanna M., and Marcus Hasselhorn. 2016. Individual differences in moral development: Does intelligence really affect children’s moral reasoning and moral emotions? Frontiers in Psychology 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brandt, Richard. 1979. A Theory of the Good and the Right. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Brooks, David. 2023. How to Know a Person. New York: Random House. [Google Scholar]
- Camara-Pastor, Tomas. 2021. Sensitivida Etica: Un Studio Comparative Entre Escolares con Alta Capacidad Intellectual y Escolares Tipicos [Ethical Sensitivity: A Comparative Study Between Students with High Intellectual Capacity and Students with Average Intellectual Capacity]. TFM. Unpublished work. Logrono: Universidad de La Rioja. [Google Scholar]
- Cattell, Raymond B. 1950. Culture Fair Intelligence Test: A Measure of “g”. Savory: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing. [Google Scholar]
- Cattell, Raymond B. 1963. Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology 54: 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cianciolo, Anna T., and Robert J. Sternberg. 2004. Intelligence: A brief history. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Cleghorn, Paul, and Stephanie Baudet. 2002. Thinking Through Philosophy: Book 4. Blackburn: Educational Printing Services. [Google Scholar]
- Colom, Roberto, Richard J. Haier, Kevin Head, Juan Álvarez-Linera, Maria Ángeles Quiroga, Pei C. Shih, and Rex E. Jung. 2009. Gray matter correlates of fluid, crystallized, and spatial intelligence: Testing the P-FIT model. Intelligence 37: 124–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Colom, Roberto, Sherif Karama, Rex E. Jung, and Richard J. Haier. 2010. Human intelligence and brain networks. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 12: 489–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Crabbe, John C., and Tamara J. Phillips. 2003. Mother nature meets mother nurture. Nature Neuroscience 6: 440–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Creane, Michael. 2025. Deceptively fair: How critical thought, teacher engagement, and classroom policy choices impact moral reasoning among elementary students. In Critical Thinking in Moral Development: Philosophy, Psychology, Education, and Assessment. Edited by Daniel Fasko and Frank Fair. Boston: De Gruyter Brill, pp. 163–84. [Google Scholar]
- Dabdoub, Juan P., and Marvin Berkowitz. 2025. Reasoning at the core of moral development: Where Kohlberg and Aristotle meet. In Critical Thinking in Moral Development: Philosophy, Psychology, Education, and Assessment. Edited by Daniel Fasko and Frank Fair. Boston: De Gruyter Brill, pp. 120–42. [Google Scholar]
- de Brigard, Felipe, and Walter Sinnott-Armstrong. 2022. Neuroscience and Philosophy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- De Neys, Wim. 2023. Advancing theorizing about fast-and slow thinking. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 46: e111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Derryberry, William P., Travis Wilson, Hannah Snyder, Tony Norman, and Brian Barger. 2005. Moral judgment developmental differences between gifted youth and college students. The Journal of Secondary Gifted Education 17: 6–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eisenberg, Nancy. 1982. The Development of Prosocial Behavior. New York: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Eisenberg, Nancy. 2001. The core and correlates of affective social competence. Social Development 10: 120–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fair, Frank, Lory Haas, Carol Gardosik, Daphne Johnson, Debra Price, and Olena Leipnik. 2015a. Socrates in the schools from Scotland to Texas: Replicating a study on the effects of a philosophy for children program. Journal of Philosophy in Schools 2: 18–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fair, Frank, Lory Haas, Carol Gardosik, Daphne Johnson, Debra Price, and Olena Leipnik. 2015b. Socrates in the schools: Gains at three-year follow-up. Journal of Philosophy in Schools 2: 5–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fasko, Daniel, and Frank Fair, eds. 2025. Critical Thinking in Moral Development: Philosophy, Psychology, Education, and Assessment. Boston: De Gruyter Brill. [Google Scholar]
- Feldman, Robert S. 2017. Life-Span Development: A Topical Approach, 3rd ed. Boston: Pearson. [Google Scholar]
- Gardner, Howard. 1983. Frames of Mind: The Theory of Multiple Intelligences. New York: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
- Gert, Bernard. 1970. The Moral Rules. New York: Harper & Row. [Google Scholar]
- Gert, Bernard. 2005. Morality: Its Nature and Justification, rev. ed. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Gert, Bernard, Charles Culver, and K. Danner Clouser. 2006. Bioethics: A Systematic Approach, 2nd ed. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Gibbs, John. 2019. Moral Development and Reality, 4th ed. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Gibbs, John. 2025. Moral development as growth beyond the superficial. In Critical Thinking in Moral Development: Philosophy, Psychology, Education, and Assessment. Edited by Daniel Fasko and Frank Fair. Boston: De Gruyter Brill, pp. 81–96. [Google Scholar]
- Glück, Judith, and Nic Westrate. 2022. Wisdom researchers and the elephant: An integrative model of wise behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review 26: 342–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldstein, Sam. 2015. The evolution of intelligence. In Handbook of Intelligence: Educational Theory, Historical Perspective, and Current Concepts. Edited by Sam Goldstein, Dana Princiotta and Jack A. Naglieri. New York: Springer, pp. 3–7. [Google Scholar]
- Goriounova, Natalia A., and Huibert D. Mansvelder. 2019. Genes, cells and brain areas of intelligence. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 13: 44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gosser, David, Jack Kampmeier, and Pratibha Varma-Nelson. 2010. Peer-led team learning: 2008 James Flack Norris Award Address. Journal of Chemical Education 87: 374–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gray, Kurt. 2025. Outraged: Why We Fight About Morality and Politics and How to Find Common Ground. New York: Pantheon Books. [Google Scholar]
- Greene, Joshua. 2023. Dual-process moral judgment beyond fast and slow. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 46: e123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Grossmann, Igor, Nic Westrate, Monika Ardelt, Justin Brienza, Mengxi Dong, Michael Ferrari, Marc Fournier, Chao Hu, Howard Musbaum, and John Vervaeke. 2020. The science of wisdom in a polarized world: Knowns and unknowns. Psychological Inquiry 31: 103–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haidt, Jonathan. 2012. The Righteous Mind. New York: Vintage Books. [Google Scholar]
- Hartshorne, Hugh, and Mark A. May. 1930. A summary of the work of the character education inquiry. Religious Education 25: 607–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Helwig, Charles. 2006. Rights, civil liberties, and democracy across cultures. In Handbook of Moral Development. Edited by Melanie Killen and Judith G. Smetana. Mahwah: Erlbaum, pp. 185–210. [Google Scholar]
- Hendrickson, Kenneth, John de Castro, Douglas Krienke, Frank Fair, Maria Botero, and William Kerr. 2024. Peer-led team learning (PLTL) and the humanities: Refreshing freshman composition classes with peer-led team learning. Advances in Peer-Led Learning 4: 3–17. [Google Scholar]
- Heyes, Cecilia. 2018. Cognitive Gadgets. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Hoffman, Stanley. 1977. Intelligence and the development of moral judgment in children. The Journal of Genetic Psychology 130: 27–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Howard-Hamilton, Mary, and Bridget A. Franks. 1995. Gifted adolescents: Psychological behaviors values, and developmental implications. Roeper Review 17: 186–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Immordino-Yang, Mary Helen. 2025. Growing the adolescent mind. Scientific American 332: 48–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Immordino-Yang, Mary Helen, Christina Kundrak, Douglas Knecht, and Jamaal Matthews. 2024. Civic reasoning depends on transcendent thinking: Implications of adolescent brain development for SEL. Social and Emotional Learning: Research, Practice, and Policy 4: 100067. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jung, Rex, and Richard Haier. 2007. The parieto-frontal integration theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: Converging neuroimaging evidence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30: 135–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahneman, Daniel. 2013. Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Farrar, Straus, & Giroux. [Google Scholar]
- Kail, Robert, and Timothy A. Salthouse. 1994. Processing speed as a mental capacity. Acta Psychologica 86: 199–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kant, Immanuel. 1959. Foundations of the Metaphysics of Morals. New York: Bobbs-Merrill. First published 1785. [Google Scholar]
- Karnes, Frances A., and K. Eliot Brown. 1981. A short form of the WISC-R for gifted students. Psychology in the Schools 18: 169–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Killen, Melanie, and Judith Smetana. 2006. Handbook of Moral Development, 1st ed. Mahwah: Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Killen, Melanie, and Judith Smetana. 2007. The biology of morality. Human Development 50: 241–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kohlberg, Lawrence. 1958. Moral Judgment in the Years Ten to Sixteen. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Kohlberg, Lawrence. 1969. Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental approach to socialization. In Handbook of Socialization: Theory and Research. Edited by David A. Goslin. Chicago: Rand McNally, pp. 347–480. [Google Scholar]
- Kohlberg, Lawrence. 1981. The Philosophy of Moral Development: Moral Stages and the Idea of Justice. San Francisco: Harper & Row. [Google Scholar]
- Kohlberg, Lawrence. 1984. The Psychology of Moral Development: The Nature and Validity of Moral Stages. San Francisco: Harper & Row. [Google Scholar]
- Kristjansson, Kristjan, and Blaine Fowers. 2024. Phronesis: Retrieving Practical Wisdom in Psychology, Philosophy, and Education. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kristjansson, Kristjan, Shane McLoughlin, and Steve Thoma. 2023. Phronesis: Developing and Validating a Short Measure of Practical Wisdom. Birmingham: University of Birmingham. [Google Scholar]
- Lewis, Thomas, Fari Amini, and Richard Lannon. 2000. A General Theory of Love. New York: Vintage. [Google Scholar]
- Malti, Tina, Michaela Gummerum, Monika Keller, and Marlis Buchmann. 2009. Children’s moral motivation, sympathy, and prosocial behavior. Child Development 80: 442–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maor, Dana, Hans-Werner Kaas, Kurt Strovink, and Ramesh Srinivasan. 2024. The Journey of Leadership. New York: Penguin Random House. [Google Scholar]
- Maslow, Abraham. 1987. Motivation and Personality, 3rd ed. New York: Pearson Longman. [Google Scholar]
- May, Joshua. 2019. Precis of Regard for reason in the moral mind. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 42: e146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- May, Joshua, Clifford Workman, Julia Haas, and Hyemin Han. 2022. The neuroscience of moral judgment: Empirical and philosophical developments. In Neuroscience and Philosophy. Edited by Felipe de Brigard and Walter Sinnott-Armstrong. Cambridge: MIT Press, pp. 17–48. [Google Scholar]
- Mischel, Walter, Ebbe B. Ebbesen, and Antonette R. Zeiss. 1972. Cognitive and attentional mechanisms in delay of gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 21: 204–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moran, Seana, Mindy Kornhaber, and Howard Gardner. 2006. Orchestrating multiple intelligences. Educational Leadership 64: 22–27. [Google Scholar]
- Narvaez, Darcia. 1993. High achieving students and moral judgment. Journal for Education of the Gifted 16: 268–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Narvaez, Darcia. 2001. Ethical Sensitivity: Activity Booklet 1. Available online: https://cee.nd.edu/curriculum/documents/actbklt1.pdf (accessed on 18 June 2025).
- Narvaez, Darcia. 2010. The emotional foundations of high moral intelligence. In Children’s Moral Emotions and Moral Cognition: Developmental and Educational Perspectives. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development. Edited by Brigitte Latzko and Tina Malti. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, vol. 129, pp. 77–94. [Google Scholar]
- Narvaez, Darcia, and Jenny L. Vaydich. 2008. Moral development and behaviour under the spotlight of the neurobiological sciences. Journal of Moral Education 37: 289–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Neisser, Ulric, Gwyneth Boodoo, Thomas J. Bouchard, Jr., A. Wade Boykin, Nathan Brody, Stephen J. Ceci, Diane F. Halpern, John C. Loehlin, Robert Perloff, Robert J. Sternberg, and et al. 1996. Intelligence: Knowns and unknowns. American Psychologist 51: 77–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ongley, Sophia F., and Tina Malti. 2014. The role of moral emotions in the development of children’s sharing behavior. Developmental Psychology 50: 1148–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Paulus, Markus, Maria Licata, Susanne Kristen-Antonow, Claudia Thoermer, Amanda Woodward, and Beate Sodian. 2015. Social understanding and self-regulation predict pre-schoolers’ sharing with friends and disliked peers: A longitudinal study. International Journal of Behavioral Development 39: 53–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perkins, David. 1995. Outsmarting IQ: The Emerging Science of Learnable Intelligence. New York: The Free Press. [Google Scholar]
- Piaget, Jean. 1965. The Moral Judgment of the Child. New York: Free Press. [Google Scholar]
- Piechowski, Michael M. 2009. The inner world of the young and bright. In Morality, Ethics, and Gifted Minds. Edited by Don Ambrose and Tracy Cross. New York: Springer Nature, pp. 177–94. [Google Scholar]
- Rest, James R. 1974. Manual for the Defining Issues Test. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota. [Google Scholar]
- Rest, James R. 1983. Morality. In Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol. 3, Cognitive Development. Edited by Paul Mussen. New York: Wiley, pp. 556–629. [Google Scholar]
- Roeper, Annemarie, and Linda K. Silverman. 2009. Giftedness and moral promise. In Morality, Ethics and Gifted Minds. Edited by Don Ambrose and Tracy Cross. New York: Springer Nature, pp. 251–64. [Google Scholar]
- Ross, Detlef H., and Tatiana Czeschlik. 1994. The psycho-social adjustment of gifted children in middle-childhood. European Journal of Psychology of Education 9: 15–25. [Google Scholar]
- Russell, Daniel. 2009. Practical Intelligence and the Virtues. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sastre-Riba, Sylvia, and Tomas Camara-Pastor. 2022. Ethical regulation and high intellectual ability. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19: 2689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sauer, Hanno. 2024. The Invention of Good and Evil. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Schwartz, Barry, and Kenneth Sharpe. 2010. Practical Wisdom: The Right Way to do the Right Thing. New York: Riverhead Books. [Google Scholar]
- Shtulman, Andrew. 2023. Learning to Imagine. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Shumsky, Abraham. 1956. Emotional Adjustment and Moral Judgment in Children. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Shweder, Richard A., Nancy C. Much, Manamohan Mahapatra, and Lawrence Park. 1997. The ‘big three’ of morality (autonomy, community, divinity) and the ‘big three’ explanations of suffering. In Morality and Health. Edited by Allan M. Brandt and Paul Rozin. New York: Routledge, pp. 119–69. [Google Scholar]
- Slavin, Robert. 2018. Educational psychology: Theory and practice, 12th ed. London: Pearson Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Smetana, Judith G. 2006. Social-cognitive domain theory: Consistencies in children’s moral and social judgments. In Handbook of Moral Development. Edited by Melanie Killen and Judith G. Smetana. Mahwah: Erlbaum, pp. 119–54. [Google Scholar]
- Stanovich, Keith. 2009. What Intelligence Tests Miss. New Haven: Yale University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 1985. Beyond IQ: A Triarchic Theory of Human Intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 2018. Theories of intelligence. In APA Handbook of Giftedness and Talent. Edited by Steven T. Pfeiffer, Elizabeth Shaunessy-Dedrick and Megan Foley-Nicpon. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 145–61. [Google Scholar]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 2024. Sketch of a TOP (Tree of Philosophy) theory. Review of General Psychology 28: 47–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J. 2025. A trilogy theory of moral intelligence. Review of General Psychology. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sternberg, Robert J., and Judith Glück. 2022. Wisdom: The Psychology of Wise Thoughts, Words, and Deeds. New York: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Tanner, Carmen, and Markus Christen. 2014. Moral intelligence—A framework for understanding moral competences. In Empirically Informed Ethics. Edited by Markus Christen, Carel van Schaik, Johannes Fischer, Markus Huppenbauer and Carmen Tanner. Zurich: Springer International, pp. 119–36. [Google Scholar]
- Tirri, Kirsi. 2011. Combining excellence and ethics: Implications for moral education for the gifted. Roeper Review 33: 59–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Topping, Keith, and Steven Trickey. 2007a. Collaborative philosophical enquiry for school children: Cognitive effects at 10–12 years. British Journal of Educational Psychology 77: 271–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Topping, Keith, and Steven Trickey. 2007b. Collaborative philosophical inquiry for schoolchildren: Cognitive gains at 2-year follow-up. British Journal of Educational Psychology 77: 787–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Trickey, Steven, and Keith Topping. 2006. Collaborative philosophical enquiry for school children: Socio-emotional effects at 11–12 years. School Psychology International 27: 599–614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tuomela, Raimo. 2007. The philosophy of sociality. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Turiel, Elliot. 1998. The development of morality. In Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol. 3. Social, Emotional, and Personality Development, 5th ed. Edited by William Damon and Nancy Eisenberg. New York: Wiley, pp. 863–932. [Google Scholar]
- Urraca-Martinez, Maria L., Sylvia Sastre-Riba, and Lourdes Viana-Saenz. 2021. World perception and high intellectual ability: A comparative study. Psicologia Educativa 27: 21–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wagner, Paul, Daphne Johnson, Frank Fair, and Daniel Fasko. 2018. Thinking Ahead: Engaging All Teachers in Critical Thinking. Lanham: Rowman & Littlefield. [Google Scholar]
- Wainryb, Cecilia. 2006. Moral development in culture: Diversity, tolerance, and justice. In Handbook of Moral Development. Edited by Melanie Killen and Judith G. Smetana. Mahwah: Erlbaum, pp. 211–40. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler, David. 1974. Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation. First published 1949. [Google Scholar]
- Weiß, Rudolf H., and Jürgen Osterland. 1997. Grundintelligenztest Skala 1 (CFT 1). 5., Revidierte Auflage. Culture Fair Intelligence Test—Scale 1. Gottingen: Hogrefe, vol. 5. [Google Scholar]
- Wiggam, Albert. E. 1941. Do brains and character go together. School and Society 54: 261–65. [Google Scholar]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Fair, F.; Fasko, D. Intelligence and Moral Development: A Critical Historical Review and Future Directions. J. Intell. 2025, 13, 72. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence13070072
Fair F, Fasko D. Intelligence and Moral Development: A Critical Historical Review and Future Directions. Journal of Intelligence. 2025; 13(7):72. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence13070072
Chicago/Turabian StyleFair, Frank, and Daniel Fasko. 2025. "Intelligence and Moral Development: A Critical Historical Review and Future Directions" Journal of Intelligence 13, no. 7: 72. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence13070072
APA StyleFair, F., & Fasko, D. (2025). Intelligence and Moral Development: A Critical Historical Review and Future Directions. Journal of Intelligence, 13(7), 72. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence13070072