4. Informational Label Related Data and Analysis
This section presents and analyzes data regarding informational labels. These labels present details in a manner similar to food nutrition fact labels and are designed to allow viewers to consider the relevant information and then to decide whether they want to consume the content or not. For each label, five types of data were collected and are analyzed in terms of three metrics. Respondents were asked about each label’s helpfulness, annoyingness, whether they would use the label, whether others would use the label and whether the label would help in assessing article trustworthiness. Respondents could answer yes, no or unsure. The data from these questions is analyzed, in this section, in terms of respondents’ age, education level and gender.
The helpfulness of informational label 1 (shown in
Figure 4), when it appears underneath a news article automatically, is considered in
Figure 5,
Figure 6 and
Figure 7. Respondents answered the question “would you find this label helpful?”.
In terms of age, there is a decline in perceived helpfulness as age increases. There are slight spikes in yes responses at the 40–44, 55–59, and 65 and older age groups. The number of uncertain responses shows no discernible pattern. Age groups other than 35–39, 45–49, and 60–64 show at least 50% answering yes even when uncertainty is factored in. When uncertainty is not considered, only the 60–64 age group maintains less than 50% yes responses.
By education level, there is a larger decline as education level increases from the some high school up to the bachelor’s degree education levels. There is a spike at the master’s degree level, which is maintained at the doctoral degree level, when uncertainty is not factored in. When uncertainty is introduced, doctoral degree holders’ support is less pronounced than master’s degree holders, due to a higher level of uncertainty amongst doctoral degree holders. Education groups, other than associate’s and bachelor’s degree holders, have at least 50% answering yes, even when uncertainty is factored in. Both of these groups show at least 50% answering “yes” when uncertainty is not considered.
By gender, there are more yes answers among females than male respondents and nearly equal levels of uncertainty. Both groups have at least 50% of respondents answering yes, even when uncertainty is factored in.
Figure 8,
Figure 9 and
Figure 10 consider annoyingness of informational label 1, with respondents answering the question “would you find this label annoying?”.
In terms of age, there is no clear pattern of decline or increase in perceived annoyingness as age increases. There are drops in yes responses at the 25–29, 50–54, and 55–59 age groups and an increase at the 60–64 age group. The number of uncertain responses also shows no discernable pattern. All age groups have less than 50% answering yes, when uncertainty is factored in. Only the 60–64 age group has greater than 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is not considered. This indicates a low level of annoyingness overall, amongst most age groups.
By education level, there is an increase in perceived annoyingness up to the associate’s degree level, then a decline up to the master’s degree level. Finally, there is a spike at the doctoral degree level. The spike at the doctoral degree level is less pronounced, once uncertainty is factored in, as doctoral degree holders show the largest level of uncertainty. All education groups have less than 50% of respondents answering yes, with uncertainty factored in. Only doctoral degree holders have at least 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is not considered. This indicates a low level of annoyingness overall, amongst most education groups.
By gender, there are more yes responses among males than females and more uncertainty among female respondents. Both groups have less than 50% of respondents answering yes, even when uncertainty is not considered. This indicates a low level of annoyingness overall, amongst both gender groups.
Figure 11,
Figure 12 and
Figure 13 consider likelihood that respondents will personally use informational label 1, with respondents answering the question “would you review this label when viewing news articles on social media?”.
In terms of age, there is no clear pattern of decline or increase in perceived personal usage as age increases. There are spikes in yes responses at the 30–34, 35–39, and 50–54 age groups. The number of uncertain responses also shows no discernible pattern. All age groups, other than 40–44, 45–49 and 55–59, have at least 50% of respondents answering yes, even when uncertainty is factored in. All groups have at least 50% yes responses when uncertainty is not considered.
By education level, there is an overall increase in yes responses, as education level increases. The lowest percentage of yes responses is at the some high school education level, and while the percentage of yes responses declines from the some college to bachelor’s degree levels, it increases again up to its peak at the doctoral degree level. Only three of the seven education groups (some college, master’s degree, and doctoral degree) have at least 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is factored in. All groups have at least a 50% level of yes responses, when uncertainty is not considered.
By gender, there are more yes responses among females than males and slightly higher uncertainty among females. Both groups have greater than 50% answering yes, even with uncertainty.
Figure 14,
Figure 15 and
Figure 16 consider respondents’ perception of the likelihood of others to use informational label 1, with respondents answering the question “would others review this label when viewing news articles on social media?”.
In terms of age, there is no clear pattern of decline or increase in perceived use by others as age increases. There is a notable drop in yes responses for the 45–49 age group. The number of uncertain responses also shows no discernible pattern, but there is a high level of uncertainty amongst all groups. All age groups other than the 25–29 and 30–34 groups have less than 50% answering yes, when factoring in uncertainty. When uncertainty is not considered, only the 45–49 age group answers yes less than 50% of the time.
By education level, there is an increase in uncertainty level as education level increases. The number of yes responses declined from the some college to the Bachelor’s degree levels, but then it increases up to the doctoral degree level. While only two education groups, the some high school and some college groups, have at least 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is factored in, all groups show at least 50% yes responses when uncertainty is not considered.
By gender, there are slightly more yes responses among female respondents and a nearly identical level of uncertainty between males and females. Both groups have less than 50% yes responses, with uncertainty factored in and greater than 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is not considered.
Figure 17,
Figure 18 and
Figure 19 consider the value of informational label 1 in gauging trustworthiness, with respondents answering the question “would it be useful for judging the trustworthiness of news articles?”.
In terms of age, there is a decline in yes responses from the 25–29 age group, up to the 60–64 age group. Against this trend, there is a downward spike at the 18–24 age group and an upward spike at the 65 and older age group. The uncertainty level shows no discernible pattern. The only four age groups to have at least 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is factored in, are the 18–24, 25–29, 30–34, and 65 and older groups. All age groups have at least 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is not considered.
By education level, there is a decline in yes responses from the high school degree to the bachelor’s degree levels followed by a spike at the master’s degree and doctoral degree levels. Much of the increase in yes responses at the doctoral degree level is not present when uncertainty is factored in. While only three groups (high school, master’s degree, and doctoral degree) have at least 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is factored in, all groups have at least 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is not considered.
By gender, females have an increase in the number of yes responses and are the only group to have at least 50% yes responses, even when uncertainty is considered. Both groups have at least 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is not factored in.
The helpfulness of informational label 2 (shown in
Figure 20), when it pops up in front of a news article automatically, is considered in
Figure 21,
Figure 22 and
Figure 23. Respondents answered the question “would you find this label helpful?”.
In terms of age, there are two waves of decline in yes responses, as age decreases. The first is from the 18–24 age group to the 35–39 age group. This is followed by a spike, and then another decline from the 40–44 to 60–64 age groups, followed by another spike. These waves remain consistent, even with uncertainty considered. While the 55–59 and 60–64 age groups have less than 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is considered, even these two groups have at least 50% yes responses when uncertainty is not factored in.
By education level, there is a gradual decline in yes responses as education level increases. There is a slight increase at the master’s degree level, and a very high level of uncertainty in the some high school group. The doctoral degree holders group reports less than 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is considered. When uncertainty is removed, all groups have at least 50% yes responses.
By gender, females have more yes responses, as well as a higher uncertainty level. Both gender groups have at least 50% yes responses, even when uncertainty is factored in.
Figure 24,
Figure 25 and
Figure 26 consider annoyingness of informational label 2, with respondents answering the question “would you find this label annoying?”.
In terms of age, there are three peaks of yes responses. There is an increase from ages 18–24 to 35–39, followed by a decline to the 45–49 age group. Then, oscillating increases and decreases are present, up to the 65 and older age group. The uncertainty responses show no discernible pattern. Only the 30–34, 35–39 and 60–64 age groups have at least 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is considered. Without uncertainty factored in, the 50–54 age group additionally has at least 50% yes responses. This indicates a low level of annoyingness, amongst most age groups.
When considering education level, there is a spike in perceived annoyingness at the some high school education level. Amongst other education levels, the number of yes responses peaks at the associate’s degree level. Uncertainty levels, similarly, have a valley at the associate’s degree level, with fewer than 5% of associate’s degree respondents reporting uncertainty. When uncertainty is factored in, only the some high school and associate’s degree education levels have at least 50% yes responses. Bachelor’s degree holders also report 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is not considered. This indicates a low level of annoyingness amongst most education groups.
By gender, females have less yes responses, as well as a greater uncertainty level. Both gender groups report less than 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is considered, though male respondents report greater than 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is not factored in. This indicates that males find the label more annoying than females.
Figure 27,
Figure 28 and
Figure 29 consider respondents likelihood to personally use informational label 2, with respondents answering the question “would you review this label when viewing news articles on social media?”.
In terms of age, there are two peaks of yes responses. There is an increase from the 18–24 to 30–34 age groups, followed by a decline to the 35–39 age group. Then, there is an increase, at the 40–44 age group, followed by a general decline. The decline at higher age groups, becomes an increase, when uncertainty is factored in. The 65 and older age group’s level of uncertainty accounts for this shift. All age groups, other than 18–24, answered at least 50% yes, even when uncertainty is factored in. The 18–24 age group remains below 50%, even without uncertainty considered.
By education level, the number of yes responses remains relatively consistent, when uncertainty is not considered. Uncertainty decreases as education level increases, up to the master’s degree level, then it increases sharply at the doctoral degree level. All groups other than the some high school and doctoral degree levels have at least 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is factored in. Without uncertainty, all groups have at least 50% yes responses.
By gender, the levels of support are almost equal, except for a higher level of uncertainty being reported among female respondents. Both groups have at least a 50% level of yes responses, even with uncertainty factored in.
Figure 30,
Figure 31 and
Figure 32 consider respondents’ perception of likelihood of others to use informational label 2, with respondents answering the question “would others review this label when viewing news articles on social media?”.
There is no clear pattern of increasing or decreasing support as age increases. There are spikes in the number of yes responses at the 30–34, 40–44, and 50–54 age groups, which are apparent even with uncertainty factored in. Overall, the level of uncertainty is relatively high. While only three age groups (30–34, 40–44, and 50–54) have at least 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is factored in, all groups have at least 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is not considered.
By education level, responses are relatively consistent, except for a surprising level (100%) of support in the some high school group, when uncertainty is not considered. It should be noted, however, that the some high school group reports approximately 40% uncertainty, so the apparent level of enthusiasm is not as strong, given the higher level of uncertainty surrounding this question. While only the some high school group has at least 50% yes responses, with uncertainty factored in, all education level groups have at least 50% yes responses, without considering uncertainty.
By gender, the levels of support are almost equal, even with uncertainty considered. While both genders have less than 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is considered, both groups have greater than 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is removed.
Figure 33,
Figure 34 and
Figure 35 consider the value of informational label 2 for gauging trustworthiness, with respondents answering the question “would it be useful for judging the trustworthiness of news articles?”.
In terms of age, there are three peaks in yes responses: one is at the 25–29 and 30–34 age groups. A second is at the 45–49 age group. A final peak is at the 65 and older age group. There is a spike in uncertainty for the 35–39 age group and an increase in uncertainty from the 40–44 to 60–64 age groups. The uncertainty level for this question is relatively high. When uncertainty is factored in, only the 18–24, 35–39, 55–59 and 60–64 age groups have less than 50% yes responses. Without uncertainty, all age groups report at least 50% yes responses.
By education level, yes responses are consistent, except for a spike at the master’s degree level, followed by a drop at the doctoral degree level. Uncertainty exhibits two valleys, with one low point at the associate’s degree level and a second at the master’s degree level. Only the some college, associate’s degree, and master’s degree education levels report at least 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is considered. Without uncertainty, all but the doctoral degree group report at least 50% yes responses.
By gender, female respondents have a higher number of yes responses than makes but also a higher level of uncertainty. With uncertainty factored in, only the female respondents have at least 50% yes responses. Without uncertainty, both groups reach this threshold.
The helpfulness of informational label 3 (shown in
Figure 36), when it appears underneath a news article automatically, is considered in
Figure 37,
Figure 38 and
Figure 39. Respondents answered the question “would you find this label helpful?”.
In terms of age, there are two plateaus in yes responses, with a drop at the 35–39 age group. These plateaus remain consistent even when uncertainty is included. All age groups report at least 50% yes responses even when uncertainty is considered.
By education level, there is a spike in support by the some high school group, when uncertainty is not considered. When uncertainty is factored in, this spike is not present, due to a high level of uncertainty at the some high school education group; however, a new spike appears at the master’s degree group, due to their relatively low uncertainty. Even when uncertainty is included, all education groups report at least 50% yes responses.
By gender, female respondents report a significantly higher level of yes responses, while uncertainty is similar for both groups. Both groups have at least 50% yes responses, even when including uncertainty.
Figure 40,
Figure 41 and
Figure 42 consider annoyingness of informational label 3, with respondents answering the question “would you find this label annoying?”.
In terms of age, there is a decline in yes responses from the 18–24 to 30–34 age groups. This is followed by an increase from the 30–34 to 50–54 age groups. Support oscillates over the 55–59, 60–64 and 65 and older age groups. Only four age groups (45–49, 50–54, 60–64, and 65 and older) have at least 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is factored in. When uncertainty is not considered, the 35–39, 40–44, and 55–59 age groups also reach this threshold. This indicates that there is a moderate feeling of annoyance towards the label, across the 35–39 and older age groups.
By education level, there is no clear pattern of increase or decrease as education level increases. The highest level of uncertainty is seen amongst those with some high school education, while the master’s degree education level group reports no uncertainty. When uncertainty is factored in, only the associate’s degree education level group has at least 50% yes responses. Without uncertainty, the high school degree, bachelor’s degree, and doctoral degree groups also report at least 50% yes responses. This shows no clear pattern of increasing or decreasing levels of annoyance, with changing education levels.
By gender, males report a slightly higher level of annoyance, while females report a higher level of uncertainty. Neither group reports at least a 50% yes response level, when uncertainty is considered; however, male respondents meet this threshold when uncertainty is removed.
Figure 43,
Figure 44 and
Figure 45 consider the likelihood of respondents personally using informational label 3, with respondents answering the question “would you review this label when viewing news articles on social media?”.
In terms of age, there is no clear pattern of increase or decrease, as age increases. The most noticeable drops in yes responses occurs with the 18–24, 35–39, and 45–49 age groups. Amongst these, while the 18–24 and 35–39 groups report a high level of uncertainty, the 45–49 age group reports almost no uncertainty. All but the 18–24 and 35–39 age groups report at least 50% yes responses when uncertainty is considered. With uncertainty removed, every age group meets the 50% yes threshold.
By education level, responses are relatively consistent, when uncertainty is not considered except for another surprising 100% yes response from the some high school education level group. Again, the uncertainty level of this education group tends to place it closer to the other groups, while the master’s degree education group spikes, due to a low level of uncertainty. Recalling
Figure 31, which considered the likelihood of others to use informational label 2, an almost identical dynamic of responses was found amongst age groups. However, in this case in
Figure 44, the question is regarding personal use, rather than the usage of others. When uncertainty is considered, only the associate’s degree education level group has fewer than 50% yes responses. With uncertainty eliminated, even this group reaches above the 50% yes response level threshold.
By gender, females report more yes responses than males, as well as a higher level of uncertainty. Both groups report at least 50% yes responses, even when uncertainty is included.
Figure 46,
Figure 47 and
Figure 48 consider respondents’ perception of the likelihood of others to use informational label 3, with respondents answering the question “would others review this label when viewing news articles on social media?”.
In terms of age, when uncertainty is not factored in, there is a gradual decline in yes responses from the 25–29 to 65 and over age groups, with the exception of a spike at the 40–44 age group and a slight recovery at 50–54 age group. The lowest point for yes responses, though, is at the 18–24 age group. When uncertainty is included, the percentage of yes responses shows the same pattern of decline, but with no spike at the 40–44 age group, a steep drop at the 35–39 age group, and some recovery at the 65 and older age group. The uncertainty level for this question is relatively high. When uncertainty is considered, only four age groups (25–29, 30–34, 40–44, and 45–49) report at least a 50% level of yes responses. When uncertainty is removed, all but the 18–24 age group reach the 50% yes threshold.
By education level, there is a consistent decline in yes responses, as education level increases. As one exception, there is a sharper drop at the high school degree age group. Uncertainty levels are mostly consistent across education groups. While only the some high school and some college (no degree) groups report at least 50% “yes” responses, when uncertainty is factored in. All groups reach this threshold, when uncertainty is removed.
By gender, female respondents report yes more frequently than males, while the two groups share approximately the same level of uncertainty. Neither group exceeds 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is included; however, both groups reach this threshold when uncertainty is removed.
Figure 49,
Figure 50 and
Figure 51 consider the value of informational label 3 for gauging articles’ trustworthiness, with respondents answering the question “would it be useful for judging the trustworthiness of news articles?”.
In terms of age, there is no clear pattern of yes responses increasing or decreasing as age increases. There are noticeable drops in yes responses at the 18–24, 35–39 and 50–54 age groups and a spike in uncertainty for the 35–39 age group. Only the 30–34 and 50–54 age groups report fewer than 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is included. Without uncertainty factored in, all age groups report at least 50% yes responses.
By education level, there is a consistent decline in yes responses, as education level increases, except for a slight recovery at the master’s degree level. Uncertainty levels are highest for the some high school group and lowest for the associate’s degree and master’s degree groups. However, they are otherwise relatively consistent. All groups, other than the doctoral degree holders, report at least 50% yes responses, even when uncertainty is considered. With uncertainty removed, even the doctoral degree holders reach the 50% yes threshold.
By gender, there are significantly greater yes responses among female respondents, as well as slightly higher uncertainty, among females. Both groups report at least 50% yes responses, even when uncertainty is considered.
5. Warning Label Related Data and Analysis
In addition to the informational labels, which present salient details in a neutral manner, labels which provide a specific caution or warning statement to viewers were also considered. Respondents’ perspectives regarding these labels are discussed in this section. Again, respondents were asked about the helpfulness, annoyingness, whether they would use the label, whether they thought others would use the label and whether they thought that the label would aid in assessing article trustworthiness. The data from these questions was analyzed in terms of respondents’ age, education level and gender.
The helpfulness of warning label 1 (
Figure 52), when it appears on top of an article that is deemed unsafe, is considered in
Figure 53,
Figure 54 and
Figure 55. Respondents were asked to answer the question “would you find this label helpful?”.
In terms of age, there is a sudden drop in yes responses at the 35–39 age group, followed by a steady increase. Otherwise, responses are generally consistent when uncertainty is not considered. Even when uncertainty is not considered, all age groups report at least 50% yes responses.
By education level, there is a slow decline in yes responses as education level increases. Uncertainty is most pronounced at the lowest and highest education levels. However, even with uncertainty factored in, all age groups report at least 50% yes responses.
By gender, female respondents have both the highest yes response rate and the highest uncertainty level. Both gender groups have at least 50% yes responses, even when uncertainty is included.
Figure 56,
Figure 57 and
Figure 58 consider respondents’ perceptions of the annoyingness of warning label 1, with respondents answering the question “would you find this label annoying?”.
In terms of age, there is a peak in yes responses at the 40–44 age group, with yes responses rising steadily from the 18–24 to 40–44 age groups and then dropping again to the 55–59 age group. There is then a second, smaller peak from the 55–59 age group to the 65 and older age group. Whether uncertainty is included or not, the only age group to exceed 50% yes responses is the 40–44 age group. This indicates an overall low level of annoyance across most age groups.
By education level, yes responses spike slightly for the some high school (no degree) and doctoral degree groups, when uncertainty is not considered, and otherwise remains relatively consistent. Results are consistent, even for these groups, when uncertainty is considered, as there are spikes in uncertainty for both groups. The percentage of respondents answering yes exceeds 50% only for these two groups, and only when uncertainty is not considered.
By gender, male and female yes response levels are nearly equal, with a slightly higher yes response and uncertainty level for females. Both groups remain under 50% yes responses, even without uncertainty included.
Figure 59,
Figure 60 and
Figure 61 consider respondents’ likelihood of personally using warning label 1, with respondents answering the question “would you review this label when viewing news articles on social media?”.
In terms of age, there are peaks at the 25–29, 50–54, and 60–64 age groups. The steepest decline in yes responses occurs between the 30–34 and 35–39 age groups. This drop is even more pronounced, when uncertainty is also included. Only the 40–44 age group has below 50% yes responses, and then only when uncertainty is included.
By education level, there is a small peak at the associate’s degree education level. Due to a higher-than-average level of uncertainty, the some high school (no degree) group drops under 50% yes, when uncertainty is included. Without uncertainty considered, all groups report above 50% yes responses.
By gender, there is an increase in the number of yes responses among female respondents in addition to a higher level of uncertainty. Both groups report greater than 50% yes responses, even when uncertainty is included.
Figure 62,
Figure 63 and
Figure 64 consider respondents’ perspective as to the likelihood of others to use warning label 1, with respondents answering the question “would others review this label when viewing news articles on social media?”.
In terms of age, results appear somewhat consistent, when uncertainty is not considered. There is a plateau from the 45–49 to 65 and older age groups. There is a slow decline from the 25–29 to 40–44 age groups, and a drop at the 18–24 age group. Uncertainty levels are generally high. Once uncertainty is introduced, the results change significantly, with three waves of increase at 18–24 to 30–34, 35–39 to 45–49, and 50–54 to 65 and older. Each of these waves bottoms out either just above or just below 50% yes responses, with only two (35–39 and 50–54) dropping below 50%, with uncertainty included. Without uncertainty, all age groups exceed 50% yes responses.
By education level, there is a decline in the level of yes responses, as education level increases, when uncertainty is not considered. With the consideration of uncertainty, results are instead relatively consistent. The exceptions, in both cases, are spikes at the some college (no degree) and bachelor’s degree education level groups. All education groups meet or exceed 50% yes responses, even when uncertainty is considered.
By gender, females report a higher level of uncertainty and slightly lower level of yes responses; though female yes responses are a higher proportion when uncertainty is not considered. Both groups exceed 50% yes responses, even when uncertainty is included.
Figure 65,
Figure 66 and
Figure 67 consider the value of warning label 1 for gauging article trustworthiness, with respondents answering the question “would it be useful for judging the trustworthiness of news articles?”.
In terms of age, there are three waves of increasing yes responses, when uncertainty is considered, at the 18–24 to 30–34, 35–39 to 45–49, and 40–54 to 65 and older age groups. Similar waves exist, when uncertainty is removed, though there is a spike at the 25–29 age group which is due to a higher level of uncertainty for that group. Only the 35–39 age group has under 50% yes responses, and then only when uncertainty is included.
By education level, the some high school (no degree) group and doctoral degree group show higher than average uncertainty levels. As a result, while there appears to be a gradual decrease in yes responses, when uncertainty is not included, this becomes a gradual increase (not including doctoral degree holders), when uncertainty is introduced. Only doctoral degree holders have less than 50% yes responses, and then only when uncertainty is included.
By gender, females have a higher percentage of yes responses, while both groups have approximately the same level of uncertainty. Both groups report higher than 50% yes responses, even when uncertainty is included.
The helpfulness of warning label 2 (shown in
Figure 68), when it appears underneath a news article that is deemed unsafe, is considered in
Figure 69,
Figure 70 and
Figure 71. Respondents answer the question “would you find this label helpful?”.
In terms of age, results are relatively consistent, except for drops in yes response levels at the 35–39, 45–49, 60–64 and 65 and older age groups. These drops appear even when uncertainty is considered, though only the 45–49 age group drops below 50% yes response levels. When uncertainty is not included, all age groups exceed 50% yes responses.
By education level, there is a gradual decline in yes responses, as education level increases and a graduate decrease in uncertainty from the some high school (no degree) level up to the associate’s degree holders education level. All education levels report a greater than 50% yes response rate, even when uncertainty is included.
By gender, female respondents report higher both a higher number of yes responses and a higher level of uncertainty. Both groups have a greater than 50% yes response rate, even when uncertainty is considered.
Figure 72,
Figure 73 and
Figure 74 consider the annoyingness of warning label 2, with respondents answering the question “would you find this label annoying?”.
In terms of age, there is no apparent general pattern of increase or decrease as age increases. There is a decline from the 18–24 to 35–39 age groups, when uncertainty is not included; however, this decline is less notable, when uncertainty is introduced. There is a spike at the 40–44 age group, a drop at the 50–54 age group, and another spike at the 60–64 age group. All groups report less than 50% yes responses, even when uncertainty is not included, meaning that annoyance is relatively low, across all age groups.
By education level, there is a decline in yes responses from the high school degree educational level up to master’s degree holders, with a drop at the some high school (no degree) level and a spike for doctoral degree holders. Uncertainty is highest for the some high school (no degree) and doctoral degree groups as well. While yes responses remain below 50% for all education groups, when uncertainty is included, doctoral degree holders exceed 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is removed. This indicates a low level of annoyance, across most education levels.
By gender, male respondents report a higher percentage of yes responses and a higher uncertainty level than females. Neither group exceeds 50% yes responses, even when uncertainty is not included, meaning that annoyance is relatively low regardless of gender.
Figure 75,
Figure 76 and
Figure 77 consider likelihood of respondents’ to personally use warning label 2, with respondents answering the question “would you review this label when viewing news articles on social media?”.
In terms of age, there is no apparent general pattern of increase or decrease as age increases. There are sharp drops at the 18–24, 35–39 and 45–49 age groups, even when uncertainty is included; however, no age group drops below 50% yes responses, even considering uncertainty.
By education level, the some high school (no degree) and doctoral degree levels have the highest uncertainty. When uncertainty is not considered, there is a large spike in yes responses for the some high school (no degree) group. Otherwise, the results are relatively consistent across education levels. Yes responses remain at or above 50% for all education levels, even when uncertainty is considered.
By gender, females report a higher percentage of yes responses and a higher uncertainty level than males. Both groups have over 50% yes responses, even when uncertainty is included.
Figure 78,
Figure 79 and
Figure 80 consider respondents’ perception of the likelihood of others to use warning label 2, with respondents answering the question “would others review this label when viewing news articles on social media?”.
In terms of age, there are three peaks in yes responses at the 30–34, 40–44, and 55–59 age groups. These are apparent whether uncertainty is considered or not. The peaks are more gradual, though, when uncertainty is included. There are high levels of uncertainty for the 50–54 and 55–59 age groups, which smooth the curve from the 45–49 to the 65 and older age groups. Uncertainty is relatively high across all age groups. With uncertainty considered, the 35–39, 45–49, 50–54, 60–64 and 65 and older age groups all have below 50% yes responses. When uncertainty is not included, all age groups exceed 50% yes responses, indicating the magnitude of uncertainty present.
By education level, yes responses are relatively consistent, when uncertainty is considered. This is due to an exceptionally high level of uncertainty amongst doctoral degree holders. When uncertainty is omitted, yes responses for doctoral degree holders appear to spike. All education levels other than high school degree and master’s degree have at least a 50% yes response rate, even when uncertainty is included. When uncertainty is omitted, all education levels exceed 50% yes responses.
By gender, female respondents report a higher percentage of yes responses, while male respondents report a higher level of uncertainty. Consequently, male respondents fall below 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is included. Both groups exceed 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is ignored.
Figure 81,
Figure 82 and
Figure 83 consider the value of warning label 2 for gauging article trustworthiness, with respondents answering the question “would it be useful for judging the trustworthiness of news articles?”.
In terms of age, there is no apparent pattern of increase or decrease as age increases. The percentage of yes responses drops for the 35–39 age group, while it spikes at the 25–29, 30–34, and 55–59 age groups. Only the 35–39 age group has below 50% yes responses, and this is only when uncertainty is included. Without uncertainty’s inclusion, all age groups exceed 50% yes responses.
By education level, there is a general decline in uncertainty from the some high school (no degree) level to the master’s degree level, followed by a sharp increase at the doctoral degree level which matches the peak seen at the some high school (no degree) level. Like prior results in this study, it may be inferred that, for most label styles, doctoral degree holders reach a point in their education where they are more likely to question their own beliefs, and that otherwise certainty tends to increase as education level increases. There is a spike in yes responses for the some high school (no degree) group, when uncertainty is not included. Only doctoral degree holders have below 50% yes responses, and then only when uncertainty is included.
By gender, females report a higher percentage of yes responses, while uncertainty levels are similar for both groups. Both groups exceed 50% yes responses, even when uncertainty is included.
The helpfulness of warning label 3 (shown in
Figure 84), when it appears after clicking a link to an article but before the article’s contents are displayed, is considered in
Figure 85,
Figure 86 and
Figure 87. Respondents answer the question would you find this label helpful?
In terms of age, there are three waves of decline: from 18–24 to 25–29, from 30–34 to 45–49, and from 50–54 to 65 and older. There is a slight recovery at the 65 and older group, when uncertainty is considered, due to a very low level of uncertainty for that age group. Only the 45–49 age group has below 50% yes responses, and then only when uncertainty is included.
By education level, yes response levels are relatively consistent, when uncertainty is included, other than a drop for doctoral degree holders. Due to a very high level of uncertainty, the some high school (no degree) education level has an apparent spike in the proportion of yes responses, when uncertainty is not considered. Only doctoral degree holders drop below 50% yes responses, and then only when uncertainty is included.
By gender, female respondents are far more likely to report yes despite similar uncertainty levels for both genders. Both genders report above 50% yes responses, even with uncertainty included.
Figure 88,
Figure 89 and
Figure 90 consider annoyingness of warning label 3, with respondents answering the question “would you find this label annoying?”.
In terms of age, there is a clear curve peaking at the 40–44 age group, whether uncertainty is included or not. Only the 40–44 and 45–49 age groups exceed 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is included. When uncertainty is not included, only these two groups and the 35–39 age group exceed a 50% yes response level. As such, for most age groups the level of annoyance is relatively low.
By education level, there are two peaks at the some college (no degree) and master’s degree levels, whether uncertainty is included or otherwise. Uncertainty peaks at the some high school (no degree) group. All groups have below a 50% yes response level, when uncertainty is included. When uncertainty is not included, only the some college (no degree) group exceeds a 50% proportion of yes responses. This shows that the annoyance level is relatively low, across education levels.
By gender, female and male responses are nearly identical, both in terms of the proportion of yes responses and the level of uncertainty. Both have under 50% yes responses, whether uncertainty is considered or not.
Figure 91,
Figure 92 and
Figure 93 consider likelihood of respondents to personally use warning label 3, with respondents answering the question “would you review this label when viewing news articles on social media?”.
In terms of age, there is no clear pattern of increase or decrease in yes responses, as age increases. Uncertainty tends to increase from the 30–34 to 50–54 age groups. It then declines up to the 65 and older age group. There are spikes in yes responses for the 30–34, 40–44, and 50–54 age groups, with a gradual decline from the 50–54 to 65 and older age groups. Only the 35–39 and 45–49 age groups have below 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is included. When uncertainty is not included, the proportion of yes responses remains over 50% for all age groups.
By education level, there results are relatively consistent, when uncertainty is included except for a decline from the bachelor’s degree to doctoral degree education levels. There is a spike in the proportion of yes responses for the some high school (no degree) group, when this group’s high level of uncertainty is included. Only the doctoral degree holders have below 50% yes responses, and then only when uncertainty is included.
By gender, there are more yes responses among females, while uncertainty remains similar for both groups. Both groups have above 50% yes responses, even when uncertainty is included.
Figure 94,
Figure 95 and
Figure 96 consider respondents’ perception of the likelihood of others to use warning label 3, with respondents answering the question “would others review this label when viewing news articles on social media?”.
In terms of age, there is no clear pattern of consistent increase or decrease in yes responses, as age increases. Uncertainty tends to decrease as age increases, with sharp spikes in uncertainty at the 35–39 and 50–54 age groups. Despite the similarly in uncertainty levels, the 35–39 age group shows a sharp drop in the proportion of yes responses, relative to most age groups, while the 50–54 age group shows a sharp increase. Another sharp drop in yes responses is seen at the 18–24 age group. Uncertainty levels are relatively high across all age groups. When uncertainty is included, only a subset of age groups (25–29, 30–34, 40–44, 50–54, and 60–64) have at least 50% yes responses. When uncertainty is removed, only the 18–25 age group has a proportion of yes responses below 50%.
By education level, there is a slight peak in support at the bachelor’s degree level, when uncertainty is included. Uncertainty is at its highest for the some high school (no degree) and doctoral degree groups. The some high school (no degree) group has an apparent spike in the proportion of yes responses, when uncertainty is omitted. With uncertainty included, only the some college (no degree) and bachelor’s degree education groups have at least 50% yes responses. When uncertainty is removed, all groups exceed a 50% proportion of yes responses.
By gender, there are more yes responses among females and slightly greater uncertainty among males. Males report less than 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is included. Both groups exceed a 50% proportion of yes responses, when uncertainty is removed.
Figure 97,
Figure 98 and
Figure 99 consider the value of warning label 3 for gauging articles’ trustworthiness, with respondents answering the question “would it be useful for judging the trustworthiness of news articles?”.
In terms of age, there are three peaks in yes responses: at the 25–29, 40–44, and 50–54 age groups. The 35–39 age group has a particularly pronounced drop in yes responses, in addition to a higher-than-average level of uncertainty. Only the 35–39 and 45–49 age groups have below 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is included. Only the 35–39 age group has a proportion of yes responses below 50%, when uncertainty is not considered.
By education level, results are relatively consistent, with a drop in yes responses for doctoral degree holders. The some high school (no degree) group has a higher proportion of yes responses, when uncertainty is not considered. The some high school (no degree) and doctoral degree groups have the highest uncertainty levels. With uncertainty considered, only these two groups have below 50% yes responses. Without uncertainty, the proportion of yes responses is at or above 50% for all education levels.
By gender, females have a higher percentage of “yes” responses, while uncertainty levels are similar for both groups. Both groups have a percentage of yes responses at or above 50%, even when uncertainty is included.
6. Supplemental Information Label Related Data and Analysis
Finally, a third type of labels—supplemental information labels—are considered. These labels provide additional details about the content of a page and a link to a location where more details can be obtained from a trusted news source. Again, respondents were asked about the helpfulness, annoyingness, whether they would use the label, whether they thought others would use the label and whether they thought the label would be helpful for assessing articles’ trustworthiness. Respondents’ answers to these questions were analyzed in terms of their age, education level and gender in this section.
The helpfulness of the supplemental information label (shown in
Figure 100), when it is appended to any article, regardless of its accuracy, is considered in
Figure 101,
Figure 102 and
Figure 103. Respondents answered the question “would you find this label helpful?”.
In terms of age, there are two clear curves, with the larger curve peaking at the 30–34 age group and the smaller curve peaking at the 50–54 and 55–59 age groups. Uncertainty levels show no clear pattern, as age increases. It reaches its the highest level at the 40–44 age group and is at the 50–54 age group. The 40–44 and 45–49 age groups have under 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is included. All groups exceed a 50% proportion of yes responses, when uncertainty is not included.
By education level, yes responses remain relatively consistent with a slight spike at the associate’s degree level and a decline at the doctoral degree level. Uncertainty spikes for the some high school (no degree) education level, though all groups have at least a 50% level of yes responses, even when uncertainty is included.
By gender, there is a significantly higher level of yes responses for female responses, as well as a slightly higher level of uncertainty. Both groups have over 50% yes responses, even when considering uncertainty.
Figure 104,
Figure 105 and
Figure 106 consider the annoyingness of the supplemental information label, with respondents answering the question “would you find this label annoying?”.
In terms of age, there is a peak at the 40–44 age group followed by a decline and consistency, at higher age groups. Uncertainty is particularly low for the 30–34 and 50–54 age groups, while uncertainty is highest for the 18–24 and 55–59 age groups. Only the 40–44 age group exceeds 50% yes responses, with uncertainty included. The proportion of yes responses also reaches 50% for the 55–59 age group, when uncertainty is excluded. This demonstrates that the level of annoyance is low across most age groups.
By education level, there is a gradual decline in yes responses, as education level increases. There is a spike at the some college (no degree) group. As with most prior questions, the some high school (no degree) group shows the highest level of uncertainty. Interestingly, the master’s degree group reports 0% uncertainty. No group reaches the threshold of 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is considered. When uncertainty is removed, only the some high school (no degree) and some college (no degree) groups exceed a 50% proportion of yes responses. This indicates a low level of annoyance across most education levels.
By gender, males have a higher percentage of yes responses, while females have a slightly higher level of uncertainty. Neither group exceeds 50% yes responses, even when uncertainty is not included.
Figure 107,
Figure 108 and
Figure 109 consider respondents’ likelihood to personally use the supplemental information label, with respondents answering the question “would you review this label when viewing news articles on social media?”.
In terms of age, there is a clear curve with yes responses peaking at the 30–34 age group. This group also has the lowest level of uncertainty. When uncertainty is considered, four groups fail to reach the 50% threshold for yes responses: 18–24, 45–49, 55–59, and 65 and older. Without uncertainty, only the 65 and older age group falls below a 50% proportion of yes responses.
Responses are relatively flat across education levels, though uncertainty is again highest for the some high school (no degree) group. Three groups (some high school (no degree), bachelor’s degree, and master’s degree) have below a 50% yes response level, when uncertainty is included. When uncertainty is excluded, all educational levels exceed a 50% proportion of yes responses.
By gender, females respond yes more frequently than males, in addition to reporting a higher level of uncertainty. The male respondents have below 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is included. Both groups exceed a 50% proportion of yes responses, when uncertainty is not considered.
Figure 110,
Figure 111 and
Figure 112 consider respondents’ perception of the likelihood of others to use the supplemental information label, with respondents answering the question “would others review this label when viewing news articles on social media?”.
In terms of age, there are spikes at three age groups, when uncertainty is included: the 25–29 and 30–34 age groups (jointly forming one peak), the 40–44 age group, and the 50–54 age group. These same peaks exist when uncertainty is removed, though the 55–59 age group also shows a peak in this case, due to it’s higher-than-average uncertainty. In general, uncertainty across all groups is relatively high for this question. Consequently, while only two groups (25–29 and 30–34) exceed 50% yes responses when considering uncertainty, all groups—other than the 65 and older age group—exceed a 50% proportion of yes responses, when uncertainty is excluded.
Responses are relatively consistent across education levels, with the lowest point at the doctoral degree level. Unlike most other questions, where the some high school (no degree) and doctoral degree education levels are frequently the high points for uncertainty, on this question uncertainty is relatively consistent (but pronounced) across all education levels. Only the some college (no degree) group exceeds 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is included. All groups have at least a 50% proportion of yes responses, when uncertainty is removed.
By gender, females more frequently respond with yes than males, while males report a higher level of uncertainty. Males have below 50% yes responses when uncertainty is included. Both gender groups exceed a 50% proportion of yes responses, when uncertainty is eliminated.
Figure 113,
Figure 114 and
Figure 115 consider the value of the supplemental information label for gauging article trustworthiness, with respondents answering the question “would it be useful for judging the trustworthiness of news articles?”.
In terms of age, the level of yes responses is relatively consistent, except for a spike at the 25–29 and 30–34 age groups. Uncertainty is at its lowest level at the 50–54 age group but seems to vary unpredictably as age increases. Less than half of age groups report at least 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is included. These groups tend to be younger, including the 18–24, 25–29, 30–34 and 40–44 age groups. When uncertainty is eliminated, all age groups other than 65 and older exceed a 50% proportion of “yes” responses.
Responses are relatively consistent across education levels when uncertainty is included. The proportion of yes responses trends downward, as education level increases, when uncertainty is ignored. The highest uncertainty level is exhibited by the some high school (no degree) education level, once again, while associate’s degree holders express the lowest level of uncertainty, in this case. Only two education levels (bachelor’s degree and doctoral degree) have below 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is included. All groups exceed a 50% proportion of yes responses, when uncertainty is not considered.
By gender, females respond far more frequently with yes than do males, while males reported a higher level of uncertainty. Males have less than 50% yes responses, when uncertainty is included. Both groups exceed a 50% proportion of yes responses, when uncertainty is excluded.
7. Broader Analysis and Analysis of Implications
This section discusses trends across the different label types, demographics and questions. Notably, respondents were overall very positive about the use of labels. In most cases, the majority of respondents indicated answers supportive of the use of labels, such finding them helpful, not annoying, indicating that they and others would use them and saying that they would be useful for evaluating articles’ trustworthiness.
Of course, some labels were better received than others. In the informational labels, for example, the third informational label was the best received by the youngest age groups, with approximately 70% of those between 18 and 34 finding the first informational label helpful (not considering those indicating uncertainty), versus an average of approximately 75% for the second informational label and 85% for the third. Notably, different trends existed between these labels as well, for these groups. The first had relative similarity between the three age groups (18–24, 25–29 and 30–34), while the second exhibited a downward trend with age and the third had an increase between the first two age groups, followed by a decline between the second and third labels. Most labels exhibited a drop in support at the 35–39 demographic; however, this was notably less pronounced for warning label 3, which has only a small difference between the 30–34 and 35–39 age groups and continues falling from the 35–39 vale at the 40–49 age levels. The supplemental information label shows a drop at 35–39; however, it continues dropping at 40–44, while—in many other cases, such as warning label 2—the support rebounds in the next age level up.
Table 4 provides an overview of the trends present, by demographic, for all of the label types and questions. Notably, there is not a consistent theme of declining or increasing by age or education level. In some cases, no clear trend is present. In others, conflicting trends are seen for a given metric at different age or education levels. Differences in trend type are also present across the different labels and questions.
Overall, the age-correlated responses show the most variability between responses. The education level data (which, of course, does have an implicit but imperfect correlation with age), shows a more moderate level of fluctuations. The gender-correlated data, on the other hand, shows that there is a limited amount of difference between genders, for most questions, with several label-question combinations having results between males and females which differ between them.
Uncertainty is also measured and, in many cases, decreases—at least partially across the range—with additional age or education. Males and females exhibit different levels of uncertainty across various label and question combinations; however, there is not a consistent pattern to which gender is more or less uncertain that perfectly correlates with specific labels or question types. In general, though, females indicate greater levels of uncertainty (having greater uncertainty reported in 25 out of 35 label question combinations). Females also indicate stronger support for labeling (indicated by greater yes responses for all questions, except annoyingness, and no for annoyingness), responding with support in 28 out 35 label-question combinations.
For all labeling categories, the annoyingness level is either the same for both males and females or higher for males than females. Conversely, the reverse is observed with regard to helpfulness, across all label styles.
There are also gender differences by label style. More males than females indicated that they would use informational label 2, while females indicated this more with respect to all of the other label styles. Females also indicated being more confident than males that others would use each labeling style (including informational label 2). Finally, except for informational label 1, more females than males indicated that each label style would be useful in judging the trustworthiness of a news article.
While some gender-difference is shown in specific label preference, the trend is broader than being related to any single label. This demonstrates that the higher level of support shown by females is likely unrelated to specific elements of the design of particular labels.
The lack of a clear pattern of responses or the presence of conflicting patterns is present for many of the demographic-analyzed individual label question responses. Of the 105 demographic-question-label combinations, just under a third (33) have no clear pattern or evidence of conflicting trends. Slightly more (36) of the combinations have no clear pattern or conflicting trends related to uncertainty. In approximately two-thirds (22) of these, there is a lack of a clear pattern (or conflicting trends) in both the demographic responses and the uncertainty.
Considering the four categories that are associated with label support (all except annoyingness), 24 demographic-question-label combinations have a decreasing association of support with increased age or education level. Four of the annoyingness demographic-question-label combinations show an increase with age/education, a similar indication of support-declining with increasing age or education. Alternately, only six combinations (outside of the annoyingness question, which has three support-increasing decrease response combinations) show a trend of increasing with greater age or education. Only one demographic-question-label combination (informational label 2′s self-use) has only minimal change amongst levels.
This data suggests that the age and education demographics of an online content labeling system user are very important, when choosing the type of label to use, to maximize the efficacy of the system. However, the limited number of overarching trends, which run the entire spectrum of the age or education range, mean that system designers and administrators will need to make nuanced decisions based on specific users’ demographics. The data presented herein, when multiple label types’ absolute values are compared for particular demographic values, can inform these decisions. Of course, these initial heuristic decisions should also be refined based on the behavior of a given user, learned over time, as any given user’s behaviors may not align perfectly with others in the particular demographic group being assessed.
8. Conclusions and Future Work
This paper has analyzed data from a national study of American’s attitudes towards online content labels, in terms of age, education level and gender. It has shown that females are more supportive of labels, generally, than males; however, they also indicate greater confusion regarding their efficacy. Additionally, while females show more support, the difference in support levels between the two genders is—for many labels and considerations—relatively limited. The impact of gender on label efficacy appears to be broader than an association with specific label styles and elements, as females evidence stronger support than males across label styles and survey questions, with a very limited number of exceptions.
In terms of education level and age, it has been shown that the perceived efficacy of labels and support for them generally decreases with age; however, a majority of respondents at all ages and education levels indicated support for the labels (when excluding responses indicating uncertainty). Label annoyingness, was shown to have a positive correlation, for four labels. This perhaps indicates that some respondents found the information to be unneeded for their age and experience. A few labels were shown to have a positive correlation between age/education and support.
As youth have been identified as a key demographic that may benefit from online content labeling, it is beneficial that this study shows that the labels may be particularly useful for this demographic. Furthermore, the study has identified certain labels that may be particularly beneficial for younger users, such as informational label 3. Other age and education levels, though, may be better served with other labels.
It is clear that age and education level have a significant impact on label efficacy; however, the impact is more nuanced than an overarching trend. In some cases, conflicting trends are shown at different points along the age or education level spectrum, which may indicate gaining more (or less) benefit, up until a point, and then having that benefit decline. There may also be generational and lifestyle factors that are responsible for some of the discontinuous changes within the data. There is also a possibility of unknown confounding variables being present. In any case, the data presented and analyzed herein can inform label-selection decision making, based on the demographics of the individual being targeted to use the label.
Building upon this work, needed future work includes conducting observations of respondent’s decision making when using a simulated system to ascertain whether individuals predicted behaviors and their actual ones align, with regard to the topic of this study. A variety of activities are also needed in the broader context of online content labeling. These include the development of new and enhanced technologies to detect intentionally deceptive content, new labels designs to assess the efficacy of and policy analysis to consider how content labeling can be most effectively implemented in real-world environments.