Next Article in Journal
Neutrophil Extracellular Traps and By-Products Play a Key Role in COVID-19: Pathogenesis, Risk Factors, and Therapy
Next Article in Special Issue
Innovative Therapeutic Approaches for Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy
Previous Article in Journal
Timing of Osteoporotic Vertebral Fractures in Lung and Heart Transplantation: A Longitudinal Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
Therapeutic Strategies Targeting DUX4 in FSHD
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Longitudinal Evaluation of Working Memory in Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy

by
Mathula Thangarajh
1,*,
Gary L. Elfring
2 and
Panayiota Trifillis
2 on behalf of the Ataluren Phase b Study Group
1
Department of Neurology, Virginia Commonwealth University, 1101 East Marshall Street, P.O. Box 980599, Richmond, VA 23298, USA
2
PTC Therapeutics Inc., South Plainfield, NJ 07080, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9(9), 2940; https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9092940
Submission received: 26 May 2020 / Revised: 16 July 2020 / Accepted: 2 September 2020 / Published: 11 September 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Innovative Therapies in Neuromuscular Diseases)

Abstract

:
Objective: The developmental maturation of forward and backward digit spans—indices of working memory—in boys with nonsense (nm) Duchenne muscular dystrophy (DMD) (nmDMD) was assessed using prospective, longitudinal data. Methods: Fifty-five boys of the 57 subjects with genetically confirmed nmDMD—who were from the placebo arm of a 48-week-long phase 2b clinical trial—were evaluated. Forward and backward digit spans were obtained every 12 weeks for a total of five assessments in all study subjects. Changes in forward and backward digit spans were evaluated based on age, corticosteroid treatment, and DMD mutation location. Results: Boys with nmDMD had lower mean scores on normalized forward digit span. Normalized forward digit spans were comparable between subjects stratified by age and between corticosteroid-naïve and corticosteroid-treated subjects. When stratified by DMD mutation location, normalized forward digit spans were lower in nmDMD subjects with mutations downstream of DMD exon 30, exon 45, and exon 63, both at baseline evaluation and at follow-up evaluation at 48 weeks. On average, normalized backward digit span scores were stable over 48 weeks in these subjects. Developmental growth modeling showed that subjects with nmDMD mutations upstream of DMD exon 30, upstream of DMD exon 45, and upstream of DMD exon 63 appeared to make better gains in working memory than subjects with mutations downstream of DMD exon 30, downstream of DMD exon 45, and downstream of DMD exon 63. Conclusion: Performance in working memory shows deficits in nmDMD and differed based on nmDMD location. Maturation in cognition was seen over a 48-week period. The developmental trajectory of working memory in this cohort was influenced by DMD mutation location.

1. Introduction

Duchenne muscular dystrophy (DMD) is a multisystem genetic disorder affecting the brain and skeletal and cardiac muscles [1,2]. Clinically, DMD is characterized by progressive skeletal muscle weakness, which results in boys becoming wheelchair bound in their early to mid-teenage years; cardiopulmonary compromise results in death in the third-to-fourth decade of life. DMD is caused by mutations in the dystrophin gene located on the X-chromosome which affect the expression of dystrophin—a large cytoskeletal protein with myriad functional properties—in various tissues [3,4]. Through tissue- and cell-specific promoters, full-length dystrophin (dp427) and shorter dystrophin proteins (dp260, dp140, and dp71) are differentially expressed [5,6]. The unique first exons for the shorter dystrophin proteins are exon 30 for dp260, exon 45 for dp140, and exon 63 for dp71. The full-length dystrophin and shorter dystrophin proteins are expressed in the brain by neurons and astroglial cells [7,8]. In cortical neurons, dystrophin associates with postsynaptic density 95, a large scaffold protein that anchors receptors, ion channels, and other signaling molecules, to influence synaptogenesis [9]. A signaling role for dystrophin has been proposed in glial lineage cells [10,11,12]. Thus, dystrophin in the brain may influence synaptogenesis, synaptic efficiency, and synaptic tuning directly through neurons and indirectly through astroglial cells. In skeletal and cardiac muscles, dystrophin with the dystrophin–glycoprotein complex connects the intracellular actin cytoskeleton to the extracellular matrix and contributes to muscle membrane integrity [13,14,15].
Cognitive abnormalities in DMD begin early [16,17], persists [18], and are comorbid with other neurodevelopmental conditions [19,20,21]. Cotton et al. performed a meta-analysis of 1224 individuals with muscular dystrophy from 32 research studies and showed that age-related intellectual gains were most apparent in verbal intelligence compared to performance intelligence [22]. Some of the research studies included in the meta-analysis were conducted as early as the 1960s, thus limiting the ascertainment of a genetic diagnosis of DMD, as molecular testing of the dystrophin gene (DMD) did not become available until the late 1980s [3]. To date, published studies on cognition during childhood in DMD have been mostly cross-sectional [23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31]. There is a single report of 41 boys with DMD followed longitudinally over a mean of 1.5 years [18]; this study utilized two different cognitive batteries to assess neurodevelopmental outcomes at the two time points (ages 4 and 6 years). Further, previous studies have also used intelligence quotient (IQ) as an indicator of cognitive ability. IQ is not a sensitive indicator of cognition in DMD because even boys with a normal IQ have disproportionate deficit in working memory [32]. Working memory impairment in DMD is well-documented [33,34]. While previous literature suggests that cognition (as measured by IQ) may show age-related improvement, more contemporaneous scientific literature suggests that individuals with DMD may have impairments in working memory skills despite normal IQ [32]. There exists a knowledge gap in our understanding of developmental gains of core cognitive skills such as working memory in DMD. Addressing this knowledge gap will allow for more completeness of cognitive surveillance and rehabilitation in individuals with DMD. Identifying whether the cognitive trajectory of individuals with DMD matches that of their peers is the first critical step to plan for early and effective personalized interventions to mitigate the negative consequences of cognitive impairment on educational and vocational outcomes.
We had previously reported that subjects with nonsense DMD mutations (nmDMD) downstream of DMD exon 45 perform poorly in forward and backward digit spans, indices of working memory [34]. Non-pharmacological cognitive rehabilitation strategies targeted towards the improvement of working memory have been employed in several chronic childhood disorders [35,36,37]. We sought to prospectively evaluate the developmental trajectory of working memory in DMD. Establishing the developmental trajectory of working memory is the first critical step towards the empirical evaluation of cognitive training in DMD, as this understanding allows us to determine that the gains in working memory is due to the intervention and not simply reflective of age-related maturation. We prospectively measured forward and backward digit spans in a cohort of genetically homogenous, well-characterized cohort of 55 subjects with nmDMD enrolled in a 48-week-long phase 2b clinical trial. Our data provide scientific evidence that subjects with nmDMD show developmental gains in working memory and that the developmental trajectory of working memory is influenced by DMD mutation location.

2. Experimental Section

2.1. Methods

2.1.1. Standard Protocol Approvals, Registrations, and Patient Consents

The parent study was approved by the local institutional review board/ethics committee at all participating sites. All participants provided signed written consent. The study conformed to the Declaration of Helsinki (2000) and the Principles of Good Clinical Practice according to the International Conference on Harmonization. The study is registered under ClinicalTrials.Gov (NCT02090959).

2.1.2. Study Design and Subjects

The parent study was an international, multicenter, prospective, phase 2b clinical trial which was conducted between 2004 and 2010. Supplementary Tables S1 and S2 list the Principal Investigators and Clinical Evaluators who were involved in the clinical trial. The study design and methodology has been previously published [38]; the primary outcome of the trial was the change in 6-min walk distance in nmDMD boys treated with placebo versus ataluren, a premature termination codon readthrough molecule. Ambulatory boys inclusive of ages 5–20 years with nmDMD had digit span assessments every 12 weeks for a total of 48 weeks. In this current report, we present data from 55 of the 57 subjects with nmDMD who were in the placebo arm of the phase 2b clinical trial from whom data was available. This secondary data analysis was performed only from subjects in the placebo arm to avoid possible confounding treatment effects of ataluren.

2.1.3. Study Measures

Forward and backward digit spans were evaluated as previously described [34]. Briefly, subjects hear a sequence of numerical digits and are to recall them in the same presented order (forward span) and in the reverse order (backward span). Raw scores were converted to an age-normed score using normative values [39]. Scaled scores have a population mean of 10 and a standard deviation of 3. Scaled scores for digit span are available in three-month increments beginning at age 6 years (i.e., 6 to 6.3 months, 6.4 months to 6.7 months, etc.). In the parent phase 2b clinical trial, subjects were stratified into two groups based on age into subjects less than 9 years of age and into subjects equal or older than 9 years of age. The same age categorization was maintained for the present data analysis.

2.1.4. Study Participant Grouping Based on Location of nmDMD Mutation

All study participants had confirmatory gene sequencing for nmDMD. Based on their individual nmDMD mutation location, subjects were grouped into those with nm upstream or downstream of DMD exon 30, upstream or downstream of DMD exon 45, and upstream or downstream of DMD exon 63, as previously described [34]. Mutations upstream of any stated DMD exon are noted as ≤ and downstream of any stated DMD exon are noted as >.

2.2. Statistical Analyses

A student t-test was used to compare mean normalized scores between nmDMD locations. The change in mean normalized score at baseline and week 48 was performed using mixed model repeated analyses. The correlation between digit span and DMD genotype was analyzed using linear regression. To model developmental growth, within-subject change in normalized forward digit span over time was performed. All analyses were performed with SAS software version 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA). Tests were two-sided, and the level of statistical significance was set at 0.05.

3. Results

3.1. Subject Demographics

The mean age at study enrollment for the 55 subjects (on whom data was available at baseline) was 8 years (range, 5 to 16 years). At study enrollment, 31 subjects were <9 years of age and 24 subjects were ≥9 years. The number of subjects who were corticosteroid-naïve and corticosteroid-treated were 17 and 38, respectively. The normalized forward and backward digit span scores at each of the five assessments over the course of the study are summarized in Table 1 and Table 2, respectively.

3.2. Developmental Change in Working Memory as a Function of Age, Oral Corticosteroid Treatment, and nmDMD Mutation Location

At baseline evaluation, the mean normalized forward digit span was 3.13 (SD 1.57, range, 1 to 7) in subjects <9 years and 2.46 (SD 1.77, range, 1 to 6) in subjects ≥9 years (Figure 1) (n = 55). The mean normalized backward digit span was 1.30 (SD 0.7, range, 1 to 4) in subjects <9 years and 1 (SD 0, range, 1) in subjects ≥9 years. Normalized forward digit spans at baseline and at 48 weeks were comparable between subjects who were on corticosteroid treatment (n = 38), and subjects who were not on corticosteroid (n = 17) (Figure 2). Subjects with nmDMD mutations downstream of DMD exon 30 (n = 29), downstream of DMD exon 45 (n = 17), and downstream of DMD exon 63 (n = 9) scored lower on forward digit span both at baseline and at follow-up at 48 weeks compared to those with mutations upstream of DMD exon 30 (Figure 3a), upstream of DMD exon 45 (Figure 3b), and upstream of DMD exon 63 (Figure 3c). The location of the nmDMD mutations by DMD exon are listed in Table 3. As shown, the number of subjects with nmDMD mutations upstream and downstream of DMD exon 30 were 26 and 29, respectively. When nmDMD mutations were assessed upstream and downstream of DMD exon 45, the number of subjects were 38 and 17, respectively. Mutations downstream of DMD exon 30 and DMD exon 45 will affect the expression of the shorter dystrophin isoforms dp260 and dp140, respectively. Nine subjects had nmDMD downstream of DMD exon 63 that affect the expression of dp71, and 46 subjects had nmDMD upstream of DMD exon 63.
Over the 48-week evaluation, the mean normalized forward and backward digit spans did not differ significantly based on age (p value 0.5) or on nmDMD mutation location (DMD exon 30 (p value 0.6), DMD exon 45 (p value 0.9), and DMD exon 63 (p value 0.5)).
To determine that developmental attenuation in working memory is not due to developmental regression or poor effort during testing, the raw scores of forward digit span were evaluated at baseline and at 48 weeks. Subjects made gains, albeit marginal, in forward digit span over 48 weeks (Figure 4).

3.3. Developmental Growth of Working Memory Based on nmDMD Mutation Location

To model developmental growth, normalized forward digit spans at all five assessments were time-fitted using a linear growth model. This modeling was performed by categorizing nmDMD mutation location into nmDMD mutations upstream and downstream of DMD exon 30, DMD exon 45, and DMD exon 63. Figure 5a shows that, as a cohort, subjects with nmDMD mutation upstream of DMD exon 30 have better normalized forward digit span scores than subjects with mutations downstream of DMD exon 30. Similarly, subjects with nmDMD mutation upstream of DMD exon 45 and upstream of DMD exon 63 scored better on normalized forward digit span scores than subjects with mutations downstream of DMD exon 45 and downstream of DMD exon 63 (Figure 5b,c).

4. Discussion

We had shown earlier that subjects with nmDMD mutations downstream of DMD exon 45 perform poorly in forward and backward digit spans [34]. In this study, we extend our earlier cross-sectional findings and present scientific evidence that developmental gains in working memory in nmDMD are marginal and continue longitudinally but that developmental gains differ based on nmDMD mutation location. Subjects with nmDMD mutations upstream of DMD exon 45 demonstrate better gains in working memory compared to subjects with mutations downstream of DMD exon 45.
We found that, as a whole group, subjects with nmDMD mutations scored two standard deviations below the mean on forward digit span. Only 16 of the subjects in our cohort scored one standard deviation below the mean on forward digit span. This finding is striking. Leaffer et al. evaluated 170 boys with DMD mutations and noted that the mean Z-score was −0.34 (SD 1.1), with range −2.3 to +3.3 (comparable scaled score ranging from 4 to 16) signifying that performance ranges from being deficient to very superior [33]. One possible explanation as to why our study results are different from that of Leaffer et al. is that, in line with the reading-frame rule, nmDMD mutations tend to have severe disease phenotype [40,41]. Also, noteworthy is that Leaffer et al. did not distinguish between different DMD mutation locations, DMD mutation types, or age.
Our data show that subjects with nmDMD have significant deficits in working memory compared to typically developing peers, with older subjects (≥9 years) actually performing comparatively poorer than the younger subjects, though this difference did not reach statistical significance. Typically, developing children between ages 9–10 years can hold 5–6 digits span, reaching an adult capacity at an average of 7 digits span by ages 12–13 years [42]. Could low working memory score, as seen in our cohort, be simply reflective of global cognitive delay in nmDMD? If so, we would expect that the gap between actual performance and age-based accentuation increases with age due to cumulative impact of attenuated developmental progress. A developmental stagnation could plausibly explain why older subjects underperformed compared to the younger group on working memory score. While we cannot exclude with certainty that our cohort had lower intellectual capacity, as a gold-standard neuropsychological assessment was not performed concurrently, a seminal study by Hinton et al. showed that working memory is a cognitive domain that is disproportionately vulnerable in DMD, regardless of intellectual capacity [32].
Converging evidence from developmental cognitive neuroscience and imaging studies show that there is substantial improvement in working memory during infancy, childhood, and adolescence, reaching developmental maturity by late adolescence [43]. Visuospatial working memory maturation parallels progressive myelination of the frontoparietal areas that increases from late childhood through young adulthood. There are distinct neural substrates for working memory in children compared to adults. Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex is activated in both children and adults during working memory tasks, while adolescents and adults (ages 12 and above) recruit the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [44]. Likewise, using functional MRI in a large cohort of children and young adults, ages 8–22 years, Kolskar demonstrated that, with developmental maturation, global effectiveness of connectivity improves with efficiency in transfer of information across global networks [45]. This maturation resembles network connectivity during tasks of executive function in adults. Thus, developmental maturation of cognition is associated with an increasing focused processing network [46]. Against this backdrop, our results suggest that the absence of dystrophin in the brain has negative impacts on the development of frontoparietal networks. While the clinical trial of our study did not include concurrent imaging, Doorenweerd et al. noted that boys with DMD and in particular those with mutations downstream of DMD exon 45 had smaller total and gray matter volume and lower white matter fractional anisotropy and performed worse on psychometric measures [25]. Similar to their study and congruent with our initial observations [34], we found that the longitudinal change in working memory was influenced by the location of the nmDMD mutation. Subjects with nmDMD mutations downstream of DMD exon 30, downstream of DMD exon 45, and downstream of DMD exon 63 showed lower scores in forward digit span both at baseline and at follow-up. Developmental growth modeling showed that subjects with nmDMD mutations upstream of DMD exon 30, upstream of DMD exon 45, and upstream of DMD exon 63 appeared to make better gains in working memory than subjects with mutations downstream of DMD exon 30, downstream of DMD exon 45, and downstream of DMD exon 63. Collectively, these data suggest specific roles for full-length dystrophin and shorter dystrophin proteins in brain development and brain network connectivity. Without overinterpreting our findings, we postulate that the cumulative loss of tissue-specific full-length and shorter dystrophin proteins may account for the lower digit span scores. For example, the absence of dystrophin in neurons and glial cells may alter neuronal-glial communication and may compromise neural connectivity. It can be hypothesized that DMD mutations affecting the expression of dystrophin dp140 can affect the neural circuity serving working memory.
We found that digit span stayed stable or continued to show slight improvement over time in all subjects. An important context while considering neuropsychological performance over time in children is the extent of practice effects. Earlier studies have shown that tests that assess semantic memory and word fluency show more practice effects compared to digit span [47,48]. Likewise, Lezak et al. noted that “tests that have a large speed component require an unfamiliar or infrequently practiced mode of response or have a single solution—particularly if it can be easily conceptualized once it is attained”—and are more likely to show practice effects [49]. Slade et al. evaluated approximately 500 children between ages 8–12 years to study the developmental maturation and practice effects of certain neuropsychological measures, including digit span [50]. These authors noted age-related increases in raw scores in digit span, with minimal susceptibility of digit span to practice effects, a finding reported by multiple research groups [48,51,52]. Thus, our data demonstrating stable or slight improvements in forward digit span over 48 weeks is unlikely to be due to practice effects. Hellebrekers et al. in a recent publication demonstrated a developmental stagnation in verbal span capacity in DMD, especially in boys with mutations downstream of DMD exon 45 [53].
Cognitive rehabilitation strategies have not been systematically evaluated in DMD. Our data suggests that individuals with DMD do make gains in working memory over time, although they differ in their developmental skills compared to typically developing children. This finding is encouraging and suggests that the window of intervention may be longer than previously thought in DMD. Among cognitive skills, working memory is “malleable” and can be improved by cognitive training. Several childhood medical conditions are associated with impairment in working memory including sickle cell disease, epilepsy, attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder, and childhood cancer survivors [35,54,55,56,57,58]. Intervention studies in these cohorts have improvements in working memory following cognitive training. The importance of working memory is that it is critical for learning and for academic success. Working memory capacity and function positively correlate to achievement tests in mathematics, reading, comprehension, speech, and computational skills [59,60]. Academic achievements are significantly lower in boys with DMD compared to their age-matched siblings (and accounting for environmental factors) [61]. Boys with DMD scored 10 points lower than the mean standard score in reading, writing, and mathematics, with mathematics showing the largest overall effect. There is further emerging evidence that executive function, the cognitive domain in which working memory is one of the principal components, determines health maintenance. Better executive functioning allows a mature ability to forecast future self or future consequences in light of immediate and salient competing or behavioral options [62]. Individual differences in working memory have been shown to relate to myopic versus disciplined health behaviors [63,64].
There are several strengths in our study. We conducted serial digit span measurement in a group of large number of subjects, all of whom are genetically homogenous. Additionally, the study evaluated subjects with DMD across a wide developmental epoch (ages 5 to 16 years). These attributes provide a “natural history of forward digit span” in nmDMD and will allow comparison to change following an intervention. There are also some limitations to our study. First, this clinical trial was not designed to evaluate cognition in a comprehensive manner, and therefore, our data is limited only to indices of verbal working memory. A full neuropsychological evaluation was not performed in the clinical trial participants. We acknowledge that comprehensive evaluation of several domains of cognition is pertinent in this population and should not be restricted only to indices of verbal working memory.
Our study is contemporaneous for several reasons. The average life expectancy in DMD now extends to the third-to-fourth decade of life [65,66], and corticosteroids and standard-of-care guidelines addressing the many facets of the disease have improved clinical outcomes in DMD. Adeno-associated-virus-based gene therapy to restore skeletal and cardiac health is currently in phase 2 clinical trial; this new treatment frontier combined with approval of newborn screening for DMD by the Food and Drug Administration Agency offers the opportunity to strategize treatment options for those showing cognitive deficits while planning for ways to prevent developmental problems before they arise [67]. The research intensity in DMD continues to be focused overwhelmingly on skeletal and cardiac health. Recognizing that cognition plays a role in health maintenance and affects long-term health outcomes urges us to reassess research priorities in this population. Lastly, cognitive clinical outcome measures are needed in DMD and will meet some of the unmet needs of this population.
While we continue to make tremendous progress, several fundamental questions remain: (i) what is the time course of developmental increase in cognitive ability in DMD; (2) how does the structure of cognition change with age; and (3) how do changes in different cognitive domains affect each other? Some potential suggestions included the following: First, consensus on the frequency of neuropsychological evaluation across the lifespan is needed. Young boys are more likely to be screened, given the developmental and cognitive concerns that they may present with. However, academic performance in this cohort is poor [61], suggesting that these boys may benefit from additional screening. Second, uniformity in the assessment of cognition is mandatory. Some of the neuropsychological measures used in earlier studies do not offer a continuity of measurement. In a lead study, we show that the NIH Toolbox—a scientifically robust neurobehavioral health measure—is a valid analytic tool for cognitive surveillance in DMD [68]. Last, well-powered natural history studies of cognition through a team-science approach will be extremely beneficial in DMD.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/2077-0383/9/9/2940/s1, Supplementary Table S1. Co-investigators from Study PTC124-GD-007-DMD, Supplementary Table S2. The Clinical Evaluator Training Group for Study PTC124-GD-007-DMD.

Author Contributions

M.T. was involved in data analysis and data interpretation and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. G.L.E. performed data analysis and data visualization and revised the manuscript. P.T. was involved in study design and revised the manuscript. Ataluren Phase 2b Study Group were involved in data collection. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study received no external funding.

Conflicts of Interest

Mathula Thangarajh has no conflict of interest. Gary Elfring and Panayiota Trifillis are employees of PTC Therapeutics, South Plainfield, NJ.

References

  1. Darras, B.T.; Urion, D.K.; Ghosh, P.S. Dystrophinopathies. In GeneReviews; Adam, M.P., Ardinger, H.H., Eds.; University of Washington: Seattle, WA, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  2. Birnkrant, D.J.; Bushby, K.; Bann, C.M.; Apkon, S.D.; Blackwell, A.; Colvin, M.K.; Cripe, L.; Herron, A.R.; Kennedy, A.; Kinnett, K.; et al. DMD Care Considerations Working Group. Diagnosis and management of Duchenne muscular dystrophy, part 3: Primary care, emergency management, psychosocial care, and transitions of care across the lifespan. Lancet Neurol. 2018, 17, 445–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Hoffman, E.P.; Brown, R.H., Jr.; Kunkel, L.M. Dystrophin: The protein product of the duchenne muscular dystrophy locus. Cell 1987, 51, 919–928. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Muntoni, F.; Torelli, S.; Ferlini, A. Dystrophin and mutations: One gene, several proteins, multiple phenotypes. Lancet Neurol. 2003, 2, 731–740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Lidov, H.G.; Selig, S.; Kunkel, L.M. Dp140: A novel 140 kDa CNS transcript from the dystrophin locus. Hum. Mol. Genet. 1995, 4, 329–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Feener, C.A.; Koenig, M.; Kunkel, L.M. Alternative splicing of human dystrophin mRNA generates isoforms at the carboxy terminus. Nature 1989, 338, 509–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Blake, D.J.; Hawkes, R.; Benson, M.A.; Beesley, P.W. Different dystrophin-like complexes are expressed in neurons and glia. J. Cell Biol. 1999, 147, 645–658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Aranmolate, A.; Tse, N.; Colognato, H. Myelination is delayed during postnatal brain development in the mdx mouse model of Duchenne muscular dystrophy. BMC Neurosci. 2017, 18, 63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Kim, T.W.; Wu, K.; Black, I.B. Deficiency of brain synaptic dystrophin in human Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Ann. Neurol. 1995, 38, 446–449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Eyermann, C.; Czaplinski, K.; Colognato, H. Dystroglycan promotes filopodial formation and process branching in differentiating oligodendroglia. J. Neurochem. 2012, 120, 928–947. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Galvin, J.; Eyermann, C.; Colognato, H. Dystroglycan modulates the ability of insulin-like growth factor-1 to promote oligodendrocyte differentiation. J. Neurosci. Res. 2010, 88, 3295–3307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Domingues, H.S.; Portugal, C.C.; Socodato, R.; Relvas, J.B. Oligodendrocyte, Astrocyte, and Microglia Crosstalk in Myelin Development, Damage, and Repair. Front. Cell Dev. Biol. 2016, 4, 71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Ervasti, J.M.; Campbell, K.P. A role for the dystrophin-glycoprotein complex as a transmembrane linker between laminin and actin. J. Cell Biol. 1993, 122, 809–823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Rybakova, I.N.; Patel, J.R.; Ervasti, J.M. The dystrophin complex forms a mechanically strong link between the sarcolemma and costameric actin. J. Cell Biol. 2000, 150, 1209–1214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Ervasti, J.M.; Campbell, K.P. Dystrophin and the membrane skeleton. Curr. Opin. Cell Biol. 1993, 5, 82–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Connolly, A.M.; Florence, J.M.; Cradock, M.M.; Malkus, E.C.; Schierbecker, J.R.; Siener, C.A.; Wulf, C.O.; Anand, P.; Golumbek, P.T.; Zaidman, C.M.; et al. MDA DMD Clinical Research Network. Motor and cognitive assessment of infants and young boys with Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy: Results from the Muscular Dystrophy Association DMD Clinical Research Network. Neuromuscul. Disord. 2013, 23, 529–539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Nichols, B.; Takeda, S.; Yokota, T. Nonmechanical Roles of Dystrophin and Associated Proteins in Exercise, Neuromuscular Junctions, and Brains. Brain Sci. 2015, 5, 275–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Chieffo, D.; Brogna, C.; Berardinelli, A.; D’Angelo, G.; Mallardi, M.; D’Amico, A.; Alfieri, P.; Mercuri, E.; Pane, M. Early Neurodevelopmental Findings Predict School Age Cognitive Abilities in Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy: A Longitudinal Study. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0133214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Ricotti, V.; Mandy, W.P.; Scoto, M.; Pane, M.; Deconinck, N.; Messina, S.; Mercuri, E.; Skuse, D.H.; Muntoni, F. Neurodevelopmental, emotional, and behavioral problems in Duchenne muscular dystrophy in relation to underlying dystrophin gene mutations. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2016, 58, 77–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Wu, J.Y.; Kuban, K.C.; Allred, E.; Shapiro, F.; Darras, B.T. Association of Duchenne muscular dystrophy with autism spectrum disorder. J. Child Neurol. 2005, 20, 790–795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Hendriksen, J.G.; Vles, J.S. Neuropsychiatric disorders in males with duchenne muscular dystrophy: Frequency rate of attention-deficient hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), autism spectrum disorder, and obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Child Neurol. 2008, 23, 477–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Cotton, N.M.; Voudouris, N.J.; Greenwood, K.M. Association between intellectual functioning and age in children and young adults with Duchenne muscular dystrophy: Further results from a meta-analysis. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2005, 47, 257–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Ogasawara, A. Downward shift in IQ in persons with Duchenne muscular dystrophy compared to those with spinal muscular atrophy. Am. J. Ment. Retard. 1989, 93, 544–547. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  24. Billard, C.; Gillet, P.; Signoret, J.L.; Uicaut, E.; Bertrand, P.; Fardeau, M.; Barthez-Carpentier, M.A.; Santini, J.J. Cognitive functions in Duchenne muscular dystrophy: A reappraisal and comparison with spinal muscular atrophy. Neuromuscul. Disord. 1992, 2, 371–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Doorenweerd, N.; Straathof, C.S.; Dumas, E.M.; Spitali, P.; Ginjaar, I.B.; Wokke, B.H.; Schrans, D.G.; van den Bergen, J.C.; van Zwet, E.W.; Webb, A.; et al. Reduced cerebral grey matter and altered white matter in boys with Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Ann. Neurol. 2014, 76, 403–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Felisari, G.; Martinelli Boneschi, F.; Bardoni, A.; Sironi, M.; Comi, G.P.; Robotti, M.; Turconi, A.C.; Lai, M.; Corrao, G.; Bresolin, N. Loss of Dp140 dystrophin isoform and intellectual impairment in Duchenne dystrophy. Neurology 2000, 55, 559–564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Dorman, C.; Hurley, A.D.; D’Avignon, J. Language and learning disorders of older boys with Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 1988, 30, 316–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Wingeier, K.; Giger, E.; Strozzi, S.; Kreis, R.; Joncourt, F.; Conrad, B.; Gallati, S.; Steinlin, M. Neuropsychological impairments and the impact of dystrophin mutations on general cognitive functioning of patients with Duchenne muscular dystrophy. J. Clin. Neurosci. 2011, 18, 90–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Cyrulnik, S.E.; Fee, R.J.; Batchelder, A.; Kiefel, J.; Goldstein, E.; Hinton, V.J. Cognitive and adaptive deficits in young children with Duchenne muscular dystrophy (DMD). J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2008, 14, 853–861. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  30. Hinton, V.J.; De Vivo, D.C.; Nereo, N.E.; Goldstein, E.; Stern, Y. Selective deficits in verbal working memory associated with a known genetic etiology: The neuropsychological profile of Duchenne muscular dystrophy. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2001, 7, 45–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Cyrulnik, S.E.; Fee, R.J.; De Vivo, D.C.; Goldstein, E.; Hinton, V.J. Delayed developmental language milestones in children with Duchenne’s muscular dystrophy. J. Pediatr. 2007, 150, 474–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Hinton, V.J.; De Vivo, D.C.; Nereo, N.E.; Goldstein, E.; Stern, Y. Poor verbal working memory across intellectual level in boys with Duchenne dystrophy. Neurology 2000, 54, 2127–2132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Leaffer, E.B.; Fee, R.J.; Hinton, V.J. Digit Span Performance in Children with Dystrophinopathy: A Verbal Span or Working Memory Contribution? J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2016, 22, 777–784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Thangarajh, M.; Elfring, G.L.; Trifillis, P.; McIntosh, J.; Peltz, S.W.; Ataluren Phase 2b Study Group. The relationship between deficit in digit span and genotype in nonsense mutation Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Neurology 2018, 91, e1215–e1219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Hardy, K.K.; Willard, V.W.; Allen, T.M.; Bonner, M.J. Working memory training in survivors of pediatric cancer: A randomized pilot study. Psychooncology 2013, 22, 1856–1865. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Conklin, H.M.; Ogg, R.J.; Ashford, J.M.; Scoggins, M.A.; Zou, P.; Clark, K.N.; Martin-Elbahesh, K.; Hardy, K.K.; Merchant, T.E.; Jeha, S.; et al. Computerized Cognitive Training for Amelioration of Cognitive Late Effects among Childhood Cancer Survivors: A Randomized Controlled Trial. J. Clin. Oncol. 2015, 33, 3894–3902. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Kerr, E.N.; Blackwell, M.C. Near-transfer effects following working memory intervention (Cogmed) in children with symptomatic epilepsy: An open randomized clinical trial. Epilepsia 2015, 56, 1784–1792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Bushby, K.; Finkel, R.; Wong, B.; Barohn, R.; Campbell, C.; Comi, G.P.; Connolly, A.M.; Day, J.W.; Flanigan, K.M.; Goemans, N.; et al. Ataluren treatment of patients with nonsense mutation dystrophinopathy. Muscle Nerve 2014, 50, 477–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Weschler, D. Weschler Intelligence Scale for Children, 4th ed.; The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Aartsma-Rus, A.; Van Deutekom, J.C.; Fokkema, I.F.; Van Ommen, G.J.; Den Dunnen, J.T. Entries in the Leiden Duchenne muscular dystrophy mutation database: An overview of mutation types and paradoxical cases that confirm the reading-frame rule. Muscle Nerve 2006, 34, 135–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Juan-Mateu, J.; González-Quereda, L.; Rodríguez, M.J.; Verdura, E.; Lázaro, K.; Jou, C.; Nascimento, A.; Jiménez-Mallebrera, C.; Colomer, J.; Monges, S.; et al. Interplay between DMD point mutations and splicing signals in Dystrophinpathy phenotypes. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e59916. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Gardner, R.A. Digits forward and digits backward as two separate tests: Normative data on 1567 school children. J. Clin. Child Psychol. 1981, 10, 131–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Gómez, C.M.; Barriga-Paulino, C.I.; Rodríguez-Martínez, E.I.; Rojas-Benjumea, M.A.; Arjona, A.; Gómez-González, J. The neurophysiology of working memory development: From childhood to adolescence and young adulthood. Rev. Neurosci. 2018, 29, 261–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Crone, E.A.; Wendelken, C.; Donohue, S.; van Leijenhorst, L.; Bunge, S.A. Neurocognitive development of the ability to manipulate information in working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2006, 103, 9315–9320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Kolskår, K.K.; Alnæs, D.; Kaufmann, T.; Richard, G.; Sanders, A.M.; Ulrichsen, K.M.; Moberget, T.; Andreassen, O.A.; Nordvik, J.E.; Westlye, L.T. Key Brain Network Nodes Show Differential Cognitive Relevance and Developmental Trajectories during Childhood and Adolescence. eNeuro 2018, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Durston, S.; Davidson, M.C.; Tottenham, N.; Galvan, A.; Spicer, J.; Fossella, J.A.; Casey, B.J. A shift from diffuse to focal cortical activity with development. Dev. Sci. 2006, 9, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Benedict, R.H.; Zgaljardic, D.J. Practice effects during repeated administrations of memory tests with and without alternate forms. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 1998, 20, 339–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Wilson, B.A.; Watson, P.C.; Baddeley, A.D.; Emslie, H.; Evans, J.J. Improvement or simply practice? The effects of twenty repeated assessments on people with and without brain injury. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2000, 6, 469–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Lezak, M.D.; Howieson, D.B.; Loring, D.W. Neuropsychological Assessment, 3rd ed.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1995; ISBN 978-0195090314. [Google Scholar]
  50. Slade, P.D.; Townes, B.D.; Rosenbaum, G.; Martins, I.P.; Luis, H.; Bernardo, M.; Martin, M.D.; Derouen, T.A. The serial use of child neurocognitive tests: Development versus practice effects. Psychol. Assess. 2008, 20, 361–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Brown, S.J.; Rourke, B.P.; Cicchetti, D.V. Reliability of tests and measures used in the neuropsychological assessment of children. Clin. Neuropsychol. 1989, 3, 353–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Dikmen, S.S.; Heaton, R.K.; Grant, I.; Temkin, N.R. Test-retest reliability and practice effects of expanded Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 1999, 5, 346–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Hellebrekers, D.M.J.; Doorenweerd, N.; Sweere, D.J.J.; van Kuijk, S.M.J.; Aartsma-Rus, A.M.; Klinkenberg, S.; Vles, J.S.H.; Hendriksen, J.G.M. Longitudinal follow-up of verbal span and processing speed in Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Eur. J. Paediatr. Neurol. 2020, 25, 120–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Hardy, S.J.; Hardy, K.K.; Schatz, J.C.; Thompson, A.L.; Meier, E.R. Feasibility of Home-Based Computerized Working Memory Training with Children and Adolescents with Sickle Cell Disease. Pediatr. Blood Cancer 2016, 63, 1578–1585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Conklin, H.M.; Ashford, J.M.; Clark, K.N.; Martin-Elbahesh, K.; Hardy, K.K.; Merchant, T.E.; Ogg, R.J.; Jeha, S.; Huang, L.; Zhang, H. Long-Term Efficacy of Computerized Cognitive Training among Survivors of Childhood Cancer: A Single-Blind Randomized Control Trial. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 2017, 42, 220–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Fuentes, A.; Kerr, E.N. Maintenance effects of working memory intervention (Cogmed) in children with symptomatic epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 2017, 67, 51–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Bigorra, A.; Garolera, M.; Guijarro, S.; Hervás, A. Long-term far-transfer effects of working memory training in children with ADHD: A randomized controlled trial. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2016, 25, 853–867. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Gathercole, S.E.; Pickering, S.J.; Knight, C.; Stegmann, Z. Working memory skills and educational attainment: Evidence from national curriculum assessments at 7 and 14 years of age. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2003, 18, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Alloway, T.P.; Alloway, R.G. Investigating the predictive roles of working memory and IQ in academic attainment. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2010, 106, 20–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Sedek, G.; Krejtz, I.; Rydzewska, K.; Kaczan, R.; Rycielski, P. Three functional aspects of working memory as strong predictors of early school achievements: The review and illustrative evidence. Pol. Psychol. Bull. 2016, 47, 103–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Hinton, V.J.; De Vivo, D.C.; Fee, R.; Goldstein, E.; Stern, Y. Investigation of Poor Academic Achievement in Children with Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy. Learn. Disabil. Res. Pract. 2004, 19, 146–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Allan, J.L.; McMinn, D.; Daly, M. A Bidirectional Relationship between Executive Function and Health Behavior: Evidence, Implications, and Future Directions. Front. Neurosci. 2016, 10, 386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Hinson, J.M.; Jameson, T.L.; Whitney, P. Impulsive decision making and working memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2003, 29, 298–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Bickel, W.K.; Yi, R.; Landes, R.D.; Hill, P.F.; Baxter, C. Remember the future: Working memory training decreases delay discounting among stimulant addicts. Biol. Psychiatry 2011, 69, 260–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  65. Saito, T.; Kawai, M.; Kimura, E.; Ogata, K.; Takahashi, T.; Kobayashi, M.; Takada, H.; Kuru, S.; Mikata, T.; Matsumura, T.; et al. Study of Duchenne muscular dystrophy long-term survivors aged 40 years and older living in specialized institutions in Japan. Neuromuscul. Disord. 2017, 27, 107–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Kieny, P.; Chollet, S.; Delalande, P.; Le Fort, M.; Magot, A.; Pereon, Y.; Perrouin Verbe, B. Evolution of life expectancy of patients with Duchenne muscular dystrophy at AFM Yolaine de Kepper centre between 1981 and 2011. Ann. Phys. Rehabil. Med. 2013, 56, 443–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Baker, M.; Griggs, R.; Bryne, B.; Connolly, A.M.; Finkel, R.; Grajkowska, L.; Haidet-Phillips, A.; Hagerty, L.; Ostrander, R.; Orlando, L.; et al. Maximizing the Benefit of Life-Saving Treatments for Pompe Disease, Spinal Muscular Atrophy, and Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy Through Newborn Screening: Essential Steps. JAMA Neurol. 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Thangarajh, M.; Kaat, A.J.; Bibat, G.; Mansour, J.; Summerton, K.; Gioia, A.; Berger, C.; Hardy, K.K.; Wagner, K.R. The NIH Toolbox for cognitive surveillance in Duchenne muscular dystrophy. Ann. Clin. Transl. Neurol. 2019, 6, 1696–1706. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Developmental maturation of forward digit span over 48 weeks.
Figure 1. Developmental maturation of forward digit span over 48 weeks.
Jcm 09 02940 g001
Figure 2. Forward digit span based on oral corticosteroid treatment.
Figure 2. Forward digit span based on oral corticosteroid treatment.
Jcm 09 02940 g002
Figure 3. (ac) Forward digit span based on nonsense Duchenne muscular dystrophy (nmDMD) mutation location.
Figure 3. (ac) Forward digit span based on nonsense Duchenne muscular dystrophy (nmDMD) mutation location.
Jcm 09 02940 g003
Figure 4. Developmental maturation of working memory in nmDMD.
Figure 4. Developmental maturation of working memory in nmDMD.
Jcm 09 02940 g004
Figure 5. (ac) Developmental modeling of forward digit span based on nonsense DMD (nmDMD) mutation location.
Figure 5. (ac) Developmental modeling of forward digit span based on nonsense DMD (nmDMD) mutation location.
Jcm 09 02940 g005aJcm 09 02940 g005b
Table 1. Summary of normalized forward digit span over time in subjects.
Table 1. Summary of normalized forward digit span over time in subjects.
Time of EvaluationNumber of SubjectsMean, Median (SD)Range
Week 0552.84, 3.00 (1.68)1–7
Week 12523.19, 2.50 (2.47)1–12
Week 24553.16, 3.00 (2.00)1–8
Week 36543.15, 2.50 (2.10)1–10
Week 48523.15, 3.00 (2.21)1–10
Table 2. Summary of normalized backward digit span over time in subjects.
Table 2. Summary of normalized backward digit span over time in subjects.
Time of EvaluationNumber of SubjectsMean, Median (SD)Range
Week 0541.17, 1.00 (0.54)1–4
Week 12521.23, 1.00 (0.73)1–5
Week 24541.22, 1.00 (0.77)1–5
Week 36531.13, 1.00 (0.52)1–4
Week 48521.19, 1.00 (0.66)1–4
Table 3. The location of nmDMD mutation by the DMD exon is listed.
Table 3. The location of nmDMD mutation by the DMD exon is listed.
Location of nmDMD Mutation by DMD Exon
3
4
6
7
7
7
11
12
12
14
14
15
16
18
19
20
21
21
22
23
23
24
24
25
29
29
33
33
33
35
35
38
39
40
40
44
44
44
52
55
55
55
59
60
61
61
65
66
66
68
68
70
70
70
70
70
70

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Thangarajh, M.; Elfring, G.L.; Trifillis, P., on behalf of the Ataluren Phase b Study Group. Longitudinal Evaluation of Working Memory in Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy. J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9, 2940. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9092940

AMA Style

Thangarajh M, Elfring GL, Trifillis P on behalf of the Ataluren Phase b Study Group. Longitudinal Evaluation of Working Memory in Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2020; 9(9):2940. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9092940

Chicago/Turabian Style

Thangarajh, Mathula, Gary L. Elfring, and Panayiota Trifillis on behalf of the Ataluren Phase b Study Group. 2020. "Longitudinal Evaluation of Working Memory in Duchenne Muscular Dystrophy" Journal of Clinical Medicine 9, no. 9: 2940. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9092940

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop