Next Article in Journal
Metabolic Syndrome Prevalence in Students Attending West Virginia University
Next Article in Special Issue
The Effect of Mental Fatigue on Cognitive and Aerobic Performance in Adolescent Active Endurance Athletes: Insights from a Randomized Counterbalanced, Cross-Over Trial
Previous Article in Journal
Incidence Rates and Risk of Hospital Registered Infections among Schizophrenia Patients before and after Onset of Illness: A Population-Based Nationwide Register Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
Applications of Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (fNIRS) Neuroimaging in Exercise–Cognition Science: A Systematic, Methodology-Focused Review
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Experimental Effects of Acute Exercise on Long-Term Emotional Memory

Exercise & Memory Laboratory, Department of Health, Exercise Science and Recreation Management, The University of Mississippi, Oxford, MS 38677, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Clin. Med. 2018, 7(12), 486; https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm7120486
Submission received: 15 November 2018 / Revised: 23 November 2018 / Accepted: 26 November 2018 / Published: 27 November 2018
(This article belongs to the Special Issue The Effects of Exercise on Cognitive Function)

Abstract

:
Emerging work suggests that acute, moderate-intensity aerobic exercise may help to subserve episodic memory of neutral stimuli. Less investigated, however, is whether acute exercise is associated with enhanced memory recognition of emotional stimuli, which was the purpose of this experiment. A parallel-group randomized controlled experiment was employed. Participants (mean age = 20 yr) were randomized into an exercise (n = 17) or control group (n = 17). The exercise group engaged in a 15-min bout of moderate-intensity treadmill walking. Emotional memory recognition was assessed via images from the International Affective Picture System, including assessments of varying degrees of valence and arousal. Memory recognition was assessed at 1 day, 7 days, and 14 days post-memory encoding. We observed a significant main effect for time (F(2) = 104.2, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.77) and a significant main effect for valence–arousal classification (F(4) = 21.39, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.40), but there was no significant time by group interaction (F(2) = 1.09, p = 0.34, η2p = 0.03), classification by group interaction (F(4) = 0.12, p = 0.97, η2p = 0.01), time by classification interaction (F(8) = 1.78, p = 0.08, η2p = 0.05), or time by classification by group interaction (F(8) = 0.78, p = 0.62, η2p = 0.02). In conclusion, emotional memory recognition decreased over the 14-day follow-up period and this rate of memory decay was not altered by acute moderate-intensity exercise engagement. We discuss these findings in the context of exercise intensity and the temporal effects of exercise.

1. Introduction

Memory function is critical for optimal daily functioning [1,2,3]. Of interest to our research group is whether exercise can subserve memory function. Emerging work from our group [4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11,12] and others [13,14,15,16,17] suggests that acute exercise may help to enhance episodic memory function.
Episodic memory, heavily influenced by the hippocampus, refers to the retrospective recall of past information, typically in a spatiotemporal context. Within the episodic memory domain, emotionally-charged memories, heavily influenced by the amygdala, are often more vividly recalled than non-emotional memories [18]. This may be attributed to various underlying mechanisms, ranging from psychological (increased attention, rehearsal, and elaboration) to physiological attributes. Regarding the latter, key neurotransmitters, including norepinephrine, appear to play an integral role in subserving emotional memories [19,20,21,22,23,24]. Of relevance to the present experiment, acute exercise, which can induce an emotionally-charged response, may also help to facilitate emotional memory function via exercise-induced enhancement in psychological attention [25,26,27,28,29] and modulation of norepinephrine [30].
On these physiological and psychological grounds, it is conceivable that acute exercise may help to subserve emotional memory function. The effects of exercise on emotional memory, however, has not been extensively evaluated [31,32,33], which we have detailed elsewhere [34]. Thus, the purpose of the present experiment was to evaluate the short- and long-term effects of acute exercise on emotional memory.

2. Methods

2.1. Study Design

A two-arm, parallel-group (between subject) randomized controlled experimental design was employed. This study was approved by the University of Mississippi’s ethics committee (#17-089) and all participants provided written informed consent prior to any participation. Through a computer-generated algorithm, participants were randomized into one of two groups, including an experimental group or a control group. The experimental group walked at a brisk intensity for 15 min, whereas the control group did not engage in any exercise. Participants completed four laboratory assessments. The first assessment involved a training phase where participants viewed 50 images from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) [35]. Three follow-up assessments occurred for an assessment of long-term emotional recognition memory, including 1-day, 7-day, and 14-day follow-up visits.

2.2. Participants

Participants were recruited utilizing a convenience-based sampling approach at the authors’ University. Female participants were exclusively recruited, as sex differences are observed in emotional memory function [36], and exercise-induced changes in cognition may be influenced by sex [37]. Each group included 17 female participants (n = 34), which aligns with our previous experimental work on this topic, demonstrating adequate statistical power with this sample size [4,5,7]. Participants included undergraduate or graduate students and were between the ages of 18 and 35 years.
Participants were excluded if they:
  • Self-reported as a daily smoker [38,39];
  • Self-reported being pregnant [40];
  • Exercised within 5 h of testing [15];
  • Consumed caffeine within 3 h of testing [41];
  • Had a concussion or head trauma within the past 30 days [42];
  • Took marijuana or other illegal drugs within the past 30 days [43];
  • Had been diagnosed with ADD/ADHD (attention deficit disorder/attention deficit hyperactivity disorder) or a learning disability [44].

2.3. Exercise Protocol

Those randomized to the exercise group walked on a treadmill for 15 min at a self-selected “brisk walk”. The minimum speed was set to 3.0 mph. Participants then increased the speed to a pace they perceived as brisk, meaning a pace they would walk as if they were late for catching the bus. This specific exercise protocol has been previously shown to enhance episodic memory function [5]. After the brisk 15-min walk, participants rested in a seated position for approximately 15-min before commencing the initial memory training task (viewing 50 IAPS images). For this 15-min break, participants in both groups (exercise and control) sat prior to the emotional memory assessment of the present study.

2.4. Memory Assessments

As stated, participants completed a training phase (baseline assessment) and three follow-up assessments for long-term emotional memory evaluation. For the training phase, participants viewed 50 IAPS images. Among these 50 images, 10 were selected from age- and gender-specific normative data [35] to elicit a negative valence-high arousal state; 10 for negative valence-low arousal; 10 for neutral valence-neutral arousal; 10 for positive valence-high arousal; and 10 for positive valence-low arousal. To confirm that our sample had similar responses to these normative valence-arousal data, which has not always been done in previous studies, our sample rated each of these 50 images during the training phase. Valence and arousal ratings were completed using the Self-Assessment Manikin (SAM) scale. For each image, participants completed the SAM happy/unhappy scale to assess valence (range, 9–1; higher score was a more positive valence) and a SAM excited/calm scale to assess arousal (range, 9–1; higher score was a more excited state).
The training phase (baseline visit) involved viewing the 50 IAPS images, with each image displayed on a computer monitor screen for 6 s. After viewing the image, participants then immediately rated the image via the SAM scale. This process continued until all 50 images were viewed.
After this baseline training visit, participants returned for three testing visits, occurring 1 day, 7 days, and 14 days after the baseline training visit. The procedures for the three follow-up visits were identical. For each follow-up visit, the participant returned to the laboratory and viewed 100 IAPS images. These 100 IAPS images included the 50 images from the training visit as well as 50 new (unseen) images that were matched (to the original 50 images) for valence and arousal levels from the five above-mentioned classifications. This matching occurred by using the age- and gender-specific normative data from the IAPS database [35].
During the three follow-up assessments, participants viewed the 100 images (presented in a random order), with each image displayed on a computer monitor screen for 3 s. After each image, participants selected one of three responses, including “remember”, “know”, or “new”. The “remember” response was defined as “The ability to become consciously aware again of some aspect or aspects of what happened or what was experienced at the time the image was presented. In other words, the image brought back to mind a particular association, image, or something more personal from the time of the study, or something about its appearance or position”. The “know” response was acceptable to select if “You recognize that the image was presented in the initial lab visit, but you cannot consciously recollect anything about its actual occurrence or what happened or what was experienced at the time of its occurrence”. Lastly, the “new” response occurred if “You are certain that you did not previously see the image in the initial lab visit”.
For each of the 3 follow-up assessment periods, five memory recognition metrics were calculated, including the summed recognition score for the images viewed during the training phase, summed recognition score for the images not viewed during the training phase, hit rate score, false rate score, and a discrimination index. For the two summed recognition scores, this involved summing the responses from the recognition memory task, with 1, 2, and 3, respectively, referring to "remember", "know", or "new" (range, 10–30; for each of the 5 valence-arousal classifications). Thus, a higher summed recognition score was a greater indication of the image being perceived as “new”. A higher summed recognition score would be expected for the images not viewed during the training session. A hit rate score was calculated as a rate of correctly indicating that they previously viewed the image during the training session (i.e., that they “remembered” or “knew” they saw the image). A false rate score was calculated as a rate of incorrectly indicating that they previously “remembered” or “knew” seeing an image that was not presented during the training phase. Lastly, the discrimination index was calculated as “hit rate-false rate”.

2.5. Additional Measurements

Body mass index was determined at baseline from measured height and weight. Further, heart rate (Polar, F1) was measured throughout the exercise and control protocol. Lastly, habitual engagement in physical activity (min/week) was assessed from the Physical Activity Vital Sign Questionnaire.

2.6. Statistical Analyses

All analyses were computed in SPSS (v. 24). A 2 (group) × 5 (valence-arousal classification) × 3 (time) repeated measures (RM) ANOVA was computed. Statistical significance was established as a nominal alpha of 0.05. Partial eta-squared (η2p) was calculated as a measure of effect size.

3. Results

Table 1 displays the characteristics of the study variables. Participants, on average, were 20 years of age, with the entire sample including females. The sample was similarly represented by non-Hispanic whites and non-Hispanic blacks. For both the exercise and control groups, resting heart rate at the baseline visit was 79 bpm; the heart rate in the exercise condition increased to approximately 130 bpm.
As noted previously, we used previously established, normative age- and gender-specific IAPS images to elicit the desired valence–arousal emotional responses. However, to confirm this normative data in our evaluated sample, participants, during the baseline training assessment, rated each image. These data are shown in Table 2; results are shown separately for the exercise and control groups. Generally, results were similar across both groups, with a slight group difference for the negative valence-high arousal classification. However, for the five valence-arousal classifications, the results for our sample aligned with the normative data. That is, the images appropriately altered valence and arousal levels. As an example, for the exercise group, and for the negative valence-high arousal classification, the mean (SD) valence score was 18.0 (6.1) and the mean (SD) arousal score was 58.9 (23.2). For the positive valence-low arousal classification, the mean valence score was 59.8 (14.4), whereas the mean arousal score was 33.9 (15.9). Indeed, a 2 (valence, arousal) × 5 (classifications) ANOVA demonstrated a significant interaction effect (F(4) = 85.8, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.72).
The recognition memory scores for both groups (exercise and control), for all five valence-arousal classifications, and across all three follow-up periods (1 day, 7 days, 14 days), are shown in Table 3. Recognition memory scores are displayed for five previously-described metrics. The main analyses for memory recognition was the discrimination index. These results are graphically displayed in Figure 1. As shown in Figure 1, with the specific numeric values displayed in Table 3, for all five valence-arousal classifications, the discrimination index decreased across the three follow-up periods. In general, and for the 1-day, 7-day, and 14-day respective time points, the discrimination index went from an approximate 0.7 to 0.5 to 0.4. Notably, the slope in the declining discrimination index was not different between the exercise and control groups. Regarding our 2 (group) × 5 (valence-arousal classification) × 3 (time) RM-ANOVA, there was a significant main effect for time (F(2) = 104.2, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.77) and a significant main effect for classification (F(4) = 21.39, p < 0.001, η2p = 0.40), but there was no significant time by group interaction (F(2) = 1.09, p = 0.34, η2p = 0.03), classification by group interaction (F(4) = 0.12, p = 0.97, η2p = 0.01), time by classification interaction (F(8) = 1.78, p = 0.08, η2p = 0.05), or time by classification by group interaction (F(8) = 0.78, p = 0.62, η2p = 0.02).

4. Discussion

The present study extends previous work that has mostly focused on the effects of exercise on non-emotional episodic memories [6]. Herein, we evaluated whether acute, moderate-intensity aerobic exercise was associated with emotional (and non-emotional) memory recognition. Our results showed that, as expected, emotional memory recognition declined over the 14 day follow-up period. This decline in memory recognition occurred for all valence and arousal stimuli. Additionally, the rate of decline in memory recognition was similar between the exercise and control groups. Thus, we did not observe any beneficial effect of acute, moderate-intensity aerobic exercise on short- or long-term emotional or non-emotional memory recognition.
Albeit limited in investigation, there is some evidence that acute exercise may enhance emotional memory. Using IAPS images, Segal et al. [30] demonstrated that 6 min of high-intensity exercise enhanced short-term emotional memory. Keyan et al. [33] demonstrated that 10 min of higher intensity walking was associated with emotional memory, evaluated up to 48 h post-exercise. Notably, both of these studies implemented the exercise bout after the memory stimuli (i.e., during the early memory consolidation stage). This differs from our study where we implemented the bout of exercise prior to memory encoding. We chose this temporal sequence as our other experimental work on non-emotional episodic memories suggests that acute exercise prior to memory encoding may be optimal when compared to other temporal periods [4,5]. However, the temporal effects of exercise on emotional and non-emotional memory may differ, and, if so, this may explain our null findings. Although speculative, it is conceivable that acute exercise prior to non-emotional episodic memories may be advantageous, as it may help to increase psychological attention and thus enhance the encoding of neutral stimuli. However, this may be less of a concern with emotional stimuli, as the stimuli itself is relatively distinct, which may help facilitate memory encoding. Exercising during the early consolidation period may help to augment levels of key neurotransmitters (e.g., norepinephrine) that are involved in the synaptic consolidation of emotional memories. Further, another possible explanation for our null findings is that we employed a moderate-intensity bout of exercise as opposed to a higher-intensity bout of exercise that was used in previous work on this topic. Although moderate-intensity exercise appears to be optimal for enhancing non-emotional memories [13], it is possible that a higher-intensity bout of exercise during the memory consolidation period is optimal for enhancing emotional memory.
A limitation of this study is the relatively high hit rate across both groups, suggesting that a ceiling effect may have contributed to our null interaction findings between groups. Another limitation of this study includes the relatively small sample size. However, it does align with the sample size of other experiments on this topic [30,31,32,33], particularly between-group experiments with one-time assessments, which is less statistically powerful than our multiple repeat assessment of memory across a two week follow-up period. It is easy to dismiss our null findings by attributing them to the relatively small sample size. However, if this were true, it is likely we would have observed a trend toward significance or a trend toward a point estimate difference between the groups, which was not the case in the present experiment. Our findings point to the suggestion that maybe exercise prior to encoding emotional memories may not enhance emotional memory, and thus, as stated previously, additional work should evaluate the temporal effects of exercise on emotional memory (e.g., exercise occurring during the memory consolidation period). The strengths of this study include the study’s novelty, the comprehensive assessment of emotional and non-emotional memory, the confirmation of the emotional response to the images, and the long-term assessment of emotional memory (up to a 14 day follow-up).
In conclusion, the present experiment does not demonstrate that acute, moderate-intensity walking is associated with enhanced memory recognition of emotional or non-emotional visual stimuli. Additional work on this topic is warranted. Such work should consider evaluating whether there is an intensity-specific effect of exercise on emotional memory, whether exercise temporality influences this paradigm, and whether there is a potential sex-specific effect of exercise on emotional memory.

Author Contributions

B.W. was involved in the conceptualization of the study, data collection, and approval of the final version of the manuscript. P.D.L. was involved in the conceptualization of the study, statistical analyses, and manuscript writing.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Kuiper, J.S.; Oude Voshaar, R.C.; Zuidema, S.U.; Stolk, R.P.; Zuidersma, M.; Smidt, N. The relationship between social functioning and subjective memory complaints in older persons: A population-based longitudinal cohort study. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry. 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Sohrabi, H.R.; Bates, K.A.; Rodrigues, M.; Taddei, K.; Martins, G.; Laws, S.M.; Lautenschlager, N.T.; Dhaliwals, S.S.; Foster, J.K.; Martins, R.N. The relationship between memory complaints, perceived quality of life and mental health in apolipoprotein Eepsilon4 carriers and non-carriers. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2009, 17, 69–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. McCabe, D.P.; Roediger, H.L.; McDaniel, M.A.; Balota, D.A.; Hambrick, D.Z. The relationship between working memory capacity and executive functioning: Evidence for a common executive attention construct. Neuropsychology 2010, 24, 222–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Frith, E.; Sng, E.; Loprinzi, P.D. Randomized controlled trial evaluating the temporal effects of high-intensity exercise on learning, short-term and long-term memory, and prospective memory. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2017, 46, 2557–2564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Sng, E.; Frith, E.; Loprinzi, P.D. Temporal effects of acute walking exercise on learning and memory function. Am. J. Health Promot. 2018, 32, 1518–1525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Loprinzi, P.D.; Frith, E.; Edwards, M.K.; Sng, E.; Ashpole, N. The Effects of exercise on memory function among young to middle-aged adults: Systematic review and recommendations for future research. Am. J. Health Promot. 2018, 32, 691–704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Crush, E.A.; Loprinzi, P.D. Dose-response effects of exercise duration and recovery on cognitive functioning. Percept. Mot. Skills 2017, 124, 1164–1193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Loprinzi, P.D.; Frith, E. A brief primer on the mediational role of BDNF in the exercise-memory link. Clin. Physiol. Funct. Imaging 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Loprinzi, P.D.; Edwards, M.K.; Frith, E. Potential avenues for exercise to activate episodic memory-related pathways: A narrative review. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2017, 46, 2067–2077. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Loprinzi, P.D.; Edwards, M.K. Exercise and implicit memory: A brief systematic review. Psychol. Rep. 2018, 121, 1072–1082. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Loprinzi, P.D.; Kane, C.J. Exercise and cognitive function: A randomized controlled trial examining acute exercise and free-living physical activity and sedentary effects. Mayo Clin. Proc. 2015, 90, 450–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Edwards, M.K.; Loprinzi, P.D. Experimental effects of acute exercise and meditation on parameters of cognitive function. J. Clin. Med. 2018, 7, 125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Roig, M.; Nordbrandt, S.; Geertsen, S.S.; Nielsen, J.B. The effects of cardiovascular exercise on human memory: A review with meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2013, 37, 1645–1666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Roig, M.; Thomas, R.; Mang, C.S.; Snow, N.J.; Ostadan, F.; Boyd, L.A.; Lundbye-Jensen, J. Time-dependent effects of cardiovascular exercise on memory. Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev. 2016, 44, 81–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Labban, J.D.; Etnier, J.L. Effects of acute exercise on long-term memory. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 2011, 82, 712–721. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Etnier, J.L.; Wideman, L.; Labban, J.D.; Piepmeier, A.T.; Pendleton, D.M.; Dvorak, K.K.; Becofsky, K. The effects of acute exercise on memory and brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF). J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2016, 38, 331–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Chang, Y.K.; Labban, J.D.; Gapin, J.I.; Etnier, J.L. The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Brain Res. 2012, 1453, 87–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Reisberg, D.; Heuer, F. Remembering the details of emotional events. In Affect and Accuracy in Recall: Studies of ‘Flashbulb’ Memories; Winograd, E., Neisser, U., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1992; pp. 162–190. [Google Scholar]
  19. McGaugh, J.L. Memory—A century of consolidation. Science 2000, 287, 248–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Hamann, S. Cognitive and neural mechanisms of emotional memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2001, 5, 394–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Ferry, B.; McGaugh, J.L. Clenbuterol administration into the basolateral amygdala post-training enhances retention in an inhibitory avoidance task. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 1999, 72, 8–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Cahill, L.; Prins, B.; Weber, M.; McGaugh, J.L. Beta-adrenergic activation and memory for emotional events. Nature 1994, 371, 702–704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Thomas, M.J.; Moody, T.D.; Makhinson, M.; O’Dell, T.J. Activity-dependent beta-adrenergic modulation of low frequency stimulation induced LTP in the hippocampal CA1 region. Neuron 1996, 17, 475–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Yang, H.W.; Lin, Y.W.; Yen, C.D.; Min, M.Y. Change in bi-directional plasticity at CA1 synapses in hippocampal slices taken from 6-hydroxydopamine-treated rats: The role of endogenous norepinephrine. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2002, 16, 1117–1128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Lanier, W.L. The afferentation theory of cerebral arousal. In Neuroanesthesia; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 1997; pp. 27–38. [Google Scholar]
  26. Lanier, W.L.; Iaizzo, P.A.; Milde, J.H. Cerebral function and muscle afferent activity following intravenous succinylcholine in dogs anesthetized with halothane: The effects of pretreatment with a defasciculating dose of pancuronium. Anesthesiology 1989, 71, 87–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Lanier, W.L.; Iaizzo, P.A.; Milde, J.H.; Sharbrough, F.W. The cerebral and systemic effects of movement in response to a noxious stimulus in lightly anesthetized dogs. Possible modulation of cerebral function by muscle afferents. Anesthesiology 1994, 80, 392–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Magnie, M.N.; Bermon, S.; Martin, F.; Madany-Lounis, M.; Suisse, G.; Muhammad, W.; Dolisi, C. P300, N400, aerobic fitness, and maximal aerobic exercise. Psychophysiology 2000, 37, 369–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Scudder, M.R.; Drollette, E.S.; Pontifex, M.B.; Hillman, C.H. Neuroelectric indices of goal maintenance following a single bout of physical activity. Biol. Psychol. 2012, 89, 528–531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Segal, S.K.; Cotman, C.W.; Cahill, L.F. Exercise-induced noradrenergic activation enhances memory consolidation in both normal aging and patients with amnestic mild cognitive impairment. J. Alzheimer’s Dis. 2012, 32, 1011–1018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Weinberg, L.; Hasni, A.; Shinohara, M.; Duarte, A. A single bout of resistance exercise can enhance episodic memory performance. Acta Psychol. 2014, 153, 13–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  32. Michalak, J.; Rohde, K.; Troje, N.F. How we walk affects what we remember: Gait modifications through biofeedback change negative affective memory bias. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 2015, 46, 121–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Keyan, D.; Bryant, R.A. Brief exercise enhances intrusive memories of traumatic stimuli. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2017, 141, 9–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Loprinzi, P.D.; Frith, E.; Edwards, M.K. Exercise and emotional memory: A systematic review. J. Cogn. Enhanc. 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Lang, P.J.; Bradley, M.M.; Cuthbert, B.N. International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Affective Ratings of Pictures and Instruction Manual; Technical Report A-8; University of Florida: Gainesville, FL, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  36. Canli, T.; Desmond, J.E.; Zhao, Z.; Gabrieli, J.D. Sex differences in the neural basis of emotional memories. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2002, 99, 10789–10794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. Barha, C.K.; Davis, J.C.; Falck, R.S.; Nagamatsu, L.S.; Liu-Ambrose, T. Sex differences in exercise efficacy to improve cognition: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials in older humans. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 2017, 46, 71–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Jubelt, L.E.; Barr, R.S.; Goff, D.C.; Logvinenko, T.; Weiss, A.P.; Evins, A.E. Effects of transdermal nicotine on episodic memory in non-smokers with and without schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology 2008, 199, 89–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  39. Klaming, R.; Annese, J.; Veltman, D.J.; Comijs, H.C. Episodic memory function is affected by lifestyle factors: A 14-year follow-up study in an elderly population. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 2017, 24, 528–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Henry, J.D.; Rendell, P.G. A review of the impact of pregnancy on memory function. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2007, 29, 793–803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Sherman, S.M.; Buckley, T.P.; Baena, E.; Ryan, L. Caffeine enhances memory performance in young adults during their non-optimal time of day. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1764. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Wammes, J.D.; Good, T.J.; Fernandes, M.A. Autobiographical and episodic memory deficits in mild traumatic brain injury. Brain Cogn. 2017, 111, 112–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Hindocha, C.; Freeman, T.P.; Xia, J.X.; Shaban, N.D.C.; Curran, H.V. Acute memory and psychotomimetic effects of cannabis and tobacco both ‘joint’ and individually: A placebo-controlled trial. Psychol. Med. 2017, 47, 2708–2719. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. LeRoy, A.; Jacova, C.; Young, C. Neuropsychological performance patterns of adult ADHD subtypes. J. Atten. Disord. 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Discrimination index (recognition memory) scores across the three time points (1 day, 7 days, 14 days), for both groups (exercise vs. control), and across all five valence classifications (negative valence-high arousal; negative valence-low arousal; neutral valence-neutral arousal; positive valence-high arousal; positive valence-low arousal). Results show that, across all valence classifications, recognition memory decreased over time, and to the same magnitude for both the exercise and control groups.
Figure 1. Discrimination index (recognition memory) scores across the three time points (1 day, 7 days, 14 days), for both groups (exercise vs. control), and across all five valence classifications (negative valence-high arousal; negative valence-low arousal; neutral valence-neutral arousal; positive valence-high arousal; positive valence-low arousal). Results show that, across all valence classifications, recognition memory decreased over time, and to the same magnitude for both the exercise and control groups.
Jcm 07 00486 g001
Table 1. Characteristics of the study variables.
Table 1. Characteristics of the study variables.
VariableExercise (n = 17)Control (n = 17)
Age, mean years20.5 (1.2)20.8 (1.8)
% Female100.0100.0
Race/Ethnicity, %
White52.964.7
Black47.135.3
BMI, mean kg/m227.6 (6.6)26.4 (6.2)
MVPA, mean min/week142.6 (135.6)162.6 (95.4)
On Birth Control Medication, %64.758.8
Heart Rate, mean bpm
Resting79.2 (17.4)78.7 (16.0)
Midpoint129.6 (14.1)83.5 (17.7)
Endpoint127.2 (18.5)83.3 (15.3)
2-Min Post90.6 (19.7)83.1 (15.3)
BMI, body mass index; MVPA, moderate to vigorous physical activity; values in parentheses are SD (standard deviation) estimates.
Table 2. Perceptual assessment of the emotional stimuli during the training session.
Table 2. Perceptual assessment of the emotional stimuli during the training session.
Image Ratings During Training SessionExercise (n = 17)Control (n = 17)
Classification
Negative Valence-High Arousal
Valence18.0 (6.1)24.2 (10.4)
Arousal58.9 (23.2)49.5 (20.7)
Negative Valence-Low Arousal
Valence27.8 (8.4)32.0 (12.9)
Arousal29.4 (15.2)29.0 (13.5)
Neutral Valence-Neutral Arousal
Valence38.7 (8.6)40.1 (11.0)
Arousal39.0 (15.7)33.6 (11.9)
Positive Valence-High Arousal
Valence50.2 (6.9)50.4 (11.2)
Arousal47.9 (12.7)43.1 (13.0)
Positive Valence-Low Arousal
Valence59.8 (14.4)60.5 (12.2)
Arousal33.9 (15.9)29.3 (12.3)
Point estimates are means and values in parentheses are SD estimates. Valence and arousal estimates range from 9 to 1 for each of the 10 items. The above scores are the summed scores for the 10 items for each classification. Thus, for the above items, the range is 10–90, with higher scores indicative of greater valence/arousal. For valence, 9 is a positive valence (happy, pleased, satisfied content, hopeful) whereas a 1 is a low valence (unhappy, annoyed, unsatisfied, despaired, bored). For arousal, 9 is a high degree of arousal (stimulated, excited, frenzied, jittery, wide-awake, aroused) whereas a 1 is a low degree of arousal (relaxed, calm, sluggish, dull, sleepy, unaroused).
Table 3. Recognition emotional memory scores.
Table 3. Recognition emotional memory scores.
AssessmentExercise (n = 17)Control (n = 17)
1-Day Follow-Up
Negative Valence-High Arousal
Summed Recognition Score Viewed Images13.94 (3.1)12.65 (2.4)
Summed Recognition Score Non-Viewed Images27.35 (1.9)27.47 (2.0)
Hit-Rate0.94 (0.08)0.94 (0.06)
False-Rate0.22 (0.17)0.18 (0.14)
Discrimination Index0.71 (0.19)0.75 (0.13)
Negative Valence-Low Arousal
Summed Recognition Score Viewed Images16.65 (3.7)14.41 (3.3)
Summed Recognition Score Non-Viewed Images28.94 (1.5)29.18 (1.1)
Hit-Rate0.92 (0.07)0.95 (0.06)
False-Rate0.10 (0.14)0.06 (0.07)
Discrimination Index0.83 (0.14)0.90 (0.09)
Neutral Valence-Neutral Arousal
Summed Recognition Score Viewed Images14.94 (3.2)13.41 (3.0)
Summed Recognition Score Non-Viewed Images29.35 (1.1)29.88 (0.33)
Hit-Rate0.95 (0.06)0.97 (0.05)
False-Rate0.05 (0.10)0.01 (0.03)
Discrimination Index0.90 (0.13)0.96 (0.06)
Positive Valence-High Arousal
Summed Recognition Score Viewed Images15.29 (3.6)12.88 (2.6)
Summed Recognition Score Non-Viewed Images28.59 (1.5)29.00 (1.0)
Hit-Rate0.92 (0.11)0.97 (0.05)
False-Rate0.13 (0.14)0.08 (0.08)
Discrimination Index0.80 (0.20)0.88 (0.11)
Positive Valence-Low Arousal
Summed Recognition Score Viewed Images16.18 (3.7)15.12 (3.0)
Summed Recognition Score Non-Viewed Images28.65 (1.8)29.35 (1.0)
Hit-Rate0.88 (0.11)0.95 (0.06)
False-Rate0.10 (0.11)0.06 (0.10)
Discrimination Index0.78 (0.17)0.89 (0.13)
7-Day Follow-Up
Negative Valence-High Arousal
Summed Recognition Score Viewed Images14.18 (3.6)13.06 (3.2)
Summed Recognition Score Non-Viewed Images21.94 (4.9)23.24 (6.0)
Hit-Rate0.94 (0.08)0.91 (0.08)
False-Rate0.52 (0.28)0.45 (0.35)
Discrimination Index0.42 (0.25)0.45 (0.30)
Negative Valence-Low Arousal
Summed Recognition Score Viewed Images15.82 (3.9)13.82 (3.4)
Summed Recognition Score Non-Viewed Images23.18 (5.4)25.12 (4.6)
Hit-Rate0.92 (0.09)0.92 (0.111)
False-Rate0.47 (0.34)0.34 (0.32)
Discrimination Index0.45 (0.30)0.58 (0.28)
Neutral Valence-Neutral Arousal
Summed Recognition Score Viewed Images15.06 (3.5)12.47 (2.6)
Summed Recognition Score Non-Viewed Images25.71 (3.8)26.47 (3.3)
Hit-Rate0.97 (0.04)0.97 (0.04)
False-Rate0.31 (0.22)0.25 (0.23)
Discrimination Index0.65 (0.22)0.71 (0.22)
Positive Valence-High Arousal
Summed Recognition Score Viewed Images16.00 (4.0)12.76 (2.8)
Summed Recognition Score Non-Viewed Images24.82 (3.9)24.06 (4.1)
Hit-Rate0.90 (0.10)0.97 (0.04)
False-Rate0.40 (0.28)0.42 (0.26)
Discrimination Index0.50 (0.27)0.54 (0.26)
Positive Valence-Low Arousal
Summed Recognition Score Viewed Images16.71 (4.5)13.76 (2.5)
Summed Recognition Score Non-Viewed Images25.47 (3.7)25.24 (3.6)
Hit-Rate0.88 (0.15)0.93 (0.07)
False-Rate0.32 (0.22)0.36 (0.22)
Discrimination Index0.55 (0.24)0.57 (0.23)
14-Day Follow-Up
Negative Valence-High Arousal
Summed Recognition Score Viewed Images15.06 (3.8)13.59 (4.06)
Summed Recognition Score Non-Viewed Images19.47 (5.7)18.76 (7.1)
Hit-Rate0.89 (0.10)0.89 (0.16)
False-Rate0.66 (0.29)0.65 (0.34)
Discrimination Index0.22 (0.26)0.24 (0.28)
Negative Valence-Low Arousal
Summed Recognition Score Viewed Images15.82 (3.5)14.41 (4.2)
Summed Recognition Score Non-Viewed Images21.29 (5.4)20.29 (6.4)
Hit-Rate0.93 (0.08)0.91 (0.14)
False-Rate0.61 (0.31)0.62 (0.37)
Discrimination Index0.32 (0.26)0.28 (0.30)
Neutral Valence-Neutral Arousal
Summed Recognition Score Viewed Images15.18 (4.1)13.35 (3.4)
Summed Recognition Score Non-Viewed Images22.65 (5.1)21.65 (6.1)
Hit-Rate0.94 (0.07)0.94 (0.07)
False-Rate0.52 (0.31)0.55 (0.35)
Discrimination Index0.41 (0.30)0.38 (0.30)
Positive Valence-High Arousal
Summed Recognition Score Viewed Images15.65 (3.9)13.29 (3.4)
Summed Recognition Score Non-Viewed Images21.47 (5.2)20.53 (6.0)
Hit-Rate0.92 (0.08)0.94 (0.10)
False-Rate0.57 (0.30)0.61 (0.33)
Discrimination Index0.35 (0.28)0.32 (0.30)
Positive Valence-Low Arousal
Summed Recognition Score Viewed Images16.41 (3.9)14.24 (2.7)
Summed Recognition Score Non-Viewed Images22.71 (5.8)22.47 (4.8)
Hit-Rate0.89 (0.12)0.92 (0.09)
False-Rate0.49 (0.34)0.55 (0.31)
Discrimination Index0.40 (0.27)0.37 (0.27)
Point estimates are means and values in parentheses are SD estimates.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Wade, B.; Loprinzi, P.D. The Experimental Effects of Acute Exercise on Long-Term Emotional Memory. J. Clin. Med. 2018, 7, 486. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm7120486

AMA Style

Wade B, Loprinzi PD. The Experimental Effects of Acute Exercise on Long-Term Emotional Memory. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2018; 7(12):486. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm7120486

Chicago/Turabian Style

Wade, Breanna, and Paul D. Loprinzi. 2018. "The Experimental Effects of Acute Exercise on Long-Term Emotional Memory" Journal of Clinical Medicine 7, no. 12: 486. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm7120486

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop