Previous Article in Journal
Phasic REM: Across Night Behavior and Transitions to Wake
Previous Article in Special Issue
Beliefs in Right Hemisphere Syndromes: From Denial to Distortion
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Emotional Status in Relation to Metacognitive Self-Awareness and Level of Functional Disability Following Acquired Brain Injury

1
Department of Human Science, LUMSA University, 00193 Rome, Italy
2
Department of Experimental Psychology, School of Psychology, Complutense University of Madrid, 28223 Madrid, Spain
3
Center for Cognitive and Computational Neuroscience, Complutense University of Madrid, 28015 Madrid, Spain
4
Institute of Knowledge Technology, Complutense University of Madrid, 28223 Madrid, Spain
5
Department of Economic, Psychological, Communication, Educational and Motor Sciences, Niccolò Cusano University, 00166 Rome, Italy
6
Department of Psychology, “Sapienza” University of Rome, 00185 Rome, Italy
7
IRCCS Istituto delle Scienze Neurologiche di Bologna, 40139 Bologna, Italy
8
Independent Researcher, 00100 Rome, Italy
9
IRCCS Fondazione Santa Lucia, 00179 Rome, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2025, 15(8), 841; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15080841 (registering DOI)
Submission received: 19 May 2025 / Revised: 22 July 2025 / Accepted: 31 July 2025 / Published: 6 August 2025
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Anosognosia and the Determinants of Self-Awareness)

Abstract

Background/Objectives: Impairment in self-awareness (ISA) is one of the common consequences of an acquired brain injury (ABI) and is associated with anosodiaphoria. Collectively, these co-occurring neuropsychological disorders pose significant obstacles in the neurorehabilitation of moderate-to-severe ABI patients. Individuals who recover from ISA may present with anxiety and/or depression as adaptive reactions to the ABI, along with related functional disabilities. The present study investigated whether the level of metacognitive self-awareness (SA) is associated with the presence of anxiety and depression, apathy, or anosodiaphoria in patients with moderate-to-severe ABI. It aimed also at investigating the possible relationship between the severity of disability and both psycho-emotional diseases and the presence of PTSD symptoms in patients with high metacognitive SA. Methods: Sixty patients with moderate-to-severe ABI and different levels of metacognitive SA completed a series of questionnaires, which assessed their self-reported metacognitive SA, anosodiaphoria, anxiety and depression, apathy, and PTSD symptoms. Results: Low-metacognitive-SA patients showed lower levels of anxiety and depression and higher anosodiaphoria than high-metacognitive-SA patients. Patients with high metacognitive SA and high levels of disability showed significant higher states of anxiety and PTSD symptoms than patients with high metacognitive SA and low levels of disability. Conclusions: The neurorehabilitation of individuals with moderate to severe ABI should address, in particular, the complex interaction between ISA and anxiety and depression in patients during the rehabilitation process.

1. Introduction

One of the more frequent consequences of a moderate-to-severe acquired brain injury (ABI) is the impairment of self-awareness (SA) [1,2]. Prigatano and Schacter [3] defined SA as the “capacity to perceive the ‘self’ in relatively ‘objective’ terms while maintaining a sense of subjectivity”.
The impairment of self-awareness (ISA) after ABI involves many different functions, e.g., cognitive, motor, social judgment, behavioural, and overall level of functional competency in everyday life [1,4,5,6,7]. This may cause failure to adopt adequate compensatory strategies, and even the implementationof ineffective or dangerous behaviours, which negatively affect the patient’s functional outcome [8,9,10]. ISA after moderate-to-severe ABI may continue after patients have been discharged home [11]; this can remain for more than 5 years [12], or be lifelong [13].
Some theoretical models have attempted to describe SA. Among them, Crosson et al. [14] proposed a pyramidal system. The base of the pyramid is intellectual SA—the patient’s ability to understand that a particular function is impaired, as well as their ability to understand the functional implications of one’s deficits. The second level, emergent SA, is the patient’s ability to recognize a problem when it occurs. The highest level of the Crosson et al. model is anticipatory SA—the patient’s ability to (a) anticipate that a problem will occur as the result of some deficit, (b) realize in advance of their actions that a given deficit will cause a particular problem in the future, and (c) realize beforehand that a given compensation would reduce the chances of a problem occurring [14].
Another model, the Dynamic Comprehensive Model of Awareness (DCMA) [15], is a non-hierarchical model of SA. The DCMA conceptualizes metacognitive awareness—knowledge of task characteristics and knowledge of one’s own capabilities (similar to the concept of intellectual SA in the Crosson et al. model) and online self-monitoring—the patient’s ability to judge their own capabilities and limitations (i.e., during a performance) in relation to the current situation. Patients with good self-monitoring skills are able to detect their errors (similarly to the aforementioned concept of emergent SA), as well as appraise the demands of the current task (similarly to the aforementioned concept of anticipatory SA). Compared to the Crosson et al. [14] model, Toglia and Kirk view the relationship between different aspects of metacognition and SA as a dynamic process rather than as a series of hierarchical levels.
These models, however, do not address the repeated clinical observation that ABI patients often demonstrate some change in their emotional status or mood [16,17]. Although ISA is one of the main challenges faced in neurorehabilitation, it is often underestimated [2,10,18], since, in many cases, the rehabilitation protocols aim at treating the specific deficits in each functional domain (e.g., cognitive, emotional–behavioural, neuromotor, etc.), regardless of the specific patient’s SA level for one or more of such deficits [19].
Different emotional features have been reported in relation to different levels of SA, regardless of the ABI severity. An increase in SA is often associated with an increase in self-reported emotional distress [20]. High SA after ABI has been associated with depression [17,21,22,23,24], anxiety [17,24,25,26], and lower self-esteem [22,24,27].
Bivona et al. [17] also reported that patients with ISA tended to be more apathetic and alexithymic than those with high SA. Importantly, in their study, patients with ISA showed significantly higher scores in self-reported anosodiaphoria [17], a phenomenon first noted by Babinski in 1914 and characterized by affective indifference towards brain injury outcomes [28]. These two interrelated disorders pose major motivational barriers to successful rehabilitation [23,25,29,30]. Indeed, a recent study [18] conducted on patients with severe ABI and healthy controls showed that, compared to both high-SA-patients and healthy controls, low-SA-patients demonstrated not only greater anosodiaphoria, but also a lower level of functioning in everyday life.
In high-SA-patients with moderate-to-severe ABI, the influence of functional disability on the psycho-emotional state has not been thoroughly investigated. Fann et al. [31] and Malec et al. [32] found that higher levels of functional disability were associated with higher levels of anxiety and depression. However, these studies were conducted on patients with varying degrees of ABI severity and assessed functional disability exclusively through subjective measures of post-injury sequelae.
Similarly, the role of functional disability on the subjective experience of post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms requires further investigation. Villalobos and Bivona, [33] documented that PTSD symptoms can occur in patients who suffered and recovered from post-traumatic amnesia (PTA). However, no studies to date have investigated the relationship between PTSD symptoms and the level of functional disability in high-SA-patients.
In the present study, we evaluated whether the level of intellectual/metacognitive (“metacognitive” from now on) SA was associated with different levels of anxiety and depression, and/or anosodiaphoria and apathy. We predicted that, when compared to patients with higher levels of metacognitive SA, low-metacognitive-SA patients would show lower levels of anxiety and depression, and higher levels of apathy or anosodiaphoria.
The study also explored the interaction between level of functional disability and some emotional correlations in patients with high metacognitive SA. We predicted that the higher the level of functional disability, the more present the anxiety and depression. We also predicted that the higher the level of functional disability, the higher the subjective traumatic impact of the event (i.e., the greater the PTSD symptoms).

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

2.1.1. Patients

Sixty-four patients with moderate-to-severe ABI due to different aetiologies [traumatic brain injury (TBI; n = 20); haemorrhage (n = 16); ischemia (n = 23), and cerebral tumour (n = 1)] who had been consecutively admitted to the Post-Coma Unit of Santa Lucia Foundation in Rome (Italy) from June 2022 to October 2024.
After enrolment, we excluded four patients from the study for the following reasons: lack of informal caregiver (“caregiver” from now on) availability (n = 2), and patient fatigue (n = 2). Thus, the final sample consisted of 60 patients (40 males and 20 females), with a mean age of 42.62 years (SD = 15.89) (ranging from 18 to 65 years), a mean educational level of 13.1 years (SD = 2.89) and a mean chronicity of 169 days (SD = 163.4). In total, 41 were inpatients and the remaining 19 were undergoing neurorehabilitation treatment in a day hospital (DH) program.
Participants were recruited according to the following inclusion criteria: (1) age ≥ 18 years; (2) diagnosis of moderate-to-severe ABI [34] score between 9 and 12 (moderate ABI) or ≤8 (severe ABI) in the acute phase]; (3) Levels of Cognitive Functioning (LCF) scale [35] score ≥ 7; (4) PTA resolution, when present in the post-acute phase; (5) distance from the ABI of at least one month; (6) ability to undergo formal psychometric evaluation despite cognitive and sensory–motor deficits; (7) availability of informed consent.
Exclusion criteria for patients were as follows: (1) a history of drug and alcohol addiction; (2) psychiatric diseases; (3) severe aphasia; (4) a history of neurological disorders; (5) recent psychological traumas (e.g., bereavement).

2.1.2. Caregivers

To evaluate patients’ level of metacognitive SA, according to the discrepancy between their report and that of the caregivers on the Patient Competency Rating Scale -Neurorehabilitation Form (PCRS-NR) [36], 60 first-degree relatives (aged ≥ 18 years) were enrolled: 22 (36.7%) were parents of the patients, 23 (38.3%) were partners, 9 (15%) were children, and 6 (10%) were siblings. All relatives were caregivers of their loved one (i.e., they continuously assisted them in the hospital or at home when they were treated in DH), and accordingly they knew the patient very well. All caregivers gave their informed consent to participate in the study.
The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Santa Lucia Foundation (Project identification code: CE/Prog.880; approval date: 28 November 2022). All participants signed a consent form for participation.

2.2. Procedure

After the enrolment, each participant was informed regarding all the characteristics of the study and, accordingly, informed consent was obtained by participants.
To administer the PCRS-NR, relative form, a psychologist met each care giver once, in a quiet room. Another psychologist met each patient once or twice, according to their degree of fatigue, to administer the PCRS-NR, patient form, and other questionnaires investigating the levels of anxiety, depression, apathy, and PTSD symptoms.
The level of metacognitive SA was determined by the discrepancy between the PCRS-NR relative form and patient form (PCRS-NRDS). A cut-off = 5 [18,37] was established to divide the sample into two groups: 43 high-metacognitive-SA patients (i.e., patients with a PCRS-NRDS under the cut-off) and 17 with low metacognitive SA (i.e., patients with a PCRS-NRDS over the cut-off) (Group 1).
High-metacognitive-SA patients were further divided into two subgroups according to their level of functional disability, based on the Disability Rating Scale (DRS) score [38] (see below for the description of the scale): Group 2, composed of 15 patients with a DRS score ranging from 7 to 21 (moderately severe-to-severe disability), and Group 3, composed of 28 patients with DRS score ranging from 0 to 6 (none-to-moderate disability).
The final groups, based on the PCRS-NR and DRS scores, were as follows:
-
Group 1: low-metacognitive-SA patients (N = 17);
-
Group 2: high-metacognitive-SA patients and moderately severe-to-severe disability (N = 15);
-
Group 3: high-metacognitive-SA patients and none-to-moderate disability (N = 28).

2.3. Measures

2.3.1. Functional Assessment

To assess the level of functional disability, we adopted the two most common scales used in patients with ABI: the LCF and the DRS.
The LCF assesses the patients’ cognitive–behavioural profile, ranging from 1 (No Response) to 8 (“Purposeful/Appropriate Response”). Importantly, an LCF score ≥ 7 (index of “Automatic/Appropriate Response”) has been used as one of the inclusion criteria.
The DRS was used to describe the patients’ level of functional disability. It evaluates disability at three levels: Impairment, captured by the first three items (“Eye Opening,” “Communication Ability” and “Motor Response”) is a slight modification of the GCS. Disability is captured by questions evaluating cognitive ability for “Feeding,” “Toileting” and “Grooming”. The final level, Handicap, captures “Level of Functioning”, using a modified measure developed by [39], and “Employability” [38].
DRS total scores allow for patient classification into the following disability categories: 0 = None; 1 = Mild; 2–3 = Partial; 4–6 = Moderate; 7–11 = Moderately Severe; 12–16 = Severe; 17–21= Extremely Severe; 22–24 = Vegetative State; 25–29 = Extreme Vegetative State. Higher DRS scores correspond to higher levels of disability [38].

2.3.2. Self-Awareness Assessment

The Patient Competency Rating Scale (PCRS) was used to assess the level of metacognitive SA [19] because of its psychometric properties and feasibility [40,41]. The PCRS was translated and validated in Italian in the past by Ciurli et al. (2010) [42]. The PCRS is a 30-item self-report questionnaire that requires patients and their relatives to make an independent judgment of perceived degree of competency demonstrated in several behavioural, cognitive and emotional situations using a 5-point Likert scale (ranging from 1, “Can’t do”, to 5, “Can do with ease”). Total PCRS scores range from 30 to 150, with higher scores indicating higher levels of perceived competency. Thus, the level of patients’ metacognitive SA (i.e., the PCRS discrepancy score—PCRSDS) is obtained by subtracting the patient’s PCRS total score from the relative’s PCRS total score [17,43].
To compare inpatients and DH patients on the same abilities, we administered the PCRS-NR, a briefer version of the PCRS used in inpatient neurorehabilitation unit [36]. The PCRS-NR excludes all items regarding patients’ competencies in activities of daily living outside the hospital. Thus, the PCRS-NR assesses metacognitive SA across three domains: cognitive (3 items), interpersonal (5 items) and emotional (5 items). Total PCRS-NR scores can range from 13 to 65.
In line with previous findings [18,37], we adopted a PCRS-NRDS cut-off score of 5 points (scores ≥ 5 indicating low metacognitive SA) to differentiate between high-metacognitive SA and low-metacognitive-SA patients.

2.3.3. Psychological and Neuropsychiatric Assessment

Anosodiaphoria
To assess both ISA and anosodiaphoria with the same measurement scale over time, Bivona et al. [18] created an extended Italian version of the PCRS-NR, that is, the “Anosodiaphoria Extension of the PCRS-NR” (AE-PCRS-NR). Anosodiaphoria is related to the patients’ subjective emotional state regarding the level of their competencies (impaired or reduced competencies).
For each of the PCRS items, patients were also asked to complete the anosodiaphoria section, which explores their level of concern about their perceived level of performance on a Likert scale ranging from 5 (“I am not disturbed/worried at all”) to 1 (“I am very disturbed/worried”). For example, after completing PCRS-NR “item 1” (“How much of a problem do I have in preparing my own meals?”), patients were also asked to answer the question (“How much am I disturbed/worried about my ability to prepare my own meals?”). The total AE-PCRS-NR score can range from 13 to 65, with higher scores indicating higher levels of anosodiaphoria [18].
To obtain an objective measure of anosodiaphoria concerning the actual level of patients’ difficulties, Bivona et al. [18] computed an Anosodiaphoria Index (AE-PCRS Index) by dividing the patients’ AE-PCRS-NR total score by the relatives’ PCRS-NR total score, considering the latter as an external/“objective” measure of patients’ difficulties. An AE-PCRS-NR Index > 1 indicates the presence of anosodiaphoria, with a maximum AE-Index score of 5 obtained in the case where a patient reports the maximum score on the AE-PCRS-NR (i.e., =65, representing total lack of concern) and their relatives report a minimum total score on the PCRS-NR (i.e., =13, representing the highest level of functional difficulties).
Apathy
To assess apathy, we administered the Italian version of the Apathy Evaluation Scale (AES) [44]. The AES [45] is an 18-item scale that assesses the behavioural, emotional, and cognitive dimensions of apathy. Each item is rated on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (“not at all true”) to 4 (“very true”). This scale can be administered in a self-report version (AES-S), by relatives or caregivers (AES-I), or by a clinician following a semi-structured interview with the patients (AES-C). In the present study, we used the AES-C version (“AES” from now on). Lane-Brown and Tate, (2009), using a sample of patients with TBI, suggested that an AES score ≥ 37 indicates the presence of apathy [46].
Anxiety
We administered the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI, X1-X2) [47] to assess self-reported anxiety. The STAI is a self-report scale that measures two distinct components of anxiety: the current state of anxiety (STAI-X1), and anxiety as a trait (STAI-X2). STAI-X1 consists of 20 descriptive statements regarding how the participant feels currently, including the moment of the interview. STAI-X2 consists of 20 descriptive statements regarding how the participant usually feels. For both sub-scales, the total score can range from 20 (very low anxiety) to 80 (very high anxiety). A score = 40 for each subscale is considered the cut-off for a pathological level of anxiety.
Depression
The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) [48] includes 21 items that assess self-reported cognitive, behavioural, affective, and somatic components of depression. Higher scores represent higher levels of reported depression. The BDI is one of the most widely used measures of depression. It has been shown to effectively differentiate between non-depressed, moderately depressed, and severely depressed adults [48] and it demonstrates good psychometric properties [49,50]. The standard cut-off scores proposed by Beck et al. [51] are as follows: 0–9 indicates minimal depression; 10–16 indicates mild depression; 17–29 indicates moderate depression; 30–63 indicates severe depression.
Post-Traumatic Stress Disorders Symptoms
To evaluate the traumatic impact of the neurological event qualifying patients for this study, based on DSM-5 criteria for PTSD, we administered the Impact of the Event Scale-revised (IES-R) [52]. The IES-R is a self-report measure of current subjective distress, that assesses the intensity of emotional and cognitive reactions in response to a specific traumatic event. It comprises three subscales representative of the major symptom clusters of PTSD: intrusion, avoidance, and hyper-arousal. A score > 24 is an index of the presence of PTSD symptoms.

2.4. Statistics

The different sample size of each patient group, visual inspection of histograms, and the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test of normality for emotional scores revealed the groups were not normally distributed (p < 0.01). Accordingly, to investigate possible between-groups differences on psychometric scales, first the Kruskal–Wallis H test was used to compare the three patient groups for each psycho-emotional score. When a significant effect was found, pairwise comparisons were performed using Mann–Whitney U tests.
Effect sizes (Rosenthal’s r) were calculated for the pairwise comparisons. We considered Cohen’s classification of the effect sizes as small (r = 0.1), medium (r = 0.3), and large (r ≥ 0.5) [53]. Finally, the sociodemographic variables and the psycho-emotional scores were submitted to bivariate correlation analysis. Spearman’s rho coefficients were computed.

3. Results

Table 1 shows the means, standard deviations, medians, and minimum and maximum values for each sociodemographic and emotional variable in the three groups.

3.1. Sociodemographic Variables and Chronicity

A Kruskal–Wallis H test was conducted to examine group differences in each sociodemographic variable among the three patient groups. Statistically significant differences emerged for age [χ2(2) = 6.544, p = 0.038] and education [χ2(2) = 7.935, p = 0.019]. As for chronicity, no significant difference [χ2(2) = 0.137, p = 0.934] was found.
As shown in Table 2, patients in Group 1 were significantly older than those in Groups 2 (Z = −2.413, p < 0.01; medium effect size) and 3 (Z = −1.776, p < 0.05; medium effect size).
In terms of education level, Group 1 and 2 were significantly different (Z = −2.456, p < 0.01; medium effect size) as were Group 2 and 3 (Z = −2.092, p < 0.05; medium effect size).

3.2. Differences Between-Groups on Psycho-Emotional Variables

Kruskal–Wallis H test was conducted to examine group differences in each variable among the three patient groups. As for apathy, no significant differences were found [χ2(2) = 2.081, p = 0.353].
Conversely, statistically significant differences emerged for anosodiaphoria [χ2(2) = 25.986, p < 0.001], state anxiety [χ2(2) = 9.444, p = 0.009] and trait anxiety [χ2(2) = 9.006, p = 0.011] (STAI-X1 and STAI-X2, respectively), depression [χ2(2) = 7.324, p = 0.026], and PTSD symptoms [χ2(2) = 12.176, p = 0.002].
Consecutively, when a significant effect was found, pairwise comparisons were performed using Mann–Whitney U tests. As shown in Table 3, for all relationships other than those involving AES (i.e., apathy), results from the Mann–Whitney U test showed significant differences in the psycho-emotional scales between Groups 1 (i.e., patients with low metacognitive SA) and 3 (i.e., patients with none-to-moderate disability), and between Groups 1 and 2 (i.e., patients with moderately severe-to-severe disability).
In particular, Group 1 exhibited higher anosodiaphoria (Z = −4.778, p < 0.001; large effect size), and lower state (Z = −1.653, p < 0.05; small effect size) and trait anxiety (Z = −2.636, p < 0.01; medium effect size), as well as lower depression scores (Z = −1.690, p < 0.05; small effect size) than Group 3.
The same pattern emerged from the comparison between Groups 1 and 2: patients in Group 1 showed significantly higher anosodiaphoria (Z = −3.929, p < 0.001), and lower state (Z = −2.798, p < 0.01) and trait (Z = −2.760, p < 0.01) anxiety and depression scores (Z = −2.764, p < 0.01) than those in Group 2. The effect size was almost large in all cases.
Finally, the comparison between Groups 2 and 3 showed significant differences only in the state anxiety STAI-X1 (Z = −1.952, p < 0.05) and in the IES-R scores (Z = −2.287, p < 0.05), indicating higher state anxiety and more PTSD symptoms, respectively, in Group 2. In both cases, the effect size was medium.

3.3. Correlation Analysis Result

The correlational analysis performed to exclude any possible influence of sociodemographic variables on the differences between groups found on the psycho-emotional features between groups showed that age correlated positively only with the PCRS-NRDS (ρ = 0.42; p < 0.001)—specifically, the higher the age, the lower the metacognitive SA. No correlations emerged between any sociodemographic variables and depression, state and trait anxiety, anosodiaphoria, apathy, and PTSD symptoms (see Table 4).

4. Discussion

The present study investigated the relationship between level of impaired self-awareness and emotional states in patients with moderate-to-severe ABI.
We also investigated the association between the level of functional disability and anxiety and depression and PTSD symptoms in high-metacognitive-SA patients. To achieve this, participants were divided into low metacognitive SA or high metacognitive SA based on a PCRS-NRDS cut-off consistent with prior literature [18,37]. We further divided participants with high metacognitive SA according to their level of functional disability, as classified by the DRS, into patients with none-to-moderate disability and moderately severe-to-severe disability. We named these three sub-groups Group 1, Group 2, and Group 3, respectively.
In line with previous studies, high-metacognitive-SA patients showed higher STAI scores—indicating higher level of anxiety [17,24,25,26], and higher BDI scores—indicating higher levels of depressive symptoms [17,21,22,23,24,32,54]. We predicted these findings believing that only patients who are aware of one or more functional (e.g., cognitive, physical, etc.) difficulties would present with anxiety and depression as congruent reactions to the event. Indeed, as stated by Prigatano et al. [55], these disorders should be considered in the patients’ adaptive reactions to the injury that, if adequately treated, can lead to better adjustment to its consequences over time, and decreased frustration, confusion, and distress provoked by the ABI. Additionally, low-metacognitive-SA patients showed significant higher anosodiaphoria scores than those in Groups 2 and 3 (i.e., high-metacognitive-SA patients). Finally, no differences in terms of apathy scores emerged between the three groups.
The presence of significantly higher anosodiaphoria (but not of apathy) scores in low-metacognitive-SA patients, compared to high-metacognitive-SA patients, aligns with the results of a recent studies with similar findings [18]. This may suggest that, although similar to each other, apathy and anosodiaphoria may be related to overlapping (e.g., the insula), but also different brain regions [54,56]. We predicted the association between low-metacognitive SA and anosodiaphoria since observing emotional concern in patients who are not self-aware of their difficulty can be considered nonsense.
Of note, some patients with good metacognitive SA also presented with anosodiaphoria (namely, some AE-PCRS-NR Index scores in the high-metacognitive-SA patients groups were above the cut-off). Indeed, as shown in Table 1, the maximum scores in Groups 2 and 3 were 1.24 and 1.39, respectively. This finding aligns with our clinical practice, since we find anosodiaphoria as a negative reaction to the ABI even in patients with good metacognitive SA and, as such, it represents another relevant obstacle in neurorehabilitation.
We did not find significant differences in levels of anxiety and depression between groups, in relation to the level of functional impairment as measured by the DRS, other than for state anxiety. This finding suggests that even low difficulties may be emotionally distressing, likely due to a patient’s fear of not recovering their complete level of pre-ABI functioning. Other studies [31,32] found divergent results, since higher levels of self-reported anxiety and depression were associated only with higher levels of functional impairment. These different results could be based on the different sample enrolled in terms of ABI severity in the acute phase (i.e., they included also patients with mild ABI), or to the measures used to assess the level of functional disability (i.e., they used subjective assessment methods of post-ABI consequences, rather than an objective measure such as the DRS, that we used in the present study).
As for the significant difference between the two groups with high metacognitive SA in terms of state anxiety (i.e., the higher the disability, the higher the state anxiety), it should be considered that patients with moderately severe-to-severe disability were young adults (with a median age of 45 years) and, as such, with a long life expectancy. Therefore, the higher state anxiety found in patients with higher DRS scores could be related to items from the assessment (e.g., dependence on others, psychosocial adaptability in terms of the possibility to return to work). Consensus on the association between anxiety and return to work is still a matter of debate [57,58], and further studies are needed.
With regard to the subjective impact of the ABI in terms of PTSD symptoms, we chose to focus only on high-metacognitive-SA patients. This was decided because ISA is a potentially confounding variable, which can make diagnosing PTSD difficult [33]. Also, since we used a self-report tool to explore PTSD symptoms, investigating this disorder in an individual with ISA may be paradoxical [8], increasing the risk of misdiagnosis [33,59]. Thus, we explored the potential presence of PTSD symptoms only in high-metacognitive-SA patients, by comparing Groups 2 and 3. We found that higher levels of functional disability were significantly associated with higher presence of PTSD symptoms (i.e., patients with moderately severe-to-severe disability lived more dramatically the brain injury experience). This aligns with our prediction, since being self-aware of higher difficulties associated with the brain injury may increase the risk of developing PTSD symptoms (i.e., intrusiveness, avoidance, hyper-arousal) as measured by the IES-R scale. In fact, at least three of the DSM-5 PTSD diagnostic criteria are compatible with severe ABI. Indeed, the brain injury can be considered “an event that is threatening to one’s wellbeing” [criterion a]. Moreover, some post-severe TBI symptoms (such as insomnia, irritability, concentration difficulties, hypervigilance, or heightened startle response) are also typical persisting signs of marked arousal, which may cause marked impairment to one’s functioning [criteria d and e] [60]. Although symptoms of PTSD have already been found in patients with moderate-to-severe ABI [33,61,62,63], to the best of our knowledge, no previous studies investigated the possible presence of PTSD symptoms according to the level of functional disability, which is an innovative contribution of the present study.
Finally, since we found some significant socio-demographic differences between-groups (i.e., in terms of age between Group 1 and both Groups 2 and 3, and in terms of educational level between Groups 1 and 3, and between Groups 2 and 3), we performed a correlational analysis to exclude any possible influence of these socio-demographic variables on the psycho-emotional feature. The analysis revealed that only age correlated with PCRS-NRDS (namely, with lower levels of metacognitive SA), whereas no other significant relationships were found.
The present study presents some important limits. First, the sample size of each sub-group was low. Furthermore, 41 out of 60 patients were inpatients, while 19 were treated in DH regimen; thus, it is possible that inpatients were more likely to have lower SA, determining as a consequence an evaluation bias. Accordingly, our findings should be considered as preliminary; further studies with larger sub-samples are needed to confirm and generalize our results.
Second, we did not provide neuroimaging data that could account for the absence of apathy in the presence of anosodiaphoria symptoms found in our study (e.g., different brain lesions sites).
Thirdly, according to the above-mentioned classificatory models [14,15], we considered only the metacognitive level of SA through the PCRS-NR discrepancy scores between patients and caregivers. Consequently, it must be considered that the subjective opinions and expectations of caregivers may also have an influence, leading (at least in part) to possible misdiagnosis. Other studies should also consider the relationship between the psycho-emotional variables here investigated and emergent/online SA, as well as the other levels of SA (i.e., declarative and real anticipatory SA) according to a partially new classificatory model proposed by Bivona and Villalobos [19].
Importantly, ISA is one of the main problems not only in the field of ABI, but also in other clinical populations (e.g., neurodegenerative [64] and psychiatrics [65,66,67]). Given the small samples size, further studies are needed to confirm our findings, through rigorous process observations and longitudinal research.
However, despite these limits, we believe that some important cues arose from our work. Our study highlighted relevant differences between groups according to the levels of metacognitive SA, in terms of anxiety and depression (significantly higher in high-metacognitive-SA patients) and anosodiaphoria (significantly higher in low-metacognitive-SA patients).
Moreover, in line with previous findings by Bivona and colleagues [17], our study confirmed a dissociation between apathy and anosodiaphoria in ISA patients, which suggests the need for an accurate differential diagnosis to implement an optimal neurorehabilitation project.
Finally, in high-metacognitive-SA patients, a complete neuropsychological and neuropsychiatric assessment should pay particular attention to the possible presence of PTSD symptoms, especially in patients with higher functional disability.

5. Conclusions

Although preliminary, the present study confirmed the importance of diagnosing, as early and specifically as possible, both levels of ISA and dysfunctional emotional reactions, such as anosodiaphoria, in patients with moderate-to-severe ABI.
We found adaptive emotional reactions (i.e., anxiety and depression symptoms) in high-metacognitive-SA patients, regardless of the level of functional disability, as well as PTSD symptoms associated with higher functional disability.
Accordingly, although the mechanisms by which SA interventions exert their effects are still a matter of debate (for details, see a recent systematic review and meta-analysis by Villalobos et al. [68]), our findings confirm the importance of first treating ISA and anosodiaphoria. Second, when patients show adequate levels of SA, the rehabilitation project should address not only the cognitive impairment, but also the psycho-emotional distress, such as anxiety and depression and the possible presence of PTSD symptoms.
In this vein, psychological support plays a crucial role in neurorehabilitation hospitals, helping to address psychological challenges closely linked to the recovery process, potentially involving not only the patients but also their families.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, V.B., D.V., A.C. and U.B.; data curation, V.B., G.G., A.Q. and A.D.; formal analysis, D.V.; investigation, V.B., G.G. and A.Q.; methodology, A.C. and U.B.; project administration, U.B.; supervision, U.B.; visualization, V.B., D.V., A.C. and U.B.; writing—original draft, V.B. and U.B.; writing—review and editing, V.B., D.V., A.C. and U.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of Santa Lucia Foundation (Project identification code: CE/Prog.880; approval date: 28 November 2022).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. Data are unavailable due to privacy or ethical restrictions.

Acknowledgments

We are sincerely grateful to Paola Ciurli, Nicoletta Fallarino, Giorgia Spina and Elisa Berardi for their help in collecting part of the data. We thank Rita Formisano for providing some important suggestions during the conceptualization of the study. Finally, we are grateful to our colleague Nicholas J. Cioe for reviewing the English.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Prigatano, G.P. The Study of Anosognosia; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bivona, U.; Ciurli, P.; Ferri, G.; Fontanelli, T.; Lucatello, S.; Donvito, T.; Villalobos, D.; Cellupica, L.; Mungiello, F.; Lo Sterzo, P.; et al. The Self-Awareness Multilevel Assessment Scale, a New Tool for the Assessment of Self-Awareness After Severe Acquired Brain Injury: Preliminary Findings. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1732. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Prigatano, G.P.; Schacter, D.L. Awareness of Deficit After Brain Injury: Clinical and Theoretical Issues; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  4. Prigatano, G.P.; Leathem, J.M. Awareness of behavioral limitations after traumatic brain injury: A cross- cultural study of New Zealand Maoris and non-Maoris. Clin. Neuropsychol. 1993, 7, 123–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Prigatano, G.P.; Altman, I.M. Impaired awareness of behavioral limitations after traumatic brain injury. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 1990, 71, 1058–1064. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  6. Sherer, M.; Bergloff, P.; Levin, E.; High, W.M.; Oden, K.E.; Nick, T.G. Impaired awareness and employment outcome after traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 1998, 13, 52–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Sherer, M.; Hart, T.; Nick, T.G. Measurement of impaired self-awareness after traumatic brain injury: A comparison of the patient competency rating scale and the awareness questionnaire. Brain Inj. 2003, 17, 25–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Bivona, U.; Riccio, A.; Ciurli, P.; Carlesimo, G.A.; Delle Donne, V.; Pizzonia, E.; Caltagirone, C.; Formisano, R.; Costa, A. Low self-awareness of individuals with severe traumatic brain injury can lead to reduced ability to take another person’s perspective. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 2014, 29, 157–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Dromer, E.; Kheloufi, L.; Azouvi, P. Impaired self-awareness after traumatic brain injury: A systematic review. Part 1: Assessment, clinical aspects and recovery. Ann. Phys. Rehabil. Med. 2021, 64, 101468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Winkens, I.; Van Heugten, C.M.; Visser-Meily, J.M.A.; Boosman, H. Impaired self-awareness after acquired brain injury: Clinicians’ ratings on its assessment and importance for rehabilitation. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 2014, 29, 153–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Millis, S.R.; Rosenthal, M.; Novack, T.A.; Sherer, M.; Nick, T.G.; Kreutzer, J.S.; High, W.M.; Ricker, J.H. Long-term neuropsychological outcome after traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 2001, 16, 343–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Kelley, E.; Sullivan, C.; Loughlin, J.K.; Hutson, L.; Dahdah, M.N.; Long, M.K.; Schwab, K.A.; Poole, J.H. Self-awareness and neurobehavioral outcomes, 5 years or more after moderate to severe brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 2014, 29, 147–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Prigatano, G.P.; Wong, J.L. Cognitive and affective improvement in brain dysfunctional patients who achieve inpatient rehabilitation goals. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 1999, 80, 77–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Crosson, B.; Barco, P.P.; Velozo, C.A.; Bolesta, M.M.; Cooper, P.V.; Werts, D.; Brobeck, T.C. Awareness and compensation in postacute head injury rehabilitation. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 1989, 4, 46–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Toglia, J.; Kirk, U. Understanding awareness deficits following brain injury. NeuroRehabilitation 2000, 15, 57–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Prigatano, G.P. Disturbances in higher order consciousness encountered in neuropsychological rehabilitation and assessment. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2024, 30, 913–922. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Bivona, U.; Costa, A.; Contrada, M.; Silvestro, D.; Azicnuda, E.; Aloisi, M.; Catania, G.; Ciurli, P.; Guariglia, C.; Caltagirone, C.; et al. Depression, apathy and impaired self-awareness following severe traumatic brain injury: A preliminary investigation. Brain Inj. 2019, 33, 1245–1256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Bivona, U.; Costa, A.; Ciurli, P.; Donvito, T.; Lombardi, G.; Misici, I.; Moretti, G.; Caltagirone, C.; Formisano, R.; Prigatano, G.P. Modification of the Patient Competency Rating Scale to Measure Anosodiaphoria after Severe Acquired Brain Injury: Preliminary Findings. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2022, 37, 753–761. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Bivona, U.; Villalobos, D. Assessment of Self-Awareness after Severe Acquired Brain Injury: Systematic Review and Recommendations for a new Classification of Offline Self-Awareness. Neuropsychologia 2025, 214, 109–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Gainotti, G. Emotional and psychosocial problems after brain injury. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 1993, 3, 259–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Carroll, E.; Coetzer, R. Identity, grief and self-awareness after traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 2011, 21, 289–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Wallace, C.A.; Bogner, J. Awareness of deficits: Emotional implications for persons with brain injury and their significant others. Brain Inj. 2000, 14, 549–562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Fleming, J.M.; Strong, J.; Ashton, R. Cluster analysis of self-awareness levels in adults with traumatic brain injury and relationship to outcome. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 1998, 13, 39–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Godfrey, H.P.D.; Partridge, F.M.; Knight, R.G.; Bishara, S. Course of insight disorder and emotional dysfunction following closed head injury: A controlled cross-sectional follow-up study. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 1993, 15, 503–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Geytenbeek, M.; Fleming, J.; Doig, E.; Ownsworth, T. The occurrence of early impaired self-awareness after traumatic brain injury and its relationship with emotional distress and psychosocial functioning. Brain Inj. 2017, 31, 1791–1798. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Sasse, N.; Gibbons, H.; Wilson, L.; Martinez-Olivera, R.; Schmidt, H.; Hasselhorn, M.; von Wild, K.; von Steinbüchel, N. Self-awareness and health-related quality of life after traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 2013, 28, 464–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Cooper-Evans, S.; Alderman, N.; Knight, C.; Oddy, M. Self-esteem as a predictor of psychological distress after severe acquired brain injury: An exploratory study. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 2008, 18, 607–626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Langer, K.G.; Levine, D.N. Babinski, J. (1914). Contribution to the study of the mental disorders in hemiplegia of organic cerebral origin (anosognosia). Translated by K.G. Langer & D.N. Levine. Translated from the original Contribution à l’Étude des Troubles Mentaux dans l’Hémiplégie Organique Cérébrale (Anosognosie). Cortex 2014, 61, 5–8. [Google Scholar]
  29. Fleming, J.; Strong, J.; Ashton, R. Self-awareness of deficits in adults with traumatic brain injury: How best to measure? Brain Inj. 1996, 10, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Villalobos, D.; Bilbao, A.; López-Muñoz, F.; Pacios, J. Self-awareness as a key process for rehabilitation of patients with acquired brain injury: A systematic review. Rev. Neurol. 2020, 70, 1–11. [Google Scholar]
  31. Fann, J.R.; Katon, W.J.; Uomoto, J.M.; Esselman, P.C. Psychiatric disorders and functional disability in outpatients with traumatic brain injuries. Am. J. Psychiatry 1995, 152, 1493–1499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Malec, J.F.; Testa, J.A.; Rush, B.K.; Brown, A.W.; Moessner, A.M. Self-assessment of impairment, impaired self-awareness, and depression after traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 2007, 22, 156–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Villalobos, D.; Bivona, U. Post-traumatic Stress Disorder after Severe Traumatic Brain Injury: A Systematic Review. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2022, 37, 583–594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Teasdale, G.; Jennett, B. Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness. A practical scale. Lancet 1974, 304, 81–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Hagen, C.; Malkmus, D.; Durham, P. Levels of Cognitive Functioning; Rancho Los Amigos Hospital: Downey, CA, USA, 1972. [Google Scholar]
  36. Borgaro, S.R.; Prigatano, G.P. Modification of the Patient Competency Rating Scale for use on an acute neurorehabilitation unit: The PCRS-NR. Brain Inj. 2003, 17, 847–853. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Prigatano, G.P.; Bruna, O.; Mataro, M.; Muñoz, J.M.; Fernandez, S.; Junque, C. Initial disturbances of consciousness and resultant impaired awareness in Spanish patients with traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 1998, 13, 29–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Rappaport, M.; Hall, K.M.; Hopkins, K.; Belleza, T.; Cope, D.N. Disability rating scale for severe head trauma: Coma to community. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 1982, 63, 118–123. [Google Scholar]
  39. Scranton, J.; Fogel, M.L.; Erdman, W.J. Evaluation of functional levels of patients during and following rehabilitation. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 1970, 51, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Hellebrekers, D.; Winkens, I.; Kruiper, S.; Van Heugten, C. Psychometric properties of the awareness questionnaire, patient competency rating scale and Dysexecutive Questionnaire in patients with acquired brain injury. Brain Inj. 2017, 31, 1469–1478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Smeets, S.M.J.; Ponds, R.W.H.M.; Verhey, F.R.; Van Heugten, C.M. Psychometric properties and feasibility of instruments used to assess awareness of deficits after acquired brain injury: A systematic review. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 2012, 27, 433–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Ciurli, P.; Bivona, U.; Barba, C.; Onder, G.; Silvestro, D.; Azicnuda, E.; Rigon, J.; Formisano, R. Metacognitive unawareness correlates with executive function impairment after severe traumatic brain injury. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2010, 16, 360–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Prigatano, G.P. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation After Brain Injury; John Hopkins University Press: Baltimore, MD, USA, 1986. [Google Scholar]
  44. Borgi, M.; Caccamo, F.; Giuliani, A.; Piergentili, A.; Sessa, S.; Reda, E.; Alleva, E.; Cirulli, F.; Miraglia, F. Validation of the Italian version of the Apathy Evaluation Scale (AES-I) in institutionalized geriatric patients. Ann. Ist. Super. Sanita 2016, 52, 249–255. [Google Scholar]
  45. Marin, R.S.; Biedrzycki, R.C.; Firinciogullari, S. Reliability and validity of the apathy evaluation scale. Psychiatry Res. 1991, 38, 143–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Lane-Brown, A.T.; Tate, R.L. Measuring apathy after traumatic brain injury: Psychometric properties of the Apathy Evaluation Scale and the Frontal Systems Behavior Scale. Brain Inj. 2009, 23, 999–1007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Spielberger, C.D.; Gorsuch, R.L.; Lushene, R.E.; Vagg, P.R.; Jacobs, G.A. Manual for the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory; Consulting Psychologist Press: Palo Alto, CA, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
  48. Beck, A.T.; Ward, C.H.; Mendelson, M.; Mock, J.; Erbaugh, J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1961, 4, 561–571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Williams, J.G.; Barlow, D.H.; Agras, W.S. Behavioral measurement of severe depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 1972, 27, 330–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Beck, A.T.; Beamesderfer, A. Assessment of depression: The depression inventory. Mod. Probl. Pharmacopsychiatry 1974, 7, 151–169. [Google Scholar]
  51. Beck, A.T.; Steer, R.A. Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory; Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
  52. Weiss, D.; Marmar, C. The impact of event scale—Revised. In Assessing Psychological Trauma and PTSD; Wilson, J., Keane, T., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 1997; pp. 399–411. [Google Scholar]
  53. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences; Routledge Academic: New York, NY, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  54. Gasquoine, P.G. Blissfully unaware: Anosognosia and anosodiaphoria after acquired brain injury. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 2016, 26, 261–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Prigatano, G.P. Emotion and motivation in recovery and adaptation after brain damage. In Brain Injury and Recovery: Theoretical and Controversial Issues; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 1988; pp. 335–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Quang, H.; Wearne, T.; Filipcikova, M.; Pham, N.; Nguyen, N.; McDonald, S. A Biopsychosocial Framework for Apathy Following Moderate to Severe Traumatic Brain Injury: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2024, 34, 1213–1234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Garrelfs, S.F.; Donker-Cools, B.H.P.M.; Wind, H.; Frings-Dresen, M.H.W. Return-to-work in patients with acquired brain injury and psychiatric disorders as a comorbidity: A systematic review. Brain Inj. 2015, 29, 550–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. van Velzen, J.M.; van Bennekom, C.A.M.; Edelaar, M.J.A.; Sluiter, J.K.; Frings-Dresen, M.H.W. Prognostic factors of return to work after acquired brain injury: A systematic review. Brain Inj. 2009, 23, 385–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Chalton, L.D.; McMillan, T.M. Can ‘partial’ PTSD explain differences in diagnosis of PTSD by questionnaire self-report and interview after head injury? Brain Inj. 2009, 23, 77–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Bryant, R.A.; Marosszeky, J.E.; Crooks, J.; Gurka, J.A. Elevated resting heart rate as a predictor of posttraumatic stress disorder after severe traumatic brain injury. Psychosom. Med. 2004, 66, 760–761. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Alway, Y.; McKay, A.; Gould, K.R.; Johnston, L.; Ponsford, J. Factors associated with posttraumatic stress disorder following moderate to severe traumatic brain injury: A prospective study. Depress. Anxiety 2016, 33, 19–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Bryant, R.A.; Marosszeky, J.E.; Crooks, J.; Baguley, I.J.; Gurka, J.A. Interaction of posttraumatic stress disorder and chronic pain following traumatic brain injury. J. Head Trauma Rehabil. 1999, 14, 588–594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Bryant, R.A.; Marosszeky, J.E.; Crooks, J.; Gurka, J.A. Posttraumatic stress disorder after severe traumatic brain injury. Am. J. Psychiatry 2000, 157, 629–631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Rosen, H.J. Anosognosia in neurodegenerative disease. Neurocase 2011, 17, 231–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Touskova, T.; Bob, P. Consciousness, awareness of insight and neural mechanisms of schizophrenia. Rev. Neurosci. 2015, 26, 295–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Coutinho, B.M.C.; Pariz, C.G.; Krahe, T.E.; Mograbi, D.C. Are you how you eat? Aspects of self-awareness in eating disorders. Personal. Neurosci. 2024, 7, e9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Kim, J.; Amaev, A.; Quilty, L.C.; Selby, P.; Shah, P.; Caravaggio, F.; Ueno, F.; Pollock, B.G.; Graff-Guerrero, A.; Gerretsen, P. A Measure to Assess Illness Awareness in Problem Gambling: Gambling Awareness and Insight Scale (GAS). J. Gambl. Stud. 2022, 38, 1029–1043. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Villalobos, D.; Bivona, U.; Botella, J. Self-Awareness Interventions After Acquired Brain Injury: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Rehabil. Psychol. 2025, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Table 1. Means, standard deviations, medians, minimum and maximum values for each sociodemographic and emotional variable in the three groups.
Table 1. Means, standard deviations, medians, minimum and maximum values for each sociodemographic and emotional variable in the three groups.
Group 1
(N = 17)
Group 2
(N = 15)
Group 3
(N = 28)
MeanSDMedianMinMaxMeanSDMedianMinMaxMeanSDMedianMinMax
Age (years)50.6512.3355266342.0715.745216538.0416.4633.51865
Education (years)14.352.781381913.82.31381712.042.9113817
Chronicity (days)168.18166.3210537698174196.77926772167.04147.948733489
AE-PCRS-NR Index1.290.161.271.11.711.030.141.020.81.240.980.180.990.51.39
STAI-X132.129.7831215544.4713.8343257936.6110.8833.52163
STAI-X232.359.1230215944.7312.1543266240.9311.5241.52066
BDI6.596.27502512.607.481233310.298.029.5029
AES30.07.7329184629.078.6429205231.787.75311847
IES-R----------26.8015.112755816.7414.6014169
Note. SD: standard deviation; Group 1: low-metacognitive-SA patients. Group 2: high-metacognitive-SA patients and moderately severe-to-severe disability. Group 3: high-metacognitive-SA patients and none-to-moderate disability. AE-PCRS-NR Index: Anosodiaphoria Extension of the Patient Competency Rating Scale-Neurorehabilitation form—Index. STAI-X1: State-Trait Anxiety Inventory—State anxiety. STAI-X2: State-Trait Anxiety Inventory—Trait anxiety. BDI: Beck Depression Inventory. AES: Apathy Evaluation Scale. IES-R: Impact of the Event Scale-Revised.
Table 2. Comparison between groups on sociodemographic variables and chronicity (Mann–Whitney U test, p value and effect size are reported).
Table 2. Comparison between groups on sociodemographic variables and chronicity (Mann–Whitney U test, p value and effect size are reported).
Group 1 vs. Group 2Group 1 vs. Group 3Group 2 vs. Group 3
Zp ValueESZp ValueESZp ValueES
Age−1.7760.038 *0.314−2.4130.008 **0.359−0.8290.2030.126
Education−0.4730.3410.084−2.4560.007 **0.366−2.0920.018 *0.319
Chronicity−0.3590.3680.063−0.0820.4670.012−0.2930.3840.045
Note. Group 1: low-metacognitive-SA patients. Group 2: high-metacognitive-SA patients and moderately severe-to-severe disability. Group 3: high-metacognitive-SA patients and none-to-moderate disability. ES: effect size (Rosenthal’s r). * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
Table 3. Comparison between groups on psycho-emotional variables (Mann–Whitney U test, p value and effect size are reported).
Table 3. Comparison between groups on psycho-emotional variables (Mann–Whitney U test, p value and effect size are reported).
Group 1 vs. Group 2Group 1 vs. Group 3Group 2 vs. Group 3
Zp ValueESZp ValueESZp ValueES
AE-PCRS-NR Index−3.929<0.001 **0.694−4.778<0.001 **0.712−0.8290.2030.126
STAI-X1−2.7980.003 **0.495−1.6530.049 *0.246−1.9520.025 *0.298
STAI-X2−2.7600.003 **0.488−2.6360.004 **0.393−0.9050.1820.138
BDI−2.7640.003 **0.489−1.6900.045 *0.252−1.2120.1120.185
AES−0.5160.3030.091−0.8010.2110.119−1.4370.0750.219
IES-R      −2.2870.011 *0.349
Note. Group 1: low-metacognitive-SA patients. Group 2: patients with high metacognitive SA and moderately severe-to-severe disability. Group 3: patients with high metacognitive SA and none-to-moderate disability. ES: effect size (Rosenthal’s r). AE-PCRS-NR Index: Anosodiaphoria Extension of the Patient Competency Rating Scale-Neurorehabilitation form—Index. STAI-X1: State-Trait Anxiety Inventory—State anxiety. STAI-X2: State-Trait Anxiety Inventory—Trait anxiety. BDI: Beck Depression Inventory. AES: Apathy Evaluation Scale. IES-R: Impact of the Event Scale-Revised; * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
Table 4. Rho Spearman bivariate correlation between sociodemographic and psycho-emotional variables.
Table 4. Rho Spearman bivariate correlation between sociodemographic and psycho-emotional variables.
PCRS-NRDSAE-PCRS
Index
STAI-X1STAI-X2BDIAESIES-R
Age0.422 **0.2480.076−0.121−0.02−0.246−0.081
Education0.1130.0870.1450.0090.072−0.160.092
Chronicity−0.0210.1940.0520.1520.0990.1840.254
Note: PCRS-NRDS: Patient Competency Rating Scale-Neurorehabilitation Form Discrepancy Score. AE-PCRS-NR Index: Anosodiaphoria Extension of the Patient Competency Rating Scale-Neurorehabilitation form—Index. STAI-X1: State-Trait Anxiety Inventory—State anxiety. STAI-X2: State-Trait Anxiety Inventory—Trait anxiety. BDI: Beck Depression Inventory. AES: Apathy Evaluation Scale. IES-R: Impact of the Event Scale-Revised; ** p < 0.01.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Bandiera, V.; Villalobos, D.; Costa, A.; Galluzzi, G.; Quinzi, A.; D’Aprile, A.; Bivona, U. Emotional Status in Relation to Metacognitive Self-Awareness and Level of Functional Disability Following Acquired Brain Injury. Brain Sci. 2025, 15, 841. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15080841

AMA Style

Bandiera V, Villalobos D, Costa A, Galluzzi G, Quinzi A, D’Aprile A, Bivona U. Emotional Status in Relation to Metacognitive Self-Awareness and Level of Functional Disability Following Acquired Brain Injury. Brain Sciences. 2025; 15(8):841. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15080841

Chicago/Turabian Style

Bandiera, Valentina, Dolores Villalobos, Alberto Costa, Gaia Galluzzi, Alessia Quinzi, Arianna D’Aprile, and Umberto Bivona. 2025. "Emotional Status in Relation to Metacognitive Self-Awareness and Level of Functional Disability Following Acquired Brain Injury" Brain Sciences 15, no. 8: 841. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15080841

APA Style

Bandiera, V., Villalobos, D., Costa, A., Galluzzi, G., Quinzi, A., D’Aprile, A., & Bivona, U. (2025). Emotional Status in Relation to Metacognitive Self-Awareness and Level of Functional Disability Following Acquired Brain Injury. Brain Sciences, 15(8), 841. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci15080841

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop