Next Article in Journal
A Comparative Study of the Impact of NO-Related Agents on MK-801- or Scopolamine-Induced Cognitive Impairments in the Morris Water Maze
Next Article in Special Issue
Eye-Movements in a Text Reading Task: A Comparison of Preterm Children, Children with Dyslexia and Typical Readers
Previous Article in Journal
Color-Coding Method Reveals Enhancement of Stereotypic Locomotion by Phenazepam in Rat Open Field Test
Previous Article in Special Issue
ERP and Behavioural Correlates of Prospective Memory in Bilinguals during L1 and L2 Processing
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Lexico-Semantic Influence on Syntactic Processing: An Eye-Tracking Study with Spanish Relative Clauses

by
Esther Álvarez-García
1,* and
José Manuel Igoa González
2,*
1
Department of Hispanic and Classic Philology, University of León, 24007 León, Spain
2
Department of Basic Psychology, Autonomous University of Madrid, 28049 Madrid, Spain
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2023, 13(3), 409; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13030409
Submission received: 30 January 2023 / Revised: 17 February 2023 / Accepted: 21 February 2023 / Published: 26 February 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Advances in Psycholinguistics and Cognition in Language Processing)

Abstract

:
This paper investigates the interaction between lexicosemantic and syntactic information in sentence processing by examining the online comprehension of Spanish relative clauses (RCs) of both restrictive and non-restrictive types. A corpus study shows that, in Spanish, a RC may be introduced by different function words (called relativizers), which differ in lexical frequency, as well as semantic features. Based on these facts, an eye-tracking experiment was conducted with the aim of analyzing whether lexicosemantic information could influence sentence processing at the early stages. The results report an early influence of lexicosemantic information not only when activating a relativizer but also when integrating it within the syntactic structure. Additionally, the semantic role played by each RC type seems to constrain sentence processing at different regions. Our results favor an interactive view of language processing, according to which language comprehension is guided from the early stages by different kinds of linguistic information rather than syntactic information alone.

1. Introduction

One of the main goals in the study of human language processing is to understand how listeners/readers recover the syntactic structure of a sentence from a string of words in order to comprehend the intended message. Assuming that structure building operations (known as parsing) are necessary for sentence understanding, it is pertinent to ask what kinds of information come into play in syntactic processing and when each one of them is accessed or becomes activated along the process. A general distinction in this regard is often made between purely structural information, on the one hand, involving word class identification and labelling and phrase structure assembly, and non-syntactic information, on the other, which encompasses a wide range of information sources such as different properties of individual lexical items, e.g., frequency of use, relevant semantic features such as animacy or thematic roles, or contextual information from previous discourse or from a visual scenario [1].
As regards the order in which the various relevant information types come into play, the issue is whether syntactic decisions are made, or at least initiated, before any extra-syntactic information becomes available, or are constrained or guided by this information in the first place. The latter may occur in two different ways, either in a cascaded fashion, according to which non-syntactic information is activated alongside syntactic information and may be used to decide between alternative parsing choices at a given point [2], or in a fully interactive way, where non-syntactic information favors certain parsing options and blocks others incompatible with it [3].
A related question that is often addressed in the literature on sentence parsing refers to the ways in which syntactic and non-syntactic information are used at different stages, either separately or in combination, and sequentially or simultaneously. This involves, on the one hand, the pace at which different parsing operations take place, either in the form of a continuous, incremental word-by word progression [4], or in a discontinuous manner, with some points in the sentence involving more costly operations and others allowing a more easy-going processing mode, depending on the memory requirements the parser faces at each point [5], and relatedly, the depth of processing attained by the parser in the face of different strategies, such as performing a shallower syntactic analysis with few initial commitments until enough information becomes available to carry out a more fine-grained analysis if or when needed [6,7].
The major aim of this article is to assess the interplay of syntactic and non-syntactic information during the comprehension of relative clauses (henceforth RCs) in Spanish. To that end, we will report a corpus study followed by an eye-tracking experiment carried out with two types of RCs, restrictive and non-restrictive, in which we manipulated (or recorded, in the case of the corpus study) the kind of relativizer that introduces the RC, taking advantage of the fact that Spanish displays interesting contrasts between different kinds of relativizers. Besides the default and more common item que (that stands for “that” in English), which lacks semantic features, Spanish has other less frequent relativizers, one of which carries gender and number features (i.e., el cual, “which” in English), and others that bear semantic features as well, such as quien (“who/whom”) or donde (“where”), which respectively encode animacy and location information. By addressing the effects of these two contrasts—RC type and relativizer type—on RC processing, we purport to find out whether non-syntactic information—i.e., structural or lexical frequency and/or semantic features—is recruited alongside purely syntactic information at different stages during sentence processing. Although RC processing has been widely studied in psycholinguistic research across many different languages and for various purposes, our study is the first to test the role of these variables in function (closed class) words.

2. Literature Review

2.1. RC Processing

Relative clauses have been of interest in the psycholinguistic research for different reasons: on the one hand, they can be manipulated to examine the resolution of local structural ambiguities (such as “The defendant examined by the lawyer turned out to be unreliable/The defendant that was examined…” [8]), but more importantly for the purpose of this paper, they allow us to analyze structure building operations and the influence of different kinds of information—both linguistic and non-linguistic—on these processes. In this regard, two main topics related to RC processing have been examined.
On the one hand, several studies have analyzed RC processing when these structures follow a complex noun phrase (NP), so the RC—between square brackets in (1)—may modify the first noun, as in (1a), thus producing a “high attachment” of the RC, or the second noun, as in (1b), resulting in a “low attachment” of the RC [9].
(1)
a.
Someone shot the maid of the actor [who was on the balcony with her husband].
b.
Someone shot the butler of the actress [who was on the balcony with her husband].
English speakers show a preference for a low attachment of the RC, reflected in fewer comprehension errors and shorter reading times when compared to a forced high attachment structure (among others, [9,10,11,12,13]). These results have been interpreted as favoring modular and locality theories of language processing, such as the garden-path model [14,15], which claims that sentence parsing depends on purely structural information, at least at early stages, and always favors an analysis that minimizes processing load. In this sense, a low attachment of the RC implies that the incoming constituent (that is, the RC) is associated with the currently processed phrase, resulting in a less demanding operation when compared to the attachment to a phrase that has already been processed and therefore closed. This locality preference is implemented in the garden-path model through a syntactic principle called “Late Closure” [16], which does not take into account non-syntactic information related to the sentence that is being processed.
Low attachment preferences have been replicated across different languages—for instance, Swedish, Norwegian, Romanian [17], or Chinese [18]; however, there are also languages that exhibit a different pattern. This is the case of Spanish, the language under study in this paper. Cuetos & Mitchell [9] first showed that Spanish speakers favor a high attachment of the RC (also [11,12,19,20]), which speaks against locality and, therefore, against syntactic principles exclusively guiding sentence processing. This preference has also been confirmed in other languages such as Dutch [21] or German [22].
In addition, some studies have also proved that attachment preferences might be influenced by non-syntactic information, such as the semantic features of the NPs (e.g., Cuetos & Mitchell [9] analyzed English RCs following non-human NPs, resulting in a preference for a high attachment of the RC), the preposition linking the two nouns of the complex NP (e.g., Gilboy et al. [23] found that replacing the Spanish preposition de (“of”) with con (“with”) results in a preference shift from high to low attachment), or prosody (e.g., Maynell [24] showed that the presence of an intonational phrase boundary before an English RC results in a preference for a high attachment). These results seem to refute modular proposals of language processing and, instead, favor an interactive view where extra-syntactic information becomes available at early stages and can even guide sentence processing [3,25].
The second main topic in the psycholinguistic literature related to RC processing has been the study of subject versus object RCs (henceforth SRC versus ORC). SRCs, that is, RCs where the relative pronoun (or relativizer) takes the subject role, as in (2a), seem to be less costly to process in languages such as English when compared to ORCs (2b), where the relativizer is the object of the relative verb—attacked in (2).
(2)
a.
The reporter [thati _i attacked the senator] admitted the error.
b.
The reporter [thati the senator attacked _i] admitted the error.
This asymmetry has been replicated across different languages—for instance, German [26], French [27], or Japanese [28], and with different experimental paradigms and measures—self-paced reading [29], eye-tracking [30,31], ERPs [32], and fMRI [33], which was initially interpreted as evidence of a universal processing mode guided by syntactic principles, such as the Accessibility Hierarchy [34], the Active Filler and Recent Filler Strategies proposed by the garden-path model [35], or the Dependency Locality Theory [36]. Leaving aside particular differences among these theories, most of them recognize a syntactic principle that relates the relativizer with its original position within the RC, so the longer the distance between these units is, the more costly its processing becomes as more incoming constituents need to be kept active by the parser until they are finally integrated. In English, as well as in other languages, the distance between the relativizer and its original position is longer in ORC when compared to SRC—identified in (2) by the dash, which would explain this processing asymmetry.
Nevertheless, and similarly to what has been previously observed in relation to high/low attachments, the asymmetry between SRCs and ORCs has not showed up in every language under study so far—e.g., in Basque, ORCs are easier to process than SRCs [37], and this pattern has also been observed in Chinese [38,39] and in some studies with Japanese [40], and more interestingly, it can be counterbalanced when manipulating certain extra-syntactic features. For example, Traxler et al. [30] (also [31,41,42,43,44]) found that the asymmetry between these two types of RCs can be neutralized when the antecedent NP (or head noun) is inanimate such as movie in (3), or similarly, when the NP within the RC is a personal pronoun such as he/him in (4) [45,46].
(3)
a.
The movie [that pleased the director] received a prize at the film festival.
b.
The movie [that the director watched] received a prize at the film festival.
(4)
a.
The reporter that attacked him admitted the error.
b.
The reporter that he attacked admitted the error.
As for Spanish, there is only one study to date that has addressed this question [47], and the results show, as in English, a SRC advantage when the head noun is animate—such as in (2) above, but a neutralization of this asymmetry when it is inanimate (3).
As previously reported, the problems of attachment ambiguities and subject-object asymmetry have become central when it comes to the study of RC processing. Considering the theoretical questions presented at the beginning of the introduction, the findings reported so far provide evidence that syntactic processing is not completely autonomous, but rather it seems that non-syntactic information plays a role when building sentence structure from early stages, regardless of whether this happens in a cascaded or fully interactive way, or in an incremental or discontinuous manner.

2.2. Function Words: Relativizers

Interestingly enough, there is a third aspect related to the structure of RCs that might be useful in order to fully understand how these sentences are processed—and therefore how different kinds of linguistic information interact when comprehending language: In languages such as Spanish, the same RC can be introduced by different relativizers, for instance, que (“that”; in English, that is considered a complementizer when introducing a RC [48], but in Spanish, que is always labeled as a relative pronoun when heading a RC [49,50]) in (5a), or donde (“where”) in (5b).
(5)
a.
Fui a visitar la ciudad en la que nací.
I went to visit the city (that) I was born in.
b.
Fui a visitar la ciudad donde nací.
I went to visit the city where I was born.
There are some syntactic contexts in which Spanish relativizers cannot vary [48], but when they do, as in example (5), this variation does not change the syntactic structure of the sentence nor its referential meaning. Nevertheless, Spanish relativizers have different linguistic features, so they can be manipulated in order to test whether and how these features influence sentence processing. First, Spanish relativizers may differ in semantic features [49,50]: Thus, the Spanish relativizers que (“that”) and el cual (“which”) lack semantic features, so they can combine with any head noun (human, object, place, time...); other relativizers, such as quien (“who”; Spanish quien and English who are not completely equivalent either, as they differ in syntactic combinations. For example, in contrast to English who, Spanish quien can only head prepositional restrictive RCs (el médico a quien llamé—the doctor whom I called)) or donde (“where”), possess intrinsic semantic features, which must be shared with the head noun: quien refers to human beings, while donde carries a locative meaning. Second, Spanish relativizers also differ in lexical frequency as it will be reported in the corpus study of Section 3. Based on these differences among Spanish relativizers, one of the aims of our study is to compare the processing of sentences in which a RC can be introduced by a more frequent relativizer—que in (6a), when compared to a less frequent one—donde in (6b), and also by a relativizer possessing semantic features—donde in (6b)—with another lacking them—que in (6a).
(6)
a.
La policía registró el barrio del que proceden los muchachos desaparecidos.
The police searched the neighborhood which the missing boys come from.
b.
La policía registró el barrio de donde proceden los muchachos desaparecidos.
The police searched the neighborhood where the missing boys come from.
Previous studies on RC processing have mainly analyzed the influence of lexicosemantic information based on the manipulation of content words, such as the antecedent NP or the NPs inside the RC; however, function words such as relativizers also differ in semantic features and lexical frequency, and so they could also be taken into account in order to examine whether and how this extra-syntactic information constrains sentence processing. However, as far as we know, there are just a few studies so far addressing these questions from the manipulation of function words.
Firstly, Tabor et al. [51] addressed the question whether the lexical frequency of an ambiguous function word such as that could be relevant when comprehending sentences. That may be interpreted as a determiner, a conjunction or a pronoun, the conjunction interpretation being the most frequent one according to Gibson [52]. Tabor et al. used a self-paced reading paradigm— which consists of presenting a written sentence in segments (words or phrases) that participants must read one at a time while they press a key in order to advance to the next segment—to compare reading times for the ambiguous word that versus an unambiguous word such as those following a verb like visited. This verb cannot take a clause as complement, so both that and those must be interpreted as determiners.
(7)
a.
The lawyer visited that cheap hotel to stay for the night.
b.
The lawyer visited those cheap hotels to stay for the night.
The authors recorded longer reading times for ambiguous sentences (7a), meaning they were more costly to process than unambiguous ones (7b). These results were interpreted as an interference of the more frequent interpretation of that as a conjunction, even though this interpretation was not possible in the syntactic context examined. Once readers realized this interpretation was not correct, they had to inhibit it in order to activate the correct one—that as a determiner—leading to longer reading times.
Gibson [52] also addressed this question, pointing out that Tabor et al. [51] did not distinguish the particular kind of frequency that influenced their results: In absolute terms, that is more frequently interpreted as a conjunction, but this is also the most frequent interpretation in the context Tabor et al. analyzed—that is, after a verb. Gibson wondered whether these results would be replicated when using a syntactic context in which the interpretation of that as a determiner is more frequent, for instance, after a preposition, as in (8).
(8)
a.
The lawyer for that skilled surgeon asked for a raise.
b.
The lawyer for this skilled surgeon asked for a raise.
Gibson [52], using also the self-paced reading technique, recorded longer reading times for ambiguous versus unambiguous sentences in both contexts—post-verb (7a versus 7b) and post-preposition (8a versus 8b), suggesting that the interpretation of that as a conjunction, although impossible in (8), was also activated. In addition, Gibson found no interaction between syntactic context and ambiguity, which means that the influence of lexical frequency occurs systematically regardless of syntactic context. Both Gibson and Tabor et al. [51] interpreted these results as supporting interactive accounts: If the parser only considered syntactic information, it would activate the syntactically correct interpretation of that, yielding no differences in the processing of ambiguous versus unambiguous sentences. However, the fact that the most frequent interpretation of the ambiguous function word was also activated shows that the parser considers non-syntactic information, such as lexical frequency, when building the structure of a sentence.
Other studies have employed experimental paradigms that are more sensitive to changes in processing as they occur along time, such as eye-tracking, which may enable to tell apart early and late decisions made by the parser. For instance, Schmauder et al. [53] compared sentences in which the target word—in italics—was a function (9) versus content word (10), with a higher or lower frequency rate (9a versus 9b; 10a versus 10b).
(9)
a.
As we looked across the crowd we could see Dad’s bright red jacket.
b.
As we looked amidst the crowd we could see Dad’s bright red jacket.
(10)
a.
The old-fashioned method was far more effective than any modern one.
b.
The old-fashioned helmet was far more effective than any modern one.
They measured the spillover effect, that is, the duration of the first fixation following the target word, which according to these authors, reflects the integration cost of the target word within the syntactic structure. The results showed longer first fixations following less versus more frequent function words (9b versus 9a); however, there were no differences between content words due to lexical frequency (10a versus 10b). Schmauder et al. [53] argued in favor of an early influence of lexical frequency on the syntactic integration of a function word: The less frequent a function word is, the more costly its syntactic integration becomes.
Nevertheless, Schmauder et al.’s study [53] has some limitations that should be taken into account. First, these authors analyzed the spillover effect for the first fixation, that is, the duration of a fixation when the eyes first meet a word. This measure has been traditionally related to lexical retrieval [54,55,56], so it could be questioned whether this effect reflects a syntactic integration cost due to the low frequency of a function word or, simply, a cost in the retrieval of a less frequent function word. Regarding this question, Rayner et al. [54] argued that first-pass duration (so named when the target region includes more than one word [57], and gaze duration when it contains only one word [58]), instead of first fixation, is a better measure to analyze syntactic integration as it shows that, after a first fixation, the reader re-fixates a word before leaving it in a regressive or progressive manner. This refixation would indicate a difficulty when establishing a relationship between the fixated word and previous ones, that is, an integration cost of the target word into the syntactic structure [56].
Similarly, Rayner et al. [54] have pointed out that a spillover effect could be related to a syntactic integration cost but also to a lexical retrieval cost: If activating a lexical unit is harder than expected, as it normally occurs with low frequency words, this cost can be dragged onto the next word, resulting in a spillover effect. In order to distinguish one cause from the other, Rayner et al. recommended to run a correlation analysis between reading times of the target word and the following one: A positive correlation between these two words would mean that the spillover effect is a consequence of a lexical retrieval cost; on the contrary, the lack of a positive correlation would denote that the spillover effect reflects a cost that goes beyond retrieving the target word from the mental lexicon and is related to syntactic integration. Unfortunately, Schmauder et al. [53] did not perform such an analysis, so the spillover effect they found could be related to either a syntactic integration cost—as they argued—or a lexical retrieval cost.
In sum, the results of the studies reviewed so far are not conclusive about the influence of function words and their lexical information on sentence processing. On the one hand, Tabor et al. [51] and Gibson [52] provided evidence for this influence but could not demonstrate that it occurs at early stages as self-paced reading techniques are not sensitive to early processing. In this sense, both modular and interactive theoretical accounts could explain their results. On the other hand, Schmauder et al.’s results [53] could be related either to a syntactic integration cost, thus reflecting some influence of lexical frequency on syntactic processing at early stages, or merely to a lexical retrieval cost. Therefore, these studies do not provide a satisfactory answer to the question at hand, that is, does lexical information related to function words constrain or guide sentence building operations? Given this state of affairs, we argue that Spanish relativizers could be useful in order to examine this question, as they differ in lexical frequency (as it will be reported in Section 3), and also in semantic information, with some of them possessing intrinsic semantic features and others lacking them. In fact, the semantic differences between relativizers allow us to analyze the influence of these features on sentence processing based on the manipulation of function words, as we have no evidence of such a study so far. E. Fernández [59] suggested a similar study to the one presented here but with a different purpose: She asked whether the preference for high or low attachment of a RC with two attachment sites within a noun phrase could be determined by the relativizer introducing it; however, such an experiment has not been conducted so far.

2.3. RC Type

Beyond relativizers, there is a second characteristic related to RCs that has scarcely been studied in relation to sentence processing: RC type. In languages such as Spanish, RCs can be classified into two different types: Restrictive (7) and Non-Restrictive (11). These two types of RCs differ, on the one hand, in their semantic function: Restrictive RCs are used to identify the referent of the head noun, which is unknown prior to introducing the embedded clause, whereas non-restrictive RCs add an extra meaning, unnecessary to identify the referent of the head noun, as this is already known by the reader (see [60,61] for different accounts of non-restrictive RCs in English, and [62] for a comprehensive review of Spanish RCs). In addition, Spanish restrictive and non-restrictive RCs also differ in frequency (as it will be reviewed in Section 3), so they can also be compared so as to examine whether the frequency of these syntactic structures influences sentence processing.
(11)
a.
La policía registró mi barrio, del que proceden los muchachos desaparecidos.
The police searched my neighborhood, which the missing boys come from.
b.
La policía registró mi barrio, de donde proceden los muchachos desaparecidos.
The police searched my neighborhood, where the missing boys come from.
Most studies on RC processing have been carried out with restrictive RCs. The comparison between restrictive and non-restrictive RCs has been addressed mainly with the interest of exploring the contribution of prosody and, to a lesser extent, punctuation marks (i.e., commas) in spoken and written RCs, respectively. As regards spoken RCs in Spanish, restrictive and non-restrictive types are distinguished on the basis of the location of pauses and changes in pitch at constituent boundaries. Thus, restrictive RCs usually bear a longer pause and a rising pitch at the end of the main clause (or the antecedent NP), plus a reset to lower pitch at the RC onset, whereas non-restrictive RCs bear a shorter pause (usually not perceptible) and a falling pitch at the clause boundary (or the antecedent NP), with reset to higher pitch at RC onset [63]. Written RCs, in turn, mirror this intonational pattern by regularly placing a comma after the head noun to identify non-restrictive RCs, while restrictive RCs conventionally follow the head noun without punctuation (see examples 7 and 11 above). Spanish is no different from other European languages in this regard. In any case, we should bear in mind that the presence of a comma right after the head noun in the written version of non-restrictive RCs (or its absence in restrictive RCs), and before the relativizer, may reveal the type of RC the reader is facing, and this has implications for the interpretation of eye-tracking data, as we shall see later.
To the best of our knowledge, only a handful of studies have addressed the contrast between restrictive and non-restrictive RCs in written modality in terms of processing difficulty. In a study by Grodner et al. [64], restrictive and non-restrictive RCs were compared in a self-paced reading task under two contextual conditions: A null context, and a supportive context preceding the target RC for each kind of RC. For restrictive RCs—e.g., “The postman that the dog bit…”, the supportive context introduced two referents compatible with the denotation of the subject noun of the target sentence, whereas for non-restrictive RCs—e.g., “The postman, who the dog bit…”, it only gave one such referent. The results showed a reversed pattern of reading times in the region following the relativizer, i.e., “the dog bit”, across RC types as a function of context: reading times were longer for restrictive RCs under the null context condition and for non-restrictive RCs under the supportive context. Therefore, when read in isolation, restrictive RCs are harder to process than non-restrictive ones, while the opposite is the case with a facilitating context. The authors take this pattern of results as evidence for the early influence of discourse context on the interpretation of unambiguous sentences.
In addition, a few studies have addressed the effects of punctuation marks on written sentence processing. In this regard, it has been shown that the presence of a comma located at phrasal or clausal boundaries of non-restrictive RCs and other structures, such as vocatives and parentheticals, tends to increase processing times in eye-tracking measures—such as fixation durations or number of fixations—on the section of the sentence preceding the comma while facilitating the processing of the following section, as shown by larger saccades into that region ([65], but see the contradictory results for non-restrictive RCs in Hirotani et al. [66]).

2.4. Present Study

Based on these two manipulations—relativizer and RC type—we set out to analyze whether and how frequency and semantic information can determine and guide RC processing in Spanish. To examine this question, we conducted an eye-tracking experiment in which participants read Spanish RCs such as (7) and (11), differing in the heading relativizer (que versus other) and the RC type (restrictive versus non-restrictive). Previous studies on RC processing had a different purpose (as reported in Section 2.1), which makes it difficult to anticipate the readers’ behavior, and underscores the exploratory nature of the current study. However, there are still some predictions that can be made taking into account different theoretical accounts.
First of all, a modular account such as the garden-path model claims that sentence processing is only determined by syntactic principles at early stages (with non-syntactic information playing a role only at late stages, when the sentence structure has already been computed), and particularly in RC processing, by two syntactic strategies: Active Filler and Recent Filler [35]. Once the parser identifies the position of a relativizer, the Active Filler Strategy is applied, so the filler formed by the relativizer needs to be kept active until a gap is found. This would fire a second strategy, Recent Filler, such that the first gap encountered would be filled by the most recent active filler. Concerning our study, this model would predict no differences in the processing of sentences (12a) and (12b), at least at early eye-tracking measures, as a consequence of the kind of relativizer heading the RC, since in both cases the parser would identify a filler (the relativizer) right after the direct object (el barrio), and this structure is taken to be common to all conditions regardless of whether the relativizer is que or other (el cual, quien, or donde). The filler would be kept active by the Active Filler Strategy until a gap was found, and the Recent Filler Strategy would try to detect a gap for this filler, which again appears at the same position in all conditions, that is, after the embedded verb. Only at a later stage and, hence, showing up in late eye-tracking measures, could non-syntactic information influence RC processing, so that differences between (12a) and (12b) due to lexical frequency and/or semantic features of the relativizer would only show up at this point.
(12)
  • La policía registró el barrio [del quei proceden hi los muchachos desaparecidos].
    The police searched the neighborhood [which the missing boys come from h].
  • La policía registró el barrio [de dondei proceden hi los muchachos desaparecidos].
    The police searched the neighborhood [where the missing boys come from h].
As for the contrast between restrictive and non-restrictive RCs, the predictions of the garden-path model would depend on the relative complexity of either type of RCs: If both types shared the same syntactic configuration, there should be no processing differences, but if restrictive and non-restrictive RCs are shown to attach at different levels—i.e., as a sister of D within the DP in restrictive RCs (see example 13a) and as a sister of DP at a higher structural level in non-restrictive RCs (13b), the parser should have more processing difficulties with the latter.
(13)
a.
Restrictive RC: [DP the [NP poets [CP who spoke French]]].
b.
Non-restrictive RC: [DP [DP the [NP poets]] [CP who spoke French]].
Other modular accounts, such as the Tuning hypothesis [9], would make moderately different predictions from those of the garden-path model. The Tuning hypothesis would accept that the frequency of a syntactic structure can influence sentence processing at early stages, so differences in the processing of restrictive versus non-restrictive RCs could be found. Particularly, those structures that are more frequent—non-restrictive RCs in most of the cases, as we shall see in the next section—would be easier to process. As for relativizers, this theory predicts no differences in the processing of RCs due to lexical or semantic features, which means that the processing of (12a) and (12b)—or, similarly, (11a) versus (11b)—would not differ, again at least at early stages. In this sense, the Tuning hypothesis can also be considered a modular account, as it preserves the autonomy of syntactic information at early stages, devoid of any semantic features.
On the other hand, interactive accounts claim that non-syntactic information can guide the process of building the syntactic structure of a sentence from the very beginning, and not only at late stages. In particular, usage-based accounts [67,68,69,70,71] argue that sentence processing is determined by distributional patterns learned over time, meaning that those patterns are easier to recognize and hence to process. One of the factors that make a pattern easier to recognize is frequency not only at a syntactic level but at any linguistic level. Consequently, usage-based accounts predict that those RCs which are more frequent and which are introduced by a more frequent relativizers would be easier to recognize and hence to process, showing shorter reading times not only at late stages but from early eye movement measures.
Similarly, usage-based accounts consider that distributional patterns may also capture semantic information, so these features might influence RC processing as well. In this case, two hypotheses could be put forward. On the one hand, it might occur that the activation of the relativizer’s semantic features—if it has any—would take some extra time, so its identification and integration would be more costly as compared to a relativizer lacking semantic features. This would result in longer reading times for those RCs headed by relativizers that possess intrinsic semantic features—donde (12b)— versus relativizers lacking semantic information—que (12a). On the other hand, it could also be predicted that sharing semantic features between the head noun and the relativizer would facilitate the activation of the latter and its integration into the syntactic structure, leading to shorter reading times in comparison to a relativizer lacking semantic features (12b versus 12a). As for the role of the semantic features of restrictive and non-restrictive RCs, interactive accounts argue that non-syntactic information can influence and constrain sentence building operations from early stages. Bearing in mind that restrictive and non-restrictive RCs perform different semantic functions in fixating the reference of their head noun, it seems plausible to consider that interactive accounts would predict processing differences between restrictive and non-restrictive RCs. However, these models do not specify how semantics constrains RC processing in particular, which makes it difficult to provide detailed predictions. Therefore, our study will hopefully help clarify whether and how the semantic function of each RC type results in processing differences.
Thus, our study purports to find out whether or not lexico-semantic information related to function words such as relativizers, as well as to RC type, can influence RC processing in Spanish. We first provide the results of a corpus study (Section 3), aimed at determining the frequency of both relativizers and RC types in Spanish. Then, we will report the results of an eye-tracking experiment (Section 4), designed to examine RC processing when manipulating relativizers and RC type. We will end up with a discussion of the results (Section 5) and some concluding remarks (Section 6).

3. Corpus Study

To control for lexical and structural frequencies, we carried out a corpus study. Previous studies have analyzed the frequency of Spanish relativizers [72,73,74,75]; however, most of them suffer from limitations, such as not discriminating the syntactic contexts in which relativizers vary, not using systematic criteria, or more frequently, not performing quantitative analyses when comparing relativizers’ frequencies [76]. For these reasons, we ran a corpus study in which we analyzed a total number of 134,018 words spanning a period of time from 2000 to 2013. These words were taken from different European Spanish corpora (Table 1) with the aim of analyzing both formal and informal registers, as well as oral and written language. This makes another difference between our study and previous ones, which have generally analyzed a particular register of a particular linguistic variety. Table 1 shows the number of words analyzed for each linguistic variety, how many of those words were relativizers, how many of those relativizers appeared in contexts where variation is allowed in Spanish, and finally, the corpus from which data was taken. Statistical analyses showed no differences in the total number of relativizers, χ2(3) = 1.21, p > 0.1, or in the total number of relativizers in variation contexts, χ2(3) = 2.72, p > 0.1, across linguistic varieties.
Table 1 displays absolute and relative frequency for all the Spanish relativizers (que, el cual, quien, donde, cuando, como, cuanto, and cuyo); however, our study is focused on the processing of RCs introduced by four of these units: que (“that”), el cual (“which”), quien (“who”), and donde (“where”). The reason why we decided to analyze only four relativizers was to include a larger number of experimental materials in the eye-tracking experiment in order to gain statistical power. Particularly, we chose the previous four relativizers for two main reasons: (1) as it will be shown, they differ in frequency rates, as well as in semantic features, which would enable us to examine the possible influence of these features on RC processing, and (2) in contrast to other relativizers, these four units make it possible to create experimental materials with the same structure across conditions and which would also sound natural.
Table 2 shows the absolute and relative frequency of the Spanish relativizers que (“that”), el cual (“which”), quien (“who”), and donde (“where”) in the contexts where they can vary for both types of RCs—restrictive and non-restrictive. As mentioned in Section 2.2, variations are not freely allowed in Spanish between these four relativizers: Que can replace the other three relativizers in any context, but these three relativizers cannot always replace que, and in addition, they cannot generally substitute each other, e.g., quien cannot replace donde or vice versa [49,50]. For this reason, we will report the results based on three relativizer contrasts: que versus el cual, que versus quien, que versus donde. In this regard, it is important to note that, in some contexts, que may be replaced by both el cual or quien and, in others, by both el cual or donde. These data have been added to the frequency of que in contrasts of que versus el cual, as well as in que versus quien (or que versus donde when the antecedent was a locative.
Taking into account count data from Table 2, we performed a Poisson regression using the glm function in R [77]. Models were built with RC type, relativizer, and their two-way interaction as fixed effects and linguistic variety as a random effect. As dependent variables, we analyzed relativizers’ absolute frequency. Models were compared conducting likelihood-ratio tests with ANOVA in R (see Tables in Section A of the Supplementary Materials for a full description of the models’ reports), and p-values were adjusted using the Holm–Bonferroni method [78].

3.1. Que versus el Cual

Figure 1 shows the frequency of que and el cual in both restrictive and non-restrictive RCs. For this contrast, there was a RC type x relativizer interaction, z = 2.89, p < 0.01, for although que is much more frequent than el cual in both types of RCs, this difference turned out to be larger for non-restrictive than restrictive RCs (69.86% versus 25.89%). Similarly, non-restrictive RCs are more frequent than restrictive RCs regardless of the heading relativizer; however, this difference seems to reach significance only for sentences with que versus el cual (43.97% versus 0.68%).

3.2. Que versus Quien

Figure 1 also displays the frequency of que and quien in both restrictive and non-restrictive RCs. For this contrast, there was a RC type x relativizer interaction, z = −2.79, p < 0.05, which shows que is more frequent than quien in both restrictive and non-restrictive RCs, but this difference proved to be much larger for non-restrictive than restrictive RCs (68.27% versus 1.09%). Additionally, and similarly to the previous contrast, non-restrictive RCs are more frequent than restrictive RCs with both relativizers, but this difference was larger for sentences with que versus quien (79% versus 11.82%).

3.3. Que versus Donde

Finally, Figure 1 also shows the frequency of que and donde in both types of RCs. For this contrast, there was a RC type x relativizer interaction, z = −4.87, p < 0.001, since que is more frequent than donde in restrictive RCs, whereas donde is more frequent than que in non-restrictive RCs. Additionally, restrictive RCs are more frequent than non-restrictive RCs in sentences headed by que, whereas the opposite pattern was found for sentences with donde: Non-restrictive RCs are more frequent than restrictive ones.
To sum up, the contrasts que versus el cual and que versus quien exhibit a similar pattern, since que turns out to be more frequent than either el cual or quien in Spanish, especially in non-restrictive RCs. In addition, non-restrictive RCs are more frequent than restrictive RCs regardless of the heading relativizer but especially when headed by que. On the other hand, the contrast que versus donde shows a different pattern, as now the frequency rate of these two relativizers is somehow determined by RC type: Non-restrictive RCs are more frequently headed by donde versus que, while restrictive RCs appear more frequently introduced by que versus donde.
Based on the results of our corpus study, Table 3 summarizes the main differences between Spanish relativizers in terms of lexical frequency and semantic features. These features were then included in our eye-tracking study in order to determine whether or not they influence sentence processing.

4. Eye-Tracking Study

4.1. Method

4.1.1. Participants

Forty-eight undergraduate students from the Autonomous University of Madrid participated in this experiment in exchange for course credits. All were Spanish native speakers and had normal or corrected to normal vision (mean age = 19.41, SD = 0.84; 7 males).

4.1.2. Design

We conducted an eye-tracking experiment with a two-way repeated measures design. Independent variables, both within-subjects, were (a) type of RC (restrictive versus non-restrictive) and (b) relativizer (que versus other), resulting in four conditions, as exemplified in Table 4: restrictive que, restrictive other, non-restrictive que, and non-restrictive other. This experimental design was applied in three contrasts, que versus el cual, que versus quien, and que versus donde, so the other conditions correspond to el cual, quien, and donde, respectively. As dependent variables, six eye movement measures were analyzed [56]: First fixation (the duration of the first fixation on a target region); first-fixation rate (probability of fixating a region during first pass); first-pass duration (the total duration of all fixations on a target region before exiting it for the first time, either in a regressive or progressive manner); first-pass regression (probability of making a regression during first pass); quasi-first-pass reading time (the total duration of all fixations on a target region before exiting it for the first time in a progressive manner) and second-pass duration (the total duration of all refixations on a target region). The first three measures are generally referred to as “early” measures, as they reflect the first contact of the eyes with the text (and in consequence, a first stage of processing), while the last measure (second-pass duration) is considered a “late” measure, as it includes any possible reanalysis of the text. As for first-pass regression and quasi-first-pass reading time, there is some controversy regarding their status: They record the first contact of the eyes with the text but purportedly reflect some difficulty when integrating a word and the resulting reanalysis performed to overcome this difficulty [56]. For this reason, we will refer to these two measures as “intermediate”. Finally, we also took into account parafoveal processing—that is, the processing of the following word (n + 1) when the eyes are fixating on the word n.

4.1.3. Materials

We created 168 experimental items—56 experimental items per contrast—and 180 filler items. Four lists of experimental items were constructed, each containing one version of each item. Participants were assigned randomly to one of the four lists, so all of them read 42 experimental items under each condition, i.e., 14 for each relativizer contrast, and only one version of each item. Similarly, each version of the items was read by twelve participants, resulting in a within- and between-subject design.
Regarding syntactic structure, experimental items included subject (region 1) + main verb (region 2) + direct object, functioning as antecedent or head noun (region 3) + RC, which was formed by a relativizer (region 4) + a verb (region 5) + a complement (region 6). Table 4 exemplifies how items were divided into six regions according to their syntactic structure in order to perform statistical analyses (see Section B of the Supplementary material for a full description of the experimental stimuli).
Conditions differed in (a) RC type as revealed in the head noun (R3), and (b) the relativizer (R4). The head noun was the same singular noun among conditions; however, in restrictive RCs it was preceded by an article—el/la (“the”), which implies that the referent is unknown and will be identified by the RC, whereas, in non-restrictive RCs, it was preceded by a possessive determiner—mi, su (“my”, “his/her”), meaning that the referent of the antecedent is already known, and by a comma following the noun, as required by Spanish orthographic rules. Relativizers also differed among conditions (que versus el cual, quien, and donde). In order to avoid large differences in the number of characters in the relativizer region (R4), all relativizers performed a prepositional syntactic function, so they were always preceded by a preposition. This aspect has important consequences for que-conditions, because the presence of a preposition requires also the use of an article before this relativizer. This phenomenon reduces to just one character the difference between que versus el cual-conditions (9.5 characters versus 10.5), as well as between que versus quien-conditions (6.03 versus 7.03). As for the contrast que versus donde, there was a mean difference of two characters between conditions (7.84 characters for que-conditions versus 5.89 for donde). In order to control for differences in the length of a region, some authors recommend the use of reading time per character, that is, the division of the total time it takes to read a region (known as raw data) by the number of characters of that region including spaces [15,79,80,81,82]. Nevertheless, we preferred to use raw data for two reasons. First, conditions differ in one or two characters, which is a negligible difference, especially when comparing short frequent words such as function words [8]. Second, reading time per character assumes a linear relation between fixations and the number of characters, which may not be generally true [8,55].
Filler items had the same length as experimental ones, but did not include RCs. They had the following structure: subject + main verb + direct object + subordinate clause, which was formed by an initial conjunction + a verb + a complement. Both types of items (experimental and filler) were randomized and followed by a comprehension question, which could refer to the content of either the main or the embedded clause.

4.1.4. Procedure

Eyelink 1000 Plus (SR Research, Ottawa, ON, Canada) was used in order to monitor participants’ eye movements while reading both experimental and filler items. The eye-tracker had a 2000 Hz. sample rating, with an angular resolution of 1.46 degrees. Materials were displayed in a MultiSync EA221WM screen, connected to the eye-tracker. Sentences and questions appeared in a single line, in the middle of the screen, on a grey background. Letters were in black, Courier New font, and size 18.
The experiment was conducted individually in two sessions with at least one week interval between sessions in order to avoid priming effects—items were different in each session, but they had the same syntactic structure. Participants sat down in front of a computer screen at an approximate distance of 60 cm. Instructions were explained before starting the task: Participants had to read the sentences on the screen in silence at a normal pace. After each sentence, a comprehension question appeared and participants had to respond “yes” or “no” by pressing a key—“s” or “n”, respectively. They also had to press a key after each sentence in order to visualize the corresponding question. While reading the material, participants placed their heads in a chin rest so as to minimize head movements. Vision was binocular during the whole experiment, but only movements from the right eye were recorded.
Before starting the experiment, the eye-tracker was calibrated in order to obtain the best possible recording of the eye movements. Calibration was repeated in the middle of each session after a short break, and also when the tracker was out of alignment. Similarly, before proceeding to the next trial a fixation point (i.e., drift correct) was displayed on the left side of the screen in order to calibrate the apparatus and to locate participants’ gaze, thus avoiding a previsualization of the sentence.

4.2. Results

4.2.1. Comprehension Task

Participants responded correctly to 95% of the comprehension questions. We performed mixed-effect logistic regression using the glmer function in R [83,84]. Mixed logit models were built with response accuracy as the dependent variable, and RC type, relativizer and their two-way interaction as fixed effects. Subjects and items were added as random effects. In order to control for individual variation in the response-reaction time (RT) relation, a random effect slope for RT was added to subjects, items or both. Models were compared conducting a likelihood-ratio test with the ANOVA function in R (see Tables in Section C of the Supplementary Materials for a full description of the models’ reports), and p-values were adjusted using the Holm–Bonferroni method [78].
Results showed no significant differences (p > 0.05) in the contrasts que versus el cual and que versus quien, meaning that participants regularly understood experimental sentences irrespective of the RC type or the relativizer heading it (Table 5). In the contrast que versus donde there was a significant interaction between the two independent variables, z = -2.54, p = 0.03, since restrictive RCs presented a higher response accuracy with que versus donde (96% versus 94%); in contrast, non-restrictive RCs showed a better performance with donde versus que (96% versus 94%). This latter result suggests participants understood better restrictive RCs with que and non-restrictive RCs with donde, thus mirroring the frequency rates of the corpus study for this contrast (see Section 2); however, the remaining two conditions (non-restrictive RCs with que and restrictive RCs with donde) also present a high response accuracy (more than 90%), confirming participants understood most of the experimental sentences in these two conditions as well.

4.2.2. Eye Movement Measures

Before analyzing the eye-tracking record, fixations longer than 2000 ms were removed. Similarly, fixations shorter than 80 ms and located at least 1.46 degrees apart from another fixation were also removed; however, when located at a distance shorter than 1.46 degrees from another fixation, they merged into one. This procedure resulted in 0.16% of the data being excluded.
Eye-tracking data was analyzed using the lme4 package in R [85]. Linear mixed-effect models were built with RC type, relativizer, and their two-way interaction as fixed effects. Additionally, we included random slopes for RC type and relativizer by subject and by item. As dependent variables, we analyzed four of the eye movement measures described above (first fixation, first-pass duration, quasi-first-pass reading time, and second-pass duration) for the three regions of interest (R3, R4, and R5, corresponding to the head noun, the relativizer, and the RC verb, respectively). The remaining two eye movement measures (first-fixation rate and first-pass regression) were analyzed using mixed-effect logistic regression in R, where the presence of a fixation was coded as 1 and its absence as 0. For the three regions of interest mentioned above, mixed logit models were built with RC type, relativizer, and their two-way interaction as fixed effects and subject and item random effect slopes for RC type and relativizer. Both linear mixed-effect models and mixed logit models were compared conducting likelihood-ratio tests with ANOVA in R, and p-values were adjusted using the Holm–Bonferroni method.
For ease of presentation, we will report the results based on the three relativizer contrasts explored in our experiment, starting with que versus el cual, then que versus quien, and finally, que versus donde. Additionally, effects will be reported by regions (R3, R4, and R5), as well as by “the way eyes move”, that is, early, intermediate, and late measures. On the other hand, and also for the sake of clarity, the subsequent discussion of the results will be framed according to the two independent variables used in our study, namely, relativizer and RC type. This will allow a clearer assessment and comparison of the role of different relativizers and RC types in the processing of these sentences, the main goal of our current research.

4.2.3. Eye-Tracking Results for Que versus el Cual

Table 6 shows the statistical effects found in the six eye-tracking measures recorded for the contrast que versus el cual at R3, R4, and R5. In addition, Figure 2, Figure 3 and Figure 4 display mean reading times of one early reading measure (first-pass duration; Figure 2) and two intermediate reading measures (quasi-first-pass time and first-pass regression; Figure 3 and Figure 4, respectively) across regions 3, 4, and 5 (see also Tables in Section D of the Supplementary Materials for a full description of the models’ reports, as well as for mean reading times of all the eye-tracking measures).
At R3 (that is, the head noun of the RC), there was a main effect of RC type for the intermediate measure first-pass regression, z = 2.88, p = 0.01, meaning that the probability of regressing from this region to previous words during the first pass was higher for non-restrictive versus restrictive RCs (22.5% versus 15.1%). No other effects were found at this region.
At the relativizer region, R4, there was an interaction of RC type x relativizer for the early measure first-fixation rate, z = 3.72, p < 0.001, since the probability of fixating this region during the first pass was higher for restrictive RCs headed by el cual versus que, while there were no differences between non-restrictive RCs (see Table 6 below).
At R4, there was also a marginal interaction of RC type x relativizer for the early measure first-pass duration, t = 1.85, p = 0.06, for although restrictive RCs received longer fixations than non-restrictive RCs when introduced by both que and el cual, this difference turned out to be larger for the latter relativizer (22.9 ms versus 3.5 ms). This same interaction was marginal for first-pass regression, z = 1.86, p = 0.06, and reached significance for quasi-first-pass time, t = 2.31, p = 0.02, both being intermediate measures (Figure 3 and Figure 4). Finally, this interaction became also marginal for the late measure second-pass duration, t = 1.87, p = 0.06, since restrictive RCs received longer refixations during second pass than non-restrictive RCs, especially when headed by el cual.
On the other hand, there was a main effect of RC type at R4 for the early measure first-pass duration, t = −2.62, p = 0.01, with longer reading times for restrictive RCs when compared to non-restrictive RCs (Figure 2). This effect was also significant for quasi-first-pass time, t = −2.96, p = 0.006 (Figure 3), and second-pass duration, t = −2.6, p = 0.01, which exhibited the same pattern.
There was also a main effect of relativizer at R4 for the first-fixation rate, z = −3.94, p < 0.001, which means that there was a higher probability of fixating this region when a RC was introduced by el cual when compared to que. This effect also showed up in reading times for early measures such as first fixation, t = −6.08, p < 0.001, and first-pass duration, t = −6.68, p < 0.001, with longer reading times for el cual when compared to que in both restrictive and non-restrictive RCs (Figure 2).
In addition, there was a main effect of relativizer for first-pass regression at R4, z = −3.26, p = 0.003, meaning that the probability of regressing form this region to previous words was higher when a RC was introduced by el cual versus que (Figure 4). This effect for first-pass regression was completed by the above-mentioned interaction of RC type x relativizer for quasi-first-pass time, for despite the fact that el cual received longer reading times than que for both types of RCs, this difference turned out to be larger for restrictive than non-restrictive RCs (68.07 ms versus 42.78 ms).
Additionally, there was a main effect of relativizer for second-pass duration at R4, t = -3.22, p = 0.003, with longer reading times for el cual versus que, especially in restrictive RCs. Altogether, the effect of relativizer at R4 seems to indicate that RCs introduced by el cual are more difficult to process than those introduced by the more frequent relativizer que in both types of RCs but, especially, in restrictive RCs.
Finally, at R5, which displayed the same RC verb across all conditions, there was a marginal effect of RC type for the intermediate measure first-pass regression, z = −2.28, p = 0.06, and the late measure second-pass duration, t = −2.34, p = 0.05, meaning that there was a higher probability of regression backwards, as well as longer reading times for the RC verb of restrictive versus non-restrictive RCs. Interestingly enough, this effect exhibited the same pattern for both R4 and R5, thus suggesting that restrictive RCs seem to impose a greater processing burden than non-restrictive RCs, but was opposite to the pattern previously observed at R3. In the Discussion section, we will try to explain this variation between regions.
To conclude, R3 showed a higher probability of making a regression from this region to previous words during the first pass for non-restrictive versus restrictive RCs. On the contrary, R4 and R5 exhibited longer reading times for restrictive versus non-restrictive RCs and for sentences introduced by el cual versus que in early measures such as first fixation or first-pass duration. Similarly, once readers reached R4, they tended to make more regressions and longer refixations on this same region before advancing to the following one when a RC was headed by el cual, especially for restrictive RCs. This latter pattern was also reflected in late measures such as the second-pass duration, which showed longer reading times for restrictive RCs with el cual.

4.2.4. Eye-Tracking Results for Que versus Quien

Table 7 displays the statistical effects of the eye-tracking measures for the contrast que versus quien. Similarly, the data of first-pass duration, quasi-first-pass reading time, and first-pass regression recorded at R3, R4, and R5 are shown in Figure 5, Figure 6 and Figure 7, respectively (see also the tables in Section E of the Supplementary Materials for models’ reports and for mean reading times of all the eye-tracking measures).
At R3, there was a main effect of RC type for the intermediate measure first-pass regression, z = 3.64, p < 0.001, since non-restrictive RCs exhibited a higher probability of making a regression from the head noun to previous regions in comparison to restrictive RCs (27% versus 15%). This effect was also significant for the late measure second-pass duration, z = 2.68, p = 0.02, with longer reading times at R3 for non-restrictive versus restrictive RCs (see Table 7 below).
At R4, there was a main effect of relativizer for the early measure first-fixation rate, z = 6.40, p < 0.001, which means that there was a higher probability of fixating R4 when a RC was introduced by quien when compared to que. This effect was also significant for first fixation, t = 7.80, p < 0.001, and first-pass duration, t = 8.09, p < 0.001, confirming longer reading times for quien versus que in both restrictive and non-restrictive RCs (see Figure 5 above).
Finally, there was also a main effect of relativizer at R4 for the intermediate measure quasi-first-pass time at R4, t = 7.83, p < 0.001, showing that, after a regression to a previous word and before advancing to the following region, readers fixated the relativizer region for a longer time when a RC was introduced by quien in comparison to que regardless of RC type (Figure 6).
As for R5, there was a marginal interaction of RC type x relativizer for the early measure first-pass duration, t = 2.16, p = 0.09, since restrictive RCs received longer fixations when they were introduced by que in comparison to quien (Table 7); in contrast, non-restrictive RCs showed slightly longer reading times when introduced by quien versus que, though this latter comparison was not significant (p > 0.1). This interaction did reach significance for first-pass regression, z = 3.38, p < 0.001, quasi-first-pass time, t = 3.24, p = 0.001, and second-pass duration, t = 2.32, p = 0.02, meaning that there was a higher probability of regressing from R5 to previous words, as well as longer (re)fixations on R5 when a restrictive RC was introduced by que versus quien; however, there were no significant differences for non-restrictive RCs (p > 0.1).
In sum, R3 exhibited a higher probability of regressing from the head noun to previous words for non-restrictive RCs in comparison to restrictive RCs, the same pattern that we found in the previous contrast que versus el cual. At R4 there were no differences between the two types of RCs, but reading times increased when a RC of any type was introduced by quien when compared to que in early measures such as first fixation or first-pass duration. Similarly, readers tended to make more regressions and longer refixations on the relativizer region before advancing to the following word (i.e., R5) when it was occupied by quien versus que. Nevertheless, once readers reached R5, they exhibited a different pattern of eye movements, since now, restrictive RCs introduced by que received longer reading times when compared to the other three conditions in early (though only marginally), intermediate and late measures.

4.2.5. Eye-Tracking Results for Que versus Donde

A full presentation of the statistical effects for this contrast is given in Table 8, and displayed for first-pass duration, quasi-first-pass reading time and first-pass regression in Figure 8, Figure 9 and Figure 10 below (see Tables in Section F of Supplementary Materials for models’ reports and for mean reading times of all the eye-tracking measures).
Similar to what was observed in the two previous relativizer contrasts, there was a marginal interaction of RC type x relativizer at R3 for the early measures first fixation, t = −2.23, p = 0.05, and first-pass duration, t = −2.10, p = 0.07, which showed longer reading times for non-restrictive RCs in comparison to restrictive RCs when introduced by the relativizer donde; however, for sentences with que restrictive RCs exhibited slightly longer reading times than non-restrictive RCs (see Table 8 and Figure 8 above).
Additionally, similarly to the previous contrasts, there was a main effect of RC type at R3 for the intermediate measure first-pass regression, z = 3.47, p = 0.001, since non-restrictive RCs exhibited a higher probability of making a regression from the head noun to previous words in comparison to restrictive RCs (22% versus 14%).
On the other hand, at R4 there was a marginal interaction of RC type x relativizer for first fixation, t = 2.17, p = 0.09, for although que received longer reading times when compared to donde for both types of RCs, this difference proved to be larger for non-restrictive RCs (23.33 ms versus 5.62 ms). No other effects were found for the remaining eye movement measures (p > 0.1).
As for R5, there was a marginal effect of relativizer for first-pass duration, t = 2.16, p = 0.09, since the RC verb received longer fixations during the first pass when following que versus donde. This main effect reached significance for first-pass regression, z = 3.40, p = 0.001, which indicates that there was a higher probability of making a regression from the RC verb to previous words when a RC was introduced by que when compared to donde for both types of RCs (Figure 10). This effect was also significant for the intermediate measure quasi-first-pass time, t = 3.00, p = 0.008 (Figure 9), and the late measure second-pass duration, t = 2.41, p = 0.04, showing that, after a regression, readers fixated for a longer time the RC verb when it followed que versus donde (see Table 8 above).
Finally, no main effect of RC type or interaction of RC type x relativizer was found for any of the eye movement measures at R5 (p > 0.1).
To sum up, R3 exhibited longer first fixations for non-restrictive RCs with donde and for restrictive RCs with que, although these differences were only marginal. In addition, readers made more regressions from the head noun (i.e., R3) to previous words for non-restrictive RCs in comparison to restrictive RCs. Unlike the two previous contrasts, once they reached R4, the relativizer region showed longer first fixations when it was occupied by que versus donde, especially for non-restrictive RCs. Finally, and similarly to the contrast que versus quien, R5 received longer reading times after a regression when it followed que in contrast to donde for both types of RCs.

5. Discussion

The following discussion of the results reported in the previous section will focus on the effects of each independent variable manipulated in our study (relativizer and RC type) on the relevant eye-tracking measures and also in relation to the data from the corpus study previously reported.

5.1. Effect of Relativizer

The results showed a main effect of relativizers at both R4 and R5 for different eye movement measures in all contrasts tested in our experiment. These eye movement measures reflect different aspects of sentence processing, which we will address in turn.

5.1.1. Lexical Activation

First of all, in the contrasts que versus el cual and que versus quien, there was a main effect of relativizer at R4 for first fixation and first-fixation rate, measures commonly related to lexical activation. Longer first fixations for el cual (“which”) and quien (“who”) therefore suggest that these relativizers were more costly to activate than que (“that”). Corpus studies showed that the former relativizers were less frequent than the latter, so this higher activation cost might be caused by their lower lexical frequency rate [53,86,87]. In addition, these results were common to both restrictive and non-restrictive RCs, showing that lexical activation cost was consistent across RC type. Nevertheless, in the contrast que versus quien, the conditions differ not only in relativizers’ lexical frequency but also in their semantic features, so it could be argued that longer first fixations for quien versus que might be caused by a greater cost when activating the semantic features associated with the former relativizer. Despite the fact that both lexical frequency and semantic feature accounts are compatible, the former seems to have more credence, as the contrast que versus el cual also showed a main effect of relativizer, and we should recall that these two relativizers lack semantic features. Thus, if lexical frequency determines the activation of que and el cual, it should also play a role when activating quien, regardless of whether semantic features influence its activation as well.
In regard to these results, we could ask whether readers activated relativizers only once they fixated R4 or this process began earlier while fixating R3, that is, whether or not there was parafoveal processing. The parafoveal hypothesis implies that some features of the relativizers were activated while readers were fixating R3, so once they moved forward to R4, the activation process was somehow alleviated. If this hypothesis were true, it should be expected that parafoveal processing would benefit from lexical frequency [88,89]: The fact that que is more frequent might have facilitated its lexical activation in the parafovea, so as readers moved to R4, they would not need to fixate it for such a long time, yielding shorter first fixations for que versus el cual and quien. Moreover, parafoveal processing normally involves an increase in reading times at the previous region, that is, the region where parafoveal processing takes place. However, according to our data, there was no effect of relativizer at R3 for the contrasts que versus el cual and que versus quien, which makes it difficult to ascertain whether relativizers could be activated foveally at R4 or parafoveally at R3 (at least, some of their features). In this respect, results for the contrast que versus donde may help clarify this issue.
For the contrast que versus donde, we found opposite patterns of results at regions 3 and 4 across RC types. Thus, at R3 non-restrictive RCs exhibited longer reading times when the following relativizer was donde versus que, while the opposite was true for restrictive RCs. As for R4, non-restrictive RCs showed longer reading time measures for sentences headed by que versus donde. Thus, taking into account the results at regions 3 and 4 together, we could argue for parafoveal processing only in non-restrictive RCs: In this type of RCs, readers spent more time fixating R3 when the following relativizer was donde versus que, so they gained a more effective preview of the former relativizer than the latter. Consequently, when they moved to R4, fixations at donde were shorter in comparison to que as it could have been activated, at least partially, at R3. This does not mean that que was not activated parafoveally, but its activation level would have been lower, requiring more time to be fully activated later at R4. Nevertheless, these results need to be taken with caution. Firstly, we may ask why evidence in favor of parafoveal processing only shows up in non-restrictive RCs, but not in restrictive RCs: In the latter que exhibited longer reading times than donde at R3, as well as R4, which downplays the role of parafoveal processing; if readers fixated R3 longer for the que condition because they were parafoveally processing this relativizer, we should then expect a decrease in reading times for que at R4, which did not occur in our data. Secondly, differences for the contrast que versus donde were only marginal at both R3 and R4, so new studies are needed to further explore this issue. Finally, previous studies have provided inconsistent evidence about the influence of lexical features on parafoveal word processing: While few studies confirm that reading times on word n may vary as a function of the frequency of n + 1 [90], most of them deny this possibility [91,92,93,94,95,96]. As previously pointed out, this does not mean there is no parafoveal processing of n + 1; however, the influence of lexical variables over this phenomenon seems to be mainly reflected at n + 1 rather than at word n.
In sum, the eye-tracking measures usually related to lexical activation, i.e., first-fixation rate and first fixation, show differences in the processing of relativizers for the contrasts que versus el cual and que versus quien; hence, the relativizer with higher frequency rates (que) exhibited shorter reading times than the ones with lower frequency rates (el cual and quien). These results suggest that the activation of function words such as relativizers is governed, among other features, by their frequency [53,86,87]. Additionally, these results could be interpreted in terms of an influence of either absolute or relative frequency, as que is more frequent than el cual and quien together but also in each individual contrast [52]. Finally, it seems more likely that the influence of lexical frequency upon relativizers’ activation occurs at the relativizer region (that is, foveally), and is not mainly dependent on parafoveal processing.

5.1.2. Syntactic Integration

The main effect of relativizer was also significant at R4 for early and intermediate eye movement measures—first-pass duration and quasi-first-pass reading time—in the contrast que versus el cual and que versus quien. First-pass duration shows that, after a first fixation, readers fixate again the target region before exiting it—either in a regressive or progressive manner. Similarly, quasi-first-pass reading time shows that, after a first fixation, readers regress to previous words and then re-fixate the target region before proceeding to subsequent words. Therefore, these measures are generally related to a syntactic integration process: Once a word is activated, readers try to establish a syntactic relationship between it and previous words, leading to new fixations or to refixations when this integration process becomes more difficult [55,56,97]. The results showed longer reading times at R4 for these measures when a RC was introduced by el cual or quien versus que. As shown in the Section 2, the syntactic structure of the experimental items was the same across the four conditions, so longer reading times cannot be explained by a difference in their syntactic structure. The only common difference between conditions lies in the introducing relativizer, and more specifically, in its lexical frequency. Therefore, it seems that establishing a syntactic relationship between a relativizer and previous words is more costly when this unit has a lower lexical frequency rate (el cual, quien), leading to longer (re)fixations. These results indicate that lexical frequency related to function words may also influence sentence processing from relatively early stages. Interestingly, this pattern was common to both restrictive and non-restrictive RCs, which indicates that the influence of lexical frequency when integrating a relativizer into a syntactic structure is systematic regardless of RC type [52].
At R5 (that is, the RC verb region), there was also a main effect of relativizer for the contrasts que versus quien and que versus donde, which showed that now RC verbs received longer fixations after the relativizer que versus quien and donde for both intermediate and late measures—quasi-first-pass reading time, first-pass regression, and second-pass duration. These results would mean that establishing a syntactic relationship between the RC verb and previous words is more costly when this verb comes after relativizer que versus quien and donde. These results cannot be explained by the relativizers’ lexical frequency, as the opposite pattern should then have ensued (that is, longer reading times, at least after quien). Therefore, now differences between conditions might be caused by the semantic features of the relativizers: quien and donde possess semantic features, while que lacks them. These semantic features are shared with the head noun of the RC, so they might have helped to recognize the referent to which the RC verb alluded and, in consequence, to establish a syntactic relationship between these words. Interestingly, for the contrast que versus quien, longer reading times at the RC verb (i.e., R5) after que versus quien were only significant for restrictive RCs, and we should recall that the function of this type of RCs is, precisely, to identify the referent of the head noun. Hence, the results at R5 suggest that semantic features related to function words may also guide sentence processing and alleviate its related cost. This hypothesis is not inconsistent with the fact that effects at R5 for the contrast que versus el cual exhibited the opposite pattern, since neither of these relativizers possesses semantic features (and, therefore, the lexical frequency would keep its influence over RC processing).
Alternatively, the effect brought about by relativizers with semantic features might arise from the adjacent position of the relativizer to the RC verb, as shown by the fact that this facilitatory effect vanishes when an intervening constituent is placed between the constituents with overlapping features [98]. This alternative explanation casts doubt on claims of the influence of semantic features of words on parsing, so there remains an open question for future research whether this alleged semantic effect would remain long enough to facilitate retrieval of the antecedent NP of the RC when disrupted by an intervening constituent. Still, early on in this section, we acknowledged that the lexical frequency in and of itself may account for the immediate effects of the relativizer on RC processing, given the robust effects of semantically underspecified relativizers such as que and el cual.

5.2. Effect of RC Type

Our results show a main effect of RC type at R3 (that is, the head noun) for first-pass regression. This effect was significant across all conditions in the three contrasts, meaning that readers tended to make more regressions from R3 to previous words (and before proceeding to following words) in non-restrictive RCs than in restrictive RCs. This pattern indicates a higher processing cost associated with the head noun of non-restrictive RCs, which could be caused by two factors, not necessarily incompatible. On the one hand, the head noun of non-restrictive RCs was always followed by a comma in contrast to the head noun of restrictive RCs. Previous studies have shown that punctuated words normally exhibit longer reading times than non-punctuated words [65,66,99], so the higher probability of making a regression from the head noun of non-restrictive RCs could be related to the presence of this punctuation mark. Interestingly, our results agree with Grodner et al.’s [64], who also recorded an advantage for non-restrictive RCs in English, but may contradict somehow Hirotani et al.’s results [66]. These authors also compared restrictive versus non-restrictive RCs in English but did not obtain a significant difference between these two types of RCs. Nevertheless, it is important to note that they did not analyze the eye-tracking measure first-pass regression; on the contrary, they focused on other reading measures (first-pass reading time, quasi-first pass reading time and total reading time), and for these measures, neither did we obtain significant results.
At this point, we must recall that the presence of a comma after the head noun of non-restrictive RCs is justified by the role they play. As mentioned in Section 2.3, non-restrictive RCs add an extra meaning to the referent of the head noun, which is already known; in contrast, restrictive RCs are used to identify the referent of the head noun. Therefore, the antecedent of non-restrictive RCs (but not that of restrictive RCs) needs to be identified at R3, and this process could entail a higher cost and, hence, a higher probability of regressing from this region to previous words. In this sense, we could interpret these results as a wrap-up effect—that is, a processing cost associated with syntactic and semantic integration at the end of a clause or sentence [66]. In non-restrictive RCs, readers need to compute a semantic representation of the main clause (and, therefore, of the head noun) at R3, but in restrictive RCs, this process is done while reading the RC itself. In order to substantiate this account, we performed an additional analysis over the total reading times—that is, the sum of all the fixations on a region during both the first and second pass [56]—of R4, R5, and R6, which correspond to the whole structure of the RC. Linear mixed-effect models were built with the RC type, relativizer and their two-way interaction as fixed effects. Random slopes for the RC type and relativizer were also included by subject and by items. As the dependent variable, we analyzed the total reading time over R4, R5, and R6. Linear mixed-effect models were compared conducting likelihood-ratio tests with ANOVA in R, and the p-values were adjusted using the Holm–Bonferroni method (the tables in Section G of the Supplementary Materials show a full description of the models’ reports). Table 9 shows the mean reading times for the eye-tracking measure total reading time in the three contrasts.
As can be seen in Table 9, restrictive RCs exhibit longer reading times than non-restrictive RCs when considering all three regions of the RC. This effect was marginal for the contrasts que versus el cual, t = −2.3, p = 0.06, and significant for que versus quien, t = −4.19, p < 0.001; for the third contrast (que versus donde), restrictive RCs also show longer reading times than non-restrictive RCs, but this difference did not reach statistical significance (p > 0.1). These results suggest that restrictive RCs seem to be harder to process than non-restrictive RCs when reading the full RC, and this extra processing load could also result from a wrap-up effect—that is, readers were computing a syntactic and semantic representation of the restrictive RCs at this point. Obviously, it could be argued that the structure of non-restrictive RCs also needs to be computed at the very end of the RC (e.g., R6); however, and in contrast to restrictive RCs, a part of the structure of non-restrictive RCs has already been computed at R3, thus reducing its processing cost. Taken together, these results suggest that restrictive and non-restrictive RCs are somehow processed in a different way as readers seem to compute a syntactic and semantic structure of each type of RCs at different regions.
To understand this difference, we should also pay attention to the effect caused by punctuated words, which would lead us to reevaluate the parafoveal hypothesis examined and dismissed in the previous section. However, we will reexamine it now with regard to the role of RC types. From this point of view, the claim that the effect of RC type at R3 reflects parafoveal processing—i.e., that readers spend more time at R3 of non-restrictive RCs because they are also processing R4, though plausible, can be ruled out on several grounds. Firstly, parafoveal processing is normally observed in reading measures that report duration data (especially, in early reading measures), but in our study, this effect was significant in first-pass regression—that is, the probability of regressing backward before moving to the next region. Therefore, it does not seem reasonable to relate parafoveal processing of R4 to a measure associated with looking backwards to previous words. Secondly, if readers benefited more from parafoveal processing of R4 when fixating R3 in non-restrictive RCs, it would then be expected that R4 should exhibit shorter reading times for non-restrictive RCs in comparison to restrictive RCs. This effect was significant for the contrast que versus el cual, where R4 showed shorter reading times for non-restrictive versus restrictive RCs, but it did not yield significant differences for the other two contrasts (que versus quien, and que versus donde; see Section 4.2.4 and Section 4.2.5, respectively). If parafoveal processing were the core cause behind the effect of RC type at R3, then the same effect at R4 ought to show up in all contrasts, and not only in one of them. Finally, it could be questioned why readers benefited more from parafoveal processing in non-restrictive RCs than in restrictive RCs, since R3 in non-restrictive RCs always displays a final comma, and this punctuation mark might prevent, rather than promote, parafoveal processing.
On the other hand, in Section 2, we also considered the hypothesis that RC processing may be determined by the frequency rates of RC types. Our corpus study showed that non-restrictive RCs are more frequent than restrictive RCs for the contrasts que versus el cual and que versus quien; as for the contrast que versus donde, there are differences between the two relativizers: Restrictive RCs are more frequent than non-restrictive ones when headed by que, whereas non-restrictive RCs are more frequent than restrictive ones when headed by donde. The effect of RC type found at R3 cannot be explained by the frequency of RC types because at this point participants have not started reading the RC and therefore cannot know its type. On the contrary, the effect of RC type found at R4, R5, and R6 together for the total reading time could be related to frequency rates, at least for the contrasts que versus el cual and que versus quien, as restrictive RCs were less frequent, thus yielding longer reading times (see Table 9). As for que versus donde, there were no significant differences between the two RC types in any eye-tracking measure, which obviously raises the question why, for this contrast, the frequency rates do not seem to play a role. This latter result downplays the explanation of the effect of RC type being caused by structural frequency patterns.
In sum, our results show that there are processing differences between the two types of RCs: Non-restrictive RCs are harder to process at the head noun region (i.e., R3), while restrictive RCs exhibit a higher processing load at the RC itself (i.e., R4, R5, and R6), at least for two contrasts: que versus el cual and que versus quien. Similar results were reported in the study by Grodner et al. [64] described above (see Section 2.3), where a processing advantage for non-restrictive over restrictive RCs was found at the RC verb region when the sentences were presented in isolation, though this study did not test RCs with different relativizers but only the contrast between that (in restrictive RCs) versus who (in non-restrictive RCs), which somehow parallels the que versus quien contrast in Spanish. Our results could be explained by the fact that readers compute a representation of the sentence structure at different regions depending on RC type. In non-restrictive RCs, the comma placed after the head noun marks the end of the main clause and therefore the need to establish its syntactic and semantic representation at R3, which might explain the higher probability of making a regression from this region in non-restrictive versus restrictive RCs. On the contrary, the process of computing the structure of restrictive RCs seems to take place while reading the RC itself, yielding longer reading times for restrictive versus non-restrictive RCs at R4, R5, and R6 together. Accordingly, the effects of RC type found in our data seem to reflect where along the sentence readers compute a syntactic and semantic interpretation of the clause or sentence.
Obviously, this account is not incompatible with the punctuation hypothesis, according to which the presence of a comma after the head noun of non-restrictive RCs may involve an extra processing cost in comparison to the head noun of restrictive RCs. However, this second hypothesis would only explain the effect of RC type found at R3 for non-restrictive RCs, but it would leave unanswered why restrictive RCs exhibited longer reading times at regions 4, 5, and 6 together. In this regard, we consider that punctuation marks could elicit the identification process associated with non-restrictive RCs at R3, adding some processing load, but this hypothesis by itself cannot explain the effect of RC type found for both restrictive and non-restrictive RCs.
On the other hand, frequency rates could also explain the effect of RC type found at regions 4, 5, and 6 together, since as restrictive RCs are less frequent than non-restrictive RCs (at least for the contrasts que versus el cual and que versus quien), they become more difficult to process. Nevertheless, this explanation leaves unanswered why this effect does not arise in the contrast que versus donde (as restrictive and non-restrictive RCs also exhibit different frequency patterns in this contrast), as well as the effect of RC type found at R3.
Finally, a finding that is hard to accommodate by any of the previous explanations is that the effects of RC type reached statistical significance in different eye-tracking measures. At R3, this effect was significant for first-pass regression, whereas over the whole RC encompassing regions 4, 5, and 6, it was significant for total reading time.
Bringing together the relativizer and RC type effects, we will finally examine our results in the light of the hypotheses put forward by the parsing models reviewed in the opening section of this paper. First, a modular account such as the garden-path theory would have difficulties to account for most of the results reported. On the one hand, this theory would expect no differences in the early processing of RCs due to the heading relativizer and its lexico-semantic features. Therefore, this account cannot explain the effect of relativizer found at different regions. Our results showed differences in both early and intermediate eye-tracking measures, such that RCs introduced by a more frequent relativizer exhibited a lower processing load, both when activating the relativizer and when integrating it into the syntactic structure. In addition, the effect of relativizer at R5 for the contrasts que versus quien and que versus donde indicates that the semantic features of the relativizers—where they have—might facilitate sentence processing, so integrating the RC verb with previous constituents is less costly when this verb follows a relativizer that possesses semantic features. Similarly, the garden-path theory would allegedly predict more processing difficulties for non-restrictive RCs, as these RCs are attached to a higher structural level in comparison to restrictive RCs (see example 13 above). It is true that, according to our data, non-restrictive RCs were harder to process than restrictive RCs, but this effect was only significant for first-pass regression at R3, and at this point, participants cannot know they are reading a RC, much less its type. In addition, when considering the structure of the RC (i.e., R4, R5, and R6), the effect of RC type showed longer reading times for restrictive versus non-restrictive RCs, displaying the opposite pattern to the one predicted by the Garden-Path theory.
Alongside the garden-path theory, the Tuning hypothesis would also find it problematic to explain most of the effects of our study. On the one hand, the Tuning hypothesis accepts the influence of coarse-grained frequency at early processing stages, so that those structures that are more frequent would be easier to process. Our corpus study showed that non-restrictive RCs are more frequent than restrictive RCs for the contrasts que versus el cual and que versus quien and also for the contrast que versus donde when these sentences are headed by donde. In this regard, the Tuning hypothesis could accommodate the effect of RC type found at R4, R5, and R6, according to which restrictive RCs exhibited longer reading times; however, this account cannot explain why non-restrictive RCs were harder to process at R3. On the other hand, the Tuning hypothesis also leaves unanswered the effect of relativizer found at different regions and for different eye-tracking measures, as this model, similar to the garden-path theory, predicts no differences in sentence processing due to the lexical frequency and the semantic features of the words integrating it, at least at the early stages.
Finally, interactive accounts seem to accommodate most of the effects found in our study. First of all, the effect of relativizer can be interpreted as supporting interactive accounts and, particularly, usage-based accounts. These models claim that those distributional patterns that are more frequent in a language are easier to recognize and, in consequence, to process, which squares with our finding of shorter fixations in early measures for the contrasts que versus el cual and que versus quien and the fact that que is more frequent (both in absolute and relative terms) [52,67,68,69,70,71]. Similarly, the results at R5 for the contrasts que versus quien and que versus donde show that integrating the RC verb with previous constituents is less costly when this verb follows a relativizer that possesses semantic features. These results can be also better accommodated under usage-based accounts, as they claim that the parser records distributional patterns at any linguistic level, so that semantic features may also help to recover the pattern underlying a message. As our results show, semantic features related to function words such as relativizers can also influence the recognition of a pattern and, consequently, the way it is processed. However, we must stress at this point that our results do not support an unconstrained version of interactive models where all extra-syntactic information is brought to bear on the parsing process at once and from the very beginning, for according to our data, the constraints from other sources of information appear to be ranked along a timeline, with the influence of the relativizer frequency coming ahead of that of their semantic features.
Second, the effect of RC type may reflect where along the sentence readers begin to compute a syntactic and semantic interpretation of the sentence. For non-restrictive RCs, this process occurs first at R3—that is, at the end of the main clause, probably sparked by the comma at the end of this region, whereas, for restrictive RCs, it occurs while reading the structure of the RC (R4, R5, and R6). This does not mean that non-restrictive RCs are not computed when reading the structure of the RC (that is, R4, R5, and R6), but in comparison to restrictive RCs, this process seems to be less costly, as a part of the structure of non-restrictive RCs has already been established. In any case, these results suggest that readers are not only considering syntactic information when processing RCs but also their semantic representation, and particularly, this latter information seems to constrain the interpretation of the sentence by showing where certain identification operations need to be completed.

6. Conclusions

The aim of our study was to analyze RC processing by taking into account two particular characteristics of these structures in Spanish (and also in other languages): namely, that they can be headed by relativizers with different linguistic features and that they can be of two different types (restrictive or non-restrictive). These questions have been scarcely examined so far in the psycholinguistic literature, hence the exploratory nature of our study. Nevertheless, these questions can be of interest, among other reasons, because they help us to analyze whether non-syntactic information could determine or guide syntactic processing when manipulating certain features of RCs. We were particularly interested in analyzing the influence of two types of non-syntactic information: frequency and semantics. With this aim in mind, we compared, on the one hand, RCs introduced by different relative pronouns, which differ in lexical frequency and/or semantic features, though their variation does not substantially modify the sentence structure nor its general meaning. The second manipulation concerns RC type: restrictive and non-restrictive, as they play different semantic roles regarding the RC head noun and also exhibit different frequency rates.
In the first place, our results show that extra-syntactic information does play a role when computing structure building operations related to RCs. Particularly, the effect of relativizer shows that RC processing differed among conditions, being less costly when the introducing relativizer is more frequent, especially in the relativizer contrasts que versus el cual and que versus quien and, to a lesser extent, in the contrast que versus donde. This pattern was recorded in measures related to lexical activation [53,86,87], as well as related to syntactic integration [51,52,53]. Similarly, there were also differences in the processing of RCs due to the semantic features of the relativizers, such that integrating the RC verb into a sentence was less demanding when following a relativizer that possesses semantic features (quien versus que, and donde versus que). Additionally, the effect of RC type found in our data reflects processing difficulties at different regions for restrictive and non-restrictive RCs, which would be caused by the fact that readers are integrating different linguistic information in order to compute the sentence representation. Consequently, our results suggest that both frequency and semantics influence syntactic processing when comprehending RCs in Spanish.
Turning to the order in which this linguistic information becomes available, several of the effects found in our study became significant for early and intermediate eye-tracking measures—that is, before readers had computed the whole structure of the RC. In this regard, our results suggest that lexico-semantic information becomes available at an early processing stage and thus plays a relevant role in the decisions taken by the parser as the sentence unfolds.
Taken together, the results of our experiment seem to be more compatible with an interactive view of language processing [25,43,46,67,100,101], according to which, syntactic processing is not blind to non-syntactic information. On the contrary, non-syntactic information, such as lexical frequency and semantic features, seems to influence and guide syntactic structure building from the beginning and not only at the late stages. Therefore, these results agree with previous studies that have manipulated non-syntactic information related to content words in order to analyze their influence on syntactic processing [31,102,103,104,105]. Thus, the main contribution of our research is the finding that lexico-semantic features of function words are activated at the initial stages of processing and may guide parsing decisions from early on, an issue that has scarcely been investigated so far in the sentence processing literature. Lastly, although the research reported in this paper leaves a few questions open for further research, it provides the first evidence of the online processing of relative clauses based on relativizer and RC-type contrasts in a language with a rich paradigm of relative pronouns such as Spanish.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/brainsci13030409/s1.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, E.Á.-G. and J.M.I.G.; methodology, E.Á.-G. and J.M.I.G.; software, E.Á.-G.; validation, E.Á.-G. and J.M.I.G.; formal analysis, E.Á.-G.; investigation, E.Á.-G. and J.M.I.G.; resources, E.Á.-G. and J.M.I.G.; data curation, E.Á.-G.; writing—original draft preparation, E.Á.-G. and J.M.I.G.; writing—review and editing, E.Á.-G. and J.M.I.G.; visualization, E.Á.-G.; supervision, E.Á.-G. and J.M.I.G.; project administration, J.M.I.G.; funding acquisition, J.M.I.G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was partially funded by the Spanish Ministry of Science, Innovation and University, grant number PID2019-111198GB-I00.

Institutional Review Board Statement

At the time we administered the experiment reported in our article, the Autonomous University of Madrid did not issue any certificate of approval by the Ethics Committee. They only required a document of informed consent issued by the researcher and signed by him/her and the participant, in the case of experiments that did not use any invasive or otherwise cumbersome procedures for participants. The informed consent document included information about the experiment, and explicitly mentioned that the undersigned was fully aware of the kind of research in which he/she agreed to participate and of its procedures, and guaranteed to safeguard the anonymity of the participant, as well as his/her right to abandon the experiment at any time.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, E.Á.-G., upon request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Van Gompel, R.; Pickering, M.J. Syntactic parsing. In The Oxford Handbook of Psycholinguistics; Gaskell, M.G., Ed.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2007; pp. 289–307. ISBN 9780198568971. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bornkessel, I.; Schlesewsky, M. The extended argument dependency model: A neurocognitive approach to sentence comprehension across languages. Psychol. Rev. 2006, 113, 787–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. MacDonald, M.C.; Pearlmutter, N.J.; Seidenberg, M.S. Lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychol. Rev. 1994, 101, 676–703. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Altmann, G.T.M.; Mirković, J. Incrementality and prediction in human sentence processing. Cogn. Sci. 2009, 33, 583–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Swinney, D.; Love, T. The Processing of Discontinuous Dependencies in Language and Music. Music Percept. Interdiscip. J. 1998, 16, 63–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Townsend, D.J.; Bever, T.G. Sentence Comprehension: The Integration of Habits and Rules; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2001; ISBN 9780262316156. [Google Scholar]
  7. Ferreira, F.; Lowder, M.W. Prediction, information, structure, and Good-Enough language processing. Psychol. Learn. Motiv. 2016, 65, 217–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Trueswell, J.C.; Tanenhaus, M.K.; Garnsey, S.M. Semantic influences on parsing: Use of thematic role information in syntactic ambiguity resolution. J. Mem. Lang. 1994, 33, 285–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Cuetos, F.; Mitchell, D.C. Cross-linguistic differences in parsing: Restrictions on the use of the late closure strategy in Spanish. Cognition 1988, 30, 73–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Frazier, L. Parsing modifiers: Special purpose routines in the human sentence processing mechanism? In Comprehension Processes in Reading; Balota, D.A., Flores d’Arcais, G.B., Rayner, K., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 1990; pp. 303–330. ISBN 0-8058-0653-9. [Google Scholar]
  11. Carreiras, M.; Clifton, C. Relative clause interpretation preferences in Spanish and English. Lang. Speech 1993, 36, 353–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Carreiras, M.; Clifton, C. Another word on parsing relative clauses: Eyetracking evidence from Spanish and English. Mem. Cogn. 1999, 27, 826–833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Dussias, P.E. Sentence parsing in fluent Spanish-English bilinguals. In One Mind, Two Languages: Bilingual Language Processing; Nicol, J.L., Ed.; Blackwell: Oxford, UK, 2001; pp. 159–176. ISBN 978-0-631-22098-5. [Google Scholar]
  14. Frazier, L. Sentence processing: A tutorial review. In Attention and Performance, XII: The Psychology of Reading; Colheart, M., Ed.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: London, UK, 1987; pp. 559–586. ISBN 9781138641556. [Google Scholar]
  15. Frazier, L.; Rayner, K. Resolution of syntactic category ambiguities: Eye movements in parsing lexically ambiguous sentences. J. Mem. Lang. 1987, 26, 505–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Frazier, L. On Comprehending Sentences: Syntactic Parsing Strategies. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
  17. Ehrlich, K.; Fernández, E.; Fodor, J.D.; Stenshoel, E.; Vinereanu, M. Low attachment of relative clauses: New data from Swedish, Norwegian and Romanian. In Proceedings of the 12th CUNY Annual Conference on Human Sentence Processing, New York, NY, USA, 18–20 March 1999. [Google Scholar]
  18. Shen, X. Late Assignment of Syntax Theory: Evidence from Chinese and English. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Exeter, Exeter, UK, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  19. Igoa, J.M.; Carreiras, M.; Meseguer, E. A study on late closure in Spanish: Principle-grounded vs. frequency-based accounts of attachment preferences. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1998, 51, 561–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Carreiras, M.; Betancort, M.; Meseguer, E. Relative clause attachment in Spanish: Do readers use different strategies when disambiguating by gender and number. In Proceedings of the 14th CUNY Annual Conference for Human Sentence Processing, Philadelphia, PA, USA, 15–17 March 2001. [Google Scholar]
  21. Brysbaert, M.; Mitchell, D.C. Modifier attachment in sentence parsing: Evidence from Dutch. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 1996, 49, 664–695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Hemforth, B.; Konieczny, L.; Scheepers, C.; Strube, G. Syntactic ambiguity resolution in German. In Sentence Processing: A Crosslinguistic Perspective; Syntax and Semantics; Hillert, D., Ed.; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 1998; Volume 31, pp. 293–312. [Google Scholar]
  23. Gilboy, E.; Sopena, J.M.; Clifton, C.; Frazier, L. Argument structure and association preferences in Spanish and English complex NPs. Cognition 1995, 54, 131–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Maynell, L.A. Effect of Pitch Accent Placement on Resolving Relative Clause Ambiguity in English. In Proceedings of the 12th Annual CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, New York, NY, USA, 18–20 March 1999. [Google Scholar]
  25. Gennari, S.P.; MacDonald, M.C. Linking production and comprehension processes: The case of relative clauses. Cognition 2009, 111, 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Mecklinger, A.; Schriefers, H.; Steinhauer, K.; Friederici, A.D. Processing relative clauses varying on syntactic and semantic dimensions: An analysis with event-related potentials. Mem. Cogn. 1995, 23, 477–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Holmes, V.M.; O’Regan, J.K. Eye fixation patterns during the reading of relative-clause sentences. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 1981, 20, 417–430. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Ueno, M.; Garnsey, S.M. An ERP study of the processing of subject and object relative clauses in Japanese. Lang. Cogn. Process. 2008, 23, 646–688. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. King, J.; Just, M.A. Individual differences in syntactic processing: The role of working memory. J. Mem. Lang. 1991, 30, 580–602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Traxler, M.J.; Morris, R.K.; Seely, R.E. Processing subject and object relative clauses: Evidence from eye movements. J. Mem. Lang. 2002, 47, 69–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Mak, W.M.; Vonk, W.; Schriefers, H. Animacy in processing relative clauses: The hikers that rocks crush. J. Mem. Lang. 2006, 54, 466–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Weckerly, J.; Kutas, M. An electrophysiological analysis of animacy effects in the processing of object relative sentences. Psychophysiology 1999, 36, 559–570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Caplan, D.; Vijayan, S.; Kuperberg, G.; West, C.; Waters, G.; Greve, D.; Dale, A.M. Vascular responses to syntactic processing: Event-related fMRI study of relative clauses. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2002, 15, 26–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Keenan, E.L.; Comrie, B. Noun phrase accessibility and universal grammar. Linguist. Inq. 1977, 8, 63–99. [Google Scholar]
  35. Frazier, L.; Clifton, C. Successive cyclicity in the grammar and the parser. Lang. Cogn. Process. 1989, 4, 93–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Gibson, E. The dependency locality theory: A distance-based theory of linguistic complexity. In Image, Language, Brain: Papers from the First Mind Articulation Project Symposium, Tokyo, Japan, 16–17 November 1998; Marantz, A., Miyashita, Y., O’Neil, W., Eds.; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2000; pp. 94–126. [Google Scholar]
  37. Carreiras, M.; Duñabeitia, J.A.; Vergara, M.; de la Cruz-Pavía, I.; Laka, I. Subject relative clauses are not universally easier to process: Evidence from Basque. Cognition 2010, 115, 79–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Hsiao, F.; Gibson, E. Processing relative clauses in Chinese. Cognition 2003, 90, 3–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Gibson, E.; Wu, H.H. Processing Chinese relative clauses in context. Lang. Cogn. Process. 2013, 28, 125–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Ishizuka, T.; Nakatani, K.; Gibson, E. Relative clause extraction complexity in Japanese. In Proceedings of the 16th CUNY Annual Conference on Human Sentence Processing, Cambridge, MA, USA, 27–29 March 2003. [Google Scholar]
  41. Traxler, M.J.; Williams, R.S.; Blozis, S.A.; Morris, R.K. Working memory, animacy, and verb class in the processing of relative clauses. J. Mem. Lang. 2005, 53, 204–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Mak, W.M.; Vonk, W.; Schriefers, H. The influence of animacy on relative clause processing. J. Mem. Lang. 2002, 47, 50–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Kidd, E.; Brandt, S.; Lieven, E.; Tomasello, M. Object relative made easy: A cross-linguistic comparison of the constraints influencing young children’s processing of relative clauses. Lang. Cogn. Process. 2007, 22, 860–897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Gennari, S.P.; Mirković, J.; Macdonald, M.C. Animacy and competition in relative clause production: A cross-linguistic investigation. Cogn. Psychol. 2012, 65, 141–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Gordon, P.C.; Hendrick, R.; Johnson, M.; Lee, Y. Similarity-based interference during language comprehension: Evidence from eye-tracking during reading. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Lang. 2006, 32, 1304–1321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Reali, F.; Christiansen, M.H. Processing of relative clauses is made easier by frequency of occurrence. J. Mem. Lang. 2007, 57, 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Betancort, M.; Carreiras, M.; Sturt, P. The processing of subject and object relative clauses in Spanish: An eye-tracking study. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2009, 62, 1915–1929. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Huddleston, R.; Pullum, G. The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2002; ISBN 978-0521431460. [Google Scholar]
  49. Real Academia Española y Asociación de Academias de la Lengua Española [RAE-ASALE]. Nueva Gramática de la Lengua Española; Espasa: Madrid, Spain, 2009; ISBN 978-84-670-3245-1. [Google Scholar]
  50. Brucart, J.M. La estructura del sintagma nominal: Las oraciones de relativo. In Gramática Descriptiva de la Lengua Española; Bosque, I., Demonte, V., Eds.; Espasa-Calpe: Madrid, Spain, 1999; Volume 1, pp. 395–522. ISBN 84-239-7918-0. [Google Scholar]
  51. Tabor, W.; Juliano, C.; Tanenhaus, M.K. Parsing in a dynamical system: An attractor-based account of the interaction of lexical and structural constraints in sentence processing. Lang. Cogn. Process. 1997, 12, 211–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Gibson, E. The interaction of top-down and bottom-up statistics in the resolution of syntactic category ambiguity. J. Mem. Lang. 2006, 54, 363–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Schmauder, A.R.; Morris, R.K.; Poynor, D.V. Lexical processing and text integration of function and content words: Evidence from priming and eye fixations. Mem. Cogn. 2000, 28, 1098–1108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Rayner, K.; Sereno, S.C.; Morris, R.K.; Schmauder, A.R.; Clifton, C. Eye movements and on-line language comprehension processes. Lang. Cogn. Process. 1989, 4, 21–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Rayner, K. Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychol. Bull. 1998, 124, 372–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Clifton, C.; Staub, A.; Rayner, K. Eye movements in reading words and sentences. In Eye Movements: A Window on Mind and Brain; van Gompel, R., Fisher, M., Murray, W., Hill, R.L., Eds.; Elsevier Science: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2007; pp. 341–371. ISBN 978-0-08-044980-7. [Google Scholar]
  57. Pickering, M.J.; Traxler, M.J.; Crocker, M.W. Ambiguity resolution in sentence processing: Evidence against frequency-based accounts. J. Mem. Lang. 2000, 43, 447–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Rayner, K.; Duffy, S.A. Lexical complexity and fixation times in reading: Effects of word frequency, verb complexity, and lexical ambiguity. Mem. Cogn. 1986, 14, 191–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  59. Fernández, E.M. Bilingual Sentence Processing; Relative Clause Attachment in English and Spanish; John Benjamins Publishing Company: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2003; ISBN 10: 9027224986. [Google Scholar]
  60. Fabb, N. The difference between English restrictive and non-restrictive relative clauses. J. Linguist. 1990, 26, 57–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Arnold, D. Non-restrictive relatives are not orphans. J. Linguist. 2007, 43, 272–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Álvarez García, E. Clasificación de las oraciones de relativo: Límites, soluciones y nueva propuesta. Nueva Rev. Filol. Hispánica 2021, 69, 499–540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Quilis, A. Fonética Acústica de la Lengua Española; Gredos: Madrid, Spain, 1981; ISBN 9788424901318. [Google Scholar]
  64. Grodner, D.; Gibson, E.; Watson, D. The influence of contextual contrast on syntactic processing: Evidence for strong-interaction in sentence comprehension. Cognition 2005, 95, 275–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Hill, R.L.; Murray, W.S. Commas and spaces: Effects of punctuation on eye movements and sentence parsing. In Reading as a Perceptual Process; Kennedy, A., Radach, R., Heller, D., Pynte, J., Eds.; North-Holland/Elsevier Science Publishers: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2000; pp. 565–589. ISBN 978-0-08-043642-5. [Google Scholar]
  66. Hirotani, M.; Frazier, L.; Rayner, K. Punctuation and intonation effects on clause ad sentence wrap-up: Evidence from eye-movements. J. Mem. Lang. 2006, 54, 425–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. MacDonald, M.C.; Seidenberg, M.S. Constraint satisfaction accounts of lexical and sentence comprehension. In Handbook of Psycholinguistics, 2nd ed.; Traxler, M.J., Gernsbacher, M.A., Eds.; Elsevier/Academic Press: London, UK, 2006; pp. 581–611. ISBN 9780123693747. [Google Scholar]
  68. MacDonald, M.C.; Thornton, R. When language comprehension reflects production constraints: Resolving ambiguities with the help of past experiences. Mem. Cogn. 2009, 37, 1177–1186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Wells, J.B.; Christiansen, M.H.; Race, D.S.; Acheson, D.J.; MacDonald, M.C. Experience and sentence processing: Statistical learning and relative clause comprehension. Cogn. Psychol. 2009, 58, 250–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Reali, F. Frequency affects object relative clause processing: Some evidence in favor of usage-based accounts. Lang. Learn. 2014, 64, 685–714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Hsiao, Y.; MacDonald, M.C. Production predicts comprehension: Animacy effects in Mandarin relative clause processing. J. Mem. Lang. 2016, 89, 87–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Lope Blanch, J.M. Nuevos Estudios de Lingüística Hispánica; Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México: México City, México, 1993; ISBN 9789683633217. [Google Scholar]
  73. Herrera Santana, J.L. Estudio Sociolingüístico de los Relativos en el Español de Santa Cruz de Tenerife; Servicio de Publicaciones de la Universidad de La Laguna: San Cristóbal de la Laguna, Spain, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  74. Santana Marrero, J. Los nexos relativos en la norma lingüística culta de Las Palmas de Gran Canaria. In Estudios Sobre el Español de Canarias; Díaz, C., Morera, M., Ortega, G., Eds.; Academia Canaria de la Lengua: Islas Canarias, Spain, 2003; pp. 477–493. ISBN 978-8-4960-5910-8. [Google Scholar]
  75. Navarro, M. La frecuencia de los relativos en el habla de Valencia (Venezuela). Boletín De Lingüística 2006, 18, 66–99. [Google Scholar]
  76. Álvarez García, E. Métodos estadísticos en lingüística. In El Legado Hispánico: Manifestaciones Culturales y sus Protagonistas; Lobato, A., de los Reyes, E., Pereira, I., García, C., Eds.; Servicio de Publicaciones de la Universidad de León: León, Spain, 2017; Volume 2, pp. 159–173. ISBN 978-84-9773-869-9. [Google Scholar]
  77. Zeileis, A.; Kleiber, C.; Jackman, S. Regression models for count data in R. J. Stat. Softw. 2008, 27, 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  78. von der Malsburg, T.; Angele, B. False positives and other statistical errors in standard analyses of eye movements in reading. J. Mem. Lang. 2017, 94, 119–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  79. Rayner, K.; Carlson, M.; Frazier, L. The interaction of syntax and semantics during sentence processing: Eye movements in the analysis of semantically biased sentences. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 1983, 22, 358–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Ferreira, F.; Clifton, C. The independence of syntactic processing. J. Mem. Lang. 1986, 25, 348–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Garnsey, S.M.; Pearlmutter, N.J.; Myers, E.; Lotocky, M.A. The contributions of verb bias and plausibility to the comprehension of temporarily ambiguous sentences. J. Mem. Lang. 1997, 37, 58–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Boland, J.E.; Blodgett, A. Understanding the constraints on syntactic generation: Lexical bias and discourse congruency effects on eye movements. J. Mem. Lang. 2001, 45, 391–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  83. Jaeger, T.F. Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. J. Mem. Lang. 2008, 59, 434–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  84. Davidson, D.J.; Martin, A.E. Modeling accuracy as a function of response time with the generalized linear mixed effects model. Acta Psychol. 2013, 144, 83–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  85. Bates, D.; Mächler, M.; Bolker, B.; Walker, S. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using lme4. J. Stat. Softw. 2015, 67, 1–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Just, M.A.; Carpenter, P.A. A theory of reading: From eye fixations to comprehension. Psychol. Rev. 1980, 87, 329–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. White, S.J.; Drieghe, D.; Liversedge, S.P.; Staub, A. The word frequency effect during sentence reading: A linear or nonlinear effect of log frequency? Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2018, 71, 46–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  88. Balota, D.A.; Pollatsek, A.; Rayner, K. The interaction of contextual constraints and parafoveal visual information in reading. Cogn. Psychol. 1985, 17, 364–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  89. Inhoff, A.W.; Rayner, K. Parafoveal word processing during eye fixations in reading: Effects of word frequency. Percept. Psychophys. 1986, 40, 431–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  90. Angele, B.; Slattery, T.J.; Chaloukian, T.L.; Schotter, E.R.; Rayner, K. Dissociating effects of parafoveal preprocessing from effects of the sentence context. In Proceedings of the 24th CUNY Annual Conference on Human Sentence Processing, Palo Alto, CA, USA, 24–26 March 2011. [Google Scholar]
  91. Carpenter, P.A.; Just, M.A. What your eyes do while your mind is reading. In Eye Movements in Reading: Perceptual and Language Processes; Rayner, K., Ed.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1983; pp. 275–307. ISBN 9780323146289. [Google Scholar]
  92. Henderson, J.M.; Ferreira, F. Eye movement control during reading: Fixation measures reflect foveal but not parafoveal processing difficulty. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 1993, 47, 201–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  93. Inhoff, A.W.; Starr, M.; Shindler, K.L. Is the processing of words during eye fixations in reading strictly serial? Percept. Psychophys. 2000, 62, 1474–1484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  94. Calvo, M.G.; Meseguer, E. Eye movements and processing stages in reading: Relative contribution of visual, lexical, and contextual factors. Span. J. Psychol. 2002, 5, 66–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  95. Angele, B.; Schotter, E.R.; Slattery, T.J.; Tenenbaum, T.L.; Bicknell, K.; Rayner, K. Do successor effects in reading reflect lexical parafoveal processing? Evidence from corpus-based and experimental eye movement data. J. Mem. Lang. 2015, 79–80, 76–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  96. Brothers, T.; Hoversten, L.J.; Traxler, M.J. Looking back on reading ahead. J. Mem. Lang. 2017, 96, 9–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Inhoff, A.W. Two stages of word processing during eye fixations in the reading of prose. J. Verbal Learn. Verbal Behav. 1984, 23, 612–624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. McElree, B.; Foraker, S.; Dyer, L. Memory structures that subserve sentence comprehension. J. Mem. Lang. 2003, 48, 67–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Pynte, J.; Kennedy, A. The influence of punctuation and word class on distributed processing in normal reading. Vis. Res. 2007, 47, 1215–1227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. MacDonald, M.C.; Christiansen, M.H. Reassessing working memory: Comment on Just and Carpenter (1992) and Waters and Caplan (1996). Psychol. Rev. 2002, 109, 35–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. MacDonald, M.C. How language production shapes language form and comprehension. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  102. MacDonald, M.C. Probabilistic constraints and syntactic ambiguity resolution. Lang. Cogn. Process. 1994, 9, 157–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Trueswell, J.C. The role of lexical frequency in syntactic ambiguity resolution. J. Mem. Lang. 1996, 35, 566–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  104. Johnson, M.L.; Lowder, M.W.; Gordon, P.C. The sentence-composition effect: Processing of complex sentences depends on the configuration of common noun phrase versus unusual noun phrases. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2011, 140, 707–724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  105. Fedorenko, E.; Piantadosi, S.T.; Gibson, E. The interaction of syntactic and lexical information sources in language processing: The case of the noun-verb ambiguity. J. Cogn. Sci. 2012, 13, 211–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Pairwise comparisons of the relative frequency of the Spanish relativizers que, el cual, quien, and donde in restrictive and non-restrictive RCs in Spanish corpora.
Figure 1. Pairwise comparisons of the relative frequency of the Spanish relativizers que, el cual, quien, and donde in restrictive and non-restrictive RCs in Spanish corpora.
Brainsci 13 00409 g001
Figure 2. Reading time in milliseconds for first-pass duration at regions 3, 4, and 5 for the contrast que versus el cual in restrictive and non-restrictive RCs. Asterisks indicate that an effect is significant at the following alpha levels: * = p < 0.05; *** = p < 0.001.
Figure 2. Reading time in milliseconds for first-pass duration at regions 3, 4, and 5 for the contrast que versus el cual in restrictive and non-restrictive RCs. Asterisks indicate that an effect is significant at the following alpha levels: * = p < 0.05; *** = p < 0.001.
Brainsci 13 00409 g002
Figure 3. Reading time in milliseconds for quasi-first-pass reading times at regions 3, 4, and 5 for the contrast que versus el cual, in restrictive and non-restrictive RCs. Asterisks indicate that an effect is significant at the following alpha levels: ** = p < 0.01; *** = p < 0.001.
Figure 3. Reading time in milliseconds for quasi-first-pass reading times at regions 3, 4, and 5 for the contrast que versus el cual, in restrictive and non-restrictive RCs. Asterisks indicate that an effect is significant at the following alpha levels: ** = p < 0.01; *** = p < 0.001.
Brainsci 13 00409 g003
Figure 4. Probability of regression during the first-pass at regions 3, 4, and 5 for the contrast que versus el cual, in restrictive and non-restrictive RCs. Asterisks indicate that an effect is significant at the following alpha levels: * = p < 0.05; ** = p < 0.01.
Figure 4. Probability of regression during the first-pass at regions 3, 4, and 5 for the contrast que versus el cual, in restrictive and non-restrictive RCs. Asterisks indicate that an effect is significant at the following alpha levels: * = p < 0.05; ** = p < 0.01.
Brainsci 13 00409 g004
Figure 5. Reading time in milliseconds for first-fixation duration at regions 3, 4, and 5 for the contrast que versus quien in restrictive and non-restrictive RCs. Asterisks indicate that an effect is significant at p < 0.001.
Figure 5. Reading time in milliseconds for first-fixation duration at regions 3, 4, and 5 for the contrast que versus quien in restrictive and non-restrictive RCs. Asterisks indicate that an effect is significant at p < 0.001.
Brainsci 13 00409 g005
Figure 6. Reading time in milliseconds for quasi-first-pass reading times at regions 3, 4, and 5 for the contrast que versus quien in restrictive and non-restrictive RCs. Asterisks indicate that an effect is significant at p < 0.001.
Figure 6. Reading time in milliseconds for quasi-first-pass reading times at regions 3, 4, and 5 for the contrast que versus quien in restrictive and non-restrictive RCs. Asterisks indicate that an effect is significant at p < 0.001.
Brainsci 13 00409 g006
Figure 7. Probability of regression during the first-pass at regions 3, 4, and 5 for the contrast que versus quien in restrictive and non-restrictive RCs. Asterisks indicate that an effect is significant at p < 0.001.
Figure 7. Probability of regression during the first-pass at regions 3, 4, and 5 for the contrast que versus quien in restrictive and non-restrictive RCs. Asterisks indicate that an effect is significant at p < 0.001.
Brainsci 13 00409 g007
Figure 8. Reading time in milliseconds for the first-pass duration at regions 3, 4, and 5 for the contrast que versus donde in restrictive and non-restrictive RCs.
Figure 8. Reading time in milliseconds for the first-pass duration at regions 3, 4, and 5 for the contrast que versus donde in restrictive and non-restrictive RCs.
Brainsci 13 00409 g008
Figure 9. Reading time in milliseconds for the quasi-first-pass reading time at regions 3, 4, and 5 for the contrast of que versus donde in restrictive and non-restrictive RCs. Asterisks indicate that an effect is significant at p < 0.01.
Figure 9. Reading time in milliseconds for the quasi-first-pass reading time at regions 3, 4, and 5 for the contrast of que versus donde in restrictive and non-restrictive RCs. Asterisks indicate that an effect is significant at p < 0.01.
Brainsci 13 00409 g009
Figure 10. Probability of regression during the first-pass at regions 3, 4, and 5 for the contrast que versus donde in restrictive and non-restrictive RCs. Asterisks indicate that an effect is significant at p < 0.01.
Figure 10. Probability of regression during the first-pass at regions 3, 4, and 5 for the contrast que versus donde in restrictive and non-restrictive RCs. Asterisks indicate that an effect is significant at p < 0.01.
Brainsci 13 00409 g010
Table 1. Corpus study data by linguistic level. Relative frequency appears in parenthesis. For the total number of relativizers, the relative frequency was calculated over total number of words; for the total number of relativizers in variation contexts, the relative frequency was calculated over the total number of relativizers.
Table 1. Corpus study data by linguistic level. Relative frequency appears in parenthesis. For the total number of relativizers, the relative frequency was calculated over total number of words; for the total number of relativizers in variation contexts, the relative frequency was calculated over the total number of relativizers.
Linguistic VarietyTotal Number of WordsTotal Number of RelativizersTotal Number of Relativizers in Variation ContextsCorpus
Oral formal33,3191905 (5.72%)538 (28.24%)CREA
Oral informal33,3891890 (5.66%)391 (20.69%)Val.Es.Co and COSER
Written formal34,8621812 (5.2%)790 (43.6%)CREA
Written informal32,4481771 (5.46%)653 (36.87%)Blogs
Table 2. Absolute and relative frequencies (in parentheses) of relativizers in restrictive and non-restrictive RCs according to the three contrasts of interest: que versus el cual, que versus quien, and que versus donde. Relative frequency was calculated over the total number of relativizers in the contexts where two of them can vary.
Table 2. Absolute and relative frequencies (in parentheses) of relativizers in restrictive and non-restrictive RCs according to the three contrasts of interest: que versus el cual, que versus quien, and que versus donde. Relative frequency was calculated over the total number of relativizers in the contexts where two of them can vary.
ContrastRelativizerRestrictive RCNon-Restrictive RC
que versus el cualque494 (25.89%)1333 (69.86%)
cual34 (1.78%)47 (2.46%)
que versus quienque13 (2.84%)374 (81.84%)
quien8 (1.75%)62 (13.57%)
que versus dondeque133 (28.85%)62 (13.45%)
donde120 (26.03%)146 (31.67%)
Table 3. Linguistic features of Spanish relativizers according to our corpus study.
Table 3. Linguistic features of Spanish relativizers according to our corpus study.
ContrastRelativizerLexical FrequencySemantic Feature
que versus el cualqueHigherNo
el cualLowerNo
que versus quienqueHigherNo
quienLowerYes
que versus dondequeHigher in restrictive RCsNo
dondeHigher in non-restrictive RCsYes
Table 4. Division of the experimental items into six regions according to the syntactic structure. “Res” refers to restrictive RCs and “Non” to non-restrictive RCs.
Table 4. Division of the experimental items into six regions according to the syntactic structure. “Res” refers to restrictive RCs and “Non” to non-restrictive RCs.
ContrastConditionRegion 1Region 2Region 3Region 4Region 5Region 6
que versus el cualRes_QueMi madreperdióel bolsoen el queguardabalas llaves del coche.
Res_CualMi madreperdióel bolsoen el cualguardabalas llaves del coche.
Non_QueMi madreperdiósu bolsoen el queguardabalas llaves del coche.
Non_CualMi madreperdiósu bolsoen el cualguardabalas llaves del coche.
que versus quienRes_QueEl entrenadorse enfadócon el jugadoral quefichóal final de la temporada.
Res_QuienEl entrenadorse enfadócon el jugadora quienfichóal final de la temporada.
Non_QueEl entrenadorse enfadócon su jugadoral quefichóal final de la temporada.
Non_QuienEl entrenadorse enfadócon su jugadora quienfichóal final de la temporada.
que versus dondeRes_QueLa policíaregistróel barriodel queprocedenlos muchachos desaparecidos.
Res_DondeLa policíaregistróel barriode dondeprocedenlos muchachos desaparecidos.
Non_QueLa policíaregistrómi barriodel queprocedenlos muchachos desaparecidos.
Non_DondeLa policíaregistrómi barriode dondeprocedenlos muchachos desaparecidos.
Table 5. Mean accuracy rates (%) and SD (in parentheses) to comprehension questions by contrast and condition. “Res” refers to restrictive RCs and “Non-Res” to non-restrictive RCs. Similarly, “Other” refers to the relativizer that is compared with que in each contrast: El cual, quien, or donde.
Table 5. Mean accuracy rates (%) and SD (in parentheses) to comprehension questions by contrast and condition. “Res” refers to restrictive RCs and “Non-Res” to non-restrictive RCs. Similarly, “Other” refers to the relativizer that is compared with que in each contrast: El cual, quien, or donde.
ContrastRes_QueRes_OtherNon-Res_QueNon-Res_Other
que versus el cual97.03 (6.85)95.24 (9.19)96.58 (7.40)97.32 (6.33)
que versus quien96.58 (6.79)94.79 (8.06)95.54 (7.56)95.39 (7.61)
que versus donde96.73 (6.71)94.94 (9.03)94.79 (8.32)96.58 (7.10)
Table 6. Estimated values from the linear mixed-effect models and mixed-effect logistic regression for the contrast que versus el cual at R3, R4, and R5. Eye movement measures are first-fixation rate (FFR), first fixation (FF), first-pass duration (FPD), first-pass regression (FPR), quasi-first-pass reading time (QFP), and second-pass duration (SPD). t-values refer to reading time measures, and z-values refer to probability measures.
Table 6. Estimated values from the linear mixed-effect models and mixed-effect logistic regression for the contrast que versus el cual at R3, R4, and R5. Eye movement measures are first-fixation rate (FFR), first fixation (FF), first-pass duration (FPD), first-pass regression (FPR), quasi-first-pass reading time (QFP), and second-pass duration (SPD). t-values refer to reading time measures, and z-values refer to probability measures.
RegionMeasureFactorEstimated βSDt/zp
R3FFRIntercept3.130.319.80<0.001
RC type−0.300.28−1.070.84
Relativizer−0.050.27−0.211.00
RC type x relativizer0.140.300.451.00
FFIntercept242.6810.90622.25<0.001
RC type4.017.5680.531.00
Relativizer0.996.7590.141.00
RC type x relativizer−1.309.426−0.131.00
FPDIntercept270.7813.2920.36<0.001
RC type1.309.690.131.00
Relativizer3.198.410.371.00
RC type x relativizer−2.2211.42−0.191.00
FPRIntercept−2.070.197−10.52<0.001
RC type0.500.172.880.01
Relativizer0.220.171.270.4
RC type x relativizer−0.060.21−0.30.76
QFPIntercept285.7615.4518.48<0.001
RC type−3.5110.98−0.321.00
Relativizer4.389.610.451.00
RC type x relativizer0.9212.490.071.00
SPDIntercept55.578.916.23<0.001
RC type17.5915.151.160.73
Relativizer11.8414.840.790.73
RC type x relativizer19.3417.801.080.73
R4FFRIntercept3.460.3110.89<0.001
RC type−0.720.27−2.580.009
Relativizer−1.030.26−3.94<0.001
RC type x relativizer1.080.293.72<0.001
FFIntercept281.1013.1421.39<0.001
RC type−15.257.94−1.920.11
Relativizer−51.648.48−6.08<0.001
RC type x relativizer14.389.671.480.13
FPDIntercept299.5214.6520.44<0.001
RC type−22.908.73−2.620.017
Relativizer−61.689.22−6.68<0.001
RC type x relativizer19.4010.461.850.06
FPRIntercept−1.690.19−8.83<0.001
RC type−0.650.20−3.100.003
Relativizer−0.600.18−3.260.003
RC type x relativizer0.450.241.860.06
QFPIntercept309.7516.1419.19<0.001
RC type−26.839.05−2.960.006
Relativizer−68.069.72−6.99<0.001
RC type x relativizer25.2810.912.310.02
SPDIntercept71.1511.016.46<0.001
RC type−24.999.58−2.600.01
Relativizer−32.4810.08−3.220.003
RC type x relativizer21.8911.701.870.06
R5FFRIntercept2.130.229.66<0.001
RC type0.160.210.780.79
Relativizer−0.240.20−1.170.71
RC type x relativizer−0.190.23−0.840.79
FFIntercept184.158.6321.32<0.001
RC type−2.385.70−0.411.00
Relativizer−4.505.19−0.861.00
RC type x relativizer−6.296.81−0.921.00
FPDIntercept225.7414.9715.07<0.001
RC type−2.458.60−0.280.88
Relativizer6.448.340.770.88
RC type x relativizer−17.9610.35−1.730.24
FPRIntercept−2.470.21−11.26<0.001
RC type−0.540.24−2.280.06
Relativizer−0.050.22−0.220.82
RC type x relativizer0.400.291.370.33
QFPIntercept242.6917.5213.84<0.001
RC type−8.689.66−0.891.00
Relativizer−0.069.10−0.0071.00
RC type x relativizer−9.8811.60−0.851.00
SPDIntercept66.1410.846.10<0.001
RC type−28.8812.33−2.340.05
Relativizer−22.0712.12−1.820.13
RC type x relativizer27.5515.861.730.13
Table 7. Estimated values from the linear mixed-effect models and mixed-effect logistic regression for the contrast que versus quien at R3, R4, and R5. Eye movement measures are first-fixation rate (FFR), first fixation (FF), first-pass duration (FPD), first-pass regression (FPR), quasi-first-pass reading time (QFP), and second-pass duration (SPD). t-values refer to reading time measures, and z-values refer to probability measures.
Table 7. Estimated values from the linear mixed-effect models and mixed-effect logistic regression for the contrast que versus quien at R3, R4, and R5. Eye movement measures are first-fixation rate (FFR), first fixation (FF), first-pass duration (FPD), first-pass regression (FPR), quasi-first-pass reading time (QFP), and second-pass duration (SPD). t-values refer to reading time measures, and z-values refer to probability measures.
RegionMeasureFactorEstimated βSDt/zp
R3FFRIntercept28.40.2311.92<0.001
RC type0.470.291.600.32
Relativizer−0.0040.26−0.010.98
RC type x relativizer0.490.34−1.430.32
FFIntercept264.768.8829.81<0.001
RC type12.037.821.530.34
Relativizer−12.437.88−1.570.34
RC type x relativizer6.9710.470.660.50
FPDIntercept297.7711.0726.88<0.001
RC type9.449.470.990.63
Relativizer−17.959.53−1.880.17
RC type x relativizer12.2012.400.980.63
FPRIntercept−1.900.17−10.78<0.001
RC type0.650.183.64<0.001
Relativizer−0.020.17−0.130.89
RC type x relativizer0.160.200.810.83
QFPIntercept308.3112.3724.91<0.001
RC type16.2910.121.600.32
Relativizer−11.7110.00−1.170.48
RC type x relativizer6.4613.250.480.62
SPDIntercept62.349.156.81<0.001
RC type49.1418.292.680.02
Relativizer9.0514.840.611.00
RC type x relativizer1.1118.300.061.00
R4FFRIntercept0.950.204.66<0.001
RC type0.140.140.970.33
Relativizer1.150.176.40<0.001
RC type x relativizer−0.330.21−1.600.21
FFIntercept156.4710.2915.19<0.001
RC type2.716.610.410.68
Relativizer50.816.517.80<0.001
RC type x relativizer−8.278.11−1.010.61
FPDIntercept168.6511.7114.39<0.001
RC type−0.797.41−0.100.91
Relativizer57.687.128.09<0.001
RC type x relativizer−14.059.17−1.530.25
FPRIntercept−2.870.23−12.21<0.001
RC type−0.350.31−1.130.77
Relativizer−0.140.30−0.471.00
RC type x relativizer0.210.380.561.00
QFPIntercept172.7412.1014.26<0.001
RC type−2.507.94−0.310.75
Relativizer60.607.737.83<0.001
RC type x relativizer−15.779.64−1.630.20
SPDIntercept25.296.254.04<0.001
RC type0.749.210.080.93
Relativizer7.739.760.790.85
RC type x relativizer−12.2410.99−1.110.79
R5FFRIntercept2.340.2119.78<0.001
RC type−0.320.19−1.670.28
Relativizer−0.190.19−0.960.33
RC type x relativizer0.390.241.620.28
FFIntercept191.367.2526.36<0.001
RC type−9.514.88−1.940.15
Relativizer1.865.190.350.96
RC type x relativizer4.586.550.690.96
FPDIntercept251.7212.5520.05<0.001
RC type−16.938.28−2.040.09
Relativizer−18.008.50−2.110.09
RC type x relativizer23.0510.672.160.09
FPRIntercept−1.850.18−10.28<0.001
RC type−1.200.28−4.29<0.001
Relativizer−1.130.26−4.33<0.001
RC type x relativizer1.220.363.38<0.001
QFPIntercept280.0714.0519.93<0.001
RC type−36.3710.02−3.62<0.001
Relativizer−33.979.10−3.73<0.001
RC type x relativizer38.4611.843.240.001
SPDIntercept90.3411.837.63<0.001
RC type−57.1312.87−4.44<0.001
Relativizer−38.2412.01−3.180.002
RC type x relativizer38.4316.532.320.02
Table 8. Estimated values from the linear mixed-effect models and mixed-effect logistic regression for the contrast que versus donde at R3, R4, and R5. Eye movement measures are first-fixation rate (FFR), first fixation (FF), first-pass duration (FPD), first-pass regression (FPR), quasi-first-pass reading time (QFP), and second-pass duration (SPD). t-values refer to reading time measures and z-values refer to probability measures.
Table 8. Estimated values from the linear mixed-effect models and mixed-effect logistic regression for the contrast que versus donde at R3, R4, and R5. Eye movement measures are first-fixation rate (FFR), first fixation (FF), first-pass duration (FPD), first-pass regression (FPR), quasi-first-pass reading time (QFP), and second-pass duration (SPD). t-values refer to reading time measures and z-values refer to probability measures.
RegionMeasureFactorEstimated βSDt/zp
R3FFRIntercept2.690.1913.62<0.001
RC type0.110.250.451.00
Relativizer0.080.240.341.00
RC type x relativizer−0.100.30−0.361.00
FFIntercept239.188.0729.61<0.001
RC type19.767.652.580.02
Relativizer9.546.791.400.16
RC type x relativizer−21.009.39−2.230.05
FPDIntercept275.4513.2420.79<0.001
RC type24.2610.922.220.07
Relativizer11.609.161.260.20
RC type x relativizer−27.2212.96−2.100.07
FPRIntercept−1.960.17−10.91<0.001
RC type0.560.163.470.001
Relativizer−0.070.19−0.391.00
RC type x relativizer0.030.210.171.00
QFPIntercept293.6515.4319.03<0.001
RC type24.9412.312.020.12
Relativizer10.5810.491.000.31
RC type x relativizer−22.5314.15−1.590.22
SPDIntercept65.369.317.01<0.001
RC type33.1416.002.070.11
Relativizer4.5313.870.321.00
RC type x relativizer−0.0117.44−0.0011.00
R4FFRIntercept1.950.1910.06<0.001
RC type−0.170.17−1.010.62
Relativizer−0.190.23−0.840.62
RC type x relativizer0.280.221.280.59
FFIntercept190.217.5525.16<0.001
RC type−6.825.86−1.160.49
Relativizer5.629.770.570.56
RC type x relativizer17.618.112.170.09
FPDIntercept203.569.1522.24<0.001
RC type−5.456.78−0.800.84
Relativizer2.0110.210.190.84
RC type x relativizer13.209.001.460.42
FPRIntercept−3.140.26−11.95<0.001
RC type0.340.271.250.41
Relativizer0.540.262.090.11
RC type x relativizer−0.220.31−0.700.48
QFPIntercept212.839.4222.59<0.001
RC type−6.167.34−0.840.80
Relativizer−2.6910.39−0.260.80
RC type x relativizer14.679.731.500.39
SPDIntercept30.478.283.67<0.001
RC type11.8912.010.990.96
Relativizer6.5111.180.581.00
RC type x relativizer−7.3814.57−0.501.00
R5FFRIntercept2.150.2010.57<0.001
RC type−0.100.19−0.541.00
Relativizer−0.010.19−0.071.00
RC type x relativizer0.020.230.081.00
FFIntercept188.507.3125.78<0.001
RC type−1.575.83−0.261.00
Relativizer1.285.420.231.00
RC type x relativizer−7.206.64−1.080.83
FPDIntercept223.2013.5716.44<0.001
RC type10.178.081.250.26
Relativizer21.9010.132.160.09
RC type x relativizer−16.5711.02−1.500.26
FPRIntercept−3.400.27−12.21<0.001
RC type0.070.350.190.89
Relativizer1.020.303.400.001
RC type x relativizer−0.290.39−0.750.89
QFPIntercept228.2014.4615.77<0.001
RC type9.688.801.100.27
Relativizer33.8411.273.000.008
RC type x relativizer−20.6211.53−1.780.14
SPDIntercept27.838.053.450.002
RC type−0.9011.86−0.071.00
Relativizer31.4913.052.410.04
RC type x relativizer2.2415.900.141.00
Table 9. Mean total reading times (ms) by condition over R4, R5, and R6 for the three contrasts. The numbers in parentheses indicate the standard deviation.
Table 9. Mean total reading times (ms) by condition over R4, R5, and R6 for the three contrasts. The numbers in parentheses indicate the standard deviation.
ContrastRESTRICTIVE RCsNON-RESTRICTIVE RCs
QUEOTHERQUEOTHER
que versus el cual525.28 (242.17)556.45 (224.63)482.03 (229.80)520.47 (238.93)
que versus quien542.51 (274.79)534.50 (253.47)488.12 (271.72)508.76 (264.02)
que versus donde536.34 (213.35)510.81 (237.39)516.67 (213.83)504.99 (246.04)
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Álvarez-García, E.; Igoa González, J.M. Lexico-Semantic Influence on Syntactic Processing: An Eye-Tracking Study with Spanish Relative Clauses. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 409. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13030409

AMA Style

Álvarez-García E, Igoa González JM. Lexico-Semantic Influence on Syntactic Processing: An Eye-Tracking Study with Spanish Relative Clauses. Brain Sciences. 2023; 13(3):409. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13030409

Chicago/Turabian Style

Álvarez-García, Esther, and José Manuel Igoa González. 2023. "Lexico-Semantic Influence on Syntactic Processing: An Eye-Tracking Study with Spanish Relative Clauses" Brain Sciences 13, no. 3: 409. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13030409

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop