Next Article in Journal
Reelin Signaling in Neurodevelopmental Disorders and Neurodegenerative Diseases
Previous Article in Journal
Actin Alpha 2, Smooth Muscle (ACTA2) Is Involved in the Migratory Potential of Malignant Gliomas, and Its Increased Expression at Recurrence Is a Significant Adverse Prognostic Factor
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Brief Report

Hebbian Control of Fixations in a Dyslexic Reader: A Case Report

1
Laser Physics Laboratory, University of Rennes, CEDEX, 35042 Rennes, France
2
Quantum Electronics and Chiralities Laboratory, 20 Square Marcel Bouget, 35700 Rennes, France
3
Unité de Formation et de Recherche Sciences et Propriétés de la Matière, University of Rennes, CEDEX, 35042 Rennes, France
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2023, 13(10), 1478; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13101478
Submission received: 28 July 2023 / Revised: 23 September 2023 / Accepted: 15 October 2023 / Published: 19 October 2023
(This article belongs to the Section Behavioral Neuroscience)

Abstract

:
When reading, dyslexic readers exhibit more and longer fixations than normal readers. However, there is no significant difference when dyslexic and control readers perform only visual tasks on a string of letters, showing the importance of cognitive processes in reading. This linguistic and cognitive processing requirement in reading is often perturbed for dyslexic readers by perceived additional letters and word mirror images superposed on the primary images on the primary cortex, inducing internal visual crowding. Here, we show that while for a normal reader, the number and the duration of fixations remain invariant whatever the nature of the lighting, the excess of fixations and total duration of reading can be controlled for a dyslexic reader using the Hebbian mechanisms to erase extra images in optimized pulse-width lighting. In this case, the number of fixations can then be reduced by a factor of about 1.8, recovering the normal reading experiment.

1. Introduction

Fixational eye movements are a fundamental aspect of vision [1,2,3]. Even when a compound eye like that of drosophila is rigidly attached to the skeleton, muscles have recently been shown to move the retina itself [4]. Anomalous eye movements have been discussed for some time now, in particular for children and adults with dyslexia [2,5] who lag behind normal readers on embedded letters and on words [6]. Binocular coordination of saccades [2,7] and vergence anomaly [8] have also been observed. Moreover, many studies have found that an excessive number of overly long fixations generally perturb dyslexics in reading but not in visual tasks [9,10,11]. Similar results have also been observed in languages with a higher grapheme–phoneme correspondence, such as German [12,13,14], and even in logographic languages with a deep orthography, such as Chinese [15]. Indeed, reading makes additional demands with respect to linguistic and cognitive processing. However, the saccadic patterns observed in readers with dyslexia seem to be the result and not the cause of their reading disabilities [2,16].
This excess of fixations in dyslexia, which has been studied by many groups [2,5,10,17,18,19,20,21,22] using different currently available eye-tracking systems [23], seems to provide a useful guide for detecting and predicting dyslexia using machine learning based on the eye-tracking technique [24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31]. The role of external crowding [32,33] has been studied and discussed in developmental dyslexia [34,35]. Various remediation methods, including increasing letter spacing [36,37], the use of colour filters [38,39], and e-reading with spaced letters [40], have been shown to help people with dyslexia.
A possible role for the lack of asymmetry between the Maxwell centroids in dyslexia inducing an absence of ocular dominance and the frequent existence of perceived extra mirror or duplicated images has also been proposed [41]. The associated internal visual crowding due to callosal interhemispheric projections of letters and words can perturb brain connectivity [42], particularly in the reading process. The aim of this paper is to show the role of this internal visual crowding in eye movements, especially in eye fixations during reading. With a higher number of fixations being an undisputed symptom of dyslexia, it is tempting to try to control them using the Hebbian mechanisms [43] at the synapses of the primary cortex. Taking into account the small delay associated with the interhemispheric transfer through the corpus callosum, the mirror images can be weakened by pulsed stimuli during silent reading.

2. Methods

To investigate this possibility, we have electronically modified a computer screen equipped with an eye tracker to optimize the lighting regime and hence control the internal visual crowding. The presence or absence of this internal visual crowding could then worsen or improve the fixational movements, suggesting a causal relationship [44] with the reading deficits.

2.1. Participants

We tested two male undergraduate students (21 years old) following the same physics courses in the third year of a Bachelor’s degree at the University of Rennes. The two students were native French speakers with normal vision. Neither of them had been diagnosed with a psychiatric disorder or any other form of cognitive disorder. The student with dyslexia and the student with normal reading characteristics were aware of the purpose of the study and gave informed written consent before participating. The entire investigation was conducted according to the principles expressed in the Declaration of Helsinki.

2.2. Foveascope

The setup described in [41] was adopted to investigate the two Maxwell centroid profiles, i.e., the blue cone-free areas at the centre of the foveas, and to record their asymmetry (Figure 1). The contrast of the Maxwell centroid entoptic image is optimized by using a blue-green exchange filter. Each observer adjusts the modulation frequency around 0.2 Hz to suit his own vision.

2.3. Noise-Activated Negative Afterimages

Retinal neurons are non-linear and bistable; therefore, they are sensitive to noise [45]. In general, closed eyelids allow 2% of incident light to pass through. This diffuse light leads to noise falling on the retina, which can activate the retinal cells and the primary images arriving on layer 4 of the primary cortex, which is the only layer that is sensitive to diffuse light [46] and receives most of the signals from the retinas. After fixating for a few seconds on a stimulus (Figure 2a), such as the word “NEURONS” placed on a window illuminated by daylight, closing their eyes, blocking out all light with by placing their hands over the eyes, and then shifting them periodically apart, the observer perceives the negative afterimage of the stimulus, as shown in Figure 2b for the normal reader and in Figure 2c for the dyslexic reader.

2.4. Eye Tracking Movements with the Stimuli Provided by an Electronically Modified Computer Screen

The computer screen (Hewlett Packard Compaq LE 2202x, Bloeblingen, Germany) was electronically modified so as to work in the continuous lighting regime (CW) or in a pulse-width modulated regime with a variable frequency from 60 to 120 Hz. The optimal frequency for the dyslexic reader was obtained by continuously varying the frequency between 60 Hz and 120 Hz, and it corresponds to a delay of about 10 ms for the interhemispheric transfer through the corpus callosum. The dyslexic reader looks at the screen while the frequency is varied and is able to optimize for himself the frequency at which disturbing secondary mirror images disappear. The eye tracker used is a commercial infrared system (Tobii dynavox PCEye Plus, Version 1.3) with a sampling frequency of 60 Hz that is used with software (Tobii Dynavox Gaze Viewer, V.1.2.0.63881) for rendering data as images and movies with gaze plots. The eye tracking analysis algorithm provides the total number of fixations and the total reading time. Calibration was carried out using a 9-point routine. Participants were seated about 60 cm from the screen, which is within the optimal range for recording, as described in the eye tracker manual. A chin and forehead rest was used in order to limit head movement. The experiment was carried out in a dark room. Figure 3a shows the whole system with the corresponding screen luminance versus time recorded in the continuous (left side of Figure 3b) and pulsed regimes (right side of Figure 3b) using an N-type Si ultrafast FND 100 photodiode. The mean luminance was the same in both regimes and was measured using a lux meter (Roline model Ro 1332, Taiwan). In the pulsed regime, the cyclic ratio can be adjusted continuously. We used a French text adapted for the assessment of dyslexia in adults [47]. We divided this into four parts of similar length and difficulty. These four texts were presented successively on the computer screen and were written in black Times New Roman font on a white background. Each text started with a capital letter and was 5 lines and approximately 291–299 characters (51.8 ± 2.4 words) long. The mean character width was about 0.45°. The participants were asked to read the texts silently.

3. Results

3.1. The Asymmetry of Maxwell’s Centroids

The two students recorded the profiles of their two Maxwell’s centroids, as shown in Figure 1. The ellipticity of each profile ε R and ε L for the right and left eye, respectively, is measured using the osculating ellipse. The asymmetry is defined by ε = ε R ε L . For the normal reader, the asymmetry was ε   0.5 , with a quasi-circular profile in the right eye, which is thus his dominant eye (Figure 1a). In contrast [41], for the dyslexic reader, the two profiles are similar (Figure 1b) and quasi-circular ( ε R   ε L   1 ) , and the lack of asymmetry induces an absence of ocular dominance and an internal visual crowding (Figure 2b). Note that for a normal reader, when the blue cone topographies are different in the two foveas, the green and red cone topographies are also automatically slightly perturbed. The asymmetry induces two slightly different retinal images and ocular dominance, as well as two slightly encoded different retinoptic maps on layer 4 of the primary cortex, where virtually all signals from the retinas arrive [48,49].

3.2. Internal Visual Crowding

After a binocular fixation on a stimulus such as NEURONS (Figure 2a), the normal reader perceived only the primary negative afterimage (Figure 2b), whereas the dyslexic reader with mirror images perceived the superposition of the primary and mirror images as in Figure 2c. Although the mirror image is weaker, confusion of letters is possible, and syllables are difficult to decipher. In another study, mirror images corresponding to symmetric projections between the two hemispheres were observed in 60% of a cohort of 160 dyslexic children, whereas duplicated images corresponding to non-symmetric projections were observed in 35% of the children [50]. As noted previously [41], small lateral shifts in the projected images generally occur, leading to different levels of severity of internal visual crowding.

3.3. Fixations during Reading

The eye movement patterns during reading are shown in Figure 4 for the two readers under the continuous wave (CW) and pulsed light regime for two texts. While for the normal reader, 50 fixations are necessary independently of the light regime (Figure 4b), for the dyslexic reader, 95 fixations are necessary in the usual continuous regime (top of Figure 4a); however, for the latter, only 46 are necessary in the optimized pulsed light regime at 82 Hz (bottom of Figure 4a), similar to the normal reader.
Repeating the experiment for four different texts yielded the results schematized in Figure 5. The error bars represent the estimated errors. The errors were estimated in a way similar to that used in [11], taking into account the inaccuracies of the recording. For the dyslexic reader (Figure 5a), the number of fixations was reduced by a factor of about 1.8 in the pulsed regime. Without the internal visual crowding, the level of the reader with dyslexia improves to that of a normal reader (Figure 5b).
The total reading times of the two readers are shown in Figure 6a. While the reading time is invariant for the normal reader, the total time is reduced by a factor of about 1.6 in the pulsed regime for the dyslexic reader but remains longer than that of the normal reader. The fixation durations are shown in Figure 6b. For both readers, the duration times are quasi invariant, but the fixation duration remains longer for the dyslexic reader by about 30%.

4. Discussion

Our eye tracking experiment confirms that the eye movements of the reader with dyslexia are different from those of a normal reader (Figure 4). In particular, the dyslexic reader makes more and longer fixations (about twice as many as a normal reader) and has longer reading times. Such observations have been made in different languages [12,13,15,30]. However, the causal relationship remains the subject of debate [44]. A lack of asymmetry between the Maxwell centroids of the two foveas has been shown to induce internal visual crowding in many readers with dyslexia [41,50], together with postural instabilities [51]. Indeed, the retinal images of the two eyes are excessively similar, inducing excessively similar retinoptic maps on layer 4 of the primary cortex, where the ganglion cells of the retinas reach the cortex. The interhemispheric projections through the corpus callosum between the excessively similar neuronal topographies in the two hemispheres are stronger than those for a normal reader with an asymmetry. When this is the case, the symmetric projections lead to superposed primary and mirror images and are perceived by the dyslexic reader not only for letters but also for words, as shown in Figure 2b. Internal visual crowding was absent for the normal reader (Figure 2c), and internal visual crowding cannot be geometrically weakened by spacing effects like external crowding, which can also induce impairments in reading [34].
In contrast, internal visual crowding has been shown to be erasable using the Hebbian mechanisms [43] at the synapses of the primary cortex [41]. Indeed, as the projected mirror images have to travel through the corpus callosum, they are delayed by about 10 milliseconds, corresponding to the transit time between the two hemispheres [52]. Pulse-width modulation of the light of the computer screen at frequencies beyond the visible flicker allows the mirror images to be weakened, restoring a single primary image similar to that perceived by a normal reader. When the modulation frequency is optimized for the dyslexic reader (here at 82 Hz), the internal visual crowding is completely erased and the number of fixations is immediately reduced to the normal reader regime. The responses of the normal reader remain invariant whatever the light regime as there is no internal visual crowding (see Figure 5 and Figure 6). The causality relationship between the internal visual crowding and the number of fixations is objectively established with an immediate and quantitative effect.
To conclude, the lack of asymmetry between the two Maxwell centroids in a reader with dyslexia, which results in a lack of ocular dominance and the existence of internal visual crowding, leads to a greater number of fixations and longer reading durations. Indeed, the excessively strong interhemispheric projections generally induce either perceived extra mirror or duplicated images [50], which make reading difficult by increasing linguistic and cognitive processing demands, in contrast to other visual tasks. Using the Hebbian mechanisms at the synapses in the primary cortex (activated by an optimized pulsed light regime from an electronically modified computer screen), the problematic internal crowding can be reduced, and excessive fixations controlled to regain the level of reading demonstrated by normal readers. Although we have compared the results for only one dyslexic reader with those of a normal reader in this paper, since the method uses common tracking features, we hope that the results will be confirmed by other research groups using larger samples. Other potential brain correlates can probably be observed, but ocularly tracking the fixations provides an immediate, precise, and objective quantification of the reduction in the number of fixations in reading and suggests a causality relationship between the reading deficit and internal visual crowding. Moreover, the reduction in the excess of fixations in reading observed here could be used as a diagnostic tool for dyslexia.

5. Patents

A patent has been filed by the University of Rennes for the modified computer screen.

Author Contributions

A.L.F. designed the study and wrote the manuscript. G.R. and A.L.F. developed the methods and the apparatus and contributed to the interpretation of the data and discussed the results. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The Relais Handicap of the University of Rennes, which assists dyslexic students, approved the non-invasive optical tests carried out in this study.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from the two subjects involved in the study. They gave informed written consent before participating. The entire investigation process was conducted in accordance with the principles expressed in the Declaration of Helsinki.

Data Availability Statement

This article has no additional data.

Acknowledgments

We thank the two students for their kind participation, the University of Rennes for providing access to its facilities, J. R. Thébault for his technical assistance, R. Even for calling our attention to the Tobii system, and K. Dunseath for reading the manuscript and for his comments.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Martinez-Conde, S.; Macknik, S.L.; Hubel, D.H. The Role of Fixational Eye Movements in Visual Perception. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2004, 5, 229–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Rayner, K. Eye Movements in Reading and Information Processing: 20 Years of Research. Psychol. Bull. 1998, 124, 372–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Rucci, M.; Iovin, R.; Poletti, M.; Santini, F. Miniature Eye Movements Enhance Fine Spatial Detail. Nature 2007, 447, 852–855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Fenk, L.M.; Avritzer, S.C.; Weisman, J.L.; Nair, A.; Randt, L.D.; Mohren, T.L.; Siwanowicz, I.; Maimon, G. Muscles That Move the Retina Augment Compound Eye Vision in Drosophila. Nature 2022, 612, 116–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Pavlidis, G. Eye Movements in Dyslexia. J. Learn. Disabil. 1985, 18, 42–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Bouma, H.; Legein, C.P. Foveal and Parafoveal Recognition of Letters and Words by Dyslexics and by Average Readers. Neuropsychologia 1977, 15, 69–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Kirkby, J.A.; Blythe, H.I.; Drieghe, D.; Liversedge, S.P. Reading Text Increases Binocular Disparity in Dyslexic Children. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e27105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Ghassemi, E.; Kapoula, Z. Is Poor Coordination of Saccades in Dyslexics a Consequence of Reading Difficulties? A Study Case. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2013, 6, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Adler-Grinberg, D.; Stark, L. Eye Movements, Scanpaths, and Dyslexia. Optom. Vis. Sci. 1978, 55, 557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Prado, C.; Dubois, M.; Valdois, S. The Eye Movements of Dyslexic Children during Reading and Visual Search: Impact of the Visual Attention Span. Vision Res. 2007, 47, 2521–2530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Bucci, M.P.; Nassibi, N.; Gerard, C.-L.; Bui-Quoc, E.; Seassau, M. Immaturity of the Oculomotor Saccade and Vergence Interaction in Dyslexic Children: Evidence from a Reading and Visual Search Study. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e33458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Hutzler, F.; Kronbichler, M.; Jacobs, A.M.; Wimmer, H. Perhaps Correlational but Not Causal: No Effect of Dyslexic Readers’ Magnocellular System on Their Eye Movements during Reading. Neuropsychologia 2006, 44, 637–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Trauzettel-Klosinski, S.; Koitzsch, A.M.; Dürrwächter, U.; Sokolov, A.N.; Reinhard, J.; Klosinski, G. Eye Movements in German-Speaking Children with and without Dyslexia When Reading Aloud. Acta Ophthalmol. 2010, 88, 681–691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Fischer, B.; Hartnegg, K. Instability of Fixation in Dyslexia: Development—Deficits—Training. Optom. Vis. Dev. 2009, 40, 221–228. [Google Scholar]
  15. Zhao, J.; Liu, M.; Liu, H.; Huang, C. The Visual Attention Span Deficit in Chinese Children with Reading Fluency Difficulty. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2018, 73, 76–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. American Academy of Pediatrics, Section on Ophthalmology, Council on Children with Disabilities; American Academy of Ophthalmology; American Association for Pediatric Ophthalmology and Strabismus; American Association of Certified Orthoptists. Learning Disabilities, Dyslexia, and Vision. Pediatrics 2009, 124, 837–844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. De Luca, M.; Di Pace, E.; Judica, A.; Spinell, D.; Zoccolotti, P. Eye Movement Patterns in Linguistic and Non-Linguistic Tasks in Developmental Surface Dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 1999, 37, 1407–1420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Lennerstrand, G.; Ygge, J.; Jacobsson, C. Control of Binocular Eye Movements in Normals and Dyslexics. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1993, 682, 231–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Bucci, M.P. Visual Training Could Be Useful for Improving Reading Capabilities in Dyslexia. Appl. Neuropsychol. Child 2021, 10, 199–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Bonifacci, P.; Tobia, V.; Sansavini, A.; Guarini, A. Eye-Movements in a Text Reading Task: A Comparison of Preterm Children, Children with Dyslexia and Typical Readers. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Vagge, A.; Cavanna, M.; Traverso, C.; Iester, M. Evaluation of Ocular Movements in Patients with Dyslexia. Ann. Dyslexia 2015, 65, 24–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Weiss, B.; Nárai, Á.; Vidnyánszky, Z. Lateralization of Early Orthographic Processing during Natural Reading Is Impaired in Developmental Dyslexia. Neuroimage 2022, 258, 119383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Holmqvist, K.; Örbom, S.L.; Hooge, I.T.C.; Niehorster, D.C.; Alexander, R.G.; Andersson, R.; Benjamins, J.S.; Blignaut, P.; Brouwer, A.-M.; Chuang, L.L.; et al. Eye Tracking: Empirical Foundations for a Minimal Reporting Guideline. Behav. Res. Methods 2023, 55, 364–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Rello, L.; Ballesteros, M. Detecting Readers with Dyslexia Using Machine Learning with Eye Tracking Measures. In Proceedings of the 12th International Web for All Conference, Florence Italy, 18–20 May 2015; pp. 1–8. [Google Scholar]
  25. Benfatto, M.N.; Seimyr, G.Ö.; Ygge, J.; Pansell, T.; Rydberg, A.; Jacobson, C. Screening for Dyslexia Using Eye Tracking during Reading. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0165508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Asvestopoulou, T.; Manousaki, V.; Psistakis, A.; Smyrnakis, I.; Andreadakis, V.; Aslanides, I.M.; Papadopouli, M. DysLexML: Screening Tool for Dyslexia Using Machine Learning. arXiv 2019, arXiv:1903.06274. [Google Scholar]
  27. Raatikainen, P.; Hautala, J.; Loberg, O.; Kärkkäinen, T.; Leppänen, P.; Nieminen, P. Detection of Developmental Dyslexia with Machine Learning Using Eye Movement Data. Array 2021, 12, 100087. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. El Hmimdi, A.E.; Ward, L.M.; Palpanas, T.; Kapoula, Z. Predicting Dyslexia and Reading Speed in Adolescents from Eye Movements in Reading and Non-Reading Tasks: A Machine Learning Approach. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 1337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. JothiPrabha, A.; Bhargavi, R.; Deepa Rani, B.V. Prediction of Dyslexia Severity Levels from Fixation and Saccadic Eye Movement Using Machine Learning. Biomed. Signal Process. Control 2023, 79, 104094. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Vajs, I.; Papić, T.; Ković, V.; Savić, A.M.; Janković, M.M. Accessible Dyslexia Detection with Real-Time Reading Feedback through Robust Interpretable Eye-Tracking Features. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Weiss, B.; Szalma, J.; Vidnyánszky, Z. Data-Driven Detection of Developmental Dyslexia: A Machine Learning Approach Based on Behavioral and Eye-Movement Features. PsyArXiv 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Pelli, D.G. Crowding: A Cortical Constraint on Object Recognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2008, 18, 445–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Levi, D.M. Crowding—An Essential Bottleneck for Object Recognition: A Mini-Review. Vis. Res. 2008, 48, 635–654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Martelli, M.; Di Filippo, G.; Spinelli, D.; Zoccolotti, P. Crowding, Reading, and Developmental Dyslexia. J. Vis. 2009, 9, 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Bellocchi, S. Developmental dyslexia, visual crowding and eye movements. In Eye Movements: Developmental Perspectives, Dysfunctions and Disorders in Humans; Steward, L.C., Ed.; Nova Science Publishers: New York, NY, USA, 2013; ISBN 978-1-62808-601-0. [Google Scholar]
  36. Bertoni, S.; Franceschini, S.; Ronconi, L.; Gori, S.; Facoetti, A. Is Excessive Visual Crowding Causally Linked to Developmental Dyslexia? Neuropsychologia 2019, 130, 107–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Zorzi, M.; Barbiero, C.; Facoetti, A.; Lonciari, I.; Carrozzi, M.; Montico, M.; Bravar, L.; George, F.; Pech-Georgel, C.; Ziegler, J.C. Extra-Large Letter Spacing Improves Reading in Dyslexia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2012, 109, 11455–11459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Hall, R.; Ray, N.; Harries, P.; Stein, J. A Comparison of Two-Coloured Filter Systems for Treating Visual Reading Difficulties. Disabil. Rehabil. 2013, 35, 2221–2226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Razuk, M.; Perrin-Fievez, F.; Gerard, C.L.; Peyre, H.; Barela, J.A.; Bucci, M.P. Effect of Colored Filters on Reading Capabilities in Dyslexic Children. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2018, 83, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Schneps, M.H.; Thomson, J.M.; Chen, C.; Sonnert, G.; Pomplun, M. E-Readers Are More Effective than Paper for Some with Dyslexia. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e75634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Le Floch, A.; Ropars, G. Left-Right Asymmetry of the Maxwell Spot Centroids in Adults without and with Dyslexia. Proc. R. Soc. B 2017, 284, 20171380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Finn, E.S.; Shen, X.; Holahan, J.M.; Scheinost, D.; Lacadie, C.; Papademetris, X.; Shaywitz, S.E.; Shaywitz, B.A.; Constable, R.T. Disruption of Functional Networks in Dyslexia: A Whole-Brain, Data-Driven Analysis of Connectivity. Biol. Psychiatry 2014, 76, 397–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Hebb, D.O. The Organization of Behavior; a Neuropsychological Theory; Wiley: Oxford, UK, 1949. [Google Scholar]
  44. Werth, R. Dyslexia: Causes and Concomitant Impairments. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Longtin, A.; Bulsara, A.; Pierson, D.; Moss, F. Bistability and the Dynamics of Periodically Forced Sensory Neurons. Biol. Cybern. 1994, 70, 569–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Hubel, D.H. Eye, Brain, and Vision; Scientific American Library: New York, NY, USA, 1988; ISBN 978-0-7167-5020-8. [Google Scholar]
  47. Lauga-Mandon, C. Création d’un Test de Lecture Pour l’évaluation Des Adultes Porteurs d’une Pathologie Développementale Du Language Écrit. 2016. Available online: https://dumas.ccsd.cnrs.fr/dumas-01487977 (accessed on 27 July 2023).
  48. Hubel, D.H.; Wiesel, T.N. Ferrier Lecture—Functional Architecture of Macaque Monkey Visual Cortex. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 1997, 198, 1–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Crick, F. Astonishing Hypothesis: The Scientific Search for the Soul, 1st ed.; Scribner: London, UK, 1995; ISBN 978-0-684-80158-2. [Google Scholar]
  50. Le Floch, A.; Ropars, G. Le manque d’asymétrie des centroïdes de Maxwell, et de dominance oculaire, chez les dyslexiques. Rev. Francoph. D’orthoptie 2020, 13, 134–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Le Floch, A.; Henriat, S.; Fourage, R.; Ropars, G. Postural Instability in a Young Dyslexic Adult Improved by Hebbian Pulse-Width Modulated Lighting. Am. J. Intern. Med. 2020, 8, 267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Caminiti, R.; Carducci, F.; Piervincenzi, C.; Battaglia-Mayer, A.; Confalone, G.; Visco-Comandini, F.; Pantano, P.; Innocenti, G.M. Diameter, Length, Speed, and Conduction Delay of Callosal Axons in Macaque Monkeys and Humans: Comparing Data from Histology and Magnetic Resonance Imaging Diffusion Tractography. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 14501–14511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Maxwell’s centroid profiles. (a) For a normal reader. (b) For a dyslexic reader. The ellipticity ε is defined by the ratio of the lengths of the two axes of the osculating ellipse (dotted line). The profiles show the asymmetry for the normal reader with an ellipticity difference between the two eyes ε = ε R ε L 0.5 and the lack of asymmetry for the dyslexic reader with ε = ε R ε L 0 . The corresponding mean diameters on the retinas are between 100 to 150 µm.
Figure 1. Maxwell’s centroid profiles. (a) For a normal reader. (b) For a dyslexic reader. The ellipticity ε is defined by the ratio of the lengths of the two axes of the osculating ellipse (dotted line). The profiles show the asymmetry for the normal reader with an ellipticity difference between the two eyes ε = ε R ε L 0.5 and the lack of asymmetry for the dyslexic reader with ε = ε R ε L 0 . The corresponding mean diameters on the retinas are between 100 to 150 µm.
Brainsci 13 01478 g001
Figure 2. Noise-activated negative afterimages. The negative afterimages are perceived when the diffuse noise through the eyelids falls on the retina. The negative afterimages are then reconstructed in each case. (a) Stimulus: NEURONS. (b) Noise-activated negative afterimage perceived by the normal reader. (c) Noise-activated negative afterimage perceived by the dyslexic reader. The mirror image induces internal visual crowding.
Figure 2. Noise-activated negative afterimages. The negative afterimages are perceived when the diffuse noise through the eyelids falls on the retina. The negative afterimages are then reconstructed in each case. (a) Stimulus: NEURONS. (b) Noise-activated negative afterimage perceived by the normal reader. (c) Noise-activated negative afterimage perceived by the dyslexic reader. The mirror image induces internal visual crowding.
Brainsci 13 01478 g002
Figure 3. (a) The eye tracker system with an electronically modified screen. (b) Screen luminance versus time in the continuous regime (left side) and in the pulsed regime (right side). The mean illuminance (dotted line) is exactly the same as in the continuous regime.
Figure 3. (a) The eye tracker system with an electronically modified screen. (b) Screen luminance versus time in the continuous regime (left side) and in the pulsed regime (right side). The mean illuminance (dotted line) is exactly the same as in the continuous regime.
Brainsci 13 01478 g003
Figure 4. Eye movement patterns during reading. (a) Results for the dyslexic reader in the continuous (top) and pulsed regime (82 Hz—bottom). (b) Results for the control reader in the continuous (top) and pulsed regime (82 Hz—bottom).
Figure 4. Eye movement patterns during reading. (a) Results for the dyslexic reader in the continuous (top) and pulsed regime (82 Hz—bottom). (b) Results for the control reader in the continuous (top) and pulsed regime (82 Hz—bottom).
Brainsci 13 01478 g004aBrainsci 13 01478 g004b
Figure 5. Number of fixations for four different texts. (a) Results for the dyslexic reader in the two regimes. (b) Results for the normal reader in the two regimes. The red zones correspond to the continuous regime, and the blue zones correspond to the pulsed regime.
Figure 5. Number of fixations for four different texts. (a) Results for the dyslexic reader in the two regimes. (b) Results for the normal reader in the two regimes. The red zones correspond to the continuous regime, and the blue zones correspond to the pulsed regime.
Brainsci 13 01478 g005
Figure 6. Comparative durations for the dyslexic and control readers. (a) Total duration for four texts for the two readers in the two lighting regimes. For the dyslexic reader, at 82 Hz, the total duration is reduced by a factor 1.6. (b) Invariance of the fixation durations for the two readers in the two lighting regimes; the fixation durations remain longer for the dyslexic reader. The red zones correspond to the continuous regime and the blue zones correspond to the pulsed regime.
Figure 6. Comparative durations for the dyslexic and control readers. (a) Total duration for four texts for the two readers in the two lighting regimes. For the dyslexic reader, at 82 Hz, the total duration is reduced by a factor 1.6. (b) Invariance of the fixation durations for the two readers in the two lighting regimes; the fixation durations remain longer for the dyslexic reader. The red zones correspond to the continuous regime and the blue zones correspond to the pulsed regime.
Brainsci 13 01478 g006
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Le Floch, A.; Ropars, G. Hebbian Control of Fixations in a Dyslexic Reader: A Case Report. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 1478. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13101478

AMA Style

Le Floch A, Ropars G. Hebbian Control of Fixations in a Dyslexic Reader: A Case Report. Brain Sciences. 2023; 13(10):1478. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13101478

Chicago/Turabian Style

Le Floch, Albert, and Guy Ropars. 2023. "Hebbian Control of Fixations in a Dyslexic Reader: A Case Report" Brain Sciences 13, no. 10: 1478. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci13101478

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop