Next Article in Journal
Brain–Immune Interactions as the Basis of Gulf War Illness: Clinical Assessment and Deployment Profile of 1990–1991 Gulf War Veterans in the Gulf War Illness Consortium (GWIC) Multisite Case-Control Study
Next Article in Special Issue
Changes in Default Mode Network Connectivity in Resting-State fMRI in People with Mild Dementia Receiving Cognitive Stimulation Therapy
Previous Article in Journal
Magnetic Resonance Imaging Measurement of Entorhinal Cortex in the Diagnosis and Differential Diagnosis of Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease
Previous Article in Special Issue
Functional Connectivity-Derived Optimal Gestational-Age Cut Points for Fetal Brain Network Maturity
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Ageing and the Ipsilateral M1 BOLD Response: A Connectivity Study

1
Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, University College London, London WC1N 3AR, UK
2
MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 7EF, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2021, 11(9), 1130; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11091130
Submission received: 27 July 2021 / Revised: 19 August 2021 / Accepted: 23 August 2021 / Published: 26 August 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Neural Networks and Connectivity among Brain Regions)

Abstract

:
Young people exhibit a negative BOLD response in ipsilateral primary motor cortex (M1) when making unilateral movements, such as button presses. This negative BOLD response becomes more positive as people age. In this study, we investigated why this occurs, in terms of the underlying effective connectivity and haemodynamics. We applied dynamic causal modeling (DCM) to task fMRI data from 635 participants aged 18–88 from the Cam-CAN dataset, who performed a cued button pressing task with their right hand. We found that connectivity from contralateral supplementary motor area (SMA) and dorsal premotor cortex (PMd) to ipsilateral M1 became more positive with age, explaining 44% of the variability across people in ipsilateral M1 responses. In contrast, connectivity from contralateral M1 to ipsilateral M1 was weaker and did not correlate with individual differences in rM1 BOLD. Neurovascular and haemodynamic parameters in the model were not able to explain the age-related shift to positive BOLD. Our results add to a body of evidence implicating neural, rather than vascular factors as the predominant cause of negative BOLD—while emphasising the importance of inter-hemispheric connectivity. This study provides a foundation for investigating the clinical and lifestyle factors that determine the sign and amplitude of the M1 BOLD response in ageing, which could serve as a proxy for neural and vascular health, via the underlying neurovascular mechanisms.

1. Introduction

Making a unilateral movement is typically associated with activation of sensorimotor cortex in the contralateral hemisphere of the brain. However, such movements also engage the ipsilateral sensorimotor cortex, according to recent reviews, [1,2]. This can result in a negative BOLD response (NBR) in the ipsilateral hemisphere, particularly in young people, as measured with fMRI. The ipsilateral NBR becomes less negative as people get older [3,4,5,6], in some cases becoming positive in sign with advanced age. This study investigates why ageing has this effect on the ipsilateral BOLD response.
The background to this question can be tackled into three parts: why is there a neural response to unilateral movements in the ipsilateral sensorimotor cortex; why is the BOLD response negative for some people; why does it become more positive with age?
Studies using EEG [7,8] and ECoG [9] have demonstrated transient reductions in neural inhibition in both the contralateral and the ipsilateral sensorimotor cortex in response to unilateral movements. This is evidenced by decreases in alpha/mu and beta band spectral power (event-related desynchronization, ERD). Ipsilateral neural activity is sufficient to decode the precise movements being performed [10,11], although it may not provide additional information beyond that encoded in the contralateral hemisphere [12]. What role does this ipsilateral neural activity play? A predominant view is that bilateral movements are the ‘default’ for the brain, with additional inter-hemispheric inhibition (IHI) required to enable unilateral movements and bimanual coordination, thereby suppressing mirror movements [13]. In support of this view, most studies using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to target M1 have identified increased IHI during unilateral motor execution [1]. These studies typically apply a stimulus to motor cortex in one hemisphere, which reduces the motor-evoked potential generated by a subsequent stimulus applied to the opposite hemisphere [14]. This IHI is hypothesised to be mediated polysynaptically, with excitatory connections through the corpus callosum driving local GABA-ergic inhibition in the target hemisphere—as evidenced by the timing of TMS effects [15], intracranial recordings in animal models [16], pharmacology in humans [17] and modeling of neural dynamics [18]. In regards to behavior, TMS studies have shown that disrupting ipsilateral motor cortex affects motor performance, for example, altering motor coordination and timing [19]. It should be noted, however, that there is considerable variability across TMS studies targeting ipsilateral motor cortex, with stimulation resulting in either increased or decreased IHI, and either facilitation or reduction of behavioral performance [1]. Nevertheless, the overall picture is that unilateral movements are associated with both hemispheres, albeit with less neural activity in the ipsilateral hemisphere. Nevertheless, the ipsilateral activity does carry information, and may have a functional role through reciprocal connectivity with the contralateral hemisphere.
Why is the BOLD response in the ipsilateral sensorimotor cortex negative in sign, relative to rest, at least in young people? Unilateral medial nerve stimulation has been used to cleanly investigate the NBR. Stimulation causes a transient reduction in cerebral blood flow (CBF) and oxygen extraction (CMRO2) in the ipsilateral sensorimotor cortex (S1/M1) [8], and similar results have been found for visual stimuli in occipital cortex [20]. With this in mind, to explain the generation of a NBR, the researcher could simply invert the typical explanation for how a positive BOLD response (PBR) is formed. A reduction in neural activity due to inhibition would cause a decrease in CBF, outweighing a smaller decrease in CMRO2. This would increase the relative level of deoxyhaemoglobin (dHB), decreasing the BOLD signal relative to the baseline. This explanation could be sufficient, were the NBR simply the inverse of the PBR. However, there is evidence that they differ in their neurovascular coupling. Higher metabolic demand (i.e., a greater CMRO2/CBF coupling ratio) has been identified for NBRs relative to PBRs [8], perhaps caused by different neural cell-types giving rise to them, each with differing metabolic demand. Thus, when identifying individual differences in the sign of the BOLD response, it is important to consider the potential for both neural and neurovascular contributions.
Having set out some key neural and haemodynamic determinants of the NBR, we next turn to ageing. Evidence from fMRI studies over the last 20 years has shown that older people display more positive BOLD responses in the ipsilateral sensorimotor cortex than younger people [3,4,5]. This effect co-locates with the ipsilateral NBR, which can be readily measured in younger, but not older, people [3]. Why might this change occur? There could be both neural and haemodynamic factors. At the neural level, inter-hemispheric connectivity changes with age. There is decline in the thickness of the corpus callosum [21] and a reduction in IHI from ipsilateral M1 to contralateral M1 [22]. This may underlie the observation that hemispheric asymmetries decrease with age, referred to as the Hemispheric Asymmetry Reduction in Older Adults (HAROLD) model [23]. An age-related shift in responses from M1 to higher level motor regions, such as dorsal premotor cortex (PMd), has also been noted. This “posterior to anterior shift in ageing” (PASA) [24] is supported by recent TMS results [22]. In parallel, ageing brings about changes in neurovascular coupling and haemodynamics. With advancing age, blood vessels stiffen, resulting in reduced cerebral blood flow dynamics (cerebrovascular reactivity) [25]. As outlined above, NBRs have higher metabolic demand than PBRs, thus they could be particularly susceptible to any reduction in haemodynamic efficacy. Therefore, in order to understand why the ipsilateral NBR changes with age, it is important to distinguish neural and haemodynamic factors.
Our ultimate aim is to investigate how specific clinical and lifestyle factors impact on the ipsilateral NBR, which we hypothesise could serve as a marker for the health of the brain and its vasculature. In this foundational paper, we use dynamic causal modeling (DCM) of fMRI data in order to quantify neural and haemodynamic contributions to the NBR. Previously, DCM has been applied successfully to study the motor system in ageing, with results that are generally consistent with findings from TMS [26,27]. Here, we applied this approach to quantify the evidence for three sets of hypotheses for why the NBR in ipsilateral M1 decreases with age. We asked to what extent age-related changes in NBR could be explained by (1) reorganisation of neural networks, particularly inter-hemispheric connections, (2) individual differences in the strength of neurovascular coupling, and/or (3) changes in the rate of blood flow.
To address these hypotheses, we leveraged a unique BOLD fMRI dataset from the Cam-CAN project [28,29], in which n = 635 participants aged 18–88 performed a simple motor response task with their right hand, in response to visual and auditory cues. The data suggested two types of subjects—those with mainly negative BOLD responses in right M1, who tended to be younger, and those with mainly positive right M1 responses, who tended to be older. We then specified biophysical models, i.e., dynamic causal models (DCMs), which included neural, neurovascular, and haemodynamic components. Each of these had parameters that we estimated from the fMRI data. We identified the parameters that could best explain differences in right M1 response across individuals of different ages, and we performed simulations to identify the minimal set of parameters that could explain the age-related shift from NBR to PBR.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants and Task

We used openly available data from the Cambridge Centre for Ageing and Neuroscience (Cam-CAN) project [28,29] (available at http://www.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/datasets/camcan/ (accessed on 4 May 2020)). The participants had been screened to ensure no Magnetic Resonance (MR) contradictions, no learning disability, no cognitive impairment (tested through Mini-Mental State Examination with a score > 24) and no long-term illness or disability. From an initial sample of 652 participants with fMRI data, we excluded nine participants with missing data or who had fMRI signal dropout. We further excluded eight participants who had suffered a stroke, giving a final sample size of n = 635.
While undergoing fMRI, participants responded to 128 trials of a simple audio/visual sensorimotor task. On each trial, a binaural auditory tone (300 Hz, 600 Hz or 1200 Hz) was played and two round checkerboards were presented simultaneously to the left and right of a central fixation cross. Auditory tones had a duration of 300 ms, and checkerboards had a duration of 34 ms. A total of 120 trials were presented (40 per auditory frequency). Additionally, there were four trials in which only an auditory tone was played and four trials wherein only checkerboards were presented. Importantly, randomly interspersed null trials were included with stimulus onset asynchronies (SOAs) from 2 s to 26 s, enabling efficient estimation of the haemodynamic response. The participant’s task was simply to press a button with the right index finger when they saw or heard a stimulus.
A recent detailed analysis of the in-scanner behavioral outcomes of this task (in these same subjects) identified no effect of age on the mean reaction time (since the task was unspeeded), but there was a significant effect of age on the variability in reaction times across trials [12]. No significant relationship between performance and the BOLD response was found, therefore we did not model reaction times in the analyses that follow.

2.2. fMRI Data and Pre-Processing

We used pre-processed fMRI data from the Cam-CAN project from the “aamod_norm_write_dartel” analysis stage. Details of scanning sequences and the image pre-processing pipeline can be found in Reference [29]. We applied an additional spatial smoothing, with an isotropic Gaussian kernel of 3 mm at its full-width half maximum, which resulted in a final estimated smoothness of the group level maps of 9 mm isotropic, where the voxel size was 3 mm. This met the assumptions of the random field theory-based multiple comparisons correction, which requires the image smoothness to be at least double the voxel size [30].

2.3. SPM Analysis

To identify brain regions responding to the task, we performed a preliminary Statistical Parametric Mapping (SPM) analysis using SPM version 12 revision 8081 (https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/ (accessed on 4 May 2020)). Each subject’s first level general linear model (GLM) included the three experimental conditions (Audio + Visual, Audio, Visual), and the 3 translations and 3 rotations from motion correction. Second level models (one sample t-tests) included covariates for the linear and non-linear effects of age (age, age squared and age cubed), in addition to potential confounds such as height, weight and hearing loss. Full details of the SPM analysis, including a table of confounding variables, are provided in the supplementary materials.

2.4. Region of Interest Definition

We defined binary masks for six regions of interest (ROIs). Right primary motor cortex (M1), right dorsal PMd and bilateral supplementary motor areas (SMA) were defined based on two criteria: (1) voxels were only included if they showed a significant effect of age on Auditory + Visual trials at p < 0.05 family-wise error corrected (as assessed by a group-level F-contrast over age, age squared and age cubed), and (2) the same voxels had to overlap with corresponding motor regions in the Human Motor Area Template (HMAT) atlas [31]. The mask for right M1 included two distinct clusters, one larger (188 voxels, peak: [39,−21,54]) and the other much smaller (14 voxels, peak: [12,−30,69]). To avoid anatomical heterogeneity across subjects, we included only the larger cluster. The remaining two regions, left M1 and left PMd, did not show age effects, and so were defined as the mirror image around the midline of right M1 and right PMd ROIs, respectively. In the remainder of this study, we refer to the left (contralateral) hemisphere regions as lM1, lPMd and lSMA, and the right (ipsilateral) hemisphere regions as rM1, rPMd and rSMA.
Given that the left hemisphere ROIs were defined by taking the mirror image of the right hemisphere ROIs, we used the HMAT atlas to ensure that the left hemisphere ROIs were positioned in the correct, anatomically defined, regions. The proportions of voxels in each left hemisphere ROI were: lM1 ROI [99% M1, 1% S1]; lPMd ROI [95% PMd, 5% M1]; lSMA ROI [100% SMA]. Thus, although alignment with the atlas was not perfect in the left hemisphere, we decided it was close enough to prioritize having symmetric masks across hemispheres, rather than defining distinctly shaped masks in each hemisphere. To validate this decision, we also ran the analyses with the lM1 and lPMd ROIs trimmed, so that all voxels fell within the respective HMAT atlas masks. This did not change the results (refer to Supplementary Materials).

2.5. Timeseries Extraction

For each subject and ROI, we extracted a representative timeseries for subsequent DCM analyses. Following standard procedures in SPM, this was the first principal component across voxels within the ROI. To reduce noise, contributing voxels were limited to those exhibiting a response to the task at a liberal statistical threshold (p < 0.001 uncorrected) at the individual subject level. Where no voxels could be found at this threshold, the threshold was iteratively relaxed to p < 0.01, p < 0.1 and finally p < 1, until at least one voxel was included. Each representative timeseries was then high-pass filtered, pre-whitened and corrected for confounds (to remove the mean and head motion) in the usual way using the SPM software.

2.6. Dynamic Causal Model

We used the standard (one-state, deterministic) DCM for fMRI, with certain changes in the parameters for this study. We began by briefly reprising the form of the model. Matrices are presented in upper case bold type ( X ), vectors are in lower case bold type ( x ) and scalars are in regular type ( x ).
The inputs to the DCM were the fMRI timeseries data and experimental timing. Matrix Y i V × R were fMRI timeseries for subject i = 1 N , with V = 261 measurements from R = 6 brain regions. Columns of the matrix U i 0 , 1 T × P encoded the onsets of the P = 3 experimental conditions (auditory + visual, auditory only, visual only) over T = 4176 time bins (16 bins per TR).
The DCM for fMRI model is split into two parts: neural and haemodynamic. The neural part has one hidden state variable per brain region—encoding the level of neural activity z i t   R . The dynamics of this activity are governed by the differential equation:
z ˙ t   = d z t d t = A z + C u t
where parameter matrix A R × R θ i is a connectivity or directed adjacency matrix and parameter matrix C R × P θ i encodes the strength of driving input due to each experimental condition to each brain region. Element A i j is the rate of change in neural activity in region j due to region i , which is called the effective connectivity and has units of Hertz (Hz). To ensure stability, the elements of the leading diagonal of matrix A (self-connections) must be negative in sign. Therefore, rather than estimate those values directly, they are replaced with log-scaling parameters λ j A , which are estimated from the data and are translated to units of Hz by the function:
A j j = 0.5   Hz   ·   exp λ j A
where the exponential ensures positivity of the scaling parameter, and −0.5 Hz is the default strength of self-inhibition.
The haemodynamic part of the model translates neural activity to predicted fMRI timeseries. It adds hidden states encoding blood volume, deoxyhaemoglobin, and cerebral blood flow. This part of the model has two types of free parameter. Rate parameter κ controls the rate of decay of the vasoactive signal (for example, nitric oxide), which couples neural activity to the haemodynamic response. It is estimated from the data via a log-scaling parameter λ κ , which is translated to units of Hz via the definition κ = 0.64   Hz   ·   exp λ κ . The remaining parameters are the haemodynamic transit times. These are time constants, τ j for each region j . They quantify the rate of blood flow through the venous compartment. They are in units of seconds and are estimated from the data via the introduction of a log-scaling parameter for each region λ j τ , defined as τ j = 2   s   ·   exp λ j τ . For the full definition of the haemodynamic model, refer to Appendix 5 of Reference [32].
Given the importance of accurate haemodynamic modeling in this study, we made several updates to the generative model in DCM for fMRI model to optimize it for our 3T fMRI data. We set the echo time (TE) to use the precise value for this experiment (0.03 s), rather than the fixed default value of 0.04 s (in the Matlab function spm_gx_fmri.m). Table 1 lists the other parameters adjusted for in this study, with details in the footnotes.

2.7. Priors on DCM Parameters

Each DCM for subject i had free parameters θ i =   A , λ A , C , λ κ , λ τ , i.e., between-region connectivity, self-connections, driving input, decay rate and transit time. The other parameters listed in Table 1 were fixed values (i.e., they had infinite precision). We specified two DCMs per subject, m 1 and m 2 , which differed in the location of the driving inputs (detailed in Section 3.3). Parameters were switched on or off in each model through specification of the prior probability density p θ i | m 1   = N μ 1 , Σ 1 and p θ i | m 2   = N μ 2 , Σ 2 , respectively. Parameters with prior variance close to zero—i.e., small values on the leading diagonal of prior covariance matrix Σ 1 or Σ 2 —were effectively ‘switched off’, fixing them at their prior expectation μ 1 or μ 2 , respectively (typically zero), whereas parameters with larger positive prior variances were ‘switched on’ and informed by the data. For full details of the priors in DCM for fMRI, see Table 3 of Reference [32].
We assigned models m 1 and m 2 the same priors on their connectivity parameters ( A , λ A ), such that all connections among the six regions were informed by the fMRI data (i.e., all connections were switched on). This was based on evidence for both homotopic and non-homotopic connections among motor cortex regions, from tract-tracing in non-human primates [36] and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) in humans [37,38]. (For a brief review of non-homotopic premotor connections, refer to Reference [22]).
We set priors on the driving inputs (parameter matrix C ) in model m 1 such that lSMA and lPMd could receive driving input by the task, and in model m 2 rSMA and rPMd could receive driving input. Any dynamics in M1 were thereby mediated by connections from SMA and PMd. This expressed the hypothesis that there is a motor hierarchy, with SMA and PMd at a higher level than M1. A similar architecture has been used in previous DCM analysis of the motor cortex [39].

2.8. Model Estimation and PEB

We fitted the two DCMs per subject to the fMRI data using the standard Bayesian scheme in SPM (Variational Laplace [40]). To attempt to rescue any subjects whose estimates were stuck in local optima, we then re-estimated every model using the group average connectivity as the starting value of the estimation (while leaving the priors untouched). The final outputs of the model estimation procedure were the posterior probability densities over the parameters for each of the two models per subject, p ( θ i | Y i ,   m 1 ) and p ( θ i | Y i ,   m 2 ) , and the log model evidences, approximated by the free energy F i , m 1 ln p ( Y i | m 1 ) and F i , m 2 ln p ( Y i | m 2 ) . Each free energy scores the quality of the model, in terms of its accuracy minus its complexity, enabling Bayesian model comparison.
We then used the Parametric Empirical Bayes (PEB) scheme in the SPM software to identify commonalities and differences in parameters at the group level. The software stacked the M parameters from all N subjects into a vector θ = θ 1 θ N and modelled them at the group level using the general linear model:
θ = X β + ϵ 2
X =   X B I M
The between-subjects design matrix X B N × 4 contained four orthogonal regressors: a constant (all ones), group (1 for negative responders, 0 for positive responders, before mean-centring), the residual effect of age after accounting for group, and the residual level of rM1 BOLD activity after accounting for group and age. The latter two regressors were ‘nuisance effects’, included to maximize the explained variance of the model, and were calculated using recursive Gram-Schmidt orthogonalization implemented in the SPM Matlab function: spm_orth.m. This procedure also mean-centred the regressors in X B , making the first regressor (the constant) interpretable as the group average connectivity. The Kronecker-tensor product with the identity matrix I M was applied automatically by the software, to replicate the four between-subject effects over the M DCM parameters per subject. The resulting design matrix X = N M × 4 M contained a regressor for each covariate on each DCM parameter. Finally, the between-subjects variability ϵ 2 was modelled using a covariance component model. We elected to have one component per field (A, C, decay, transit), allowing each type of parameter to have a different level of between-subjects variability.
We specified a PEB model for each of the two DCMs (left hemisphere driving and right hemisphere driving), and estimated each PEB model to obtain posterior probability densities over the parameters, p θ m 1 | Y , m 1 and p θ m 2 | Y , m 2 , as well as the free energy for each hierarchical model F m 1 = p Y | m 1 and F m 2 = p Y | m 2 , where Y was the complete dataset from all subjects.

2.9. Bayesian Model Comparison

We tested our hypotheses by comparing the evidence for different models (Bayesian model comparison). This procedure identified the model(s) that offered the optimal trade-off between model accuracy and complexity. We began by comparing the evidence for the two PEB models described above, which differed in whether left or right hemisphere regions served as driving inputs, i.e., the log Bayes factor was F m 1 F m 2 . We then performed a series of comparisons of a full PEB model against reduced models where different mixtures of regressors (columns of the design matrix X in Equation (3)) were switched off, by setting restrictive priors on the corresponding regression parameters ( β ). The probability for each parameter being present versus absent was calculated by performing repeated Bayesian model comparisons, with each parameter switched on versus switched off.

3. Results

3.1. Participant Demographics

Of 635 participants, 319 were male (50%), aged from 18 to 88 (18–28: n = 50, 28–38: n = 102, 38–48: n = 99, 48–58: n = 98, 58–68: n = 100, 68–78: n = 99, 78–88: n = 87).

3.2. Region of Interest (ROI) Analyses

A preliminary mass-univariate (SPM) analysis identified significant effects of age in bilateral SMA (left: 52 voxels, right: 66 voxels) and right premotor/motor regions (rPMd: 38 voxels and rM1: 188 voxels). There were no significant effects of age in left premotor/motor regions (lPMd and lM1). Consequently, we specified three ROIs per hemisphere: SMA, PMd and M1, illustrated in Figure 1A. The distribution of voxels within each ROI across subjects is shown in the Supplementary Material (Figure S3).
The effect of age on right M1 is illustrated in Figure 1B. Each subject’s response is shown as a grey dot; overlaid upon these is a third order polynomial model fit, as was used to identify the regions in the SPM analysis. (Similar curves for all regions of interest are provided in Figure S4). Visual inspection suggested at least two types of subjects. People with negative BOLD responses were more likely to be under 50, whereas people with positive BOLD responses were more likely to be over 50.
To formalise this observation, we fitted a Gaussian mixture model to the same two-dimensional data and used it to cluster the subjects into two groups (Figure 1C). The model was initialized with two clusters, one with subjects who had positive rM1 responses, and the other with subjects who had negative rM1 responses. Following model estimation using the EM algorithm, 46% of subjects were assigned to the group with mainly negative rM1 responses; they were generally younger in age (mean age 48 years, SD 17). The remaining 54% of subjects, with generally positive BOLD responses in rM1, were typically older (mean age 62 years, SD 15). We will refer to these two groups as the negative and positive responders, respectively. We compared the log evidence for this two-cluster mixture model, approximated by the AIC and BIC, against the log evidence of a control model with only a single cluster. The two-cluster model was better in both cases ( Δ AIC: 173.71, Δ BIC: 146.99). In summary, clustering the subjects into two groups served to collapse the two highly correlated variables of age and rM1 BOLD response into one principal mode of variation, simplifying the connectivity analyses that followed.
Next, we identified the neural dynamics and haemodynamics of the six brain regions shown in Figure 1A that could distinguish negative and positive responders.

3.3. First Level DCM Analysis

Our first connectivity question was whether the multivariate fMRI data from the six regions of interest were best explained as being driven by the left or right hemisphere (SMA and PMd). We formalised each of these two hypotheses as a connectivity model (DCM), as illustrated in Figure 2.
After fitting the two DCMs per subject, we checked their explained variance, which serves as a diagnostic for model fitting. More than 10% of variance explained is generally considered acceptable for DCM for fMRI in the time domain. (Although, we note that this statistic cannot be used to compare models, as it does not account for differences in model complexity.). For model 1 (left driving), the average variance explained over subjects was: lM1 = 34%, lPMd = 22%, lSMA = 24%, rM1 = 25%, rPMd = 21%, rSMA = 23%. For model 2 (right driving), the average was: lM1 = 32%, lPMd = 22%, lSMA = 24%, rM1 = 25%, rPMd = 21% and rSMA = 23%. Therefore, we were satisfied that the models explained a non-trivial amount of variance overall.

3.4. Does Left or Right Hemisphere Drive the Network?

We extended the subject-specific DCMs to the group level using the parametric empirical Bayes (PEB) framework. We specified two hierarchical (PEB) models—one for the left driving DCMs and one for the right driving DCMs. Each PEB model consisted of the subject-specific DCMs at the first level and a GLM at the second level. The design matrix of the GLM included regressors to capture the group average strength of each DCM parameter, the difference between positive/negative responder groups for each DCM parameter, and the residual effects of age and the level of rM1 activation after accounting for group.
We performed Bayesian model comparison to identify which of the two PEB models (left or right driving) better explained the group-level data. The log Bayes factor in favour of the left driving model was 2.584 × 10 4 , equivalent to a posterior probability of 1.00. Thus, there was strong evidence that the left (contralateral) hemisphere drove activity in the network. Consequently, we focus on the left driving model for the remainder of this paper.
To confirm the DCMs captured the data feature of interest i.e., negative BOLD in right (ipsilateral) M1, we plotted the predicted BOLD response from the DCMs, averaged over subjects in each of the two groups (Figure 3). The DCMs predicted negative BOLD in rM1 for the negative responder group (who were generally younger), and positive BOLD for the positive responder group (who were generally older). The peak of the response in rM1 was earlier in the positive responder group, five seconds post-stimulus, and later in the negative responder group, seven seconds post-stimulus, to the nearest half second. This pattern was similar in rSMA and rPMd. Noticeably, the two groups also differed in the BOLD response in the left (contralateral) hemisphere. Negative responders had overall lower amplitude responses in all left hemisphere regions.
In summary, the DCMs explained a non-trivial amount of variance and were able to capture the data feature of interest i.e., negative BOLD in rM1. Next, we used these models to test specific hypotheses about the causes of the group difference.

3.5. Do Neural and/or Haemodynamic Parameters Differ between Groups?

The PEB model included parameters for the effect of each covariate (average, group, residual age, residual rM1 activation) on each DCM parameter (matrices A and C from the neural model, transit τ and decay κ from the haemodynamic model). To test whether neural and/or haemodynamic parameters captured the group difference, we compared the evidence for the full hierarchical (PEB) model versus reduced models with particular mixtures of these parameters switched off (fixed at zero).
More specifically, we defined eight sets of parameters: (1) all; (2) A , C ; (3) A ,   τ , κ ; (4) C ,   τ , κ , (5) A only, (6) C only, (7) τ , κ , (8) none. We compared the evidence for 64 candidate PEB models, where each model had its group-average parameters switched on or off according to one of these eight sets, and its parameters encoding the group difference switched on or off according to one of these eight sets. For example, there was a candidate PEB model in which only neural parameters ( A , C ) were switched on at the group level, with only C parameters differing between groups.
The winning PEB model, with posterior probability 1.00, was the model with all types of parameters ( A , C ,   τ , κ ) switched on for both the commonalities across subjects (group average), as well as for the group difference. Thus, both neural and haemodynamic parameters were needed to explain the typical response to the task and the differences between groups. We next examined these neural and haemodynamic parameters in turn.

3.6. Which Neural Connections Differed between Groups?

We identified the specific neural connections that differed between the two groups. We fitted separate PEB models to the connectivity parameters (matrix A ) and driving input parameters (matrix C ) from the neural model (Equation (1)). We then performed an automatic search over thousands of reduced PEB models, iteratively pruning mixtures of connections from the model, where doing so did not reduce the free energy. Finally, the parameters from the best models identified during the search were averaged (weighted by the evidence from the contributing models), shown in Figure 4. Herein, the bars indicate the estimated difference in neural parameters for negative responders minus positive responders. Thus, positive bars indicate effects that were more positive in the younger subjects, whereas negative bars indicate effects that were more positive for the older subjects. (The average connectivity over subjects is additionally reported in supplementary results Section 2.4.)
The task-related input entering the model was more negative for the negative responders, particularly in lSMA (Figure 4i). Thus, increased age was associated with greater activity in the overall network.
The self-inhibition parameter for each region controlled its gain or sensitivity to input. Negative responders had stronger self-inhibition in all left hemisphere regions, whereas positive responders had stronger self-inhibition in rM1 (Figure 4ii). Thus, increasing age was associated with increased activity (disinhibition) of the left hemisphere, and more rapid extinguishing of activity in rM1.
The remaining panels show the between-region connections. The age-related increase in left hemisphere activity, described above, was accompanied by increased inter-hemispheric connectivity from lPMd and lSMA to all right hemisphere regions (Figure 4iii). The largest of these effects were lPMd → rM1 and lSMA → rM1. Conversely, connectivity from lM1 to all right hemisphere was more excitatory in the younger group (positive responders), with the largest effect being lM1 → rM1. Thus, ageing was associated with opposing patterns of inter-hemispheric connectivity: more negative connectivity from lM1 and more positive connectivity from lPMd/lSMA. The intra-hemispheric connections of the left hemisphere showed a similar pattern (Figure 4v).
Finally, any connections originating in the right hemisphere—intra- and inter-hemispheric (Figure 4iv,vi)—were more positive in the negative responder group (Figure 4iv). Ageing was, therefore, associated with decreased (or more negative) efferents from right hemisphere regions.
In summary, in this cross-sectional dataset, the age-related shift from negative to positive BOLD in rM1 was accompanied by, (1) increased activity in the left hemisphere (i.e., greater driving input and disinhibition), (2) more positive inter-hemispheric drive from lSMA and lPMd to the right hemisphere, paired with more negative inter-hemispheric drive from lM1, and (3) decreased inter-hemispheric and intra-hemispheric connectivity from the right hemisphere.

3.7. Which Haemodynamic Parameters Differed between Groups?

The model includes two kinds of haemodynamic parameters. The decay parameter pertains to the neurovascular coupling part of the model. This is a unitless log-scaling parameter controlling the rate of decay of the vasoactive signal. The transit parameters control the mean transit time of venous blood, i.e., the average time it takes for blood to transverse the venous compartment. Again, these are unitless log-scaling parameters. We had a single decay parameter pooled across brain regions, whereas transit parameters were estimated on a per-region basis.
We fitted a PEB model to these haemodynamic parameters and applied an automatic search over reduced models to remove any parameters not contributing to the free energy. The parameters of the best models from this search were averaged, and the results are shown in Figure 5. Plots in the top row show the raw estimated log-scaling parameters. Within the model, the exponential of these parameters was taken to make them positive in sign, and then they were multiplied by a default value (2 s for transit, 0.64 Hz for decay). To aid interpretation, the plots in the bottom row show the same parameters with this transform applied, for each group of subjects separately.
Negative responders had a longer transit time in all three regions of the right hemisphere, with the largest effect in rM1. Thus, blood flow was more sustained in negative responders, who tended to be younger. There was no difference between groups in left hemisphere regions. Additionally, negative responders had a reduced rate of neurovascular decay ( κ ), i.e., their vasoactive signal was more sustained.

3.8. Which Parameters Explain Negative BOLD in rM1?

After having identified model parameters that differed between groups, we next identified which of these parameters determined the sign of the BOLD response in rM1. We specified a DCM that was supplied with the average parameters of subjects in the negative responder group. Then, we systematically varied each parameter around its estimated value, and recorded the effect on the predicted rM1 BOLD response.
There were only four parameters that, when varied in isolation, were able to switch the virtual subject from a negative to a positive rM1 BOLD response (Figure 6). These were all from the neural, rather than haemodynamic part of the model. Three of these were inter-hemispheric neural connections: lM1 → rM1, lPMd → rM1 and lSMA → rM1. By making any of these connections more positive, we also made the rM1 BOLD more positive, and delayed the peak of the response. The amplitude of the rM1 BOLD response was most sensitive to input from lM1, then lPMd, and it was least sensitive to input from lSMA. The fourth parameter was the driving effect of task on lPMd, which had a negative relationship with rM1 BOLD—the more positive the input, the more negative the rM1 BOLD, and vice versa.
Notably, three of these four parameters were those we earlier identified as having group differences: lPMd → rM1, lSMA → rM1 and lM1 → rM1. Therefore, these inter-hemispheric connections were sufficient to explain the change in sign in rM1 BOLD between negative and positive responders.
For completeness, we performed the same analysis but starting from the average parameters of the positive responder group—i.e., we asked whether a typical older subject could have their rM1 BOLD flipped to a negative BOLD response by varying any neural or haemodynamic parameters. We surmised that the same three inter-hemispheric connections (lM1 → rM1, lPMd → rM1 and lSMA → rM1) could flip the sign of the response from positive to negative; however, we determined that the lPMd driving input could not (detailed in supplementary results Section 2.5).
Figure 7 summarises the three connectivity parameters that showed a group difference and could determine the sign of the BOLD response. The largest between-groups differences were lSMA → rM1 and lPMd → rM1, whereas the group difference in lM1 → rM1 was about half the amplitude (Figure 7b). Negative responders, who tended to be younger, had negative inputs to rM1 from both lPMd and lM1 (Figure 7c), whereas positive responders, who tended to be older, had far stronger positive inputs from both lSMA and lPMd, with a negative input from lM1. Therefore, we conclude that in this model, the combined action of increased lSMA → rM1 and lPMd → rM1 with increasing age pushes the rM1 BOLD from negative to positive sign, and this outweighed the smaller negative drive from lM1.
To conclude the analyses, we plotted the estimated values for the three inter-hemispheric connections identified above against the amplitude of the response in rM1, as estimated by SPM (Figure 8). Individually, the lSMA → rM1 connection explained 31% of the variance in rM1 response across subjects (R = 0.56), the lPMd → rM1 connection explained 28% (R = 0.53) and the lM1 → rM1 connection explained close to zero variance (R = 0.03). By placing both the lSMA → rM1 and lPMd → rM1 connections into the same regression model, the overall variance explained in the rM1 response across subjects was 44% (R = 0.66).

4. Discussion

We investigated why the negative BOLD response (NBR) in ipsilateral M1, elicited by unilateral right-hand button presses, becomes more positive with age. We grouped 635 subjects, aged 18–88, into positive and negative rM1 responders and applied dynamic causal modeling (DCM). While there were many neural and haemodynamic parameters that differed between negative and positive responders, simulations demonstrated that only three of the parameters with group differences were sufficient to switch the sign of the rM1 BOLD response. These were neural inter-hemispheric connectivity parameters: lM1 → rM1, lSMA → rM1 and lPMd → rM1. The between-groups differences in two of these parameters, lSMA → rM1 and lPMd → rM1, were particularly strong and they correlated positively with the rM1 BOLD response. Together, these two connections explained 44% of the variance in rM1 response across people.
Ageing has a particularly striking effect on the ipsilateral NBR, with only a modest effect on contralateral PBR. Our preliminary SPM analysis, presented in the supplementary materials, confirmed that there was an effect of ageing on the NBR in ipsilateral rM1 and rPMd, as well as bilateral SMA, with no significant effect on contralateral lM1 or lPMd. We were able to capture this difference in BOLD response between hemispheres using dynamic causal models (Figure 3). Ageing, therefore, does not have a uniform effect on the brain, but rather it alters specific neural populations, brain regions or cognitive processes. This argues against a purely vascular origin for the effects of ageing. To test this hypothesis and compare the evidence for different underlying mechanisms, we used DCM to partition the variance in subjects’ fMRI data into neural, neurovascular and haemodynamic contributions.
Using DCM, we sought to identify the best model of the fMRI data, where ‘best’ has a precise meaning in a Bayesian setting. Models are compared based on their log evidence, also called the log marginal likelihood, ln p y | m . This is the log probability of having observed the data y given the model m . This quantity, which is approximated in DCM by the free energy, can be decomposed into the accuracy of the model minus its complexity. Therefore, by comparing models of the fMRI data and identifying those with the highest log evidence, we sought to identify models that offered the optimal trade-off between accuracy and simplicity.
We started by specifying two biophysical models (DCMs) for each subject’s data. We opted to use the tried-and-tested deterministic DCM for the fMRI model [42,43], which made minimal assumptions about neural dynamics, coupled with an established model of haemodynamics [43]. We then extended these models to include between-subjects effects, using the Parametric Empirical Bayes (PEB) framework. Using Bayesian model comparison, we determined that the data were best explained by a network driven by the contralateral, rather than ipsilateral hemisphere. Note that a caveat of this result, and those which follow, is that the fMRI task only involved responses with the right hand. Therefore, we could not separate the left/right hemispheres from the contralateral/ipsilateral hemispheres with these data.
We then used the winning model to investigate which of four types of parameters best explained the difference between groups. We tried different mixtures of the effective connectivity ( A ), the driving inputs ( C ), the rate of decay of neurovascular coupling ( κ ) and/or the haemodynamic time parameters τ . We had hoped that this Bayesian model comparison would whittle down the number of parameters differing between groups. Instead, the group difference was expressed in all four types of parameters. Given the many ways in which ageing affects the brain and its vasculature, this is perhaps not surprising. Furthermore, with such a large number of subjects, we were well placed to detect very small effects. One further technical explanation for why group differences may have been distributed across many parameters stems from the definition of complexity in the free energy. In this context, complexity is a measure of the deviation of the parameters from their prior values, weighted by their prior precisions (the KL-divergence). In the presence of co-varying parameters, moving one parameter far from its prior can have a larger complexity cost than moving many parameters a small distance from their priors [44]. This ‘many-hands-make-light-work’ phenomenon can cause experimental effects to be distributed across many parameters.
One clear pattern that emerged from the estimated parameters was that positive responders, who tended to be older, had stronger left hemisphere neural activity, mediated by more positive driving input entering lSMA, together with more positive inter-hemispheric connections from the left to right hemisphere. This gave rise to higher amplitude (positive) BOLD responses in all regions. These results are consistent with many previous studies, which have shown that older participants have stronger activation in response to motor tasks than younger participants [4,5]. Our results are also largely consistent with two previous DCM studies [26,27], which found stronger connectivity from the contralateral motor regions to ipsilateral M1 with increasing age. An earlier DCM study identified that SMA inhibits M1 during motor imagery [45], and it would be interesting to determine whether the ageing effects we observed are specific to motor execution, or if they would also pertain to imagery.
A key advantage of having a generative model of the fMRI data is the ability to perform virtual experiments—lesioning or varying the strength of particular connections—and observing the resulting effects on particular data features. Herein, we varied all neural and haemodynamic parameters and recorded the effect on the ipsilateral rM1 BOLD response. We found that only neural, not haemodynamic, parameters were able shift the sign of the BOLD response from negative to positive, and three of these connections also showed differences between groups: lM1 → rM1, lSMA → rM1 and lPMd → rM1. Increasing any of these connections was sufficient to progress from a negative to positive rM1 BOLD response, in a similar manner to the process of ageing.
Of these three connections, the lM1 → rM1 connection had the smallest difference between groups (around half the effect size of the other two connections), and the estimated strength of this connection did not correlate with the overall level of activity in rM1 across subjects. This is consistent with tractography studies in non-human primates, which have found little or no transcallosal connectivity between the finger areas of left and right M1 [36,46]. Tractography studies have also found a lack of direct transcallosal connections between SMA and M1 [36,47]. Therefore, the lM1 → rM1 and lSMA → rM1 effective connectivity identified here is most likely to be mediated polysynaptically. Although in this study, we took an unconstrained approach by allowing all regions to directly interact, future studies could investigate the anatomical path that supports this polysynaptic connectivity, by comparing the evidence for DCMs with different anatomically-constrained priors on the connectivity parameters. For example, a model could be specified that forces lSMA → rM1 effective connectivity to be mediated by rSMA, recognising that there are dense transcallosal SMA → SMA connections [36,47]. The Matlab scripts provided with this paper are intended to make it straightforward to test hypotheses of this sort using the Cam-CAN dataset. Furthermore, non-invasive tractography findings in humans (diffusion-weighted imaging) could be integrated into the DCM model by setting empirical priors on the connectivity parameters [48,49].
Why might inter-hemispheric connections become stronger with age? One possibility is that they compensate for age-related decline [50,51], which could account for the decrease in functional asymmetry across hemispheres (‘HAROLD’). However, this compensation story has recently been challenged. Using the same Cam-CAN dataset as we used in this study, Knights et al. (2021) found that the level of ipsilateral activity was not associated with behavioral performance, as may be expected if there was a need for compensation that was not entirely fulfilled. Furthermore, including the ipsilateral hemisphere motor activity in a statistical model provided no additional information about the movement being performed, beyond including only the contralateral neural activity [12]. A similar result was found in a study investigating compensation as an explanation for the shift from posterior to anterior regions with age (‘PASA’) [52].
One caveat with those studies, and our study, is that the tasks used have been very simple. Previous work has demonstrated that increasing task complexity is associated with stronger BOLD response in ipsilateral motor cortex—specifically, pressing buttons in sequence or as chords in comparison to simple tapping [53]. Increasing the level of force applied also increases the ipsilateral M1 response and inter-hemispheric functional connectivity [54]. With TMS, it has been found that as the force of unilateral wrist flexion movements increases, there is a decrease in local inhibition within ipsilateral M1 (short-interval intracortical inhibition, SICI), which is modulated by the level of inter-hemispheric inhibition (IHI) from contralateral to ipsilateral M1 [55]. Thus, with the simple button pressing task applied here, which involved only minimal force, it is likely that we elicited only the minimal level of activity that would be expected in ipsilateral motor cortex.
There may be other physiological, rather than cognitive, explanations for the change in neural inter-hemispheric connectivity, and the resulting de-differentiation of the hemispheres. As set out in the introduction, an age-related reduction in the thickness of the corpus callosum is often noted [21], with associated reduction in IHI, which may alter the dynamics of inter-hemispheric communication. Another candidate mechanism for age-related reduction in brain asymmetry comes from recent work in epigenetics [56]. That study found higher DNA (CpH) methylation in the left hemisphere of the brain than the right, a marker that correlates with the repression of enhancers and promoters in neurons. This asymmetry decreased with age, with the epigenome of the right hemisphere becoming more similar to the left over the years. This may give rise to (as yet unknown) changes in the neurons of the right hemisphere, reducing hemispheric specialisation.
Neural explanations such as these are consistent with our finding that only neural connectivity parameters, rather than neurovascular or haemodynamic parameters, could account for the shift in ipsilateral BOLD response with age. This also aligns with previous studies that have found a tight coupling between negative BOLD and decreases in neural firing relative to spontaneous activity, in humans [8,57] and in non-human primates [58]. Additionally, using the Cam-CAN dataset, Tsvetanov et al. found that age-related increases ipsilateral motor responses could not be explained by vascular reactivity, as approximated using the amplitude of resting state fMRI [25]. Nevertheless, there are limitations to our study that may have reduced our ability to detect effects that were specifically neurovascular or haemodynamic. First, we only used one type of data—BOLD fMRI—with no independent measures of neural activity (EEG/MEG) or blood flow (ASL fMRI). We, instead, separated out neural and non-neural effects using a generative model, which had both region-specific parameters and neurovascular coupling parameters shared across regions. This complemented an efficient experimental design that sampled a wide range of inter-stimulus intervals, enabling the characterisation of non-linearities in the haemodynamic response. Nevertheless, to confidently distinguish vascular effects from neural responses would require the integration of multiple modalities of neuro-imaging data. The Cam-CAN dataset includes direct neural recordings—magnetoencephalography (MEG)—of the same subjects who underwent fMRI while performing the motor tasks. These data could be integrated with the fMRI data via a ‘Bayesian fusion’ approach, whereby a common generative model gives rise to both kinds of data [59,60]. This would enable the evidence to be formally assessed for between-subjects effects having neuronal and/or haemodynamic origins. We plan to apply these methods in future.
A second limitation of our study pertains to the generative model we used. It enabled us to estimate the strength of neurovascular coupling on a per-subject level, however it could not account for excitatory and inhibitory neural populations within each brain region having distinct neurovascular coupling. This may be relevant, given findings of differential metabolic demand for positive relative to negative BOLD responses [8]. This limitation was first addressed in an extension to DCM by Havlicek et al. [61], who re-parameterised the neurovascular coupling model in tandem with the use of a two-state-per-region neural model. A further step-up in biological detail would be to apply a laminar haemodynamic model [62], which together with high-resolution fMRI data (likely at 7T or above) and a laminar model of neural dynamics, could enable a more spatially fine-grained investigation into the genesis of the NBR and its age-related changes.
While the connections that we identified could account for a significant amount of the inter-subject variability in rM1 response, there remains a lot of variance to explain. It is clear from the data (Figure 1B) that some 30-year-olds had rM1 responses similar to those of 80-year-olds, while some 80-year-olds had responses similar to the youngest participants in the dataset. Our long-term goal is to understand the reason for these inter-subject differences and to determine whether they correlate with health and clinical measures. We anticipate this will be aided by having a sensitive characterisation of the physiological processes causing the fMRI data, such as the estimates of connection strengths established here. Accompanying this paper, we provide the connectivity estimates from each subject, as well as the scripts needed to reproduce the analyses. Hypotheses can then be tested by correlating these model parameters against selected variables from the Cam-CAN dataset, which includes hundreds of lifestyle variables, demographic data as well as physiological measures.

5. Conclusions

The novelty of this study was the use of generative models (DCM for fMRI), with parameters fitted to an exceptionally rich fMRI dataset, in order to quantify the neural, neurovascular and haemodynamic contributions to the negative ipsilateral BOLD response across the lifespan. With a simple button-pressing task conducted with the right hand, we found that the negative BOLD response in ipsilateral rM1, but not contralateral lM1, became more positive with age. Participants with positive rM1 BOLD had stronger inter-hemispheric effective connectivity from lSMA and lPMd to rM1, and these two connections were able to explain 44% of the variance in the rM1 response across participants. Therefore, we conclude that inter-hemispheric neural connectivity from lSMA and lPMd are major contributors to the age-related change in the sign of the rM1 BOLD response.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/brainsci11091130/s1, supplementary methods (preparation of behavioural covariates; SPM analysis), supplementary results (SPM: average response to auditory + visual trials; SPM: effects of age; neural parameters; simulations in the positive responder group; re-analysis with adjusted lSMA and lM1 masks) and supplementary tables (Table S1: covariates used in the behavioural analysis and their definition; Table S2: effects of age on whole-brain fMRI results). The supplementary figures are Figure S1: the main effect of task; Figure S2: effects of age on the response to auditory + visual trials; Figure S3: voxels that were used to generate a representative timeseries for each region of interest (ROI); Figure S4: correlation between age and response to Auditory + Visual trials; Figure S5: posterior parameter estimates of the neural part of the model; Figure S6: simulations based on the average model parameters of positive responder subjects; Figure S7: summary of connections that showed a group difference and could determine the sign of the BOLD response, as demonstrated using simulations.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, P.Z.; investigation, Y.W.T. and P.Z.; data curation, E.K. and R.H.; writing—original draft preparation, Y.W.T. and P.Z.; writing—review and editing, P.Z., Y.W.T., E.K. and R.H.; supervision, P.Z. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging (P.Z. and Y.W.T.) is supported by core funding from Wellcome [203147/Z/16/Z]. Data collection and sharing for this project was provided by the Cambridge Centre for Ageing and Neuroscience (Cam-CAN). Cam-CAN funding was provided by the UK Biotechnology and Biological Sciences Research Council (grant number BB/H008217/1), together with support from the UK Medical Research Council and University of Cambridge, UK. Cam-CAN receives additional funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (‘LifeBrain’, Grant Agreement No. 732592), which supported E.K.; R.N.H. was supported by the Medical Research Council (SUAG/046 G101400).

Institutional Review Board Statement

Data used for this study were previously collected by Cam-CAN and made openly available to researchers. These data were collected in compliance with the Helsinki Declaration and approved by the Cambridgeshire 2 (now East of England-Cambridge Central) Research Ethics Committee (reference: 10/H0308/50).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data are available on request from this website: https://camcan-archive.mrc-cbu.cam.ac.uk/dataaccess/ (accessed on 4 May 2020). Matlab scripts and models arising from this study are available from https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.15390864 (published on 23 August 2021).

Acknowledgments

Thank you to Edda Bilek for advice on figure preparation.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Chettouf, S.; Rueda-Delgado, L.M.; de Vries, R.; Ritter, P.; Daffertshofer, A. Are unimanual movements bilateral? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2020, 113, 39–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Bundy, D.T.; Leuthardt, E.C. The cortical physiology of ipsilateral limb movements. Trends Neurosci. 2019, 42, 825–839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Riecker, A.; Gröschel, K.; Ackermann, H.; Steinbrink, C.; Witte, O.; Kastrup, A. Functional significance of age-related differences in motor activation patterns. NeuroImage 2006, 32, 1345–1354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Hutchinson, S.; Kobayashi, M.; Horkan, C.M.; Pascual-Leone, A.; Alexander, M.P.; Schlaug, G. Age-related differences in movement representation. NeuroImage 2002, 17, 1720–1728. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Mattay, V.S.; Fera, F.; Tessitore, A.; Hariri, A.R.; Das, S.; Callicott, J.H.; Weinberger, D.R. Neurophysiological correlates of age-related changes in human motor function. Neurology 2002, 58, 630–635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Ward, N.S.; Swayne, O.B.; Newton, J.M. Age-dependent changes in the neural correlates of force modulation: An fMRI study. Neurobiol. Aging 2008, 29, 1434–1446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Manganotti, P.; Gerloff, C.; Toro, C.; Katsuta, H.; Sadato, N.; Zhuang, P.A.; Leocani, L.; Hallett, M. Task-related coherence and task-related spectral power changes during sequential finger movements. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. Electromyogr. Mot. Control 1998, 109, 50–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Mullinger, K.J.; Mayhew, S.D.; Bagshaw, A.P.; Bowtell, R.; Francis, S.T. Evidence that the negative BOLD response is neuronal in origin: A simultaneous EEG–BOLD–CBF study in humans. NeuroImage 2014, 94, 263–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Crone, N.E.; Miglioretti, D.L.; Gordon, B.; Sieracki, J.M.; Wilson, M.T.; Uematsu, S.; Lesser, R.P. Functional mapping of human sensorimotor cortex with electrocorticographic spectral analysis. I. Alpha and beta event-related desynchronization. Brain J. Neurol. 1998, 121, 2271–2299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Bundy, D.T.; Szrama, N.; Pahwa, M.; Leuthardt, E.C. Unilateral, 3D arm movement kinematics are encoded in ipsilateral human cortex. J. Neurosci. 2018, 38, 10042–10056. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Diedrichsen, J.; Wiestler, T.; Krakauer, J.W. Two distinct ipsilateral cortical representations for individuated finger movements. Cereb. Cortex 2013, 23, 1362–1377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Knights, E.; Morcom, A.; Henson, R.N. Does Hemispheric Asymmetry Reduction in Older Adults (HAROLD) in motor cortex reflect compensation? bioRxiv 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Stinear, J.W.; Byblow, W.D. Disinhibition in the human motor cortex is enhanced by synchronous upper limb movements. J. Physiol. 2002, 543, 307–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Ferbert, A.; Priori, A.; Rothwell, J.C.; Day, B.L.; Colebatch, J.G.; Marsden, C.D. Interhemispheric inhibition of the human motor cortex. J. Physiol. 1992, 453, 525–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Chen, R. Interactions between inhibitory and excitatory circuits in the human motor cortex. Exp. Brain Res. 2004, 154, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Asanuma, H.; Okuda, O. Effects of transcallosal volleys on pyramidal tract cell activity of cat. J. Neurophysiol. 1962, 25, 198–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Irlbacher, K.; Brocke, J.; Mechow, J.V.; Brandt, S.A. Effects of GABAA and GABAB agonists on interhemispheric inhibition in man. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2007, 118, 308–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Daffertshofer, A.; Peper, C.L.E.; Beek, P.J. Stabilization of bimanual coordination due to active interhemispheric inhibition: A dynamical account. Biol. Cybern. 2005, 92, 101–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Davare, M.; Duque, J.; Vandermeeren, Y.; Thonnard, J.L.; Olivier, E. Role of the ipsilateral primary motor cortex in controlling the timing of hand muscle recruitment. Cereb. Cortex 2007, 17, 353–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Shmuel, A.; Yacoub, E.; Pfeuffer, J.; van de Moortele, P.F.; Adriany, G.; Hu, X.; Ugurbil, K. Sustained negative BOLD, blood flow and oxygen consumption response and its coupling to the positive response in the human brain. Neuron 2002, 36, 1195–1210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Danielsen, V.M.; Vidal-Piñeiro, D.; Mowinckel, A.M.; Sederevicius, D.; Fjell, A.M.; Walhovd, K.B.; Westerhausen, R. Lifespan trajectories of relative corpus callosum thickness: Regional differences and cognitive relevance. Cortex 2020, 130, 127–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Tscherpel, C.; Hensel, L.; Lemberg, K.; Freytag, J.; Michely, J.; Volz, L.J.; Fink, G.R.; Grefkes, C. Age affects the contribution of ipsilateral brain regions to movement kinematics. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2020, 41, 640–655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Cabeza, R. Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: The HAROLD model. Psychol. Aging 2002, 17, 85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Davis, S.W.; Dennis, N.A.; Daselaar, S.M.; Fleck, M.S.; Cabeza, R. Que PASA? The posterior—Anterior shift in aging. Cereb. Cortex 2008, 18, 1201–1209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Tsvetanov, K.A.; Henson, R.N.; Tyler, L.K.; Davis, S.W.; Shafto, M.A.; Taylor, J.R.; Williams, N.; Rowe, J.B. The effect of ageing on f MRI: Correction for the confounding effects of vascular reactivity evaluated by joint f MRI and MEG in 335 adults. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2015, 36, 2248–2269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Boudrias, M.-H.; Gonçalves, C.S.; Penny, W.D.; Park, C.H.; Rossiter, H.E.; Talelli, P.; Ward, N.S. Age-related changes in causal interactions between cortical motor regions during hand grip. NeuroImage 2012, 59, 3398–3405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Michely, J.; Volz, L.J.; Hoffstaedter, F.; Tittgemeyer, M.; Eickhoff, S.B.; Fink, G.R.; Grefkes, C. Network connectivity of motor control in the ageing brain. NeuroImage Clin. 2018, 18, 443–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Shafto, M.A.; Tyler, L.K.; Dixon, M.; Taylor, J.R.; Rowe, J.B.; Cusack, R.; Calder, A.J.; Marslen-Wilson, W.D.; Duncan, J.; Dalgleish, T.; et al. The Cambridge Centre for Ageing and Neuroscience (Cam-CAN) study protocol: A cross-sectional, lifespan, multidisciplinary examination of healthy cognitive ageing. BMC Neurol. 2014, 14, 204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Taylor, J.R.; Williams, N.; Cusack, R.; Auer, T.; Shafto, M.A.; Dixon, M.; Tyler, L.K.; Henson, R.N. The Cambridge Centre for Ageing and Neuroscience (Cam-CAN) data repository: Structural and functional MRI, MEG, and cognitive data from a cross-sectional adult lifespan sample. NeuroImage 2017, 144, 262–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Worsley, K.J.; Friston, K.J. Analysis of fMRI time-series revisited—Again. NeuroImage 1995, 2, 173–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Mayka, M.A.; Corcos, D.M.; Leurgans, S.E.; Vaillancourt, D.E. Three-dimensional locations and boundaries of motor and premotor cortices as defined by functional brain imaging: A meta-analysis. NeuroImage 2006, 31, 1453–1474. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Zeidman, P.; Jafarian, A.; Corbin, N.; Seghier, M.L.; Razi, A.; Price, C.J.; Friston, K.J. A guide to group effective connectivity analysis, part 1: First level analysis with DCM for fMRI. NeuroImage 2019, 200, 174–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Heinzle, J.; Koopmans, P.J.; Ouden, H.D.; Raman, S.; Stephan, K.E. A hemodynamic model for layered BOLD signals. NeuroImage 2016, 125, 556–570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Donahue, M.J.; Hoogduin, H.; van Zijl, P.C.M.; Jezzard, P.; Luijten, P.R.; Hendrikse, J. Blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) total and extravascular signal changes and ΔR 2 * in human visual cortex at 1.5, 3.0 and 7.0 T. NMR Biomed. 2010, 24, 25–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Uludag, K.; Müller-Bierl, B.; Uğurbil, K. An integrative model for neuronal activity-induced signal changes for gradient and spin echo functional imaging. NeuroImage 2009, 48, 150–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Rouiller, E.M.; Babalian, A.; Kazennikov, O.; Moret, V.; Yu, X.-H.; Wiesendanger, M. Transcallosal connections of the distal forelimb representations of the primary and supplementary motor cortical areas in macaque monkeys. Exp. Brain Res. 1994, 102, 227–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Ruddy, K.L.; Leemans, A.; Carson, R. Transcallosal connectivity of the human cortical motor network. Brain Struct. Funct. 2017, 222, 1243–1252. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Schulz, R.; Braass, H.; Liuzzi, G.; Hoerniss, V.; Lechner, P.; Gerloff, C.; Hummel, F.C. White matter integrity of premotor-motor connections is associated with motor output in chronic stroke patients. NeuroImage Clin. 2015, 7, 82–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Pool, E.-M.; Rehme, A.K.; Fink, G.R.; Eickhoff, S.B.; Grefkes, C. Handedness and effective connectivity of the motor system. NeuroImage 2014, 99, 451–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Friston, K.; Mattout, J.; Trujillo-Barreto, N.; Ashburner, J.; Penny, W. Variational free energy and the Laplace approximation. NeuroImage 2007, 34, 220–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Xia, M.; Wang, J.; He, Y. BrainNet viewer: A network visualization tool for human brain connectomics. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e68910. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  42. Friston, K.J.; Harrison, L.; Penny, W. Dynamic causal modelling. NeuroImage 2003, 19, 1273–1302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Stephan, K.E.; Weiskopf, N.; Drysdale, P.M.; Robinson, P.; Friston, K. Comparing hemodynamic models with DCM. NeuroImage 2007, 38, 387–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Litvak, V.; Jafarian, A.; Zeidman, P.; Tibon, R.; Henson, R.N.; Friston, K. There’s no such thing as a ‘true’ model: The challenge of assessing face validity. In Proceedings of the 2019 IEEE International Conference on Systems, Man and Cybernetics (IEEE SMC 2019), Bari, Italy, 6–9 October 2019. [Google Scholar]
  45. Kasess, C.; Windischberger, C.; Cunnington, R.; Lanzenberger, R.; Pezawas, L.; Moser, E. The suppressive influence of SMA on M1 in motor imagery revealed by fMRI and dynamic causal modeling. NeuroImage 2008, 40, 828–837. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Jenny, A.B. Commissural projections of the cortical hand motor area in monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 1979, 188, 137–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Liu, J.; Morel, A.; Wannier, T.; Rouiller, E.M. Origins of callosal projections to the supplementary motor area (SMA): A direct comparison between pre-SMA and SMA-proper in macaque monkeys. J. Comp. Neurol. 2001, 443, 71–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Sokolov, A.A.; Zeidman, P.; Erb, M.; Ryvlin, P.; Pavlova, M.A.; Friston, K. Linking structural and effective brain connectivity: Structurally informed Parametric Empirical Bayes (si-PEB). Brain Struct. Funct. 2019, 224, 205–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  49. Stephan, K.E.; Tittgemeyer, M.; Knösche, T.R.; Moran, R.J.; Friston, K. Tractography-based priors for dynamic causal models. NeuroImage 2009, 47, 1628–1638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Reuter-Lorenz, P.A.; Cappell, K.A. Neurocognitive Aging and the Compensation Hypothesis. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2008, 17, 177–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Cabeza, R.; Albert, M.; Belleville, S.; Craik, F.I.M.; Duarte, A.; Grady, C.L.; Lindenberger, U.; Nyberg, L.; Park, D.C.; Reuter-Lorenz, P.A.; et al. Maintenance, reserve and compensation: The cognitive neuroscience of healthy ageing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2018, 19, 701–710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Morcom, A.M.; Henson, R.N. Increased prefrontal activity with aging reflects nonspecific neural responses rather than compensation. J. Neurosci. 2018, 38, 7303–7313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Verstynen, T.D.; Diedrichsen, J.; Albert, N.; Aparicio, P.; Ivry, R.B. Ipsilateral motor cortex activity during unimanual hand movements relates to task complexity. J. Neurophysiol. 2005, 93, 1209–1222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Andrushko, J.W.; Gould, L.A.; Renshaw, D.W.; Ekstrand, C.; Hortobágyi, T.; Borowsky, R.; Farthing, J.P. High force unimanual handgrip contractions increase ipsilateral sensorimotor activation and functional connectivity. Neuroscience 2021, 452, 111–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Perez, M.A.; Cohen, L.G. Mechanisms underlying functional changes in the primary motor cortex ipsilateral to an active hand. J. Neurosci. 2008, 28, 5631–5640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Li, P.; Ensink, E.; Lang, S.; Marshall, L.; Schilthuis, M.; Lamp, J.; Vega, I.; Labrie, V. Hemispheric asymmetry in the human brain and in Parkinson’s disease is linked to divergent epigenetic patterns in neurons. Genome Biol. 2020, 21, 61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Pasley, B.N.; Inglis, B.A.; Freeman, R.D. Analysis of oxygen metabolism implies a neural origin for the negative BOLD response in human visual cortex. NeuroImage 2007, 36, 269–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  58. Shmuel, A.; Augath, M.; Oeltermann, A.; Logothetis, N.K. Negative functional MRI response correlates with decreases in neuronal activity in monkey visual area V1. Nat. Neurosci. 2006, 9, 569–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Jafarian, A.; Litvak, V.; Cagnan, H.; Friston, K.; Zeidman, P. Comparing dynamic causal models of neurovascular coupling with fMRI and EEG/MEG. NeuroImage 2020, 216, 116734. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Jafarian, A.; Zeidman, P.; Litvak, V.; Friston, K. Structure learning in coupled dynamical systems and dynamic causal modelling. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. A Math. Phys. Eng. Sci. 2019, 377, 20190048. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Havlicek, M.; Ivanov, D.; Roebroeck, A.; Uludag, K. Determining excitatory and inhibitory neuronal activity from multimodal fMRI data using a generative hemodynamic model. Front. Neurosci. 2017, 11, 616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  62. Uludag, K.; Havlicek, M. Determining laminar neuronal activity from BOLD fMRI using a generative model. Prog. Neurobiol. 2021, 102055. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Regions of interest and the effect of age on right M1. (A) The six regions of interest are shown overlaid on a dorsal view of the cortical surface (left) and on a sagittal view of the right hemisphere (right). M1 = primary motor cortex (red), PMd = dorsal pre-motor cortex (orange), SMA = supplementary motor area (turquoise). (B) Confound-corrected relationship between age and the estimated neural response to Auditory + Visual trials in rM1, from an initial mass-univariate (SPM) analysis. The red line is the best fit of a third order polynomial, matching the SPM analysis used to identify rM1. (C) The same rM1 responses with assignments to two clusters using a Gaussian mixture model. Contours indicate the two Gaussians with colours of the individual subjects (dots) indicating cluster assignment. The younger group (with mainly negative BOLD) were described by a 2D-Gaussian with means [44.77 −3.53] for age and BOLD response, respectively, with variances [289.71 5.20] and covariance −11.16. The older group (with mainly positive BOLD) were described by a 2D-Gaussian with means [62.07 4.51], with variances [238.31 8.12] and covariance −12.43. Renderings were performed using BrainNet Viewer (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/bnv/ (accessed on 23 June 2021)) [41].
Figure 1. Regions of interest and the effect of age on right M1. (A) The six regions of interest are shown overlaid on a dorsal view of the cortical surface (left) and on a sagittal view of the right hemisphere (right). M1 = primary motor cortex (red), PMd = dorsal pre-motor cortex (orange), SMA = supplementary motor area (turquoise). (B) Confound-corrected relationship between age and the estimated neural response to Auditory + Visual trials in rM1, from an initial mass-univariate (SPM) analysis. The red line is the best fit of a third order polynomial, matching the SPM analysis used to identify rM1. (C) The same rM1 responses with assignments to two clusters using a Gaussian mixture model. Contours indicate the two Gaussians with colours of the individual subjects (dots) indicating cluster assignment. The younger group (with mainly negative BOLD) were described by a 2D-Gaussian with means [44.77 −3.53] for age and BOLD response, respectively, with variances [289.71 5.20] and covariance −11.16. The older group (with mainly positive BOLD) were described by a 2D-Gaussian with means [62.07 4.51], with variances [238.31 8.12] and covariance −12.43. Renderings were performed using BrainNet Viewer (http://www.nitrc.org/projects/bnv/ (accessed on 23 June 2021)) [41].
Brainsci 11 01130 g001
Figure 2. Candidate network architectures. The lighter arrows indicate connections among regions (matrix A of the DCM neural model, Equation (1)) and the darker arrows indicate the location of driving inputs (matrix C of the DCM neural model). Self-connections on each region are present but not shown. The left hemisphere of the brain is on the left of each diagram (and in all subsequent figures). SMA = supplementary motor area, PMd = dorsal premotor cortex, M1 = primary motor cortex.
Figure 2. Candidate network architectures. The lighter arrows indicate connections among regions (matrix A of the DCM neural model, Equation (1)) and the darker arrows indicate the location of driving inputs (matrix C of the DCM neural model). Self-connections on each region are present but not shown. The left hemisphere of the brain is on the left of each diagram (and in all subsequent figures). SMA = supplementary motor area, PMd = dorsal premotor cortex, M1 = primary motor cortex.
Brainsci 11 01130 g002
Figure 3. Predicted BOLD responses from the left driving DCM. These responses (first order Volterra kernels) are averaged over subjects from each of the two groups (Figure 1C). The negative (Nve) responder group includes younger subjects, most of whom had negative BOLD responses in rM1. The positive (Pve) responder group includes older subjects, most of whom had positive BOLD responses in rM1. SMA = supplementary motor area, PMd = dorsal premotor cortex, M1 = primary motor cortex.
Figure 3. Predicted BOLD responses from the left driving DCM. These responses (first order Volterra kernels) are averaged over subjects from each of the two groups (Figure 1C). The negative (Nve) responder group includes younger subjects, most of whom had negative BOLD responses in rM1. The positive (Pve) responder group includes older subjects, most of whom had positive BOLD responses in rM1. SMA = supplementary motor area, PMd = dorsal premotor cortex, M1 = primary motor cortex.
Brainsci 11 01130 g003
Figure 4. Group differences in neural parameters (negative minus positive responders). Within each panel, bars greater than zero indicate more positive parameters for the negative responder group (younger subjects); bars less than zero indicate more positive parameters for the positive responder group (older subjects). Each vertical axis is the estimated effect size, in units of Hz, for all panels except the self-connections (panel ii.), which are unitless log-scaling parameters. Pink error bars are 90% Bayesian credible intervals. The network diagrams, inset, illustrate where in the model the connections can be found. Each circle is a brain region—refer to Figure 2 for region names.
Figure 4. Group differences in neural parameters (negative minus positive responders). Within each panel, bars greater than zero indicate more positive parameters for the negative responder group (younger subjects); bars less than zero indicate more positive parameters for the positive responder group (older subjects). Each vertical axis is the estimated effect size, in units of Hz, for all panels except the self-connections (panel ii.), which are unitless log-scaling parameters. Pink error bars are 90% Bayesian credible intervals. The network diagrams, inset, illustrate where in the model the connections can be found. Each circle is a brain region—refer to Figure 2 for region names.
Brainsci 11 01130 g004
Figure 5. Haemodynamic parameters. (Top row) The posterior expected values and 90% credible intervals for the haemodynamic log-scaling parameters. In each plot, the first six bars are the transit parameters for each region, and the final bar is the decay parameter, which is pooled over regions. (Bottom row) The posterior expected values, transformed into the units of the underlying model (secs for transit, Hz for decay), plotted separately for each group of subjects.
Figure 5. Haemodynamic parameters. (Top row) The posterior expected values and 90% credible intervals for the haemodynamic log-scaling parameters. In each plot, the first six bars are the transit parameters for each region, and the final bar is the decay parameter, which is pooled over regions. (Bottom row) The posterior expected values, transformed into the units of the underlying model (secs for transit, Hz for decay), plotted separately for each group of subjects.
Brainsci 11 01130 g005
Figure 6. Simulations. Each plot shows the effect of varying one parameter (indicated by the title and the diagram, inset), on the predicted BOLD response in right M1. The black dotted line indicates the predicted BOLD response under the group average parameters for the negative responder group. The coloured lines are simulated BOLD responses, as a consequence of varying each parameter between its estimated value minus 1 Hz, to its estimated value plus 1 Hz.
Figure 6. Simulations. Each plot shows the effect of varying one parameter (indicated by the title and the diagram, inset), on the predicted BOLD response in right M1. The black dotted line indicates the predicted BOLD response under the group average parameters for the negative responder group. The coloured lines are simulated BOLD responses, as a consequence of varying each parameter between its estimated value minus 1 Hz, to its estimated value plus 1 Hz.
Brainsci 11 01130 g006
Figure 7. Summary of connections that showed a group difference and could determine the sign of the BOLD response, as demonstrated using simulations. Positive connections are excitatory, whereas negative connections are inhibitory.
Figure 7. Summary of connections that showed a group difference and could determine the sign of the BOLD response, as demonstrated using simulations. Positive connections are excitatory, whereas negative connections are inhibitory.
Brainsci 11 01130 g007
Figure 8. Correlation of key inter-hemispheric connections and rM1 response. In each plot, the dots are individual subjects. The horizontal axis is the posterior expected value from the individual subjects’ DCM and the vertical axis is the amplitude of the response in rM1 from the SPM analysis. The line of best fit is in red. Pearson’s correlation coefficients are provided in the titles.
Figure 8. Correlation of key inter-hemispheric connections and rM1 response. In each plot, the dots are individual subjects. The horizontal axis is the posterior expected value from the individual subjects’ DCM and the vertical axis is the amplitude of the response in rM1 from the SPM analysis. The line of best fit is in red. Pearson’s correlation coefficients are provided in the titles.
Brainsci 11 01130 g008
Table 1. Updated DCM parameters.
Table 1. Updated DCM parameters.
ParameterSPM Matlab FunctionSPM12 DefaultNew Value
epsilon 1spm_dcm_fmri_priors10.46
V0 2spm_gx_fmri8%4%
r0 3spm_gx_fmri25 Hz110 Hz
nu0 4spm_gx_fmri40.3 Hz84.8 Hz
1 The ratio of intra-vascular contribution S i to extra-vascular contribution S e to the fMRI signal. We followed the approach of Heinzle et al. [33] to derive a prior density that reflects empirical variability in this value. We considered a range of intra-vascular relaxation rates T 2 , i from 15 ms to 25 ms, and extra-vascular relaxation rates T 2 , e from 30 ms to 45 ms [34]. We computed epsilon for each combination of intra- and extra-vascular rates, in steps of 0.5 ms, according to ϵ = S i S e = exp R 2 , i TE / exp R 2 , e TE , where R 2 , i = 1 / T 2 , i and R 2 , e = 1 / T 2 , e . The echo time was TE = 30   ms . Finally, we fitted a log-normal distribution over the resulting set of ϵ values, which gave Lognormal ln 0.46 ,   0.06 . Due to the identity a X Lognormal μ + ln a , σ 2   X Lognormal μ , σ 2 , we arrived at prior density: 0.46   ·   LogNormal 0 ,   0.06 . In contrast to SPM12, wherein this parameter is estimated from the data, in this study we fixed it to its prior expectation of 0.46 to ensure stability. 2 The resting venous volume: SPM’s default value of 8% is quite high, given experimental findings from recent years. We therefore reduced it to 4%. 3 The intravascular relaxation rate, which is a function of oxygen saturation: The value for r0 can be derived from the slope of the relaxation rate of blood relative to the deoxygenation level [35]. The slope at an oxygenation level of 70% and a hematocrit of 44% yields r0 ≅ 110 Hz (3 T). 4 The frequency offset at the outer surface of magnetized vessels depends linearly on the main magnetic field strength B0: nu0 ≅ 28.265 ⋅ B0.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Tak, Y.W.; Knights, E.; Henson, R.; Zeidman, P. Ageing and the Ipsilateral M1 BOLD Response: A Connectivity Study. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 1130. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11091130

AMA Style

Tak YW, Knights E, Henson R, Zeidman P. Ageing and the Ipsilateral M1 BOLD Response: A Connectivity Study. Brain Sciences. 2021; 11(9):1130. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11091130

Chicago/Turabian Style

Tak, Yae Won, Ethan Knights, Richard Henson, and Peter Zeidman. 2021. "Ageing and the Ipsilateral M1 BOLD Response: A Connectivity Study" Brain Sciences 11, no. 9: 1130. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11091130

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop