Stopping a Response When You Really Care about the Action: Considerations from a Clinical Perspective
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Conceptualisation of Response Inhibition
2.1. Measuring Response Inhibition
2.2. Response Inhibition Tasks in Clinical Research
2.3. On Caring about a Response: Internal and External Context
3. Clinical Theorising
3.1. The Cognitive-Behavioural Model of OCD
3.2. The Utility of Action Tendencies
4. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.P.; Emerson, M.J.; Witzki, A.H.; Howerter, A.; Wager, T.D. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 2000, 41, 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dalley, J.W.; Everitt, B.J.; Robbins, T.W. Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron 2011, 69, 680–694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; American Psychiatric Association: Arlington, VA, USA, 2013; ISBN 0-89042-555-8. [Google Scholar]
- Chamberlain, S.R.; Blackwell, A.D.; Fineberg, N.A.; Robbins, T.W.; Sahakian, B.J. The neuropsychology of obsessive compulsive disorder: The importance of failures in cognitive and behavioural inhibition as candidate endophenotypic markers. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2005, 29, 399–419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morein-Zamir, S.; Robbins, T.W. Fronto-striatal circuits in response-inhibition: Relevance to addiction. Brain Res. 2015, 1628, 117–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Zilverstand, A.; Huang, A.S.; Alia-Klein, N.; Goldstein, R.Z. Neuroimaging impaired response inhibition and salience attribution in human drug addiction: A systematic review. Neuron 2018, 98, 886–903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Insel, T.; Cuthbert, B.; Garvey, M.; Heinssen, R.; Pine, D.S.; Quinn, K.; Sanislow, C.; Wang, P. Research domain criteria (RDoC): Toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am. J. Psychiatry 2010, 167, 748–751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- About RDoC. Available online: https://www.nimh.nih.gov/research/research-funded-by-nimh/rdoc/about-rdoc (accessed on 10 June 2020).
- Evenden, J. Impulsivity: A discussion of clinical and experimental findings. J. Psychopharmacol. 1999, 13, 180–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dalley, J.W.; Robbins, T.W. Fractionating impulsivity: Neuropsychiatric implications. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2017, 18, 158–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luigjes, J.; Lorenzetti, V.; de Haan, S.; Youssef, G.J.; Murawski, C.; Sjoerds, Z.; van den Brink, W.; Denys, D.; Fontenelle, L.F.; Yücel, M. Defining compulsive behavior. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2019, 29, 4–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Robbins, T.W.; Vaghi, M.M.; Banca, P.; Johnson, S.; Andersen, H.C. Review obsessive-compulsive disorder: Puzzles and prospects. Neuron 2019, 102, 27–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Willcutt, E.G.; Doyle, A.E.; Nigg, J.T.; Faraone, S.V.; Pennington, B.F. Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biol. Psychiatry 2005, 57, 1336–1346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miyake, A.; Friedman, N.P. The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 21, 8–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Abramovitch, A.; Abramowitz, J.S.; Mittelman, A. The neuropsychology of adult obsessive-compulsive disorder: A meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2013, 33, 1163–1171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Snyder, H.R.; Kaiser, R.H.; Warren, S.L.; Heller, W. Obsessive-Compulsive disorder is associated with broad impairments in executive function: A meta-analysis. Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2014, 3, 1–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Martel, M.; Nikolas, M.; Nigg, J.T. Executive function in adolescents with ADHD. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2007, 46, 1437–1444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Faraone, S.V.; Asherson, P.; Banaschewski, T.; Biederman, J.; Ramos-Quiroga, J.A.; Rohde, L.A.; Tannock, R.; Franke, B. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nat. Reviews Dis. Primers 2015, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Logan, G.D.; Cowan, W.B.; Davis, K.A. On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: A model and a method. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1984, 10, 276–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Logan, G.D.; Schachar, R.J.; Tannock, R. Impulsivity and inhibitory control. Psychol. Sci. 1997, 8, 60–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Verbruggen, F.; Aron, A.R.; Band, G.P.; Beste, C.; Bissett, P.G.; Brockett, A.T.; Brown, J.W.; Chamberlain, S.R.; Chambfers, C.D.; Colonius, H.; et al. A consensus guide to capturing the ability to inhibit actions and impulsive behaviors in the stop-signal task. Elife 2019, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aron, A.R. From reactive to proactive and selective control: Developing a richer model for stopping inappropriate responses. Biol. Psychiatry 2011, 69, e55–e68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Verbruggen, F.; Logan, G.D. Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the stop-signal paradigm. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2009, 35, 835–854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Logan, G.D. On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A users guide to the stop-signal Paradigm. In Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory, and Language; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1994; pp. 189–239. [Google Scholar]
- Castro-Meneses, L.J.; Johnson, B.W.; Sowman, P.F. The effects of impulsivity and proactive inhibition on reactive inhibition and the go process: Insights from vocal and manual stop signal tasks. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Jahfari, S.; Stinear, C.M.; Claffey, M.; Verbruggen, F.; Aron, A.R. Responding with restraint: What are the neurocognitive mechanisms? J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2010, 22, 1479–1492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Morein-Zamir, S.; Chua, R.; Franks, I.; Nagelkerke, P.; Kingstone, A. Predictability influences stopping and response control. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2007, 33, 149–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zandbelt, B.B.; Vink, M. On the role of the striatum in response inhibition. PLoS ONE 2010, 5, e13848. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vink, M.; Kaldewaij, R.; Zandbelt, B.B.; Pas, P.; du Plessis, S. The role of stop-signal probability and expectation in proactive inhibition. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2015, 41, 1086–1094. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mirabella, G.; Pani, P.; Ferraina, S. Context influences on the preparation and execution of reaching movements. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 2008, 25, 996–1010. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chamberlain, S.R.; Fineberg, N.A.; Menzies, L.A.; Blackwell, A.D.; Bullmore, E.T.; Robbins, T.W.; Sahakian, B.J. Impaired cognitive flexibility and motor inhibition in unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 2007, 164, 335–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kalanthroff, E.; Steinman, S.A.; Schmidt, A.B.; Campeas, R.; Simpson, H.B. Piloting a personalized computerized inhibitory training program for individuals with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychother. Psychosom. 2018, 87, 52–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hedge, C.; Powell, G.; Sumner, P. The reliability paradox: Why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences. Behav. Res. Methods 2018, 50, 1166–1186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eisenberg, I.W.; Bissett, P.G.; Zeynep Enkavi, A.; Li, J.; MacKinnon, D.P.; Marsch, L.A.; Poldrack, R.A. Uncovering the structure of self-regulation through data-driven ontology discovery. Nat. Commun. 2019, 10, 2319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kuelz, A.K.; Hohagen, F.; Voderholzer, U. Neuropsychological performance in obsessive-compulsive disorder: A critical review. Biol. Psychol. 2004, 65, 185–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mirabella, G. Inhibitory control and impulsive responses in neurodevelopmental disorders. Dev. Med. Child. Neurol. 2021, 63, 520–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kalanthroff, E.; Teichert, T.; Wheaton, M.G.; Kimeldorf, M.B.; Linkovski, O.; Ahmari, S.E.; Fyer, A.J.; Schneier, F.R.; Anholt, G.E.; Simpson, H.B. The role of response inhibition in medicated and unmedicated obsessive-compulsive disorder patients: Evidence from the stop-signal task. Depress. Anxiety 2017, 34, 301–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lipszyc, J.; Schachar, R. Inhibitory control and psychopathology: A meta-analysis of studies using the stop signal task. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2010, 16, 1064–1076. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Morein-Zamir, S.; Fineberg, N.A.; Robbins, T.W.; Sahakian, B.J. Inhibition of thoughts and actions in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Extending the endophenotype? Psychol. Med. 2010, 40, 263–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bruin, W.B.; Taylor, L.; Thomas, R.M.; Shock, J.P.; Zhutovsky, P.; Abe, Y.; Alonso, P.; Ameis, S.H.; Anticevic, A.; Arnold, P.D.; et al. Structural neuroimaging biomarkers for obsessive-compulsive disorder in the ENIGMA-OCD consortium: Medication matters. Transl. Psychiatry 2020, 10, 342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bari, A.; Robbins, T.W. Inhibition and impulsivity: Behavioral and neural basis of response control. Prog. Neurobiol. 2013, 108, 44–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Abramovitch, A.; Cooperman, A. The cognitive neuropsychology of obsessive-compulsive disorder: A critical review. J. Obsessive. Compuls. Relat. Disord. 2015, 5, 24–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Abramovitch, A.; Dar, R.; Hermesh, H.; Schweiger, A. Comparative neuropsychology of adult obsessive-compulsive disorder and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Implications for a novel executive overload model of OCD. J. Neuropsychol. 2012, 6, 161–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schall, J.D.; Palmeri, T.J.; Logan, G.D. Models of inhibitory control. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2017, 372, 20160193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Verbruggen, F.; Logan, G.D. Response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2008, 12, 418–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Braver, T.S.; Krug, M.K.; Chiew, K.S.; Kool, W.; Westbrook, J.A.; Clement, N.J.; Adcock, R.A.; Barch, D.M.; Botvinick, M.M.; Carver, C.S.; et al. Mechanisms of motivation–cognition interaction: Challenges and opportunities. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2014, 14, 443–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Zelazo, P.D.; Qu, L.; Kesek, A.C. Hot executive function: Emotion and the development of cognitive control. In Child Development at the Intersection of Emotion and Cognition; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2010; pp. 97–111. [Google Scholar]
- Dayan, A.; Berger, A.; Anholt, G.E. Enhanced action tendencies in high versus low obsessive-compulsive symptoms: An event-related potential study. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 2014, 224, 133–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dayan, A.; Berger, A.; Anholt, G.E. Enhanced action tendencies in obsessive-compulsive disorder: An ERP study. Behav. Res. Ther. 2017, 93, 13–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Patterson, T.K.; Lenartowicz, A.; Berkman, E.T.; Ji, D.; Poldrack, R.A.; Knowlton, B.J. Putting the brakes on the brakes: Negative emotion disrupts cognitive control network functioning and alters subsequent stopping ability. Exp. Brain Res. 2016, 234, 3107–3118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Verbruggen, F.; De Houwer, J. Do emotional stimuli interfere with response inhibition? Evidence from the stop signal paradigm. Cogn. Emot. 2007, 21, 391–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pessoa, L.; Padmala, S.; Kenzer, A.; Bauer, A. Interactions between cognition and emotion during response inhibition. Emotion 2012, 12, 192–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- De Putter, L.M.S.; Van Yper, L.; Koster, E.H.W. Obsessions and compulsions in the lab: A meta-analysis of procedures to induce symptoms of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2017, 52, 137–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morein-Zamir, S.; Simon Jones, P.; Bullmore, E.T.; Robbins, T.W.; Ersche, K.D. Prefrontal hypoactivity associated with impaired inhibition in stimulant-dependent individuals but evidence for hyperactivation in their unaffected siblings. Neuropsychopharmacology 2013, 38, 1945–1953. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bartholdy, S.; Dalton, B.; O’Daly, O.G.; Campbell, I.C.; Schmidt, U. A systematic review of the relationship between eating, weight and inhibitory control using the stop signal task. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2016, 64, 35–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Leotti, L.A.; Wager, T.D. Motivational influences on response inhibition measures. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2010, 36, 430–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Boehler, C.N.; Schevernels, H.; Hopf, J.M.; Stoppel, C.M.; Krebs, R.M. Reward prospect rapidly speeds up response inhibition via reactive control. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2014, 14, 593–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Boehler, C.N.; Hopf, J.M.; Stoppel, C.M.; Krebs, R.M. Motivating inhibition—reward prospect speeds up response cancellation. Cognition 2012, 125, 498–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Verbruggen, F.; McLaren, R. Effects of reward and punishment on the interaction between going and stopping in a selective stop-change task. Psychol. Res. 2018, 82, 353–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Morein-Zamir, S.; Papmeyer, M.; Gillan, C.M.; Crockett, M.J.; Fineberg, N.A.; Sahakian, B.J.; Robbins, T.W. Punishment promotes response control deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder: Evidence from a motivational go/no-goa task. Psychol. Med. 2013, 43, 391–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Zhukovsky, P.; Morein-Zamir, S.; Meng, C.; Dalley, J.W.; Ersche, K.D. Network failures: When incentives trigger impulsive responses. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2020, 41, 2216–2228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Barlow, D.H.; Allen, L.B.; Choate, M.L. Toward a Unified Treatment for Emotional Disorders—Republished Article. Behav. Ther. 2016, 47, 838–853. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scherer, K.R. What are emotions? And how can they be measured? Soc. Sci. Inf. 2005, 44, 695–729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beckers, T.; Krypotos, A.-M.; Boddez, Y.; Effting, M.; Kindt, M. What’s wrong with fear conditioning? Biol. Psychol. 2013, 92, 90–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morein-Zamir, S.; Papmeyer, M.; Durieux, A.; Fineberg, N.A.; Sahakian, B.J.; Robbins, T.W. Investigation of attentional bias in obsessive compulsive disorder with and without depression in visual search. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e80118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Salkovskis, P.M. Obsessional-compulsive problems: A cognitive-behavioural analysis. Behav. Res. Ther. 1985, 23, 571–583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wegner, D.M.; Schneider, D.J.; Carter, S.R.; White, T.L. Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1987, 53, 5–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van den Hout, M.; Kindt, M. Repeated checking causes memory distrust. Behav. Res. Ther. 2003, 41, 301–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- OCCWG. Cognitive assessment of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behav. Res. Ther. 1997, 35, 667–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anholt, G.; Kalanthroff, E. Letter to the editor: Recent advances in research on cognition and emotion in OCD: A Review. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 2013, 15, 416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anholt, G.; Kalanthroff, E. Do we need a cognitive theory for obsessive-compulsive disorder? Clin. Neuropsychiatry J. Treat. Eval. 2013, 11, 194–196. [Google Scholar]
- Martoni, R.M.; Risso, G.; Giuliani, M.; De Filippis, R.; Cammino, S.; Cavallini, C.; Bellodi, L. Evaluating proactive strategy in patients with OCD during stop signal task. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2018, 24, 703–714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hallion, L.S.; Tolin, D.F.; Diefenbach, G.J. Enhanced cognitive control over task-irrelevant emotional distractors in generalized anxiety disorder versus obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Anxiety Disord. 2019, 64, 71–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fruehauf, L.M.; Fair, J.E.; Liebel, S.W.; Bjornn, D.; Larson, M.J. Cognitive control in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD): Proactive control adjustments or consistent performance? Psychiatry Res. 2021, 298, 113809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Abramovitch, A.; De Nadai, A.S.; Geller, D.A. Neurocognitive endophenotypes in pediatric OCD probands, their unaffected parents and siblings. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2021, 110, 110283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kalanthroff, E.; Anholt, G.E.; Henik, A. Always on guard: Test of high vs. low control conditions in obsessive-compulsive disorder patients. Psychiatry Res. 2014, 219, 322–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kalanthroff, E.; Marsh, R.; Hassin, R.R.; Simpson, H.B. Evidence for trial-by-trial dynamic adjustment of task control in unmedicated adults with OCD. Behav. Res. Ther. 2020, 126, 103572. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Soref, A.; Liberman, N.; Abramovitch, A.; Dar, R. Explicit instructions facilitate performance of OCD participants but impair performance of non-OCD participants on a serial reaction time task. J. Anxiety Disord. 2018, 55, 56–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Giuliani, M.; Martoni, R.M.; Gregori Grgič, R.; Crespi, S.A.; Cavallini, M.C.; De’Sperati, C. Who has done it? Exploring gaze agency in obsessive-compulsive checkers. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2017, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Oren, E.; Friedmann, N.; Dar, R. Things happen: Individuals with high obsessive-compulsive tendencies omit agency in their spoken language. Conscious. Cogn. 2016, 42, 125–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gibson, J.J. The theory of affordances. In Perceiving, Acting, and Knowing: Toward an Ecological Psychology; Shaw, R., Bransford, J., Eds.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hilldale, PA, USA, 1977; pp. 67–82. [Google Scholar]
- Lowe, R.; Ziemke, T. The feeling of action tendencies: On the emotional regulation of goal-directed behavior. Front. Psychol. 2011, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Van Uijen, S.L.; Toffolo, M.B.J. Safety behavior increases obsession-related cognitions about the severity of threat. Behav. Ther. 2015, 46, 521–531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dayan-Riva, A.; Berger, A.; Anholt, G.E. Affordances, response conflict, and enhanced-action tendencies in obsessive-compulsive disorder: An ERP study. Psychol. Med. 2021, 51, 948–963. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stein, D.J.; Costa, D.L.C.; Lochner, C.; Miguel, E.C.; Reddy, Y.C.J.; Shavitt, R.G.; van den Heuvel, O.A.; Simpson, H.B. Obsessive–compulsive disorder. Nat. Rev. Dis. Prim. 2019, 5, 52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lynch, T.R.; Chapman, A.L.; Rosenthal, M.Z.; Kuo, J.R.; Linehan, M.M. Mechanisms of change in dialectical behavior therapy: Theoretical and empirical observations. J. Clin. Psychol. 2006, 62, 459–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolitzky, K.B.; Telch, M.J. Augmenting in vivo exposure with fear antagonistic actions: A preliminary test. Behav. Ther. 2009, 40, 57–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Amir, N.; Kuckertz, J.M.; Najmi, S. The effect of modifying automatic action tendencies on overt avoidance behaviors. Emotion 2013, 13, 478–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Morein-Zamir, S.; Anholt, G. Stopping a Response When You Really Care about the Action: Considerations from a Clinical Perspective. Brain Sci. 2021, 11, 979. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11080979
Morein-Zamir S, Anholt G. Stopping a Response When You Really Care about the Action: Considerations from a Clinical Perspective. Brain Sciences. 2021; 11(8):979. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11080979
Chicago/Turabian StyleMorein-Zamir, Sharon, and Gideon Anholt. 2021. "Stopping a Response When You Really Care about the Action: Considerations from a Clinical Perspective" Brain Sciences 11, no. 8: 979. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11080979
APA StyleMorein-Zamir, S., & Anholt, G. (2021). Stopping a Response When You Really Care about the Action: Considerations from a Clinical Perspective. Brain Sciences, 11(8), 979. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11080979