Next Article in Journal
Design of Parameter Adaptive Suspension Controllers with Kalman Filter for Ride Comfort Enhancement and Motion Sickness Reduction
Previous Article in Journal
Stratified Multisource Optical Coherence Tomography Integration and Cross-Pathology Validation Framework for Automated Retinal Diagnostics
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

How Long Does It Take to Stop? Are Children Able to Stop on Demand?

by
Ernst Tomasch
1,*,
Heinz Hoschopf
1,
Bernd Schneider
1,
Bettina Schützhofer
2,
Martin Söllner
2,
Barbara Krammer-Kritzer
2,
Michael Plank
3 and
Hannes Glaser
4
1
Vehicle Safety Institute, TU Graz, Inffeldgasse 13/6, 8010 Graz, Austria
2
Sicher Unterwegs Verkehrspsychologische Untersuchungen und Nachschulungen, Schottenfeldgasse 28/8, 1070 Wien, Austria
3
Bergstr. 39, 7501 Siget in der Wart, Austria
4
Limbergstrasse 54, 3500 Krems an der Donau, Austria
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Appl. Sci. 2025, 15(9), 4978; https://doi.org/10.3390/app15094978
Submission received: 21 March 2025 / Revised: 28 April 2025 / Accepted: 29 April 2025 / Published: 30 April 2025
(This article belongs to the Section Transportation and Future Mobility)

Abstract

:
Children’s physical and cognitive development plays a crucial role in their ability to react appropriately in dynamic traffic situations. One key aspect of traffic safety is the ability to stop movement quickly and accurately after receiving a stop signal. Distraction is a major contributor to road accidents, especially among children who are easily distracted and may not be fully aware of the traffic situation. It is crucial to understand that children up to a certain age may struggle to halt their movement once initiated. This study indicates that the stopping distance, time, and deceleration of children aged six to ten years after a specific stop signal at different speeds are strongly influenced by the speed of movement and the age of the children. The results show that in the “walking” test configuration, the children were able to stop within a range of 0.47 m to 0.63 m, with a shorter distance for older children. The stopping time ranges from 0.84 s to 1.21 s and correlates positively with age. The stopping time and distance of children were measured in both “running” and “walking” test configurations across different age groups. However, in the “running” test configuration, stopping distance is almost the same across all age groups, with children requiring between 1.72 m and 1.84 m and a stopping time ranging from 1.17 s to 1.28 s. In the “walking” test configuration, children are able to decelerate between 0.91 m/s2 and 1.57 m/s2, while in the “running” test configuration, they are able to decelerate between 2.24 m/s2 and 3.19 m/s2.

1. Introduction

As children reach school age, they become more independent and active road users. Despite this, they remain the most vulnerable group of road users as pedestrians [1]. It is common for children to travel by foot to various destinations, including school, friends’ houses, and sports activities [2]. The American Academy of Pediatrics recommends that children under the age of ten should not be unsupervised pedestrians in road traffic, except under special conditions [3]. However, it may not always be feasible to practice and supervise all trips. It is strongly recommended that children practice walking to school with adult supervision before being allowed to walk alone [4]. According to Limbourg [5], children up to the age of approximately eight years are unpredictable and untrustworthy as pedestrians in road traffic due to their psychological development. Even older children, aged 8–9 years, are still easily distracted. It is crucial for children to focus their attention on the hazardous situation and avoid distractions from other things [4,6,7,8]. Children crossing the road tend to focus on their immediate surroundings, often neglecting traffic [6,9]. It is vital to educate children on the significance of paying attention to traffic when crossing the road. Their attention is often drawn to non-traffic related objects such as animals or other children [4,8]. Thus, inadequate observation of traffic and failure to stop before crossing the road are major contributing factors to accidents, as highlighted by Jiang et al. and Wang et al. [6,10,11]. Accurately estimating the speed and distance of an approaching vehicle is crucial to avoid critical situations. It is important to note that speed estimation is a complex task that requires considering distance [12]. According to various studies, children’s ability to estimate distances ranges from approximately six [13] to approximately nine years [14] (as cited in Uhr [12]). Furthermore, studies have demonstrated that children between the ages of 10 and 12 possess the capacity to estimate the velocity of approaching vehicles [14] (as cited in Uhr [12]). It is worth noting that 10-year-old children tend to use distance rather than speed as a reference for their judgment [15]. Furthermore, Tapiro et al. [8] identified a correlation between the time taken to clear the roadway and the speed of the next approaching vehicle. The study indicates that younger children have a shorter escape time compared with older children. Each age group (7–8 years, 9–10 years, 11–13 years, and adults) exhibited a significant difference to the next age group. Notably, the age group of 11–13-year-olds exhibited no statistically significant variation in their results when compared with adults. The remaining escape time averaged at 0.7 s for 7–8 year olds, 2.7 s for the 9–10 year olds, and 4.9 s for the 11–13 year olds. Younger children were able to cross the road safely in significantly less time than adults, who exhibited an escape time of 5.7 s. These findings suggest that younger children perceive a shorter distance between two vehicles as safe compared with adults or older children. However, in some situations, it may still be more appropriate for all pedestrians to stop and wait for traffic to pass or for vehicles to stop. It has been demonstrated that children aged six to seven encounter difficulties in interrupting a movement once it has been initiated. This has significant implications for their ability to respond promptly in dangerous situations [16] (cited in Schlag et al. [17]). This task requires a high level of alertness, which refers to the ability to quickly establish a general readiness to react [18]. It is important to note that alertness increases significantly between the ages of 5 and 11 years [19] (cited in Limbourg [12]). However, for younger children (6–8 years), there are significant inter-individual differences. The attainment of an average adult level of alertness is customarily observed at approximately 14 years of age.
The objective of the study is to analyze the stopping distance, stopping time, and deceleration of children of varying ages in response to a specific request.

2. Literature

A considerable body of research has been dedicated to examining the impact of varying speeds of movement among study participants. The extant studies have been separated into three categories: those that include (e.g., Griffits et al. [20], Knoblauch et al. [21] cited in Rastogi et al. [22], Forde and Daniel [23], Hussein and Sayed [24]), those that include children (e.g., Vaughan and Bain [25,26], Eberhardt and Himbert [27] cited in Vaughan [26], Eubanks and Hill [28] cited in Vaughan [26]), and those that include both, adults and children (e.g., Strouhal et al. [29] cited in Bartels and Erbsmehl [30], Tarawneh [31], Kramer and Raddatz [32] cited in Bartels and Erbsmehl [30], Rahman et al. [33]). The studies categorized assessments into slow walking, normal walking, running, and jogging. These categories were separated by gender. The experimental protocol entailed the execution of trials in both real-world road traffic conditions and a controlled laboratory environment. Throughout these trials, the primary focus remained on the steady-state speed of the participants. However, the examination of stopping time or stopping distance has been undertaken in a limited number of studies. For example, Wood et al. [34] examined the stopping distance of seven males and one female. The study conducted two test runs with participants jogging and walking at their preferred speed. The subjects were prompted to stop as quickly as possible using an acoustic signal, with the timing of the signal varied to prevent preparation. Tavana et al. [35] conducted a meticulous investigation into the reaction time and stopping distance of 16 participants (nine males and seven females) at three distinct velocities. Each velocity was assessed on three separate occasions. During the test configuration, participants were instructed to follow a leading person at a comfortable distance and to respond promptly to the leader’s sudden command to stop, without overtaking. The time and location of the stop were randomly selected and were not disclosed to the participants. Zhang et al. [36] investigated the stopping distance of pedestrians (29 males and 25 females) in the context of pedestrian crowd stampede accidents. The experimental trials were executed at an assortment of temporal and velocity parameters. A comprehensive analysis of pedestrian safety was conducted, encompassing their capacity to estimate the velocity and stopping distance of oncoming vehicles [37,38], as well as to predict the pedestrian’s intended crossing path [39]. In their study, Kath et al. [40] found that vehicle drivers are willing to stop for pedestrians crossing the road. Concurrently, Rajendar et al. [41] conducted a specific examination of the detection of pedestrians and the stopping distance of a vehicle equipped with an emergency braking system.

3. Method

The experiments were conducted in a child-friendly environment to prevent hazardous situations in real traffic conditions. This space has been meticulously designed to promote experimental learning in a safe and engaging environment.
The study protocol was reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of Medizinische Universität Graz Ethikkommission.

3.1. Test Set-Up

The initial point was established at a distance of 30 cm in front of the first light beam of the time-measuring device, which is 30 cm before the commencement of time measurement (see Figure 1 and Figure 2). In order to avert the occurrence of erroneous activations of the time measurement, the initial starting point was recalibrated in accordance with preliminary tests that revealed children tend to move their arms at the initial starting point. The first light beam was triggered 30 cm subsequent to the initial starting point, thereby establishing the temporal reference point. The second light beam was located one meter after the first, and the third light beam was positioned five meters after the first. Consequently, the distance between the second and third light beams was four meters. The temporal data from all three light beams was used to calculate speed and acceleration.
The children’s movement behavior was recorded in its entirety from a uniform perspective during all tests, which were each filmed from two fixed camera positions, as shown in Figure 2.

3.2. Test Matrix

The test configurations used in this study were consistent with those previously examined in the literature—including walking, running, and sprinting. Similar configurations have been investigated by Eberhard and Himbert [27] (cited in Bartels and Erbsmehl [30]), Eubanks and Hill [28] (cited in Vaughan and Bain [25,26]), and Vaughan and Bain [25,26,42]). Prior to testing, each child received clear instructions regarding the test they were to complete. The children were instructed to walk or run as they would normally do on a road. Each child completed five tests: two assessing walking, two assessing running, and one assessing running as fast as possible (sprinting). An acoustic stop signal was given after the third light beam during the walking and running tests. The signal time was varied to prevent anticipation and ensure the children’s ability to stop remained unaffected. The children were instructed to freeze immediately upon receiving the signal. During the sprinting test, participants were instructed to run as fast as possible to the end of the track and stop. The stopping distance was not measured in the sprint tests. It was not necessary to give a stop signal during this test configuration.
Some tests had to be repeated. Reasons for this include: slowing down during the test, interrupting movement during the test, standing still before the acoustic stop signal was given, etc.

3.3. Sample

The study included a sample of 93 primary school children, consisting of 39 female and 54 male children, ranging in age from six to ten years. Primary school is divided into four school levels. In the first year children are aged 6–7, in the second year children are aged 7–8, in the third year children are aged 8–9 and in the last year children are aged 9–10. The children were grouped according to their age, not their level of schooling.
A total of 32 children were six years old (34.4%), 17 children were seven years old (18.3%), 23 children were eight years old (24.7%), 19 children were nine years old, and two children were 10 years old and grouped together (22.6%). Physiological parameters, such as height and weight, were recorded by age group and are presented in Table 1.

3.4. Data Analysis

Figure 3 illustrates the theoretical speed-distance characteristic. The children begin at the starting point (SP) and accelerate to the target speed. It is assumed that they maintain a constant steady-state speed until they reach the third light beam (LB3). Following the third light beam, an acoustic signal is given, and the deceleration phase commences.
Formula (1) was used to calculate the speed at steady state, taking into account the distance between the second (LB2) and third light beam (LB3) (s3s2) and the time difference captured at the light beams LB2 and LB3 (t3t2) being taken into account. The distance traversed by the children from the initial location to their final position was meticulously measured. The deceleration from the acoustic signal to the rest position was calculated based on the speed and the distance moved (Formula (2)). Deceleration time was not measured separately from the reaction time to the signal.
v = s 3 s 2 t 3 t 2
a = v 2 2 s E P s A S

4. Results Analysis

The walking speeds of the different age groups within the conducted tests were significantly different from each other, as determined by a single-factorial ANOVA (p = 0.005), see Table 2 (a summary of the results for boys and girls can be found in the Appendix A and Appendix B). The walking speed of seven-year-olds was significantly different from that of nine to ten-year-olds (p = 0.002) (Tukey post-hoc test), while no significant differences were observed among the other age groups (Table 3). A notable difference was found only between the seven-year-olds and the eight-year-olds (p = 0.065).
The findings further indicate a significant difference between the age groups for the “running” test configuration (single-factorial ANOVA, p = 0.003). Specifically, a statistically significant difference in speed was observed between six-year-olds and nine-year-olds in the “running” test (p = 0.001), as well as between eight-year-olds and nine-year-olds (p = 0.041) (Tukey post-hoc test).
For the “sprinting” test configuration, a single-factorial ANOVA yielded a significant difference between age groups (p = 0.000). The Tukey post-hoc test revealed significant differences between the age groups of six-year-olds and seven-year-olds (p = 0.021), six-year-olds and eight-year-olds (p = 0.000), and six-year-olds and nine-year-olds (p = 0.000).
Notably, there was no statistically significant difference found between boys and girls in terms of speed in the “walking” and “running” test configurations within each age group. A significant gender difference (p = 0.007) was found between eight-year-old boys and girls in the “sprinting” test configuration.
In the “walking” test configuration, an average stopping distance of 0.63 m was recorded for six-year-olds (Table 4). Stopping distance has been observed to decrease with increasing age, with an average value of 0.47 m reported for 9–10-year-olds. The Welch-ANOVA test revealed a statistically significant difference between age groups (p = 0.030). Subsequent analysis using the Games-Howell post hoc test (Table 5) showed that only the six-year-olds and the 9–10-year-olds exhibited a significant difference (p = 0.017). The “running” test configuration yielded a stopping distance ranging from 1.72 m to 1.85 m. No significant difference was found between age groups for the “running” test configuration (single-factorial ANOVA test, p = 0.840).
The investigation yielded no statistically significant differences in the average stopping distance between male and female subjects within the specified test configurations and single age groups.
As indicated by the results presented in Table 6, a clear trend emerges in the stopping times for both walking and running. Older children demonstrate significantly faster stopping times than younger children. A statistically significant difference in the mean stopping time was observed between the age groups for the walking condition (Welch-ANOVA test, p = 0.000). Furthermore, the Games-Howell test reveals statistically significant differences (Table 7) in stopping time between six- and seven-year-olds (p = 0.042), six- and nine-year-olds (p = 0.000), seven- and nine-year-olds (p = 0.017), and eight- and nine-year-olds (p = 0.019). A single-factorial analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed no statistically significant differences between the age groups (p = 0.533). However, it was observed that older children tended to cease running more rapidly in the designated “running” configuration.
The investigation yielded no statistically significant differences in the average stopping distance between male and female subjects within the specified test configurations and single age groups.
The analysis of deceleration shows a comparable trend, albeit with an inverse tendency. As children mature, they attain higher deceleration values in both the “walking” and “running” test configurations (Table 8). However, these results did not attain statistical significance in either configuration (p = 0.167 and p = 0.284, single-factorial ANOVA).
An examination of the mean stopping deceleration revealed no statistically significant differences between male and female subjects within the designated test configurations and single age groups.
Speed significantly affects the length of stopping distance and stopping time. The relationship between stopping distance, stopping time, and steady-state speed for the “walking” and “running” test configurations is shown in Figure 4 and Figure 5. The data clearly indicates an increase in stopping distances at higher speeds (R2 = 0.34 for “walking” and R2 = 0.40 for “running”). Notably, this trend is not observed for stopping time.

5. Discussion

During the course of the tests, we carefully considered the boundary conditions of the test setup and determined that the length of the test course differs from what is stated in the literature. While Vaughan and Bain’s complete test course [25,26,42] was 15 m long, it is assumed that the initial five meters constituted the acceleration phase. Eberhard and Himbert [27] (cited in Bartels and Erbsmehl [30]) employed a test set-up that encompassed the entire course length of 10 m. However, the length of the acceleration phase was not specified. Strouhal et al. [29] (also cited in Bartels and Erbsmehl [30]) used a course length of 10 m to determine only steady-state speed, indicating that the participants did not accelerate from a standstill. The recorded steady-state speeds of the children in different age groups are comparable with findings in the literature. However, they are lower than those reported by some previous studies (see Vaughan and Bain [25,26,42], Eberhard, and Himbert [27] (cited in Bartels and Erbsmehl [30]), Eubanks and Hill [28] (cited in Vaughan and Bain [25,26])). These differences may be attributable to variations in the test configuration. It is expected that the children will attain a constant speed subsequent to the second light beam, following an acceleration phase of 1.3 m. It is important to note that if the children do not attain a constant speed immediately after this brief acceleration phase, they will persist in accelerating, despite the assumption of a steady-state speed. It is assumed that a steady-state speed is maintained between the second and third light beams, but there is still acceleration. In the context of the walking test configuration, it has been observed that a steady-state speed is maintained between the second and third light beams. However, the acceleration phase of the running tests is characterized by a prolonged duration, with steady-state speed being attained at a subsequent stage. It is important to acknowledge the potential underestimation of the steady-state speed attained after the second light beam, which may be more than the values reported in the extant literature. As shown in Figure 6, the calculation of steady-state speed based on the distance and time between the second and third light beams results in a speed of 7.2 km/h. During the acceleration phase, a lower speed can be calculated based on the distance and time between light beams. In this particular example, the steady-state speed between the first and second light beams is calculated to be 6 km/h.
Research has demonstrated that older children, including both girls and boys, demonstrate higher speeds, a finding that aligns with existing literature on the subject. Despite the finding that steady-state speed is lower than the values reported in the extant literature, this discrepancy does not contradict the observed tendency. A number of studies have proposed a categorization based on specific age groups and gender, as evidenced by the works of Vaughan and Bain [25,26,42], Eberhard and Himbert [27] (cited in Vaughan and Bain [25,26]), and Eubanks and Hill [28] (cited in Vaughan and Bain [25,26]). The results show that older children attain higher speeds. According to Vaughan and Bain [25,26,42] as well as Eberhard and Himbert [27] (cited in Vaughan and Bain [25,26]), boys tend to exhibit higher levels of speed in comparison to girls. Nevertheless, this observation was not substantiated in the present study.
A paucity of studies exists that examine the stopping distance or deceleration of children according to their age group. However, the extant literature on deceleration behavior primarily focuses on adults and their body postures immediately before collision [43,44,45,46]. It is important to note that these postures can exert a substantial influence on the patterns and severity of injuries. Research in this field involves the prediction of movement trajectories of pedestrians [47,48] and investigates movement characteristics of healthy individuals and those with a unilateral transtibial leg prosthesis [49]. The study confidently determined a steady-state speed with a time to stop of approximately 1.6 s (Goldhammer et al. [50]), while senior citizens and children had average times to stop of 3.25 s (SD = 1.25) and 3.14 s (SD = 1.13), respectively. These observations suggest that age group may not be a significant factor in reaction time, although further research is needed to confirm this conclusion. According to Tavana et al. [35], the average time to stop was 0.58 s (SD = 0.31) with a total stopping distance of 0.63 m and a total stopping time of 1.08 s. Notably, the maximum speed recorded by Tavana et al. [35] is comparable with the steady-state speed in the “walking” test configuration and the average stopping time of this test configuration. Furthermore, the stopping distance in the “walking” configuration ranges from 0.47 to 0.63 m at a steady-state speed of between 3.1 and 4.4 km/h. The study’s “walking” test configuration indicates that older children have a significantly shorter average stopping distance than younger children. The stopping time required to decelerate from steady-state speed to standstill ranges from 0.84 s to 1.21 s. This range demonstrates a negative correlation with children’s age. Moreover, the “running” test configuration revealed that the stopping distances of the various age groups were comparable. This finding serves to reinforce the study’s conclusions. The stopping distance ranges from 1.72 m to 1.84 m, and the stopping time ranges from 1.17 s to 1.28 s. The time required to decelerate from steady-state speed to standstill decreases with age. These results indicate a clear relationship between age and stopping performance. The temporal dynamics of the acoustic signal’s perception and subsequent reaction are not explicitly evaluated; however, these parameters can be inferred from the observed stopping distance and the deceleration phase. The specific braking distance is reduced, and the deceleration values achieved are potentially higher. It has been demonstrated that children can achieve a deceleration ranging from 0.91 m/s2 (six years old) to 1.57 m/s2 (9 to 10 years old) in the “walking” test configuration. In the “running” test configuration, deceleration ranges from 2.24 m/s2 (six years old) to 3.19 m/s2 (9 to 10 years old). According to Zou et al. [51], pedestrians exhibit a median deceleration of 4.2 m/s2 in response to hazardous situations, demonstrating a clear effort to avoid collisions. The authors conducted a study on vehicle-pedestrian accidents and subsequently analyzed the pedestrian’s response prior to the collision. The imminent danger of a collision is quite different from the test configuration, in which an acoustic signal is used to trigger a stopping maneuver. Consequently, the discrepancy in the deceleration rates observed between the present study and those reported by Zou et al. [51] is not unexpected.
While the present study provides significant findings on stopping distances, stopping times, and braking behavior of children aged six to ten years under standardized, child-friendly conditions, the transferability of the results to real traffic situations must be viewed critically. In the controlled test environment, potential sources of distraction—including other road users, unpredictable vehicle movements, noise, and visual stimuli—were systematically minimized. However, it is important to note that these factors are not uncommon in real road traffic and have the potential to significantly influence children’s behavior. While the experimental results in the laboratory environment demonstrated that the children’s stopping distance after the acoustic signal in the current test configurations was up to 1.85 m, it is crucial to hypothesize that under real-world conditions, such as when children are distracted, the stopping distance could be significantly longer. This, in turn, would increase the probability of a collision.
It is particularly evident in children that the ability to direct attention and motor self-regulation is limited depending on age and can be significantly impaired by distraction, causing them to fail to stop before crossing the road [4,6,8,9]. In more complex environments, children may have difficulty stopping movements or reacting quickly to unexpected events, which may lead to longer reaction times and stopping distances in everyday situations. This discrepancy highlights the need to design future studies in such a way that they take more realistic framework conditions into account.
In order to increase the validity of future studies and improve their relevance to real-life traffic situations, methodological extensions to the study design should be considered, such as field surveys in a protected area under natural conditions, virtual reality simulations for a realistic but controlled representation of suitable traffic scenarios with targeted distraction stimuli or comparative age group studies for a more precise classification of development-related differences. These methodological extensions could help to increase the transferability of the study results to real traffic situations and to tailor specific prevention measures more precisely to age- and development-specific requirements.
The results of the present study highlight the need to focus training measures not only on motor skills, but also to specifically take real-world stress factors into account. In addition to the acquisition of motor skills, road safety education should therefore also include strategies for directing attention and dealing with distractions. Only by consistently developing methods and practical training approaches can road safety and the independent, safe behavior of young road users be sustainably improved.

6. Limitations

Despite receiving instruction on the proper methodology, the children frequently failed to adhere to the established theoretical velocity-distance characteristics. The acceleration phase was not completed before the second beam thereby resulting in an underestimation of the steady-state velocity between the second and third beams was underestimated. In some cases, the subjects began to decelerate prematurely, that is, before the third light beam was passed or before the acoustic signal was initiated. Consequently, the stopping distance is therefore underestimated.
The position of the children at the time of the acoustic signal is extracted from the video source. The position was not measured precisely with a measuring device. The position was estimated from the video data analysis; consequently, it is not recorded with precision.
The stopping distance is significantly influenced by the reaction time; however, it was not feasible to analyze the reaction times of the children in the test setup. It was hypothesized that the children would begin to decelerate immediately following the acoustic signal. However, this assertion is not entirely accurate, as there is invariably a temporal interval preceding an action. It is important to note that reaction time was not measured separately in this study.
According to the extant literature, younger children encounter difficulties in interrupting a movement once it has begun. The present study aimed to measure the stopping distance of children of different age groups in primary school. The study demonstrated that, even within the confines of a laboratory setting, children require ample space to halt. Following a thorough examination of the available evidence, it was determined that children are incapable of stopping immediately. However, the absence of a control group comprising adolescents, adults, and elderly people limits the study’s conclusions. A comparison with these groups would allow for more precise conclusions regarding any significant differences in the stopping distance of children. In order to enhance the robustness of the results, it would be advantageous to analyze adolescents, adults, and elderly people within the same test configuration.
Despite the considerable size of the overall sample, its magnitude diminishes when specific parameters are integrated, such as age groups and gender differences. Individual characteristics may exert an unequal influence on the outcomes.
The experimental tests were carried out in a non-hazardous environment. The extent to which the results can be extrapolated to real traffic conditions is not entirely clear and should be treated with care.

7. Conclusions

Our study provides key findings on the traffic-related movement control of children aged six to ten years. One of the most important findings of the study is that children up to a certain age have considerable difficulty in stopping a movement they have started in response to a stop signal. Both the speed of movement and age play a decisive role in braking behavior. The ability of volitional movement inhibition is an important component of road safety behavior in order to be able to avoid approaching dangers in a timely manner.
As part of the study, the stopping behavior, time, and distance of children in different test configurations—‘walking’ and ‘running’—were systematically recorded. In the walking configuration, the measured braking distances varied between 0.47 m and 0.63 m depending on age. Older children were able to achieve shorter braking distances and stopped faster, with braking times between 0.84 and 1.21 s. The deceleration performance when walking was between 0.91 m/s2 and 1.57 m/s2. When running, on the other hand, the braking distance was almost constant across all age groups at 1.72 to 1.84 meters, as was the braking time at 1.17 to 1.28 s. The deceleration measured here was 2.24 to 3.19 m/s2. It can be deduced from this that age-dependent differences are less pronounced when running, which indicates biomechanical limits to the ability to react and control at higher speeds. This finding highlights the fact that younger children in particular are a particularly vulnerable group in road traffic due to their still-maturing ability to inhibit movement, which requires special protection.
The results also make it clear that safety-relevant movement sequences such as stopping cannot be taught by traffic rules alone, but require intensive, independent practice under realistic conditions accompanied by adults, especially for younger children. The study thus provides both theoretically sound and practically applicable findings for promoting road safety among children and emphasizes the need for age-appropriate measures in traffic planning, policy, and education.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, E.T. and B.S. (Bettina Schützhofer); methodology, E.T. and B.S. (Bettina Schützhofer); formal analysis, E.T.; investigation, B.S. (Bettina Schützhofer), B.K.-K. and M.S.; data curation, E.T., B.S. (Bettina Schützhofer) and M.S.; writing—original draft preparation, E.T.; writing—review and editing, H.H., B.S. (Bettina Schützhofer), B.S. (Bernd Schneider), M.S., B.K.-K., M.P. and H.G.; project administration, E.T. and B.S. (Bettina Schützhofer); funding acquisition, E.T. and B.S (Bettina Schützhofer). All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was funded by the Austrian Federal Ministry for Climate Action, Environment, Energy, Mobility, Innovation and Technology (BMK) by grants (grant number GZ.BMK-2021-0.772.800) of the Austrian Road Safety Fund (VSF). The APC was funded by TU Graz Open Access Publishing Fund. Open Access Funding by the Graz University of Technology.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of Medizinische Universität Graz Ethikkommission (protocol code 34-442 ex 21/22, date of approval: 16 August 2022).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Appendix A. Girls

Table A1. Average weight, height and length of legs according to age groups of girls.
Table A1. Average weight, height and length of legs according to age groups of girls.
ParameterAge GroupNumberMeanSDMinQ1MedianQ3Max
Weight6 years1123.344.4218.5020.1822.6525.1034.10
7 years828.888.7618.5521.8926.5836.5541.65
8 years1132.177.4224.2526.7829.7036.9843.70
9–10 years936.087.9623.2031.2533.6041.8548.15
Height6 years11120.025.31112.2115.70120.00123.10128.0
7 years8126.658.70116.2122.22124.50129.68145.0
8 years11133.735.64126.1129.90133.20136.20145.0
9–10 years9142.997.76127.5138.00143.50148.90152.5
Leg length6 years1172.003.9765.070.0071.572.7579.5
7 years875.694.6870.073.0074.077.8885.0
8 years1181.054.7674.078.5081.083.2590.0
9–10 years987.005.3978.084.0087.089.0094.0
Table A2. Average steady-state speed of the different test configurations according to age groups of girls.
Table A2. Average steady-state speed of the different test configurations according to age groups of girls.
TestAge GroupNumberMeanSDMinQ1MedianQ3Max
Walking6 years113.981.032.123.143.964.785.69
7 years83.230.891.922.723.113.635.22
8 years113.700.382.933.473.643.884.62
9–10 years93.740.822.543.293.583.875.85
Running6 years119.781.177.549.1010.0010.7011.61
7 years810.991.957.469.9711.7112.2613.33
8 years1110.051.747.388.679.7011.7513.21
9–10 years910.851.588.149.7211.2512.1812.74
Table A3. Average stopping distance to standstill after the acoustic signal of the different test configurations according to age groups of girls.
Table A3. Average stopping distance to standstill after the acoustic signal of the different test configurations according to age groups of girls.
TestAge GroupNumberMeanSDMinQ1MedianQ3Max
Walking6 years110.720.240.380.510.800.911.03
7 years80.540.300.230.350.480.641.15
8 years110.560.110.400.500.550.630.75
9–10 years90.470.100.290.430.450.530.63
Running6 years111.860.391.251.562.082.142.30
7 years81.930.630.901.561.962.262.95
8 years111.730.361.201.461.731.952.40
9–10 years91.690.391.001.501.631.952.35
Table A4. Average stopping time to standstill after the acoustic signal of the different test configurations according to age groups of girls.
Table A4. Average stopping time to standstill after the acoustic signal of the different test configurations according to age groups of girls.
TestAge GroupNumberMeanSDMinQ1MedianQ3Max
Walking6 years111.290.240.891.191.261.481.61
7 years81.170.420.750.811.051.631.70
8 years111.080.240.760.891.121.251.47
9–10 years90.930.210.660.860.871.011.34
Running6 years111.360.201.041.241.411.451.80
7 years81.250.290.841.041.321.421.65
8 years111.240.111.041.181.261.311.37
9–10 years91.150.290.580.931.221.321.50
Table A5. Average stopping deceleration of the different test configurations according to age groups of girls.
Table A5. Average stopping deceleration of the different test configurations according to age groups of girls.
TestAge GroupNumberMeanSDMinQ1MedianQ3Max
Walking6 years110.890.240.500.750.861.021.26
7 years80.850.300.440.640.861.001.27
8 years111.020.260.730.810.901.261.40
9–10 years91.320.630.730.881.191.462.85
Running6 years112.040.251.421.992.102.202.30
7 years82.610.751.702.222.392.874.18
8 years112.300.401.851.992.192.573.16
9–10 years93.211.961.832.082.723.658.06

Appendix B. Boys

Table A6. Average weight, height and length of legs according to age groups of boys.
Table A6. Average weight, height and length of legs according to age groups of boys.
ParameterAge GroupNumberMeanSDMinQ1MedianQ3Max
Weight6 years2124.893.7319.2522.8024.0527.1535.45
7 years925.231.9523.1524.0025.1026.1029.40
8 years1232.576.9723.5528.5830.2537.1446.80
9–10 years1236.228.4119.9531.3536.4744.2047.15
Height6 years21121.924.79114.9118.00121.40125.90129.8
7 years9127.804.43122.0124.50127.30129.00136.5
8 years12135.437.50125.0130.95134.65137.43150.0
9–10 years12140.638.60122.3134.22142.70147.25151.5
Leg length6 years2170.983.8365.068.0070.073.5079.0
7 years976.504.2171.074.5075.578.0084.0
8 years1281.505.6075.077.6280.083.3892.0
9–10 years1285.505.4673.083.1287.589.0093.0
Table A7. Average steady-state speed of the different test configurations according to age groups of boys.
Table A7. Average steady-state speed of the different test configurations according to age groups of boys.
TestAge GroupNumberMeanSDMinQ1MedianQ3Max
Walking6 years213.540.862.112.923.324.205.41
7 years93.090.861.822.572.813.525.60
8 years123.860.722.553.303.804.255.35
9–10 years124.350.892.143.954.324.696.21
Running6 years219.491.855.128.139.2611.1212.85
7 years99.852.266.468.099.0111.7513.84
8 years1210.011.656.829.149.8010.9213.84
9–10 years1211.831.798.9410.6311.8013.3315.65
Table A8. Average stopping distance to standstill after the acoustic signal of the different test configurations according to age groups of boys.
Table A8. Average stopping distance to standstill after the acoustic signal of the different test configurations according to age groups of boys.
TestAge GroupNumberMeanSDMinQ1MedianQ3Max
Walking6 years210.570.220.350.450.500.631.23
7 years90.520.290.280.350.400.601.20
8 years120.530.130.280.490.550.610.73
9–10 years120.470.160.200.340.490.560.78
Running6 years211.670.440.801.411.731.832.40
7 years91.750.931.101.301.381.634.10
8 years121.710.820.851.191.582.013.93
9–10 years121.970.720.251.742.152.432.85
Table A9. Average stopping time to standstill after the acoustic signal of the different test configurations according to age groups of boys.
Table A9. Average stopping time to standstill after the acoustic signal of the different test configurations according to age groups of boys.
TestAge GroupNumberMeanSDMinQ1MedianQ3Max
Walking6 years211.160.270.670.991.141.281.92
7 years91.160.340.800.921.061.261.85
8 years120.990.160.630.911.001.091.20
9–10 years120.770.210.350.670.790.911.08
Running6 years211.240.280.671.061.241.441.83
7 years91.250.450.720.981.181.332.31
8 years121.200.400.650.941.211.332.17
9–10 years121.170.360.181.131.261.401.47
Table A10. Average stopping deceleration of the different test configurations according to age groups of boys.
Table A10. Average stopping deceleration of the different test configurations according to age groups of boys.
TestAge GroupNumberMeanSDMinQ1MedianQ3Max
Walking6 years210.920.350.530.700.761.091.95
7 years90.780.160.590.670.720.941.05
8 years121.130.250.750.981.131.241.62
9–10 years121.760.690.851.261.762.033.35
Running6 years212.350.731.411.822.282.753.93
7 years92.431.061.531.592.352.564.99
8 years122.570.921.772.042.252.654.65
9–10 years123.171.522.152.372.773.207.81

References

  1. Zegeer, C.V.; Bushell, M. Pedestrian crash trends and potential countermeasures from around the world. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2012, 44, 3–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Flade, A.; Limbourg, M. Das Hineinwachsen in Die Motorisierte Gesellschaft; IWU: Darmstadt, Germany, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  3. Committee on Injury, Violence, and Poison Prevention; American Academy of Pediatrics. Policy statement--Pedestrian safety. Pediatrics 2009, 124, 802–812. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Limbourg, M. Kinder Unterwegs im Verkehr—Ansätze zur Erhöhung der Verkehrssicherheit im Kindes- und Jungendalter; Verkehrswachtforum: Berlin, Germany, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  5. Limbourg, M. Der Einfluß der schulischen Verkehrserziehung auf das Verhalten von Kindern als Fußgänger im Straßenverkehr. In Proceedings of the Bericht über die 1. Tagung “Kindersicherheit: Was Wirkt?”, Vienna, Austria, 9–10 June 1994; pp. 312–324. [Google Scholar]
  6. Jiang, K.; Wang, Y.; Feng, Z.; Sze, N.N.; Yu, Z.; Cui, J. Exploring the crossing behaviours and visual attention allocation of children in primary school in an outdoor road environment. Cogn. Tech. Work 2021, 23, 587–604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Tapiro, H.; Oron-Gilad, T.; Parmet, Y. Pedestrian distraction: The effects of road environment complexity and age on pedestrian’s visual attention and crossing behavior. J. Saf. Res. 2020, 72, 101–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Tapiro, H.; Oron-Gilad, T.; Parmet, Y. The effect of environmental distractions on child pedestrian’s crossing behavior. Saf. Sci. 2018, 106, 219–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Biassoni, F.; Bina, M.; Confalonieri, F.; Ciceri, R. Visual exploration of pedestrian crossings by adults and children: Comparison of strategies. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2018, 56, 227–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Wang, H.; Tan, D.; Schwebel, D.C.; Shi, L.; Miao, L. Effect of age on children’s pedestrian behaviour: Results from an observational study. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2018, 58, 556–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Wang, H.; Schwebel, D.C.; Tan, D.; Shi, L.; Miao, L. Gender differences in children’s pedestrian behaviors: Developmental effects. J. Saf. Res. 2018, 67, 127–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Uhr, A. Entwicklungspsychologische Grundlagen: Überblick und Bedeutung für die Verkehrssicherheit; Beratungsstelle für Unfallverhütung: Bern, Switzerland, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  13. Sigl, U.; Weber, K. Hurra, Wir Sind Mobil: Mobilitätsverhalten von 5- bis 10-Jährigen Kindern in Wien, Niederösterreich und im Burgenland, Kuratorium für Verkehrssicherheit; Institut für Verkehrserziehung: Wien, Austria, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  14. Limbourg, M. Kinder Unterwegs im Straßenverkehr; Unfallklrass Nordrhein-Westfalen: Münster, Germany, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  15. Keshavarz, B.; Landwehr, K.; Baurès, R.; Oberfeld, D.; Hecht, H.; Benguigui, N. Age-Correlated Incremental Consideration of Velocity Information in Relative Time-to-Arrival Judgments. Ecol. Psychol. 2010, 22, 212–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Brück, J. Kindersicherheit: Gefahren Erkennen, Gefahren Vermeiden mit Checklisten und Tipps zur Unfallvermeidung; BEUTH: Berlin, Germany, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  17. Schlag, B.; Richter, S.; Kröling, S.; Gehlert, T. Ganzheitliche Verkehrserziehung für Kinder und Jugendliche Teil 1: Entwicklung Verkehrsrelevanter Kompetenzen im Alter von 0 bis 14 Jahren, Band 2: In Ausgewählten Verkehrssituationen, 2nd ed.; Gesamtverband der Deutschen Versicherungswirtschaft e.V. Unfallforschung der Versicherer: Berlin, Germany, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  18. Niemann, H.; Gauggel, S. Störungen der Aufmerksamkeit. In NeuroRehabilitation; Frommelt, P., Lösslein, H., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2010; pp. 145–170. [Google Scholar]
  19. Kaufmann, L.; Nuerk, H.-C.; Konrad, K.; Willmes, K. Kognitive Entwicklungsneuropsychologie; Hogrefe: Göttingen, Germany, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  20. Griffiths, J.D.; Hunt, J.G.; Marlow, M. Delays at Pedestrian Crossings; Traffic Engineering & Control; Hemming Group, Limited: London, UK.
  21. Knoblauch, R.L.; Pietrucha, M.T.; Nitzburg, M. Field Studies of Pedestrian Walking Speed and Start-Up Time. Transp. Res. Rec. 1996, 1538, 27–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Rastogi, R.; Chandra, S.; Vamsheedhar, J.; Das, V.R. Parametric Study of Pedestrian Speeds at Midblock Crossings. J. Urban Plann. Dev. 2011, 137, 381–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Forde, A.; Daniel, J. Pedestrian walking speed at un-signalized midblock crosswalk and its impact on urban street segment performance. J. Traffic Transp. Eng. (Engl. Ed.) 2021, 8, 57–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Hussein, M.; Sayed, T. Microscopic Pedestrian Interaction Behavior Analysis Using Gait Parameters. Transp. Res. Rec. 2015, 2519, 28–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Vaughan, R.; Bain, J. Acceleration and speeds of young pedestrians. Sect. 6 J. Passeng. Cars Part 1 1999, 108, 725–736. [Google Scholar]
  26. Vaughan, R.; Bain, J. Acceleration and Speeds of Young Pedestrians: Phase II. Sect. 6 J. Passeng. Cars Mech. Syst. J. 2000, 109, 1264–1287. [Google Scholar]
  27. Eberhardt, W.; Himbert, G. Bewegungsgeschwindigkeiten: Versuchsergebnisse Nichtmotorisierter Verkehrsteilnehmer; Ingenieur-Büro Simon-Himbert-Eberhardt: Saarbrücken, Germany, 1977. [Google Scholar]
  28. Eubanks, J.; Hill, P.F. Pedestrian Accident Reconstruction and Litigation, 2nd ed.; Lawyers & Judges Pub: Tucson, AZ, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  29. Strouhal, J.; Kuehnl, K.; Hein, H. Bewegungsgeschwindigkeit von Fussgängern. Verkehrsunfall Fahrzeugtechnik 1994, 32, 313–314. [Google Scholar]
  30. Bartels, B.; Erbsmehl, C. Bewegungsverhalten von Fußgängern im Straßenverkehr—Teil 1; VDA: Berlin, Germany, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  31. Tarawneh, M.S. Evaluation of pedestrian speed in Jordan with investigation of some contributing factors. J. Saf. Res. 2001, 32, 229–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Kramer, F.; Raddatz, M. Das Bewegungsverhalten von Fussgaengern im Strassenverkehr auf Basis einer experimentellen Reihenuntersuchung/The movement behaviour of pedestrians in road traffic on the basis of an experimental serial study. Verkehrsunfall Fahrzeugtechnik 2010, 48, 382–388. [Google Scholar]
  33. Rahman, K.; Ghani, N.A.; Kamil, A.A.; Mustafa, A. Analysis of Pedestrian Free Flow Walking Speed in a Least Developing Country: A Factorial Design Study. Eng. Environ. Sci. 2012, 21, 4299–4304. [Google Scholar]
  34. Wood, M.; Ayres, T.; Kelkar, R.; Khatua, R. Walking and Jogging: An Analysis of Pedestrian Stopping Times and Distances. In Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, San Francisco, CA, USA, 27 September–1 October 2010; Volume 54, pp. 1435–1439. [Google Scholar]
  35. Tavana, H.; Thompson, P.; Boyce, K.; Frantzich, H.; Nilsson, D.; McGrath, D. A novel approach to the investigation and quantification of the stop/start process for pedestrian traffic using motion capture devices. Travel Behav. Soc. 2024, 34, 100659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Zhang, Y.; Chen, X.; Li, N. Experimental Research of Relationship between Pedestrian Stopping Distance and Walking Speed to Avoid Pedestrian Crowd Stampede Accident. In ICCTP 2010; Wei, H., Wang, Y., Rong, J., Weng, J., Eds.; American Society of Civil Engineers: Reston, VA, USA, 2010; pp. 839–850. [Google Scholar]
  37. Sun, R.; Zhuang, X.; Wu, C.; Zhao, G.; Zhang, K. The estimation of vehicle speed and stopping distance by pedestrians crossing streets in a naturalistic traffic environment. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2015, 30, 97–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Shi, J.; Wu, C.; Qian, X. The Effects of Multiple Factors on Elderly Pedestrians’ Speed Perception and Stopping Distance Estimation of Approaching Vehicles. Sustainability 2020, 12, 5308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Keller, C.G.; Gavrila, D.M. Will the Pedestrian Cross? A Study on Pedestrian Path Prediction. IEEE Trans. Intell. Transp. Syst. 2014, 15, 494–506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Katz, A.; Zaidel, D.; Elgrishi, A. An Experimental Study of Driver and Pedestrian Interaction during the Crossing Conflict. Hum. Factors 1975, 17, 514–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Rajendar, S.; Rathinasamy, D.; Pavithra, R.; Kaliappan, V.K.; Gnanamurthy, S. Prediction of stopping distance for autonomous emergency braking using stereo camera pedestrian detection. Mater. Today Proc. 2022, 51, 1224–1228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Vaughan, R.; Bain, J. Speeds and accelerations of school children. Road Transp. Res. 2001, 3, 28–48. [Google Scholar]
  43. Soni, A.; Robert, T.; Beillas, P. Effects of pedestrian pre-crash reactions on crash outcomes during multi-body simulations. In Proceedings of the 2013 IRCOBI Conference Proceedings, IRCOBI, Gothenburg, Sweden, 11–13 September 2013; International Research Council on the Biomechanics of Injury: Zurich, Switzerland, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  44. Schachner, M.; Schneider, B.; Weissenbacher, F.; Kirillova, N.; Possegger, H.; Bischof, H.; Klug, C. Identifying and Extracting Pedestrian Behavior in Critical Traffic Situations. In Proceedings of the 26th IEEE International Conference on Intelligent Transportation Systems, Bilbao, Spain, 24–28 September 2023; IEEE: Piscataway, NJ, USA, 2023; pp. 3943–3949. [Google Scholar]
  45. Schachner, M.; Schneider, B.; Sinz, W.; Klug, C. Extracting Quantitative Descriptions of Pedestrian Pre-crash Postures from Real-world Accident Videos. In Proceedings of the 2020 IRCOBI Conference Proceedings, IRCOBI, Online, 8–10 September 2020; International Research Council on the Biomechanics of Injury: Zurich, Switzerland, 2020; pp. 231–249. [Google Scholar]
  46. Han, Y.; Li, Q.; He, W.; Wan, F.; Wamg, B.; Mizuno, K. Analysis of Vulnerable Road User Kinematics Before/During/After Vehicle Collisions Based on Video Records. In Proceedings of the 2017 IRCOBI Conference Proceedings, IRCOBI, Antwerp, Belgium, 13–15 September 2017; International Research Council on the Biomechanics of Injury: Zurich, Switzerland, 2017; pp. 151–165. [Google Scholar]
  47. Murphey, Y.L.; Liu, C.; Tayyab, M.; Narayan, D. Accurate pedestrian path prediction using neural networks. In Proceedings of the 2017 IEEE Symposium Series on Computational Intelligence (SSCI), Honolulu, HI, USA, 27 November–1 December 2017; IEEE: Piscataway, NJ, USA, 2017; pp. 1–7. [Google Scholar]
  48. Goldhammer, M.; Gerhard, M.; Zernetsch, S.; Doll, K.; Brunsmann, U. Early prediction of a pedestrian’s trajectory at intersections. In Proceedings of the 16th International IEEE Conference on Intelligent Transportation Systems (ITSC), Kurhaus, The Hague, The Netherlands, 6–9 October 2013; IEEE: Piscataway, NJ, USA, 2013; pp. 237–242. [Google Scholar]
  49. Miff, S.C.; Childress, D.S.; Gard, S.A.; Meier, M.R.; Hansen, A.H. Temporal symmetries during gait initiation and termination in nondisabled ambulators and in people with unilateral transtibial limb loss. J. Rehabil. Res. Dev. 2005, 42, 175–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Goldhammer, M.; Hubert, A.; Koehler, S.; Zindler, K.; Brunsmann, U.; Doll, K.; Sick, B. Analysis on termination of pedestrians’ gait at urban intersections. In Proceedings of the 17th International IEEE Conference on Intelligent Transportation Systems (ITSC), Qingdao, China, 8–11 October 2014; pp. 1758–1763. [Google Scholar]
  51. Zou, T.; Liu, Z.; Wang, D.; Chen, T. Assessing the effect of pedestrian deceleration on pedestrian autonomous emergency braking systems. Int. J. Crashworthines 2022, 27, 1368–1373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Sketch of the test set-up.
Figure 1. Sketch of the test set-up.
Applsci 15 04978 g001
Figure 2. Test set-up in the sports room.
Figure 2. Test set-up in the sports room.
Applsci 15 04978 g002
Figure 3. Theoretical velocity-distance characteristic.
Figure 3. Theoretical velocity-distance characteristic.
Applsci 15 04978 g003
Figure 4. Correlation between steady-state speed and stopping distance: (a) walking test configuration; (b) running test configuration.
Figure 4. Correlation between steady-state speed and stopping distance: (a) walking test configuration; (b) running test configuration.
Applsci 15 04978 g004
Figure 5. Correlation between steady-state speed and stopping time: (a) walking test configuration; (b) running test configuration.
Figure 5. Correlation between steady-state speed and stopping time: (a) walking test configuration; (b) running test configuration.
Applsci 15 04978 g005
Figure 6. Influence of the length of the test set-up.
Figure 6. Influence of the length of the test set-up.
Applsci 15 04978 g006
Table 1. Physiological parameters including the standard deviation of the different age groups (Separate tables for girls and boys can be found in the Appendix A and Appendix B).
Table 1. Physiological parameters including the standard deviation of the different age groups (Separate tables for girls and boys can be found in the Appendix A and Appendix B).
Parameter6 Years7 Years8 Years9–10 Years
Number of children32172321
Females118119
Males2191212
Weight [kg]24.4 (SD = 4.0)26.9 (SD = 6.2)32.4 (SD = 7.0)36.9 (SD = 8.0)
Height [cm]121.3 (SD = 5.0)127.3 (SD = 6.6)134.6 (SD = 6.6)142.3 (SD = 8.1)
Leg length [cm]71.3 (SD = 3.8)76.1 (SD = 4.3)81.3 (SD = 5.1)86.7 (SD = 5.3)
Table 2. Steady-state speed in km/h of the different test configurations according to age groups between the second and third light beam (standard deviation: SD; first quartile: Q1; third quartile: Q3).
Table 2. Steady-state speed in km/h of the different test configurations according to age groups between the second and third light beam (standard deviation: SD; first quartile: Q1; third quartile: Q3).
TestAge GroupNumberMeanSDMinQ1MedianQ3Max
Walking6 years323.690.942.113.043.444.435.69
7 years173.160.861.822.603.013.605.60
8 years233.780.582.553.443.734.155.35
9–10 years214.090.912.143.454.034.526.21
Running6 years329.591.655.128.239.4710.7612.85
7 years1710.392.176.468.4711.2111.9813.84
8 years2310.031.686.828.749.8011.5013.84
9–10 years2111.411.758.1410.0611.5712.5215.65
Sprinting6 years3211.861.447.6211.2911.9012.7413.98
7 years1712.871.1110.0612.3012.9713.3314.54
8 years2313.251.0211.4212.5213.3313.9115.65
9–10 years2113.640.6612.5213.0913.5813.9814.84
Table 3. Tukey post-hoc test results for the steady-state speed of the age groups.
Table 3. Tukey post-hoc test results for the steady-state speed of the age groups.
Test ConfigurationAge Groups6 Years7 Years8 Years9–10 Years
Walking6 yearsNA0.1110.9720.272
7 years0.111NA0.0650.002
8 years0.9720.065NA0.569
9–10 years0.2720.0020.569NA
Running6 yearsNA0.4020.7760.001
7 years0.402NA0.9130.258
8 years0.7760.913NA0.041
9–10 years0.0010.2580.041NA
Sprinting6 yearsNA0.0210.0000.000
7 years0.021NA0.7320.169
8 years0.0000.732NA0.659
9–10 years0.0000.1690.659NA
NA—Not Applicable.
Table 4. Average stopping distance in meters to standstill after the acoustic signal.
Table 4. Average stopping distance in meters to standstill after the acoustic signal.
TestAge GroupNumberMeanSDMinQ1MedianQ3Max
Walking6 years320.630.230.350.450.550.781.23
7 years170.530.280.230.350.400.631.20
8 years230.540.120.280.500.550.630.75
9–10 years210.470.140.200.380.450.550.78
Running6 years321.740.430.801.411.792.102.40
7 years171.840.790.901.331.602.184.10
8 years231.720.630.851.261.702.033.93
9–10 years211.850.610.251.501.932.232.85
Table 5. Games Howell post-hoc test results for the stopping distance of the age groups.
Table 5. Games Howell post-hoc test results for the stopping distance of the age groups.
Test ConfigurationAge Groups6 Years7 Years8 Years9–10 Years
Walking6 yearsNA0.6550.3490.017
7 years0.655NA0.9990.809
8 years0.3490.999NA0.200
9–10 years0.0170.8090.200NA
Table 6. Average stopping time in s to standstill after the acoustic signal.
Table 6. Average stopping time in s to standstill after the acoustic signal.
TestAge GroupNumberMeanSDMinQ1MedianQ3Max
Walking6 years321.210.260.671.021.201.341.92
7 years171.160.370.750.881.061.511.85
8 years231.030.200.630.891.041.171.47
9–10 years210.840.220.350.680.870.921.34
Running6 years321.280.260.671.121.271.451.83
7 years171.250.370.720.981.261.382.31
8 years231.220.290.651.101.261.312.17
9–10 years211.170.300.361.101.241.391.50
Table 7. Games Howell post-hoc test results for the stopping time of the age groups.
Table 7. Games Howell post-hoc test results for the stopping time of the age groups.
Test ConfigurationAge Groups6 Years7 Years8 Years9–10 Years
Walking6 yearsNA0.9710.0420.000
7 years0.971NA0.5610.017
8 years0.0420.561NA0.019
9–10 years0.0000.0170.019NA
Table 8. Average stopping deceleration in m/s2 to standstill after the acoustic signal.
Table 8. Average stopping deceleration in m/s2 to standstill after the acoustic signal.
TestAge GroupNumberMeanSDMinQ1MedianQ3Max
Walking6 years320.910.310.500.700.831.081.95
7 years170.810.230.440.670.770.941.27
8 years231.070.260.730.861.091.261.62
9–10 years211.570.680.731.151.451.903.35
Running6 years322.240.611.411.962.122.353.93
7 years172.510.901.532.132.352.754.99
8 years232.440.721.772.012.202.584.65
9–10 years213.191.681.832.272.723.298.06
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Tomasch, E.; Hoschopf, H.; Schneider, B.; Schützhofer, B.; Söllner, M.; Krammer-Kritzer, B.; Plank, M.; Glaser, H. How Long Does It Take to Stop? Are Children Able to Stop on Demand? Appl. Sci. 2025, 15, 4978. https://doi.org/10.3390/app15094978

AMA Style

Tomasch E, Hoschopf H, Schneider B, Schützhofer B, Söllner M, Krammer-Kritzer B, Plank M, Glaser H. How Long Does It Take to Stop? Are Children Able to Stop on Demand? Applied Sciences. 2025; 15(9):4978. https://doi.org/10.3390/app15094978

Chicago/Turabian Style

Tomasch, Ernst, Heinz Hoschopf, Bernd Schneider, Bettina Schützhofer, Martin Söllner, Barbara Krammer-Kritzer, Michael Plank, and Hannes Glaser. 2025. "How Long Does It Take to Stop? Are Children Able to Stop on Demand?" Applied Sciences 15, no. 9: 4978. https://doi.org/10.3390/app15094978

APA Style

Tomasch, E., Hoschopf, H., Schneider, B., Schützhofer, B., Söllner, M., Krammer-Kritzer, B., Plank, M., & Glaser, H. (2025). How Long Does It Take to Stop? Are Children Able to Stop on Demand? Applied Sciences, 15(9), 4978. https://doi.org/10.3390/app15094978

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop