Not All Bilinguals Are the Same: A Call for More Detailed Assessments and Descriptions of Bilingual Experiences
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Individual Differences in Bilingualism
2.1. Age of Acquisition
2.2. Proficiency
2.3. Context of Language Acquisition
2.4. Language Use
2.5. Language Switching and Language Context
3. Measuring Bilingual Experiences
3.1. Age of Acquisition
3.2. Proficiency
3.3. Language Use
3.4. Language Switching and Language Context
4. Conclusions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Bialystok, E.; Craik, F.I.; Klein, R.; Viswanathan, M. Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychol. Aging 2004, 19, 290–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Costa, A.; Hernández, M.; Costa-Faidella, J.; Sebastián-Gallés, N. On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition 2009, 113, 135–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Prior, A.; MacWhinney, B. A bilingual advantage in task switching. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2010, 13, 253–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paap, K.R.; Greenberg, Z.I. There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cogn. Psychol. 2013, 66, 232–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lehtonen, M.; Soveri, A.; Laine, A.; Järvenpää, J.; de Bruin, A.; Antfolk, J. Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? A meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 2018, 144, 394–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luk, G.; Bialystok, E. Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2013, 25, 605–621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Prior, A.; Gollan, T.H. Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: Evidence from Spanish–English and Mandarin–English bilinguals. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2011, 17, 682–691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaushanskaya, M.; Prior, A. Variability in the effects of bilingualism on cognition: It is not just about cognition, it is also about bilingualism. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2015, 18, 27–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tabori, A.A.T.; Mech, E.N.; Atagi, N. Exploiting language variation to better understand the cognitive consequences of bilingualism. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baum, S.; Titone, D. Moving toward a neuroplasticity view of bilingualism, executive control, and aging. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2014, 35, 857–894. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kapa, L.L.; Colombo, J. Attentional control in early and later bilingual children. Cogn. Dev. 2013, 28, 233–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Luk, G.; De Sa, E.; Bialystok, E. Is there a relation between onset age of bilingualism and enhancement of cognitive control? Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2011, 14, 588–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pelham, S.D.; Abrams, L. Cognitive advantages and disadvantages in early and late bilinguals. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2014, 40, 313–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paap, K.R.; Johnson, H.A.; Sawi, O. Are bilingual advantages dependent upon specific tasks or specific bilingual experiences? J. Cogn. Psychol. 2014, 26, 615–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Von Bastian, C.C.; Souza, A.S.; Gade, M. No evidence for bilingual cognitive advantages: A test of four hypotheses. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 2016, 145, 246–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bak, T.H.; Vega-Mendoza, M.; Sorace, A. Never too late? An advantage on tests of auditory attention extends to late bilinguals. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tao, L.; Marzecová, A.; Taft, M.; Asanowicz, D.; Wodniecka, Z. The efficiency of attentional networks in early and late bilinguals: The role of age of acquisition. Front. Psychol. 2011, 2, 123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Singh, N.; Mishra, R.K. Second language proficiency modulates conflict-monitoring in an oculomotor Stroop task: Evidence from Hindi-English bilinguals. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Singh, N.; Mishra, R.K. Does language proficiency modulate oculomotor control? Evidence from Hindi–English bilinguals. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2012, 15, 771–781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mishra, R.K.; Padmanabhuni, M.; Bhandari, P.; Viswambharan, S.; Prasad, S.G. Language proficiency does not modulate executive control in older bilinguals. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 2018, 1–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barac, R.; Bialystok, E. Bilingual effects on cognitive and linguistic development: Role of language, cultural background, and education. Child Dev. 2012, 83, 413–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Linck, J.A.; Hoshino, N.; Kroll, J.F. Cross-language lexical processes and inhibitory control. Ment. Lex. 2008, 3, 349–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- de Bruin, A.; Bak, T.H.; Della Sala, S. Examining the effects of active versus inactive bilingualism on executive control in a carefully matched non-immigrant sample. J. Mem. Lang. 2015, 85, 15–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- de Bruin, A.; Della Sala, S.; Bak, T.H. The effects of language use on lexical processing in bilinguals. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 2016, 31, 967–974. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jaekel, N.; Jaekel, J.; Willard, J.; Leyendecker, B. No evidence for effects of Turkish immigrant children’s bilingualism on executive functions. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0209981. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pot, A.; Keijzer, M.; de Bot, K. Intensity of Multilingual Language Use Predicts Cognitive Performance in Some Multilingual Older Adults. Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Verreyt, N.; Woumans, E.; Vandelanotte, D.; Szmalec, A.; Duyck, W. The influence of language-switching experience on the bilingual executive control advantage. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2016, 19, 181–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hartanto, A.; Yang, H. Disparate bilingual experiences modulate task-switching advantages: A diffusion-model analysis of the effects of interactional context on switch costs. Cognition 2016, 150, 10–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Green, D.W.; Abutalebi, J. Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2013, 25, 515–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Blanco-Elorrieta, E.; Pylkkänen, L. Ecological Validity in Bilingualism Research and the Bilingual Advantage. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2018, 22, 1117–1126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Bruin, A.; Samuel, A.G.; Duñabeitia, J.A. Voluntary language switching: When and why do bilinguals switch between their languages? J. Mem. Lang. 2018, 103, 28–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blanco-Elorrieta, E.; Pylkkänen, L. Bilingual language switching in the lab vs. in the wild: The spatio-temporal dynamics of adaptive language control. J. Neurosci. 2017, 37, 9022–9036. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Green, D.W.; Wei, L. A control process model of code-switching. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 2014, 29, 499–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hofweber, J.; Marinis, T.; Treffers-Daller, J. Effects of dense code-switching on executive control. Linguist. Approaches Biling. 2016, 6, 648–668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Surrain, S.; Luk, G. Describing bilinguals: A systematic review of labels and descriptions used in the literature between 2005–2015. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2019, 22, 401–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hulstijn, J.H. The construct of language proficiency in the study of bilingualism from a cognitive perspective. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2012, 15, 422–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Marian, V.; Blumenfeld, H.K.; Kaushanskaya, M. The Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q): Assessing language profiles in bilinguals and multilinguals. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2007, 50, 940–967. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Bruin, A.; Carreiras, M.; Duñabeitia, J.A. The BEST dataset of language proficiency. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- MacIntyre, P.D.; Noels, K.A.; Clément, R. Biases in self-ratings of second language proficiency: The role of language anxiety. Lang. Learn. 1997, 47, 265–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomoschuk, B.; Ferreira, V.S.; Gollan, T.H. When a seven is not a seven: Self-ratings of bilingual language proficiency differ between and within language populations. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2018, in press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gollan, T.H.; Weissberger, G.H.; Runnqvist, E.; Montoya, R.I.; Cera, C.M. Self-ratings of spoken language dominance: A Multilingual Naming Test (MINT) and preliminary norms for young and aging Spanish–English bilinguals. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2012, 15, 594–615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dunn, L.M.; Dunn, L.M. PPVT-III: Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; American Guidance Service: Circle Pines, MN, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Lemhöfer, K.; Broersma, M. Introducing LexTALE: A quick and valid lexical test for advanced learners of English. Behav. Res. Methods 2012, 44, 325–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brysbaert, M. Lextale_FR a fast, free, and efficient test to measure language proficiency in French. Psychol. Belg. 2013, 53, 23–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Izura, C.; Cuetos, F.; Brysbaert, M. Lextale-Esp: A test to rapidly and efficiently assess the Spanish vocabulary size. Psicológica 2014, 35, 49–66. [Google Scholar]
- de Bruin, A.; Della Sala, S. The importance of language use when studying the neuroanatomical basis of bilingualism. Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 2016, 31, 335–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gullifer, J.W.; Titone, D. Characterizing the social diversity of bilingualism using language entropy. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2019, in press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grosjean, F. The bilingual individual. Interpreting 1997, 2, 163–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anderson, J.A.; Mak, L.; Chahi, A.K.; Bialystok, E. The language and social background questionnaire: Assessing degree of bilingualism in a diverse population. Behav. Res. Methods 2018, 50, 250–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Anderson, J.A.; Hawrylewicz, K.; Bialystok, E. Who is bilingual? Snapshots across the lifespan. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2018, in press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gullifer, J.W.; Chai, X.J.; Whitford, V.; Pivneva, I.; Baum, S.; Klein, D.; Titone, D. Bilingual experience and resting-state brain connectivity: Impacts of L2 age of acquisition and social diversity of language use on control networks. Neuropsychologia 2018, 117, 123–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gullifer, J.W.; Titone, D. Compute Language Entropy with {languageEntropy}. 2018. Available online: https://github.com/jasongullifer/languageEntropy (accessed on 4 February 2019). [CrossRef]
- Rodriguez-Fornells, A.; Kramer, U.; Lorenzo-Seva, U.; Festman, J.; Münte, T.F. Self-assessment of individual differences in language switching. Front. Psychol. 2012, 2, 388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Soveri, A.; Rodriguez-Fornells, A.; Laine, M. Is there a relationship between language switching and executive functions in bilingualism? Introducing a within group analysis approach. Front. Psychol. 2011, 2, 183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jylkkä, J.; Soveri, A.; Laine, M.; Lehtonen, M. Assessing bilingual language switching behavior with Ecological Momentary Assessment. In Proceedings of the Talk Presented at the 20th Conference of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology, Potsdam, Germany, 3–6 September 2017. [Google Scholar]
- Jevtović, M.; Duñabeitia, J.A.; de Bruin, A. How do bilinguals switch between languages in different interactional contexts? A comparison between voluntary and mandatory language switching. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2019, in press. [Google Scholar]
- Dewaele, J.M.; Wei, L. Attitudes towards code-switching among adult mono- and multilingual language users. J. Multiling. Multicult. Dev. 2014, 35, 235–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Laine, M.; Lehtonen, M. Cognitive consequences of bilingualism: Where to go from here? Lang. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018, 33, 1205–1212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Macbeth, A.; Bruni, M.; Mech, E.N.; Sarkis, J.T.; Karan, A.; Robbins, M.L.; Chiarello, C. Using the EAR to track bilingual language use. In Proceedings of the Poster Presented at the 10th Annual Meeting of the Society for the Neurobiology of Language, Quebec City, QC, Canada, 16–18 August 2018. [Google Scholar]
© 2019 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
de Bruin, A. Not All Bilinguals Are the Same: A Call for More Detailed Assessments and Descriptions of Bilingual Experiences. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 33. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9030033
de Bruin A. Not All Bilinguals Are the Same: A Call for More Detailed Assessments and Descriptions of Bilingual Experiences. Behavioral Sciences. 2019; 9(3):33. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9030033
Chicago/Turabian Stylede Bruin, Angela. 2019. "Not All Bilinguals Are the Same: A Call for More Detailed Assessments and Descriptions of Bilingual Experiences" Behavioral Sciences 9, no. 3: 33. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9030033
APA Stylede Bruin, A. (2019). Not All Bilinguals Are the Same: A Call for More Detailed Assessments and Descriptions of Bilingual Experiences. Behavioral Sciences, 9(3), 33. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9030033