Individual Variation and the Bilingual Advantage—Factors that Modulate the Effect of Bilingualism on Cognitive Control and Cognitive Reserve
1. Bilingual Advantage in Cognitive Control
2. Cognitive Reserve Hypothesis
3. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Byers-Heinlein, K.; Lew-Williams, C. Bilingualism in the early years: What the science says. Learn. Landsc. 2013, 7, 95–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Branzi, F.M.; Della Rosa, P.A.; Canini, M.; Costa, A.; Abutalebi, J. Language control in bilinguals: Monitoring and response selection. Cereb. Cortex 2016, 26, 2367–2380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Soveri, A.; Laine, M.; Hämäläinen, H.; Hugdahl, K. Bilingual advantage in attentional control: Evidence from the forced-attention dichotic listening paradigm. Bilingualism 2011, 14, 371–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blom, E.; Küntay, A.C.; Messer, M.; Verhagen, J.; Leseman, P. The benefits of being bilingual: Working memory in bilingual Turkish-Dutch children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2014, 128, 105–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keysar, B.; Hayakawa, S.L.; An, S.G. The foreign-language effect: Thinking in a foreign tongue reduces decision biases. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 23, 661–668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marzecová, A.; Bukowski, M.; Correa, Á.; Boros, M.; Lupiáñez, J.; Wodniecka, Z. Tracing the bilingual advantage in cognitive control: The role of flexibility in temporal preparation and category switching. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2013, 25, 586–604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sullivan, M.D.; Janus, M.; Moreno, S.; Astheimer, L.; Bialystok, E. Early stage second-language learning improves executive control: Evidence from ERP. Brain Lang. 2014, 139, 84–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bellander, M.; Berggren, R.; Mårtensson, J.; Brehmer, Y.; Wenger, E.; Li, T.Q.; Bodammer, N.C.; Shing, Y.L.; Werkle-Bergner, M.; Lövdén, M. Behavioral correlates of changes in hippocampal gray matter structure during acquisition of foreign vocabulary. Neuroimage 2016, 131, 205–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, E.; Majumder, S.; Martin, M.M. Developing phonological awareness: Is there a bilingual advantage? Appl. Psycholinguist. 2003, 24, 27–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialystok, E.; Craik, F.I.M.; Klein, R.; Viswanathan, M. Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychol. Aging 2004, 19, 290–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Naeem, K.; Filippi, R.; Periche-Tomas, E.; Papageorgiou, A.; Bright, P. The importance of socioeconomic status as a modulator of the bilingual advantage in cognitive ability. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fuller-Thomson, E.; Kuh, D. The healthy migrant effect may confound the link between bilingualism and delayed onset of Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex 2014, 52, 128–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Woumans, E.; Duyck, W. The bilingual advantage debate: Moving toward different methods for verifying its existence. Cortex 2015, 73, 356–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Samuel, S.; Roehr-Brackin, K.; Pak, H.; Kim, H. Cultural effects rather than a bilingual advantage in cognition: A review and an empirical study. Cogn. Sci. 2018, 42, 2313–2341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van den Noort, M.; Struys, E.; Bosch, P.; Jaswetz, L.; Perriard, B.; Yeo, S.; Barisch, P.; Vermeire, K.; Lee, S.H.; Lim, S. Does the bilingual advantage in cognitive control exist and if so, what are its modulating factors? A systematic review. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stern, Y. Cognitive reserve: Implications for assessment and intervention. Folia Phoniatr. Logop. 2013, 65, 49–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stern, Y. Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 2012, 11, 1006–1012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Opdebeeck, C.; Martyr, A.; Clare, L. Cognitive reserve and cognitive function in healthy older people: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. B 2016, 23, 40–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Craik, F.I.M.; Bialystok, E.; Freedman, M. Delaying the onset of Alzheimer disease: Bilingualism as a form of cognitive reserve. Neurology 2010, 75, 1726–1729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Woumans, E.; Versijpt, J.; Sieben, A.; Santens, P.; Duyck, W. Bilingualism and cognitive decline: A story of pride and prejudice. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2017, 60, 1237–1239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perani, D.; Farsad, M.; Ballarini, T.; Lubian, F.; Malpetti, M.; Fracchetti, A.; Magnani, G.; March, A.; Abutalebi, J. The impact of bilingualism on brain reserve and metabolic connectivity in Alzheimer’s dementia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2017, 114, 1690–1695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fidler, J.; Lochtman, K. The effect of cognates on cognitive control in late sequential multilinguals: A bilingual advantage? Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Crystal, D. A Dictionary of Linguistics and Phonetics; Blackwell Publishing: Malden, MA, USA, 2011; p. 560. ISBN 13-978-1444356755. [Google Scholar]
- Stroop, J.R. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J. Exp. Psychol. 1935, 18, 643–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nour, S.; Struys, E.; Stengers, H. Attention network in interpreters: The role of training and experience. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fan, J.; McCandliss, B.D.; Sommer, T.; Raz, A.; Posner, M.I. Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2002, 14, 340–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Petersen, S.E.; Posner, M.I. The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2012, 35, 73–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wu, R.; Struys, E.; Lochtman, K. Relationship between language dominance and stimulus-stimulus or stimulus-response inhibition in Uyghur-Chinese bilinguals with an investigation of speed-accuracy trade-offs. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eriksen, B.A.; Eriksen, C.W. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept. Psychophys. 1974, 16, 143–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Simon, J.R.; Rudell, A.P. Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. J. Appl. Psychol. 1967, 51, 300–304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Woumans, E.; van Herck, S.; Struys, E. Shifting gear in the study of the bilingual advantage: Language switching examined as a possible moderator. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weissberger, G.H.; Gollan, T.H.; Bondi, M.W.; Clark, L.R.; Wierenga, C.E. Language and task switching in the bilingual brain: Bilinguals are staying, not switching, experts. Neuropsychologia 2015, 66, 193–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Woumans, E.; Ceuleers, E.; Van der Linden, L.; Szmalec, A.; Duyck, W. Verbal and nonverbal cognitive control in bilinguals and interpreters. J. Exp. Psychol. 2015, 41, 1579–1586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Boumeester, M.; Michel, M.C.; Fyndanis, V. Sequential multilingualism and cognitive abilities: Preliminary data on the contribution of language proficiency and use in different modalities. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Goldsmith, S.F.; Morton, J.B. Sequential congruency effects in monolingual and bilingual adults: A failure to Replicate Grundy et al., (2017). Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 2476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Haft, S.L.; Kepinska, O.; Caballero, J.N.; Carreiras, M.; Hoeft, F. Attentional fluctuations, cognitive flexibility, and bilingualism in Kindergarteners. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bialystok, E. The bilingual adaptation: How minds accommodate experience. Psychol. Bull. 2017, 143, 233–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paap, K.R.; Greenberg, Z.I. There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cogn. Psychol. 2013, 66, 232–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paap, K.R.; Johnson, H.A.; Sawi, O. Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex 2015, 69, 265–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lehtonen, M.; Soveri, A.; Laine, A.; Järvenpää, J.; de Bruin, A.; Antfolk, J. Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? A meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 2018, 144, 394–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- De Bruin, A.; Treccani, B.; Della Sala, S. Cognitive advantage in bilingualism: An example of publication bias? Psychol. Sci. 2015, 49, 287–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Festman, J.; Schwieter, J.W. Self-concepts in reading and spelling among mono- and multilingual children: Extending the bilingual advantage. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Shavelson, R.; Hubner, J.; Stanton, G. Self-concept: Validation of construct interpretations. Rev. Educ. Res. 1976, 46, 407–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martschinke, S. Identitätsentwicklung und Selbstkonzept. In Handbuch Grundschulpädagogik und Grundschuldidaktik, 3rd ed.; Einsiedler, W., Götz, M., Hartinger, A., Heinzel, F., Kahlert, J., Sandfuchs, U., Eds.; Verlag Julius Klinkhardt: Bad Heilbrunn, Germany, 2011; pp. 257–260. [Google Scholar]
- Ouzia, J.; Bright, P.; Filippi, R. Attentional control in bilingualism: An exploration of the effects of trait anxiety and rumination on inhibition. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Roberts, L.; Siyanova-Chanturia, A. Using eye-tracking to investigate topics in L2 acquisition and L2 processing. Stud. Second Lang. Acquis. 2013, 35, 213–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Eysenck, M.W.; Derakshan, N.; Santos, R.; Calvo, M.G. Anxiety and cognitive performance: Attentional control theory. Emotion 2007, 7, 336–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Eysenck, M.W.; Calvo, M.G. Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency theory. Cogn. Emot. 1992, 6, 409–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Bruin, A. Not all bilinguals are the same: A call for more detailed assessments and descriptions of bilingual experiences. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Poarch, G.J.; Krott, A. A bilingual advantage? An appeal for a change in perspective and recommendations for future research. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Román-Caballero, R.; Arnedo, M.; Triviño, M.; Lupiáñez, J. Musical practice as an enhancer of cognitive function in healthy aging: A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0207957. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Guure, C.B.; Ibrahim, N.A.; Adam, M.B.; Said, S.M. Impact of physical activity on cognitive decline, dementia, and its subtypes: Meta-analysis of prospective studies. Biomed. Res. Int. 2017, 2017, 9016924. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bak, T.H.; Nissan, J.J.; Allerhand, M.M.; Deary, I.J. Does bilingualism influence cognitive aging? Ann. Neurol. 2014, 75, 959–963. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van den Noort, M.; Vermeire, K.; Bosch, P.; Staudte, H.; Krajenbrink, T.; Jaswetz, L.; Struys, E.; Yeo, S.; Barisch, P.; Perriard, B.; et al. A systematic review on the possible relationship between bilingualism, cognitive decline, and the onset of dementia. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Pot, A.; Porkert, J.; Keijzer, M. The bidirectional in bilingual: Cognitive, social and linguistic effects of and on third-age language learning. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
© 2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
van den Noort, M.; Struys, E.; Bosch, P. Individual Variation and the Bilingual Advantage—Factors that Modulate the Effect of Bilingualism on Cognitive Control and Cognitive Reserve. Behav. Sci. 2019, 9, 120. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9120120
van den Noort M, Struys E, Bosch P. Individual Variation and the Bilingual Advantage—Factors that Modulate the Effect of Bilingualism on Cognitive Control and Cognitive Reserve. Behavioral Sciences. 2019; 9(12):120. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9120120
Chicago/Turabian Stylevan den Noort, Maurits, Esli Struys, and Peggy Bosch. 2019. "Individual Variation and the Bilingual Advantage—Factors that Modulate the Effect of Bilingualism on Cognitive Control and Cognitive Reserve" Behavioral Sciences 9, no. 12: 120. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9120120
APA Stylevan den Noort, M., Struys, E., & Bosch, P. (2019). Individual Variation and the Bilingual Advantage—Factors that Modulate the Effect of Bilingualism on Cognitive Control and Cognitive Reserve. Behavioral Sciences, 9(12), 120. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9120120