Executive Functioning Profiles in Children with Neurodevelopmental Disorders
Abstract
1. Introduction
Aim and Research Hypotheses
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Design
2.2. Participants
2.3. Measurement
2.4. Procedure
2.5. Research Variables
2.6. Analysis of Data
3. Results
- (i)
- Working memory: (a) parents, [F = 195.76, p < 0.001]; (b) teachers, [F = 199.63, p < 0.001].
- (ii)
- Emergent Metacognition Index: (a) parents, [F = 176.15, p < 0.001]; (b) teachers, [F = 187.87, p < 0.001].
- (iii)
- Executive Function Global: (a) parents, [F = 168.07, p < 0.001]; (b) teachers, [F = 207.47, p < 0.001].
- (i)
- Teachers tend to report larger effect sizes than parents, especially in global executive functions and metacognition.
- (ii)
- The highest estimates are observed in working memory, emergent metacognition, and global executive function, suggesting that these dimensions better capture the differences between groups or conditions analyzed.
- (iii)
- Scales with lower effect sizes (such as flexibility) may indicate that group differences are less pronounced in that domain.
- (iv)
- The most affected executive functions (by group, intervention, or condition) are working memory, emergent metacognition, and global executive functioning.
- (v)
- Teachers tend to detect stronger effects than parents in most dimensions, which may reflect greater observational sensitivity in structured school contexts.
- (vi)
- Although some scales, such as flexibility or emotional control, show lower effect sizes, they are not null, which still adds value to their analysis.
3.1. Parent Evaluation (Clinical Sample vs. Neurotypical Sample)
3.2. Teachers Evaluation (Clinical Sample vs. Neurotypical Sample)
4. Discussion
4.1. Limitations and Future Directions
4.2. Contribution to the Field
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Abbreviations
ADHD | Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder |
ASD | Autism spectrum disorder |
BRIEF-P | Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function in Preschool |
EFs | Executive functions |
References
- Albuquerque, C. P., Moura, O., Vilar, M., Seabra-Santos, M. J., Pereira, M., Major, S., Moreira, A., Lopes, A. F., Gaudêncio, A., & Simões, M. R. (2025). BRIEF-A: Factor structure and measurement invariance across self-report and informant forms. Applied Neuropsychology: Adult, 32(5), 1407–1417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Andersen, A. C., Sund, A. M., Thomsen, P. H., Lydersen, S., Haugan, A.-L. J., & Nøvik, T. S. (2024). Executive function measured by BRIEF in adolescents diagnosed and treated for ADHD: Problem profiles and agreement between informants. Child Neuropsychology, 30(1), 45–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Anderson, P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 8(2), 71–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Antshel, K. M., & Russo, N. (2019). Autism spectrum disorders and ADHD: Overlapping phenomenology, diagnostic issues, and treatment considerations. Current Psychiatry Reports, 21(5), 34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barrouillet, P. (2015). Theories of cognitive development: From Piaget to today. Developmental Review, 38, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bausela Herreras, E. (2020). BRIEF-P: Trastornos por Déficit de Atención Hiperactividad versus Trastorno del Espectro Autista. Revista de Psiquiatría Infanto-Juvenil, 37(1), 17–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bausela Herreras, E. (2021). Desarrollo de las funciones ejecutivas en población infantil en castellano y en euskera. Editorial Dykinson, S.L. Available online: https://books.google.es/books?id=cIZjEAAAQBAJ (accessed on 1 January 2022).
- Bausela Herreras, E. (2024a). Comparative study of the development of executive functions in children: Transition from the first cycle to the second cycle of early childhood education. Brain Sciences, 14(12), 1273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bausela Herreras, E. (2024b). Autism spectrum disorder and BRIEF-P: A review and meta-analysis. Children, 11(8), 978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bausela Herreras, E., Alonso Esteban, Y., & Alcantud Marín, F. (2023). Behavior rating inventory of executive function in preschool (BRIEF-P) and attention-deficit and hyperactivity disorders (ADHD): A systematic review and meta-analysis of floor and ceiling effects. Children, 11(1), 58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bausela Herreras, E., & Luque Cuenca, T. (2017). Evaluación conductual de la función ejecutiva-versión infantil (BRIEF-P, versión española): Fiabilidad y validez. Acta de Investigación Psicológica, 7(3), 2811–2822. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ben-Asher, E., Porter, B. M., Roe, M. A., Mitchell, M. E., & Church, J. A. (2023). Bidirectional longitudinal relations between executive function and social function across adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 59(9), 1587–1594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blair, C. (2016). Executive function and early childhood education. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 10, 102–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brignell, A., Harwood, R. C., May, T., Woolfenden, S., Montgomery, A., Iorio, A., & Williams, K. (2022). Overall prognosis of preschool autism spectrum disorder diagnoses. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 2022(9). [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bryce, D., Whitebread, D., & Szűcs, D. (2015). The relationships among executive functions, metacognitive skills and educational achievement in 5 and 7 year-old children. Metacognition and Learning, 10(2), 181–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ceravolo, R., Pagni, C., Tognoni, G., & Bonuccelli, U. (2012). The epidemiology and clinical manifestations of dysexecutive syndrome in Parkinson’s disease. Frontiers in Neurology, 3, 159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ceruti, C., Mingozzi, A., Scionti, N., & Marzocchi, G. M. (2024). Comparing executive functions in children and adolescents with autism and ADHD—A systematic review and meta-analysis. Children, 11(4), 473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, J., Zhang, L. J., Wang, X., & Zhang, T. (2021). Impacts of self-regulated strategy development-based revision instruction on english-as-a-foreign-language students’ self-efficacy for text revision: A mixed-methods study. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 670100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, S.-F. F., Chien, Y.-L. L., Wu, C.-T. T., Shang, C.-Y. Y., Wu, Y.-Y. Y., & Gau, S. S. (2016). Deficits in executive functions among youths with autism spectrum disorders: An age-stratified analysis. Psychological Medicine, 46(8), 1625–1638. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Christoforou, M., Jones, E. J. H., White, P., & Charman, T. (2023). Executive function profiles of preschool children with autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review. JCPP Advances, 3(1), e12123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cordova, M., Shada, K., Demeter, D. V., Doyle, O., Miranda-Dominguez, O., Perrone, A., Schifsky, E., Graham, A., Fombonne, E., Langhorst, B., Nigg, J., Fair, D. A., & Feczko, E. (2020). Heterogeneity of executive function revealed by a functional random forest approach across ADHD and ASD. NeuroImage: Clinical, 26, 102245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cristofori, I., Cohen-Zimerman, S., & Grafman, J. (2019). Executive functions. In Handbook of clinical neurology (Vol. 163, pp. 197–219). Elsevier. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cuartas, J., Hanno, E., Lesaux, N. K., & Jones, S. M. (2022). Executive function, self-regulation skills, behaviors, and socioeconomic status in early childhood. PLoS ONE, 17(11), e0277013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dalgleish, T., Black, M., Johnston, D., & Bevan, A. (2020). Transdiagnostic approaches to mental health problems: Current status and future directions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 88(3), 179–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Daunhauer, L. A., Fidler, D. J., Hahn, L., Will, E., Lee, N. R., & Hepburn, S. (2014). Profiles of everyday executive functioning in young children with down syndrome. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 119(4), 303–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- De Los Reyes, A., & Kazdin, A. E. (2005). Informant discrepancies in the assessment of childhood psychopathology: A critical review, theoretical framework, and recommendations for further study. Psychological Bulletin, 131(4), 483–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Demetriou, E. A., Lampit, A., Quintana, D. S., Naismith, S. L., Song, Y. J. C., Pye, J. E., Hickie, I., & Guastella, A. J. (2018). Autism spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis of executive function. Molecular Psychiatry, 23(5), 1198–1204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Demetriou, E. A., Park, S. H., Pepper, K. L., Naismith, S. L., Song, Y. J., Thomas, E. E., Hickie, I. B., & Guastella, A. J. (2021). A transdiagnostic examination of anxiety and stress on executive function outcomes in disorders with social impairment. Journal of Affective Disorders, 281, 695–707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- DeYoung, C. G., Blain, S. D., Latzman, R. D., Grazioplene, R. G., Haltigan, J. D., Kotov, R., Michelini, G., Venables, N. C., Docherty, A. R., Goghari, V. M., Kallen, A. M., Martin, E. A., Palumbo, I. M., Patrick, C. J., Perkins, E. R., Shackman, A. J., Snyder, M. E., & Tobin, K. E. (2024). The hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology and the search for neurobiological substrates of mental illness: A systematic review and roadmap for future research. Journal of Psychopathology and Clinical Science, 133(8), 697–715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diamond, A., & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science, 333(6045), 959–964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dickson, K. S., Kenworthy, L., Anthony, L., & Brookman-Frazee, L. (2023). Mental health therapist perspectives on the role of executive functioning in children’s mental health services. Evidence-Based Practice in Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 8(1), 39–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dörrenbächer-Ulrich, L., & Bregulla, M. (2024). The relationship between self-regulated learning and executive functions—A systematic review. Educational Psychology Review, 36(3), 95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duku, E., & Vaillancourt, T. (2014). Validation of the BRIEF-P in a sample of Canadian preschool children. Child Neuropsychology: A Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence, 20(3), 358–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duncan, A. F. (2023). Interventions for executive function in high-risk infants and toddlers. Clinics in Perinatology, 50(1), 103–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Durlak, J. A., Weissberg, R. P., Dymnicki, A. B., Taylor, R. D., & Schellinger, K. B. (2011). The impact of enhancing students’ social and emotional learning: A meta-analysis of school-based universal interventions. Child Development, 82(1), 405–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Escobar-Ruiz, V., Arias-Vázquez, P. I., Tovilla-Zárate, C. A., Doval, E., & Jané-Ballabriga, M. C. (2024). Advances and challenges in the assessment of executive functions in under 36 months: A scoping review. Advances in Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 8(3), 365–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Etokabeka, E. (2024). Supporting the development of executive function skills through structured play: A qualitative study of south African preschool teachers. Early Childhood Education Journal. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feijs, H. M. E., Van Aken, L., Van Der Veld, W. M., Van Der Heijden, P. T., & Egger, J. I. M. (2024). No relations between executive functions and dimensional models of psychopathology or is time the missing link? PLoS ONE, 19(3), e0288386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fortea, L., Ortuño, M., De Prisco, M., Oliva, V., Albajes-Eizagirre, A., Fortea, A., Madero, S., Solanes, A., Vilajosana, E., Yao, Y., Del Fabro, L., Solé, E., Verdolini, N., Farré-Colomés, A., Serra-Blasco, M., Picó-Pérez, M., Lukito, S., Wise, T., Carlisi, C., … Radua, J. (2025). Atlas of gray matter volume differences across psychiatric conditions: A systematic review with a novel meta-analysis that considers co-occurring disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 98(1), 76–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gavrilova, M., Karimova, A., Solopova, O., Veraksa, A., & Yakushina, A. (2024). Preschoolers’ executive function: Effect of the duration of preschool attendance and quality of teacher-child interactions. Frontiers in Education, 9, 1421037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gioia, G. A., Espy, K. A., & Isquith, P. K. (2016). BRIEF-P. Evaluación conductual de la función ejecutiva. Versión infantil (E. Bausela y T. Luque, adaptadoras). Hogrefe TEA Ediciones. [Google Scholar]
- Godefroy, O., Azouvi, P., Robert, P., Roussel, M., LeGall, D., & Meulemans, T. (2010). Dysexecutive syndrome: Diagnostic criteria and validation study. Annals of Neurology, 68(6), 855–864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Graham, S., & Harris, K. (2018). An examination of the design principles underlying a self-regulated strategy development study. Journal of Writing Research, 10(2), 139–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heilbronner, U., & Albrecht, B. (2023). Editorial: Transdiagnostic correlates of executive functions in psychiatric disorders. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 17, 1268506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hennecke, M., & Bürgler, S. (2023). Metacognition and self-control: An integrative framework. Psychological Review, 130(5), 1262–1288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Howard, S. J., & Vasseleu, E. (2020). Self-regulation and executive function longitudinally predict advanced learning in preschool. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hynd, G. W., Lorys, A. R., Semrud-Clikeman, M., Nieves, N., Huettner, M. I. S., & Lahey, B. B. (1991). Attention deficit disorder without hyperactivity: A distinct behavioral and neurocognitive syndrome. Journal of Child Neurology, 6(1), S37–S43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, M. H. (2012). Executive function and developmental disorders: The flip side of the coin. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(9), 454–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jones, D. T. (2020). Multiple aetiologies of the progressive dysexecutive syndrome and the importance of biomarkers. Brain Communications, 2(2), fcaa127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jones, D. T., & Graff-Radford, J. (2021). Executive dysfunction and the prefrontal cortex. Continuum, 27(6), 1586–1601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Karalunas, S. L., & Huang-Pollock, C. L. (2011). Examining relationships between executive functioning and delay aversion in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 40(6), 837–847. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kopp, C. B. (1982). Antecedents of self-regulation: A developmental perspective. Developmental Psychology, 18(2), 199–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krivitzky, L. S., Walsh, K. S., Fisher, E. L., & Berl, M. M. (2016). Executive functioning profiles from the BRIEF across pediatric medical disorders: Age and diagnosis factors. Child Neuropsychology, 22(7), 870–888. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kubota, M., Hadley, L. V., Schaeffner, S., Könen, T., Meaney, J.-A., Morey, C. C., Auyeung, B., Moriguchi, Y., Karbach, J., & Chevalier, N. (2023). The effect of metacognitive executive function training on children’s executive function, proactive control, and academic skills. Developmental Psychology, 59(11), 2002–2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuhn, D. (2000). Metacognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9(5), 178–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lee, D., Boulton, K. A., Sun, C., Phillips, N. L., Munro, M., Kumfor, F., Demetriou, E. A., & Guastella, A. J. (2025). Attention and executive delays in early childhood: A meta-analysis of neurodevelopmental conditions. Molecular Psychiatry, 30(5), 1906–1914. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lee, R. R., Ward, A. R., Lane, D. M., Aman, M. G., Loveland, K. A., Mansour, R., & Pearson, D. A. (2023). Executive function in Autism: Association with ADHD and ASD symptoms. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 53(2), 688–700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liebermann, D., Ploner, C. J., Kraft, A., Kopp, U. A., & Ostendorf, F. (2013). A dysexecutive syndrome of the medial thalamus. Cortex, 49(1), 40–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liu, C., Liu, R., Tao, R., Feng, J., Liang, X., Yang, Y., & Sit, C. H. (2025). Physical activity and executive functions in children and adolescents with neurodevelopmental disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Preventive Medicine, 193, 108246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lunkenheimer, E., Panlilio, C., Lobo, F. M., Olson, S. L., & Hamby, C. M. (2019). Preschoolers’ self-regulation in context: Task persistence profiles with mothers and fathers and later attention problems in kindergarten. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 47(6), 947–960. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mareva, S., Holmes, J., Lead Investigators, Astle, D., Baker, K., Gathercole, S., Holmes, J., Kievit, R., Manly, T., Team of Researchers and PhD Students, Akarca, D., Bathelt, J., Bettencourt, M., Bennett, M., Bignardi, G., Bishop, S., Bottacin, E., Bridge, L., Brkic, D., … Zhang, M. (2024). Mapping neurodevelopmental diversity in executive function. Cortex, 172, 204–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Margari, L., Craig, F., Margari, F., Legrottaglie, A., Palumbi, R., & De Giambattista, C. (2016). A review of executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 12, 1191–1201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCraw, A., Sullivan, J., Lowery, K., Eddings, R., Heim, H. R., & Buss, A. T. (2024). Dynamic field theory of executive function: Identifying early neurocognitive markers. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 89(3), 7–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meuwissen, A. S., & Carlson, S. M. (2015). Fathers matter: The role of father parenting in preschoolers’ executive function development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 140, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Michelini, G., Carlisi, C. O., Eaton, N. R., Elison, J. T., Haltigan, J. D., Kotov, R., Krueger, R. F., Latzman, R. D., Li, J. J., Levin-Aspenson, H. F., Salum, G. A., South, S. C., Stanton, K., Waldman, I. D., & Wilson, S. (2024). Where do neurodevelopmental conditions fit in transdiagnostic psychiatric frameworks? Incorporating a new neurodevelopmental spectrum. World Psychiatry: Official Journal of the World Psychiatric Association (WPA), 23(3), 333–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mirabella, G. (2021). Inhibitory control and impulsive responses in neurodevelopmental disorders. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 63(5), 520–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Moreno-Llanos, I., Guevara, I., Palacios, P., & Rodríguez, C. (2024). How to study the early development of executive function? Let’s put control back in the hands of children. New Ideas in Psychology, 72, 101049. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morgan, P. L., Farkas, G., Wang, Y., Hillemeier, M. M., Oh, Y., & Maczuga, S. (2019). Executive function deficits in kindergarten predict repeated academic difficulties across elementary school. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 46, 20–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Olsson, C., Arvidsson, P., & Blom Johansson, M. (2020). Measuring executive function in people with severe aphasia: Comparing neuropsychological tests and informant ratings. NeuroRehabilitation, 46(3), 299–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O’Reilly, F., U. Gattas, S., & Scerif, G. (2025). Improving executive function during toddlerhood: A systematic review and meta-analysis of parent-led interventions. Developmental Review, 76, 101198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ortiz Luna, J. A., & Acle Tomasini, G. (2006). Diferencias entre padres y maestros en la identificación de síntomas del trastorno por déficit de atención con hiperactividad en niños mexicanos. Revista de Neurología, 42(01), 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ossenkoppele, R., Pijnenburg, Y. A. L., Perry, D. C., Cohn-Sheehy, B. I., Scheltens, N. M. E., Vogel, J. W., Kramer, J. H., Van Der Vlies, A. E., Joie, R. L., Rosen, H. J., Van Der Flier, W. M., Grinberg, L. T., Rozemuller, A. J., Huang, E. J., Van Berckel, B. N. M., Miller, B. L., Barkhof, F., Jagust, W. J., Scheltens, P., … Rabinovici, G. D. (2015). The behavioural/dysexecutive variant of Alzheimer’s disease: Clinical, neuroimaging and pathological features. Brain, 138(9), 2732–2749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Otterman, D. L., Koopman-Verhoeff, M. E., White, T. J., Tiemeier, H., Bolhuis, K., & Jansen, P. W. (2019). Executive functioning and neurodevelopmental disorders in early childhood: A prospective population-based study. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 13(1), 38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ousley, O., & Cermak, T. (2014). Autism spectrum disorder: Defining dimensions and subgroups. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, 1(1), 20–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ozonoff, S., & Jensen, J. (1999). Brief report: Specific executive function profiles in three neurodevelopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(2), 171–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pappas, S., Ginsburg, H. P., & Jiang, M. (2003). SES differences in young children’s metacognition in the context of mathematical problem solving. Cognitive Development, 18(3), 431–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pardo-Salamanca, A., Paoletti, D., Pastor-Cerezuela, G., De Stasio, S., & Berenguer, C. (2024). Executive functioning profiles in neurodevelopmental disorders: Parent–child outcomes. Children, 11(8), 909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peng, Z., Stanton, K., Dominguez-Alvarez, B., & Watts, A. L. (2025). Where does attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder fit in the psychopathology hierarchy? A symptom-focused analysis. Journal of Psychopathology and Clinical Science, 134(2), 143–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(1), 51–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pezzoli, P., Parsons, S., Kievit, R. A., Astle, D. E., Huys, Q. J. M., Steinbeis, N., & Viding, E. (2023). Challenges and solutions to the measurement of neurocognitive mechanisms in developmental settings. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 8(8), 815–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Poletti, M., Cavallo, M., & Adenzato, M. (2017). Detecting dysexecutive syndrome in neurodegenerative diseases: Are we using an appropriate approach and effective diagnostic tools? Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 88(3), 195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rabinovici, G. D., Stephens, M. L., & Possin, K. L. (2015). Executive dysfunction. Continuum, 21(3), 646–659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rai, J. K., Abecassis, M., Casey, J. E., Flaro, L., Erdodi, L. A., & Roth, R. M. (2017). Parent rating of executive function in fetal alcohol spectrum disorder: A review of the literature and new data on aboriginal Canadian children. Child Neuropsychology, 23(6), 713–732. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ratto, A. B., Potvin, D., Pallathra, A. A., Saldana, L., & Kenworthy, L. (2020). Parents report fewer executive functioning problems and repetitive behaviors in young dual-language speakers with autism. Child Neuropsychology, 26(7), 917–933. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ribas, M. Z., Paticcié, G. F., Noleto, F. M., Ramanzini, L. G., Veras, A. D. O., Dall’Oglio, R., Filho, L. B. D. A., Martins Da Silva, J. G., Lima, M. P. P., Teixeira, B. E., Nunes De Sousa, G., Alves, A. F. C., Vieira Lima, L. M. F., Sallem, C. C., Garcia, T. F. M., Ponte De Oliveira, I. M., Rocha, R. S. D. C., Jucá, M. D. S., Barroso, S. T., & Claudino Dos Santos, J. C. (2023). Impact of dysexecutive syndrome in quality of life in Alzheimer disease: What we know now and where we are headed. Ageing Research Reviews, 86, 101866. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roebers, C. M. (2017). Executive function and metacognition: Towards a unifying framework of cognitive self-regulation. Developmental Review, 45, 31–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosas, R., Espinoza, V., Porflitt, F., & Ceric, F. (2019). Executive functions can be improved in preschoolers through systematic playing in educational settings: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roussel, M., Lhommée, E., Narme, P., Czernecki, V., Gall, D. L., Krystkowiak, P., Diouf, M., Godefroy, O., & on behalf of the GREFEX study group. (2017). Dysexecutive syndrome in Parkinson’s disease: The GREFEX study. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 24(5), 496–507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Roye, S., Calamia, M., & Robinson, A. (2022). Examining patterns of executive functioning across dimensions of psychopathology. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 77, 101778. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sadozai, A. K., Sun, C., Demetriou, E. A., Lampit, A., Munro, M., Perry, N., Boulton, K. A., & Guastella, A. J. (2024). Executive function in children with neurodevelopmental conditions: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Nature Human Behaviour, 8(12), 2357–2366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salagre, E., Arango, C., Artigas, F., Ayuso-Mateos, J. L., Bernardo, M., Castro-Fornieles, J., Bobes, J., Desco, M., Fañanás, L., González-Pinto, A., Haro, J. M., Leza, J. C., Mckenna, P. J., Meana, J. J., Menchón, J. M., Micó, J. A., Palomo, T., Pazos, Á., Pérez, V., … Vieta, E. (2019). Diez años de investigación traslacional colaborativa en enfermedades mentales: El CIBERSAM. Revista de Psiquiatría y Salud Mental, 12(1), 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- San Diego, R. J., Franke, N., Harding, J. E., & Wouldes, T. A. (2023). Cross-cultural validity and reliability of the BRIEF-P at age 2 and 4.5 years in children born at risk of neonatal hypoglycemia. Child Neuropsychology, 29(2), 340–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schneider, H., Ryan, M., & Mahone, E. M. (2020). Parent versus teacher ratings on the BRIEF-preschool version in children with and without ADHD. Child Neuropsychology: A Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence, 26(1), 113–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schraw, G., Crippen, K. J., & Hartley, K. (2006). Promoting self-regulation in science education: Metacognition as part of a broader perspective on learning. Research in Science Education, 36(1–2), 111–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scott, J. K., Nelson, J. A., & Dix, T. (2018). Interdependence among mothers, fathers, and children from early to middle childhood: Parents’ sensitivity and children’s externalizing behavior. Developmental Psychology, 54(8), 1528–1541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Snyder, H. R., Silton, R. L., Hankin, B. L., Smolker, H. R., Kaiser, R. H., Banich, M. T., Miller, G. A., & Heller, W. (2023). The dimensional structure of internalizing psychopathology: Relation to diagnostic categories. Clinical Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 11(6), 1044–1063. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Cortese, S., Fairchild, G., & Stringaris, A. (2016). Annual Research Review: Transdiagnostic neuroscience of child and adolescent mental disorders—Differentiating decision making in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, conduct disorder, depression, and anxiety. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(3), 321–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Spataro, P., Morelli, M., Pirchio, S., Costa, S., & Longobardi, E. (2024). Exploring the relations of executive functions with emotional, linguistic, and cognitive skills in preschool children: Parents vs. teachers reports. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 39(2), 1045–1067. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spiegel, J. A., Lonigan, C. J., & Phillips, B. M. (2017). Factor structure and utility of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function—Preschool Version. Psychological Assessment, 29(2), 172–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stuss, D. T., & Alexander, M. P. (2000). Executive functions and the frontal lobes: A conceptual view. Psychological Research, 63(3–4), 289–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tamm, L., & Peugh, J. (2019). Concordance of teacher-rated and performance-based measures of executive functioning in preschoolers. Child Neuropsychology, 25(3), 410–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tang, W. K., Wong, K. S. L., Mok, V. C. T., Chu, C. W. W., Wang, D., & Wong, A. (2020). Behavioural dysexecutive syndrome after stroke: Abridged secondary publication. Hong Kong Medical Journal = Xianggang Yi Xue Za Zhi, 26(6), 30–33. [Google Scholar]
- Tannock, R. (2013). Rethinking ADHD and LD in DSM-5: Proposed changes in diagnostic criteria. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46(1), 5–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Toplak, M. E., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Practitioner review: Do performance-based measures and ratings of executive function assess the same construct? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 54(2), 131–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Townes, P., Liu, C., Panesar, P., Devoe, D., Lee, S. Y., Taylor, G., Arnold, P. D., Crosbie, J., & Schachar, R. (2023). Do ASD and ADHD Have Distinct Executive Function Deficits? A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Direct Comparison Studies. Journal of Attention Disorders, 27(14), 1571–1582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vaidya, C. J., You, X., Mostofsky, S., Pereira, F., Berl, M. M., & Kenworthy, L. (2020). Data-driven identification of subtypes of executive function across typical development, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61(1), 51–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veenman, M. V. J., Van Hout-Wolters, B. H. A. M., & Afflerbach, P. (2006). Metacognition and learning: Conceptual and methodological considerations. Metacognition and Learning, 1(1), 3–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, X., & Cheng, Z. (2020). Cross-sectional studies. Chest, 158(1), S65–S71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wei, Y., Womer, F. Y., Sun, K., Zhu, Y., Sun, D., Duan, J., Zhang, R., Wei, S., Jiang, X., Zhang, Y., Tang, Y., Zhang, X., & Wang, F. (2023). Applying dimensional psychopathology: Transdiagnostic prediction of executive cognition using brain connectivity and inflammatory biomarkers. Psychological Medicine, 53(8), 3557–3567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Whitebread, D., & O’Sullivan, L. (2012). Preschool children’s social pretend play: Supporting the development of metacommunication, metacognition and self-regulation. International Journal of Play, 1(2), 197–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Williams, E. H., Thompson, N. M., McCray, G., Crespo-Llado, M. M., Bhavnani, S., Gajria, D., Mukherjee, D., Del Bianco, T., Lockwood-Estrin, G., Mason, L., Ngoma, V., Namathanga, C., Nkhata, R., Bennie, A., Ranjan, A., Kawelama, U., Midha, N., Singh, A., Mpakiza, I., … Chakrabarti, B. (2024). Scalable transdiagnostic early assessment of mental health (STREAM): A study protocol. BMJ Open, 14(6), e088263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilson, A. C., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2022). Does the autism phenotype differ when selecting groups by neurodevelopmental versus genetic diagnosis? An observational study comparing autism and sex chromosome trisomy. F1000Research, 11, 571. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wu, M., & Was, C. A. (2023). The relationship between executive functions and metacognition in college students. Journal of Intelligence, 11(12), 220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yaden, D. B., & Martinez-Yaden, C. (2023). Reintroducing “development” into theories of the acquisition and growth of early literacy: Developmental science approaches and the cultural-historical perspective of L. S. Vygotsky. In R. J. Tierney, F. Rizvi, & K. Ercikan (Eds.), International encyclopedia of education (4th ed., pp. 849–865). Elsevier. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zelazo, P. D. (2015). Executive function: Reflection, iterative reprocessing, complexity, and the developing brain. Developmental Review, 38, 55–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zelazo, P. D. (2020). Executive function and psychopathology: A neurodevelopmental perspective. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 16(1), 431–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zelazo, P. D., Qu, L., & Müller, U. (2005). Hot and cool aspects of executive function: Relations in early development. In Young children’s cognitive development: Interrelationships among executive functioning, working memory, verbal ability, and theory of mind (pp. 71–93). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. [Google Scholar]
Sample | Informant | ||
---|---|---|---|
Parents | Teachers | Subtotal | |
Clinical | 107 | 98 | 205 |
Normotypical | 1077 | 902 | 1979 |
Subtotal | 1184 | 1000 | 2184 |
Informant | Sample | Age (Years) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | Subtotal | ||
Parents | Clinical | 10 | 26 | 32 | 39 | 107 |
Normotypical | 156 | 262 | 339 | 320 | 1077 | |
Subtotal | 166 | 288 | 371 | 359 | 1184 | |
Teachers | Clinical | 9 | 24 | 32 | 33 | 98 |
Normotypical | 118 | 208 | 295 | 281 | 902 | |
Subtotal | 127 | 232 | 327 | 314 | 1000 |
Informant | Sample | Sex | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Man | Woman | Subtotal | ||
Parents | Clinical | 82 | 25 | 107 |
Normotypical | 567 | 510 | 1077 | |
Subtotal | 649 | 535 | 1184 | |
Teachers | Clinical | 74 | 24 | 98 |
Normotypical | 475 | 427 | 902 | |
Subtotal | 549 | 451 | 1000 |
Clinical Scales | Description | Examples |
---|---|---|
Inhibition | Assesses problems with controlling impulses and behavior, and difficulties stopping or appropriately regulating actions in specific moments or contexts. | During activities, the child is easily distracted from their goal. |
Flexibility | Assesses difficulties in voluntarily shifting between situations or activities and solving problems in a flexible manner. | Has trouble switching from one activity to another. |
Emotional control | Evaluates problems with adequately regulating or modulating emotional responses according to situational demands. | Becomes upset very easily. |
Working memory | Assesses difficulties in holding information in mind to complete a task or provide an appropriate response. | Has trouble remembering things even after a short period of time. |
Planning and organization | Assesses problems with anticipating future events or consequences. | Has trouble finding their belongings in their room or play area, even when given specific directions. |
Indices | Description | |
Inhibitory Self-Control Index | Sum of raw scores from the inhibition and emotional control scales. | |
Flexibility Index | Sum of raw scores from the flexibility and emotional control scales. | |
Emergent Metacognition Index | Sum of raw scores from the working memory and planning and organization scales. |
Clinical Scales and Indices | Sample | Informant | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Parents | Teachers | ||||||
Mean | Standard Deviation | Standard Error | Mean | Standard Deviation | Standard Error | ||
Inhibition | Clinical | 30.74 | 7.25 | 0.70 | 30.48 | 8.21 | 0.83 |
Normotypical | 23.92 | 5.71 | 0.17 | 21.91 | 6.25 | 0.21 | |
Subtotal | 24.54 | 6.18 | 0.18 | 22.75 | 6.95 | 0.22 | |
Flexibility | Clinical | 15.15 | 4.08 | 0.39 | 15.66 | 5.24 | 0.53 |
Normotypical | 13.48 | 3.20 | 0.10 | 12.7 | 3.16 | 0.11 | |
Subtotal | 13.63 | 3.32 | 0.10 | 12.99 | 3.53 | 0.11 | |
Emotional control | Clinical | 17.23 | 4.21 | 0.41 | 16.33 | 5.31 | 0.54 |
Normotypical | 14.48 | 3.62 | 0.11 | 13.14 | 3.73 | 0.12 | |
Subtotal | 14.73 | 3.76 | 0.11 | 13.45 | 4.03 | 0.13 | |
Working memory | Clinical | 31.51 | 7.70 | 0.75 | 32.45 | 8.92 | 0.90 |
Normotypical | 23.35 | 5.53 | 0.17 | 22.47 | 6.35 | 0.21 | |
Subtotal | 24.09 | 6.21 | 0.18 | 23.45 | 7.27 | 0.23 | |
Planning and organization | Clinical | 17.96 | 4.36 | 0.42 | 17.89 | 5.47 | 0.55 |
Normotypical | 14.38 | 3.33 | 0.10 | 13.14 | 3.53 | 0.12 | |
Subtotal | 14.71 | 3.58 | 0.10 | 13.61 | 4.02 | 0.13 | |
Inhibitory Self-Control Index | Clinical | 47.97 | 10.26 | 0.99 | 46.81 | 12.35 | 1.25 |
Normotypical | 38.4 | 8.57 | 0.26 | 35.04 | 9.181 | 0.31 | |
Subtotal | 39.27 | 9.16 | 0.27 | 36.2 | 10.15 | 0.32 | |
Flexibility Index | Clinical | 32.38 | 6.96 | 0.67 | 31.99 | 9.55 | 0.97 |
Normotypical | 27.96 | 5.76 | 0.18 | 25.83 | 5.93 | 0.20 | |
Subtotal | 28.36 | 6.01 | 0.18 | 26.44 | 6.63 | 0.21 | |
Emergent Metacognition Index | Clinical | 49.48 | 11.51 | 1.11 | 50.34 | 13.92 | 1.41 |
Normotypical | 37.73 | 8.41 | 0.26 | 35.61 | 9.60 | 0.32 | |
Subtotal | 38.79 | 9.36 | 0.27 | 37.06 | 11.00 | 0.35 | |
Executive Function Global | Clinical | 112.6 | 21.61 | 2.09 | 112.81 | 25.88 | 2.61 |
Normotypical | 89.61 | 17.03 | 0.52 | 83.35 | 18.37 | 0.61 | |
Subtotal | 91.69 | 18.69 | 0.54 | 86.24 | 21.12 | 0.67 |
ANOVA | Parents | Teachers | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
F | Sig. | F | Sig. | |
Inhibition | 131.486 | <0.001 *** | 155.526 | <0.001 *** |
Flexibility | 25.189 | <0.001 *** | 66.499 | <0.001 *** |
Emotional control | 54.718 | <0.001 *** | 58.764 | <0.001 *** |
Working memory | 195.763 | <0.001 *** | 199.632 | <0.001 *** |
Planning and organization | 106.074 | <0.001 *** | 140.485 | <0.001 *** |
Inhibitory Self-Control Index | 116.747 | <0.001 *** | 134.53 | <0.001 *** |
Flexibility Index | 55.178 | <0.001 *** | 82.615 | <0.001 *** |
Emergent Metacognition Index | 176.146 | <0.001 *** | 187.869 | <0.001 *** |
Executive Function Global | 168.072 | <0.001 *** | 207.474 | <0.001 *** |
Clinical Scales and Indices | Effect Sizes from ANOVA | Parents | Teachers | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Point Estimate | 95% Confidence Interval | Point Estimate | 95% Confidence Interval | ||||
Lower | Upper | Lower | Upper | ||||
Inhibition | Eta squared | 0.1 | 0.07 | 0.133 | 0.135 | 0.098 | 0.174 |
Epsilon squared | 0.099 | 0.069 | 0.132 | 0.134 | 0.097 | 0.173 | |
Fixed-effect omega squared | 0.099 | 0.069 | 0.132 | 0.134 | 0.097 | 0.173 | |
Random-effect omega squared | 0.099 | 0.069 | 0.132 | 0.134 | 0.097 | 0.173 | |
Flexibility | Eta squared | 0.021 | 0.008 | 0.04 | 0.062 | 0.037 | 0.093 |
Epsilon squared | 0.02 | 0.007 | 0.039 | 0.062 | 0.036 | 0.092 | |
Fixed-effect omega squared | 0.02 | 0.007 | 0.039 | 0.061 | 0.036 | 0.092 | |
Random-effect omega squared | 0.02 | 0.007 | 0.039 | 0.061 | 0.036 | 0.092 | |
Emotional control | Eta squared | 0.044 | 0.024 | 0.069 | 0.056 | 0.031 | 0.085 |
Epsilon squared | 0.043 | 0.023 | 0.068 | 0.055 | 0.03 | 0.084 | |
Fixed-effect omega squared | 0.043 | 0.023 | 0.068 | 0.055 | 0.03 | 0.084 | |
Random-effect omega squared | 0.043 | 0.023 | 0.068 | 0.055 | 0.03 | 0.084 | |
Working memory | Eta squared | 0.142 | 0.108 | 0.178 | 0.167 | 0.127 | 0.208 |
Epsilon squared | 0.141 | 0.107 | 0.178 | 0.166 | 0.126 | 0.207 | |
Fixed-effect omega squared | 0.141 | 0.107 | 0.177 | 0.166 | 0.126 | 0.207 | |
Random-effect omega squared | 0.141 | 0.107 | 0.177 | 0.166 | 0.126 | 0.207 | |
Planning and organization | Eta squared | 0.082 | 0.055 | 0.113 | 0.123 | 0.088 | 0.161 |
Epsilon squared | 0.082 | 0.054 | 0.113 | 0.123 | 0.087 | 0.161 | |
Fixed-effect omega squared | 0.082 | 0.054 | 0.112 | 0.122 | 0.087 | 0.161 | |
Random-effect omega squared | 0.082 | 0.054 | 0.112 | 0.122 | 0.087 | 0.161 | |
Inhibitory Self-Control Index | Eta squared | 0.09 | 0.061 | 0.122 | 0.119 | 0.084 | 0.156 |
Epsilon squared | 0.089 | 0.061 | 0.121 | 0.118 | 0.083 | 0.156 | |
Fixed-effect omega squared | 0.089 | 0.06 | 0.121 | 0.118 | 0.083 | 0.156 | |
Flexibility Index | Eta squared | 0.045 | 0.024 | 0.07 | 0.076 | 0.048 | 0.109 |
Epsilon squared | 0.044 | 0.024 | 0.069 | 0.076 | 0.047 | 0.109 | |
Fixed-effect omega squared | 0.044 | 0.024 | 0.069 | 0.075 | 0.047 | 0.108 | |
Random-effect omega squared | 0.044 | 0.024 | 0.069 | 0.075 | 0.047 | 0.108 | |
Emergent Metacognition Index | Eta squared | 0.13 | 0.096 | 0.165 | 0.158 | 0.12 | 0.199 |
Epsilon squared | 0.129 | 0.096 | 0.164 | 0.158 | 0.119 | 0.198 | |
Fixed-effect omega squared | 0.129 | 0.096 | 0.164 | 0.157 | 0.119 | 0.198 | |
Random-effect omega squared | 0.129 | 0.096 | 0.164 | 0.157 | 0.119 | 0.198 | |
Executive Function Global | Eta squared | 0.124 | 0.092 | 0.16 | 0.172 | 0.132 | 0.213 |
Epsilon squared | 0.124 | 0.091 | 0.159 | 0.171 | 0.131 | 0.212 | |
Fixed-effect omega squared | 0.124 | 0.091 | 0.159 | 0.171 | 0.131 | 0.212 | |
Random-effect omega squared | 0.124 | 0.091 | 0.159 | 0.171 | 0.131 | 0.212 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Herreras, E.B. Executive Functioning Profiles in Children with Neurodevelopmental Disorders. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 1256. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091256
Herreras EB. Executive Functioning Profiles in Children with Neurodevelopmental Disorders. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(9):1256. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091256
Chicago/Turabian StyleHerreras, Esperanza Bausela. 2025. "Executive Functioning Profiles in Children with Neurodevelopmental Disorders" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 9: 1256. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091256
APA StyleHerreras, E. B. (2025). Executive Functioning Profiles in Children with Neurodevelopmental Disorders. Behavioral Sciences, 15(9), 1256. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15091256