Next Article in Journal
Work–Family Boundary Fit and Employee Well-Being: The Mediating Role of Work–Family Conflict
Previous Article in Journal
The Impact of Cyber-Ostracism on Bystanders’ Helping Behavior Among Undergraduates: The Moderating Role of Rejection Sensitivity
Previous Article in Special Issue
Difficulties in Emotion Regulation as a Mediator and Gender as a Moderator in the Relationship Between Problematic Digital Gaming and Life Satisfaction Among Adolescents
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Differential Effects of Short- and Long-Term Negative Affect on Smartphone Usage: The Moderating Role of Locus of Control

1
Center for Psychological Sciences, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou 310058, China
2
Huawei Technologies Co., Ltd., Beijing 100080, China
3
Department of Psychology, Lingnan University, Hong Kong SAR, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2025, 15(8), 1121; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15081121
Submission received: 5 June 2025 / Revised: 12 August 2025 / Accepted: 14 August 2025 / Published: 18 August 2025

Abstract

In the digital age, smartphones are often used as tools for emotion regulation. While prior research has examined affective predictors of smartphone use, few studies have considered the combined impact of short-term and long-term affective states. This study investigates how daily negative emotional states and psychological distress relate to smartphone use and whether these associations are moderated by locus of control, a core belief about perceived control. Thirty-seven participants completed a one-month daily diary study combined with objective smartphone usage tracking, which yielded 837 valid observations. Multilevel analyses showed no association between daily negative emotional state and smartphone use. However, psychological distress predicted divergent behavioral patterns based on locus of control: individuals with an internal locus of control showed reduced usage under distress, whereas those with an external locus of control exhibited increased frequency of use. These findings highlight the importance of individual control beliefs in shaping technology-mediated emotion regulation and offer implications for interventions targeting excessive smartphone use.

1. Introduction

Smartphones have become essential in modern work and daily life. Despite their benefits, smartphones are also linked to a range of negative consequences, which have raised increasing concerns. These negative outcomes, including reduced attention, poor sleep quality, and mental health issues (J. Yang et al., 2020), may escalate into problematic smartphone use or even smartphone addiction (Elhai et al., 2017; James et al., 2023). These concerns have driven researchers to investigate not only the consequences but also the underlying determinants of smartphone use, particularly in relation to its role in regulating emotions. Often referred to as a “security blanket” or “first aid in the pocket”, smartphones help individuals cope with daily negative affect (Panova & Lleras, 2016; Schneider et al., 2023). Although studies have pointed to a link between negative affect (e.g., depression, anxiety) and excessive smartphone use (Elhai et al., 2020a; Tng & Yang, 2024), uncertainties remain regarding the precise nature of this relationship. Most existing work has examined either short-term or long-term negative affect in isolation (Elhai et al., 2018; Elhai & Contractor, 2018; Roggeveen et al., 2018), with little research clarifying their distinct effects. Furthermore, many studies have relied upon self-reported data and concentrated on relatively brief smartphone use periods (James et al., 2023; Ellis, 2019; Xu et al., 2024), thereby restricting their ability to capture comprehensive patterns of actual smartphone use over time.
In order to address these gaps, we conducted a one-month objective tracking of smartphone usage, incorporating daily negative emotional state assessments and a single-time measure of psychological distress. Additionally, we examined individual differences in locus of control (LOC) to investigate its moderating role in the relationship between negative affect and smartphone use. By integrating these elements, the present study advances the understanding of the impact of negative affect on smartphone usage and provides insights for effective interventions to mitigate excessive use.

1.1. Relationship Between Negative Affect and Smartphone Use

Uses and Gratifications Theory (UGT) provides a foundational framework for understanding why individuals use media (Katz et al., 1973). UGT posits that users are active individuals who selectively use media to obtain specific “gratifications” that fulfill their needs. One key gratification among these is stress relief and the regulation of emotions and mood (Wolfers et al., 2020). Compensatory Internet Use Theory (CIUT) extends UGT (Wickord & Quaiser-Pohl, 2022) by proposing that when individuals face negative situations in real life, including stress, frustration, or unmet needs, they may utilize the Internet as a compensatory mechanism to alleviate negative affect (Kardefelt-Winther, 2014). CIUT offers an essential framework for understanding the relationship between negative affect and smartphone use.
Negative affect is a broad term that encompasses a variety of negative feelings. Based on temporal stability, it ranges from state-like to trait-like forms (Geiger et al., 2019). In this context, CIUT explains the relationship between negative affect and smartphone use by addressing both the short-term emotional states that influence an individual’s smartphone use (within-person processes) and the long-term negative affect that contributes to individual distinctions in smartphone use (between-person processes). Thus far, most studies supporting CIUT have employed cross-sectional designs (Elhai et al., 2018; Elhai & Contractor, 2018), concentrating on the perspective of individual differences by measuring psychological distress (such as depression, anxiety, and stress). Psychological distress is considered a stable, long-term form of negative affect (Ridner, 2004) and is related to increased smartphone use (Elhai et al., 2017; Kara et al., 2019). However, some studies have also focused on the direct impact of individuals’ short-term emotional fluctuations on smartphone use (Roggeveen et al., 2018; Xu et al., 2024; X. Yang et al., 2023). For instance, researchers found an association between daily moods and smartphone use (Roggeveen et al., 2018; Xu et al., 2024).
In summary, while existing literature has separately confirmed that both long-term (trait-like) and short-term (state-like) negative affect are associated with smartphone use, few studies have situated both within a unified framework to systematically investigate the potentially distinct psychological mechanisms that may underlie them. Clarifying these mechanisms is crucial for developing effective interventions (Elhai et al., 2018).

1.2. Distinguishing Compensatory Use Mechanisms with a Dual-System Framework

Building on the gap identified above, this study proposes two distinct compensatory mechanisms. The first is an emotion-driven mechanism, where smartphone use is directly driven by short-term emotions, and it represents a within-person (time-varying) process. The second is habit-driven behavior, where long-term negative affect reinforces smartphone use (Elhai et al., 2017), which becomes less sensitive to momentary emotional fluctuations. It reflects a between-person (time-invariant) process that focuses on the automation and stability of behavior.
The dual-system model provides a useful lens for understanding these differences. It distinguishes two distinct psychological processing systems: a fast, automatic, and emotion-driven system (the automatic system), which governs immediate emotional reactions and rapid reward valuation; and a slow, controlled, and goal-oriented system (the deliberative system), responsible for impulse inhibition, self-regulation, and planned behavior (Frankish, 2010; Kahneman, 2003; McClure & Bickel, 2014; Shulman et al., 2016). This model is widely applied to impulse control and behavioral addiction and is supported by neural substrates (Shulman et al., 2016). Specifically, the emotion-driven mechanism involves the dynamic interplay of both systems (Gyurak et al., 2011). The automatic system rapidly generates instinctive emotional impulses, while the deliberative system may inhibit these impulses or strategically guide smartphone use toward emotion-regulation goals (Gross, 2015). The final usage behavior is, thus, an integrated outcome of impulse and regulation. By contrast, the habit-driven mechanism is primarily governed by the automatic system. When individuals experience persistent negative affect, the repeated use of a smartphone to gain temporary relief constitutes negative reinforcement (Brand, 2022). Over time, such use shifts from goal-directed action to a stimulus–response habit, becoming largely independent of deliberative oversight (Wood & Rünger, 2016).
The dual-system model not only explains the divergence between emotion-driven and habit-driven use but also emphasizes the central role of self-regulation in determining which pathway predominates. Self-regulation capacity is not random; it is systematically shaped by stable personality traits. Among these traits, LOC offers a theoretically grounded way to account for individual differences in how the two systems are engaged (Botha & Dahmann, 2024).

1.3. The Moderating Role of Locus of Control

Locus of control (LOC), originally proposed by Rotter (1966), refers to an individual’s belief regarding the extent to which their environment is controllable and outcomes are responsive to their actions. LOC is a unidimensional construct that ranges from internal to external (Farnier et al., 2021). Individuals with an internal LOC feel that they can influence outcomes through their own actions, while those with an external LOC typically believe that outcomes are more determined by external circumstances (De Freitas et al., 2023). LOC is a critical personality trait which has been shown to reliably predict affect, cognition, and behavior (Galvin et al., 2018). It significantly shapes how individuals manage stress and influences behavioral patterns (Farnier et al., 2021; Parkes, 1984).
Individuals with an internal LOC commonly engage in problem-focused coping and actively address stressors to mitigate their impact (Parkes, 1984; Wang et al., 2010). Conversely, individuals with an external LOC are more likely to rely upon emotion-focused coping, employing suppression strategies or external methods to handle emotions and stress (Ng et al., 2006). Furthermore, LOC influences how individuals engage with technology (L. H. N. Fong et al., 2017; N. Park et al., 2013). Individuals with an internal LOC may utilize smartphones more strategically, employing them as tools for obtaining information and exerting control over their environment (Casale et al., 2022). Conversely, those with an external LOC are more prone to problematic internet or smartphone use (Jacob, 2020; Y.-K. Lee et al., 2014). For instance, J. Li et al. (2015) revealed that while LOC does not influence the overall amount of smartphone use, it is significantly related to inappropriate use at unsuitable times. Similarly, Durak (2018) reported a positive correlation between external LOC and nomophobia.
Expanding on these findings, this study hypothesizes that LOC moderates the relationship between negative affect and smartphone use. Individuals with an internal LOC are more likely to engage their deliberative system, consciously choosing more adaptive coping strategies and thereby decreasing their dependence on smartphones. Conversely, those with an external LOC may be more susceptible to the automatic system, which leads them to increase their smartphone use as a maladaptive coping mechanism for managing negative affect.

1.4. Smartphone Use Indicators

Smartphone use can be quantified by duration (the total time spent using smartphones) and frequency (the number of usage episodes) (Hu et al., 2018; Venkatesh et al., 2008). Although both reflect usage intensity, they may be driven by different psychological factors. Venkatesh et al. (2008) argued that duration often reflects an intentional investment of time rooted in intrinsic motivation, whereas frequency is more determined by external factors and environmental demands. Empirical evidence showed that their shared variance is often limited (Tng & Yang, 2024), indicating that they capture partially distinct behavioral patterns (Toh et al., 2021).
Frequency is often regarded as a direct proxy for measuring the strength of habitual behavior (Naab & Schnauber, 2016; Verplanken & Orbell, 2003). In the context of smartphone use, one of the most common habitual patterns is the checking habit (Roffarello & De Russis, 2021), which typically involves constant checking or participation in a cycle of smartphone checking (Ytre-Arne et al., 2020). Checking behavior is defined as brief and frequent inspections of dynamic content (Oulasvirta et al., 2012). Such high-frequency, short-duration usage patterns reflect the impulsive and automatic nature of habitual use (Gerlach & Cenfetelli, 2020). Moreover, frequency and duration of use are functionally interrelated. For example, Wickord and Quaiser-Pohl (2025) reported that fragmented checking—even when each session is brief—can accumulate to substantially increase overall screen time. Measuring both indicators enables the analysis of average session duration (i.e., the amount of time spent in a single usage session; X. Yang et al., 2023). Notably, very short average session durations typically suggest unconscious checking, whereas longer average sessions are more likely to reflect deliberate engagement.
To accurately capture these complementary behavioral patterns and circumvent the systemic biases of self-reports (Marciano & Camerini, 2022), the present study incorporated objective tracking of both daily smartphone use duration and frequency. This dual-indicator approach enables us to test whether the proposed effects of negative affect and LOC are robust across distinct facets of smartphone use and to explore potential divergences between duration-based and frequency-based outcomes.

1.5. The Present Study

Building on this review of the prior literature, this study seeks to examine the determinants of smartphone use, focusing on short-term negative affect (daily negative emotional state, DNES), long-term negative affect (psychological distress, PD), and the moderating role of LOC. The present study employs a one-month daily diary design with continuous smartphone use tracking. We propose the following hypotheses:
H1 (Within-person effect).
On days when individuals report a higher DNES, their smartphone use will be positively correlated.
H2 (Between-person effect).
Individuals with higher levels of PD will exhibit higher smartphone use.
H3 (Cross-level moderation effect).
LOC will moderate the within-person association between DNES and smartphone use. Specifically, this positive association will be stronger for individuals with a higher external LOC.
H4 (Between-person moderation effect).
LOC will moderate the between-person association between PD and smartphone use. Specifically, this positive association will be stronger for individuals with a higher external LOC.
Additionally, we explore whether the observed patterns differ depending on whether smartphone usage is measured by duration or frequency.

2. Methods and Materials

2.1. Participants and Procedure

This study recruited 40 participants from eastern China through online convenience sampling. It was conducted from November 2021 to January 2022 using a rolling recruitment strategy, ensuring that each participant took part for approximately one month. Three participants withdrew during the study, resulting in a final sample of 37 participants. Participants were aged between 18 and 39 (M = 26, SD = 5.02), with 48.6% being female. The study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board.
The study consisted of two main components: (1) a daily diary, in which participants completed a DNES report each day; and (2) continuous smartphone use tracking, in which a custom Android application automatically recorded the app name, start time, and duration of each user-initiated smartphone use session. At the start of the study, participants also completed questionnaires to gather person-level information (e.g., LOC).
Participants had an average of 25.1 participation days (SD = 7.0), which yielded a total of 929 study days. The rate of valid day-level observations (i.e., days with both smartphone tracking and diary entries) was 90.1% (837 out of 929). Thus, participants contributed an average of 22.6 complete day-level observations. Previous research indicates that multiple imputation of missing values does not offer benefits for mixed-effects modeling (D. Y. T. Fong et al., 2013). Therefore, listwise deletion was ultimately chosen for the main analyses in this study.

2.2. Measures

DNES was assessed each evening at 10 p.m. to capture participants’ reflections on their overall emotional experiences of the day, consistent with prior daily diary methods (Xu et al., 2024). Participants completed a single-item online questionnaire, rating their daily emotional state on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (“Very Negative”) to 5 (“Very Positive”). For analysis, these scores were reverse-coded so that higher values indicated greater negative affect, consistent with the study’s focus on negative emotional states. This brief single-item measure, adapted from previous studies (e.g., X. Yang et al., 2023), was selected to reduce response burden.
PD was measured using the Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS-21) (Osman et al., 2012). The scale consists of three subscales—depression, anxiety, and stress—each comprising seven items. Example items include the following: “I couldn’t seem to experience any positive feeling at all” (depression), “I experienced breathing difficulty” (anxiety), and “I found it hard to wind down” (stress). Items are rated on a 4-point scale, which ranges from 0 (“Did not apply to me at all”) to 3 (“Applied to me very much, or most of the time”), with higher scores indicating greater symptom frequency. Following the DASS-21 manual (University of New South Wales, 2025), raw scores from the three subscales were standardized into Z-scores to derive an overall PD score. The Cronbach’s alpha for the sample was 0.92.
LOC was measured using the Internal-External Locus of Control Scale (Rotter, 1966), which includes 29 forced-choice items, with 6 unscored filler items. For each item, two options are presented, representing internal or external control orientations. Scores are calculated by totaling the items indicative of external control, resulting in an overall score ranging from 0 to 23. Higher scores indicate external LOC, while lower scores reflect internal LOC (Stiglbauer, 2017). A representative item is “Becoming a success is a matter of hard work, luck has little or nothing to do with it” (internal) versus “Getting a good job depends mainly on being in the right place at the right time” (external). The Cronbach’s alpha for this study was 0.72.
Demographics, such as gender, age, and occupation, were collected. Additionally, other measures—including those related to personality—were administered but excluded from the present analysis, as they were beyond the scope of the study.
Smartphone use was assessed as follows: (1) daily smartphone use duration (DSUD) refers to the total amount of time (in minutes) that participants used smartphone applications each day, and (2) daily smartphone use frequency (DSUF) represents the number of times participants launched any applications daily.

2.3. Data Analysis

To account for the nested structure of repeated longitudinal data, a multilevel modeling framework was employed (Snijders & Bosker, 2011). Two separate models were constructed for each DSUD and DSUF, using the same predictors. Model 1 tested the main effects, while Model 2 examined the moderation hypotheses. The model equations are as follows:
Model 1 (Main Effects):
D S U D i t , D S U F i t = β 0 i + β 1 i   ×   w p . D N E S i t + ε i t
β 0 i = γ 00 + γ 01 × b p . D N E S i + γ 02 × P D i + γ 03 × L O C i + γ 04 × A g e i + γ 05 × G e n d e r i + u 0 i
β 1 i = γ 10
Model 2 (Interaction Effects):
D S U D i t , D S U F i t = β 0 i + β 1 i × w p . D N E S i t + ε i t
β 0 i = γ 00 + γ 01 × b p . D N E S i + γ 02 × P D i + γ 03 × L O C i + γ 04 × A g e i + γ 05 × G e n d e r i + γ 07 P D i × L O C i + u 0 i
β 1 i = γ 10 + γ 11 L O C i
D S U D i t   a n d   D S U F i t represent the repeated measures of smartphone use for individual i on day t. The intercept ( β 0 i ) reflects each individual’s baseline level of smartphone use, while the slope ( β 1 i ) captures the within-person effect of a time-varying predictor. The fixed-effect coefficients ( γ ) estimate average associations at the sample level, and the random effects ( u ) are the residual unexplained between-person differences. The DNES predictor was decomposed into within-person fluctuations (wp.DNES) and between-person differences (bp.DNES). Model 1 included wp.DNES and PD as predictors, focusing on their main effects on smartphone use. Model 2 expanded Model 1 by incorporating the interaction terms (wp.DNES × LOC and PD × LOC) to examine LOC’s moderating effects.
Data analysis was conducted using R (Version 4.2.2), particularly the lme4 statistical package. All Level 1 predictors were group-mean centered, and all Level 2 predictors were grand-mean centered (Enders & Tofighi, 2007). Furthermore, based on prior research indicating that age and gender are important demographic factors in smartphone use (Meng et al., 2024; J. S. Park et al., 2019), these two variables were included as control variables in our analyses.

3. Results

3.1. Preliminary Analysis

Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics and correlations among the variables. On average, participants used their smartphones for 308.15 min per day (DSUD) and launched apps 184 times per day (DSUF). The two smartphone usage metrics were strongly correlated at both the between-person (r = 0.81, p < 0.001) and within-person levels (r = 0.77, p < 0.001). As for the measures of negative affect, the average monthly DNES and PD were positively associated (r = 0.33, p = 0.049).
Regarding the core hypotheses, there were no significant correlations between PD and the average smartphone usage metrics (Avg DSUD and Avg DSUF) at the between-person level. Similarly, at the within-person level, daily DNES was not significantly correlated with daily smartphone usage. Among the control variables, age was positively correlated with average DSUD (r = 0.39, p = 0.019) but not with DSUF. Age was also negatively correlated with average DNES (r = −0.33, p = 0.045). Gender was not significantly associated with any of the main study variables.
Finally, variance partitioning revealed intraclass correlation coefficients of 0.31 for DNES, 0.64 for DSUD, and 0.64 for DSUF, indicating substantial between-person variability and supporting the use of multilevel modeling.

3.2. Multilevel Models

The results of the multilevel linear models are presented in Table 2 and Table 3.
Model 1: In terms of DSUD, the effect of wp.DNES (β = 0.03, p = 0.151) and PD (β = 0.07, p = 0.603) was not significant. For DSUF, neither wp.DNES (β = −0.01, p = 0.829) nor PD (β = −0.05, p = 0.840) showed significant effects. The control variables of age and gender also did not show any significant effects on either DSUD or DSUF (all ps > 0.05).
Model 2: The interaction effect of wp.DNES × LOC was not significant for DSUD (β = −0.02, p = 0.312) or DSUF (β = −0.03, p = 0.194). The interaction effect of PD × LOC was marginally significant for DSUD (β = 0.27, p = 0.074) but was significant for DSUF (β = 0.47, p < 0.001). Age had a significant positive effect on DSUD (β = 0.34, p = 0.020) but not on DSUF. Gender had no significant effect on either usage metric.
To further probe the significant interaction, we conducted a simple slopes analysis. Figure 1 presents the effect of PD on smartphone use for low LOC (−1 SD, internal LOC) and high LOC (+1 SD, external LOC) (Stiglbauer, 2017). Visually, as LOC becomes more external, the effect of PD on smartphone use shifts from negative to positive. These patterns were significant for DSUF and marginal for DSUD. Simple slopes analyses for DSUF revealed that for individuals with an internal LOC, PD had a significantly negative effect on DSUF (β = −15.26, p = 0.012), while with an external LOC, PD had a significantly positive effect on DSUF (β= 16.81, p = 0.014).
Additionally, we examined the daily average session duration (calculated as DSUD divided by DSUF) as a complementary indicator of smartphone use. The interaction effect of PD × LOC was significant for average session duration (β = −0.24, p = 0.03; Figure 2), suggesting that under high PD, individuals with an internal LOC tended to have longer smartphone use per session compared to those with an external LOC—a pattern that was not significant under low PD.

4. Discussion

In order to investigate the effects of negative affect and LOC on smartphone use, this study employed a longitudinal design and objective data tracking. The results demonstrated no significant within-person association between DNES and smartphone use. However, LOC significantly moderated the relationship between PD and daily smartphone use frequency and showed a marginally significant moderating effect on daily smartphone use duration. These results offer a nuanced perspective on the complex relationships between negative affect, personality traits, and smartphone use patterns. This serves to contribute to a deeper understanding of CIUT.

4.1. The Limited Efficacy of the Emotion-Driven Pathway

This study found no significant within-person association between DNES and smartphone use (H1 was not supported). These results align with prior research that failed to detect significant within-person associations between short-term negative affect and smartphone use (Bradley & Howard, 2023; X. Yang et al., 2023). As Bradley and Howard (2023) suggested, the relationship between emotional fluctuations and smartphone use is unlikely to follow a simple “dose-response” pattern. This finding can be interpreted through the lens of the dual-system model, which highlights the significant role of the deliberative system. The deliberative system manages emotional impulses to produce divergent, goal-directed outcomes (Shulman et al., 2016). For example, in response to negative emotions, an individual might strategically use their smartphone for social support (promoting use), while at another moment, they might consciously inhibit the same urge to focus on a demanding task. It is plausible that these opposing, consciously driven behaviors co-exist throughout the day, thereby canceling each other out in aggregated daily-level analyses. This mechanism also helps to explain the importance of timescale. For example, Roggeveen et al. (2018) observed a positive association between short-term emotional states and usage when participants rated their emotions 10 times during the day. This suggests that momentary emotional urges may not systematically translate into an overall increase in daily smartphone use.
Building upon this, this study did not find a significant cross-level moderating effect of LOC on the relationship between DNES and smartphone use (H3 was not supported). The absence of a significant main effect (H1) means there was no consistent, systematic association for a moderator to act upon. Additionally, the stable dispositional trait LOC may have a limited role in moderating immediate, state-level reactions to daily emotional variations (Galvin et al., 2018). It is plausible that reactions to daily emotions are more likely influenced by situational constraints (e.g., workload, social obligations; Greenaway et al., 2018). Instead, LOC is better suited for predicting responses to chronic, persistent stressors, such as PD.
Therefore, the absence of a direct within-person effect at the daily level does not necessarily invalidate the emotion-driven pathway proposed by CIUT. Rather, it highlights its nature as a momentary, highly context-dependent process that may not aggregate into a detectable increase in total daily use.

4.2. The Habit-Driven Pathway: Locus of Control Shapes Compensatory Use

This study found no between-person association between PD and smartphone use (H2 was not supported). This finding aligns with the notable lack of consensus in the literature, which has reported both positive (Elhai et al., 2017; Kara et al., 2019) and negative (Elhai et al., 2016) correlations between PD and smartphone use. We argue that this inconsistency reflects the complexity of the relationship rather than its absence, specifically, conditional effects shaped by personality. Furthermore, the methodological differences in the literature likely contributed to these divergent results. While the present study employed objective smartphone usage data, most prior studies have relied on self-report measures. Prior research comparing self-reported and objectively logged digital behaviors has demonstrated that self-reports often misestimate actual usage (Ellis, 2019), potentially obscuring moderation effects that may be detectable with more accurate measures.
This study identified a significant interaction between PD and LOC on smartphone use frequency (providing support for H4). Although this interaction was not significant for duration, a similar pattern emerged. Specifically, individuals with an external LOC increased their smartphone use under high PD levels, whilst those with an internal LOC typically reduced their usage. This result is consistent with prior research indicating that coping strategies are influenced by LOC (Farnier et al., 2021; Petrosky & Birkimer, 1991). Moreover, this finding supports the notion that individuals with an external LOC are more prone to maladaptive usage of digital devices (Jacob, 2020; Y.-K. Lee et al., 2014). To understand this interaction pattern, it is essential to consider the coping tendencies typically associated with LOC. External LOC involves attributing problems to uncontrollable external factors (Keenan & McBAIN, 1979), lowering perceived personal control and fostering a problem-avoidance orientation (Rotter, 1966). Such orientation is associated with escape–avoidance coping (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984) and self-distraction (Carver & Scheier, 1982), which aim to distract negative emotions rather than attempt problem resolution (Stanisławski, 2019). Smartphones, with their mobility, connectivity, and multifunctionality (Wadley et al., 2020), are well suited to support emotion regulation strategies, such as mood relief (Rozgonjuk et al., 2019), digital procrastination (Reinecke & Hofmann, 2016), and social reassurance seeking (Elhai et al., 2020b; Reid & Reid, 2007). Over time, the repeated use of smartphones for emotional escape can reinforce these behaviors, gradually solidifying them into automatic habits (Elhai et al., 2017). From a dual-system perspective, habit formation reduces the reliance on deliberative processes, thereby increasing the influence of impulsive, automatic processes. This habit-driven mechanism offers a plausible explanation for the significantly elevated smartphone use observed among individuals with an external LOC under high psychological distress. Conversely, individuals with an internal LOC—who believe they can control their environment—typically adopt active, problem-focused coping strategies (Ng et al., 2006; Wang et al., 2010). In high-distress conditions, these individuals may deliberately limit smartphone use to avoid distraction and preserve cognitive resources for problem solving (A. Y. Lee et al., 2023). Such an approach requires deliberate planning and sustained attention; processes governed by deliberate systems to regulate impulses and serve long-term goals.
Taken together, our findings suggest that long-term negative affect—reflected in between-person differences—plays a more substantial role in driving overall daily usage than short-term emotional fluctuations. This pattern suggests the habit-driven mechanisms within CIUT are more relevant to smartphone use behaviors than the emotion-driven mechanisms.

4.3. The Meaning of Different Smartphone Use Indicators

DSUD and DSUF measure how much versus how often an individual uses their smartphone in a single day. The average levels of these indicators were consistent with those reported in previous studies (e.g., Bradley & Howard, 2023; Guo et al., 2022), providing evidence for the validity of our measurements and the generalizability of our findings. Although DSUD and DSUF were correlated, their divergent statistical patterns in this study suggest that they capture partially overlapping yet distinct behavioral tendencies.
Frequency of behavior is a key indicator of habit strength (Verplanken & Orbell, 2003). Consistent with this, our findings show that smartphone use frequency is more strongly influenced by the interaction between PD and LOC than duration. This suggests that frequency of use may be a more direct expression of habit-driven patterns than total duration. Moreover, under high PD, individuals with an external LOC exhibited significantly shorter daily average session durations than those with an internal LOC, which may reflect checking behaviors. This pattern supports research suggesting that checking behaviors are habitual responses to reduce the gap between an individual’s perceived and actual state in the real world (Gerlach & Cenfetelli, 2020). For individuals with an external LOC, such checking behaviors may serve as a coping mechanism to manage distress and reduce uncertainty (Liu et al., 2022).
Demographic variables further help distinguish DSUD and DSUF. In our study, age was a significant positive predictor of smartphone use duration but not frequency. This pattern suggests that older users may engage in longer, more sustained sessions of phone use—such as news reading (Busch et al., 2021)—rather than the high-frequency, brief checking behaviors typically reflected in DSUF. Consistent with this interpretation, prior objective logging research has shown that older adults have significantly longer average app session durations compared to younger users (Gordon et al., 2019).
Taken together, these results reinforce our central argument that duration and frequency are not interchangeable metrics. They likely reflect different underlying motivations and user profiles, with frequency serving as a more direct behavioral expression of the habit-driven processes examined in this study.

4.4. Implications

CIUT posits that individuals use smartphones to alleviate negative affect. Building on this, our study specifies and refines CIUT by situating its dual pathways within a dual-system framework. We found no evidence that daily emotional fluctuations systematically drive overall usage, likely because immediate affective impulses are variably regulated by reflective processes. By contrast, our results highlight the relevance of the habit-driven mechanism—a process reflecting the dominance of automatic impulses—which is shaped by the interplay between persistent psychological distress and an individual’s locus of control. This suggests that, within CIUT, long-term emotional vulnerabilities and stable individual differences are more influential in driving compensatory smartphone use than transient emotional states.
Given these differentiated pathways, interventions to mitigate excessive smartphone use should distinguish between short- and long-term negative affect. In terms of short-term negative affect, certain interventions, such as personalized notifications, can help promote immediate self-regulation (Lyngs et al., 2019). Conversely, more comprehensive support is required to address long-term negative affect, including coping-skills training (S. Li et al., 2024) and programs focused on bolstering individuals’ LOC (Zappala-Piemme et al., 2023). By addressing various affective factors, interventions can be more tailored and effective.
Moreover, this study emphasizes the importance of measurements of smartphone use. In comparison to self-reported data, logged data provides a more precise reflection of actual behavior (Parry et al., 2021). Additionally, a multifaceted measurement approach—disaggregating smartphone use into components (e.g., frequency, duration, session length)—could help capture the psychological mechanisms.

4.5. Limitations and Future Study

Despite its strengths, this study had several limitations. First, a significant limitation is the use of a single-item measure for DNES. While intended to reduce participant burden, this approach compromises construct validity, as negative affect is a multifaceted construct that a single item cannot fully capture. Crucially, this may limit the interpretability of our null findings for within-person effects. The measure might lack the sensitivity to detect subtle daily emotional fluctuations. Therefore, the absence of a significant effect could be an artifact of this measurement weakness. Future research should employ validated short-form scales (e.g., the short-form PANAS; Mackinnon et al., 1999) to allow for a more conclusive test of these hypotheses. Second, despite employing a longitudinal design, this study only measured participants’ emotional state at the end of each day. Consequently, it was not possible to draw conclusions about whether affect influences smartphone use or vice versa (Elhai et al., 2017). Future studies might consider more intensive measurements or advanced analytical methods to better capture temporal dynamics and reinforce causal inferences. Third, the sample size was relatively small and consisted primarily of Android users, which may have restricted the external validity of the findings. Future research should diversify the sample to enhance the generalizability and reliability of the results. Finally, although this study utilized objective smartphone use measures, it did not capture the broader contexts in which smartphones are used (Wei et al., 2024). Going forward, studies should refine behavioral indicators by considering factors such as usage categories and contexts (Lukoff et al., 2018). This could help to identify more precise patterns of smartphone usage and offer deeper insights into the interactive mechanisms between affect and smartphone use.

5. Conclusions

Based on CIUT, this study used objective measurements and a longitudinal design to examine the impact of short- and long-term negative affect on smartphone use. Additionally, this study incorporated LOC as a moderating factor. The results showed (1) no significant association between DNES and daily smartphone use and (2) that LOC moderated the relationship between PD and smartphone use frequency. These findings underscore the crucial role of habituation mechanisms rather than emotional triggers in smartphone use. By integrating both short- and long-term emotional factors alongside individual differences, this study provides valuable insights for developing more targeted interventions in problematic smartphone use.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, Y.L. and J.X.; methodology, S.L., Y.L. and J.X.; software, Y.L.; validation, J.L.; formal analysis, Y.C. and J.L.; investigation, S.L., Y.L. and J.X.; data curation, Y.C. and J.L.; writing—original draft preparation, Y.C.; writing—review and editing, J.L., S.L., Y.L. and J.X.; visualization, Y.C.; supervision, J.X.; project administration, Y.L. and J.X. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Joint Innovation Lab of Consumer Business Group (Huawei Technologies Co., Ltd.)—Zhejiang University.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Center for Psychological Sciences at Zhejiang University (ethics approval number: 2021-020; ethics approval date: 1 September 2021).

Informed Consent Statement

Written informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study. Each participant received appropriate monetary compensation, and participants were informed of their right to terminate their participation at any stage of the study.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors on request.

Conflicts of Interest

Author Yanfang Liu was employed by the company Huawei Technologies Co., Ltd. The authors declare that this study received funding from the Joint Innovation Lab of Consumer Business Group (Huawei Technologies Co., Ltd.)—Zhejiang University. The funder had the following involvement with the study: study design. The funder was not involved in the collection, analysis, or interpretation of data; the writing of this article; or the decision to submit it for publication.

Abbreviations

The following abbreviations are used in this manuscript:
LOCLocus of control
PDPsychological distress
DESDaily emotional state
DSUFDaily smartphone use frequency
DSUDDaily smartphone use duration

References

  1. Botha, F., & Dahmann, S. C. (2024). Locus of control, self-control, and health outcomes. SSM-Population Health, 25, 101566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Bradley, A. H. M., & Howard, A. L. (2023). Stress and mood associations with smartphone use in university students: A 12-week longitudinal study. Clinical Psychological Science, 11(5), 921. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Brand, M. (2022). Can internet use become addictive? Science, 376(6595), 798–799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Busch, P. A., Hausvik, G. I., Ropstad, O. K., & Pettersen, D. (2021). Smartphone usage among older adults. Computers in Human Behavior, 121, 106783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1982). Control theory: A useful conceptual framework for personality–social, clinical, and health psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 92(1), 111–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Casale, S., Fioravanti, G., Gioia, F., Redditi, E., & Spada, M. (2022). Fear of missing out and fear of not being up to date: Investigating different pathways towards social and process problematic smartphone use. Current Psychology, 42(26), 22847–22856. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. De Freitas, J., Agarwal, S., Schmitt, B., & Haslam, N. (2023). Psychological factors underlying attitudes toward AI tools. Nature Human Behaviour, 7(11), 1845–1854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Durak, H. Y. (2018). What would you do without your smartphone? Adolescents’ social media usage, locus of control, and loneliness as a predictor of nomophobia. Addicta: The Turkish Journal on Addictions, 5(3), 543–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Elhai, J. D., & Contractor, A. A. (2018). Examining latent classes of smartphone users: Relations with psychopathology and problematic smartphone use. Computers in Human Behavior, 82, 159–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Elhai, J. D., Dvorak, R. D., Levine, J. C., & Hall, B. J. (2017). Problematic smartphone use: A conceptual overview and systematic review of relations with anxiety and depression psychopathology. Journal of Affective Disorders, 207, 251–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Elhai, J. D., Gallinari, E. F., Rozgonjuk, D., & Yang, H. (2020a). Depression, anxiety and fear of missing out as correlates of social, non-social and problematic smartphone use. Addictive Behaviors, 105, 106335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Elhai, J. D., Levine, J. C., Dvorak, R. D., & Hall, B. J. (2016). Fear of missing out, need for touch, anxiety and depression are related to problematic smartphone use. Computers in Human Behavior, 63, 509–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Elhai, J. D., Rozgonjuk, D., Alghraibeh, A. M., Levine, J. C., Alafnan, A. A., Aldraiweesh, A. A., Aljomaa, S. S., & Hall, B. J. (2020b). Excessive reassurance seeking mediates relations between rumination and problematic smartphone use. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 84(2), 137–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Elhai, J. D., Tiamiyu, M. F., Weeks, J. W., Levine, J. C., Picard, K. J., & Hall, B. J. (2018). Depression and emotion regulation predict objective smartphone use measured over one week. Personality and Individual Differences, 133, 21–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Ellis, D. A. (2019). Are smartphones really that bad? Improving the psychological measurement of technology-related behaviors. Computers in Human Behavior, 97, 60–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Enders, C. K., & Tofighi, D. (2007). Centering predictor variables in cross-sectional multilevel models: A new look at an old issue. Psychological Methods, 12(2), 121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Farnier, J., Shankland, R., Kotsou, I., Inigo, M., Rosset, E., & Leys, C. (2021). Empowering well-being: Validation of a locus of control scale specific to well-Being. Journal of Happiness Studies, 22(8), 3513–3542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Fong, D. Y. T., Rai, S. N., & Lam, K. S. L. (2013). Estimating the effect of multiple imputation on incomplete longitudinal data with application to a randomized clinical study. Journal of Biopharmaceutical Statistics, 23(5), 1004–1022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Fong, L. H. N., Lam, L. W., & Law, R. (2017). How locus of control shapes intention to reuse mobile apps for making hotel reservations: Evidence from chinese consumers. Tourism Management, 61, 331–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Frankish, K. (2010). Dual-process and dual-system theories of reasoning. Philosophy Compass, 5(10), 914–926. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Galvin, B. M., Randel, A. E., Collins, B. J., & Johnson, R. E. (2018). Changing the focus of locus (of control): A targeted review of the locus of control literature and agenda for future research. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 39(7), 820–833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Geiger, M., Lee, J.-Y., & Geiger, M. K. (2019). The power of negative disposition: An examination of trait and state negative affect with OCBI and OCBO. Group & Organization Management, 44(6), 1136–1172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Gerlach, J. P., & Cenfetelli, R. T. (2020). Constant checking is not addiction: A grounded theory of IT-mediated state-tracking. MIS Quarterly, 44(4), 1705–1732. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Gordon, M. L., Gatys, L., Guestrin, C., Bigham, J. P., Trister, A., & Patel, K. (2019, May 4–9). APP usage predicts cognitive ability in older adults. 2019 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (pp. 1–12), Glasgow, UK. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Greenaway, K. H., Kalokerinos, E. K., & Williams, L. A. (2018). Context is everything (in emotion research). Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 12(6), e12393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: Current status and future prospects. Psychological Inquiry, 26(1), 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Guo, N., Tsun Luk, T., Wu, Y., Lai, A. Y., Li, Y., Cheung, D. Y. T., Wong, J. Y., Fong, D. Y. T., & Wang, M. P. (2022). Between- and within-person associations of mobile gaming time and total screen time with sleep problems in young adults: Daily assessment study. Addictive Behaviors, 134, 107408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Gyurak, A., Gross, J. J., & Etkin, A. (2011). Explicit and implicit emotion regulation: A dual-process framework. Cognition and Emotion, 25(3), 400–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Hu, T., Stafford, T. F., Kettinger, W. J., Zhang, X. P., & Dai, H. (2018). Formation and effect of social media usage habit. Journal of Computer Information Systems, 58(4), 334–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Jacob, J. (2020). Internet addiction, locus of control and resilience. Artha Journal of Social Sciences, 19(1), 27–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. James, R. J. E., Dixon, G., Dragomir, M. G., Thirlwell, E., & Hitcham, L. (2023). Understanding the construction of “behavior” in smartphone addiction: A scoping review. Addictive Behaviors, 137, 107503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58(9), 697–720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Kara, M., Baytemir, K., & Inceman-Kara, F. (2019). Duration of daily smartphone usage as an antecedent of nomophobia: Exploring multiple mediation of loneliness and anxiety. Behaviour & Information Technology, 40(1), 85–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Kardefelt-Winther, D. (2014). A conceptual and methodological critique of internet addiction research: Towards a model of compensatory internet use. Computers in Human Behavior, 31, 351–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Katz, E., Blumler, J. G., & Gurevitch, M. (1973). Uses and gratifications research. The Public Opinion Quarterly, 37(4), 509–523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Keenan, A., & McBAIN, G. D. M. (1979). Effects of type a behaviour, intolerance of ambiguity, and locus of control on the relationship between role stress and work-related outcomes. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 52(4), 277–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping. Springer. [Google Scholar]
  38. Lee, A. Y., Ellison, N. B., & Hancock, J. T. (2023). To use or be used? The role of agency in social media use and well-being. Frontiers in Computer Science, 5, 1123323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Lee, Y.-K., Chang, C.-T., Lin, Y., & Cheng, Z.-H. (2014). The dark side of smartphone usage: Psychological traits, compulsive behavior and technostress. Computers in Human Behavior, 31, 373–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Li, J., Lepp, A., & Barkley, J. E. (2015). Locus of control and cell phone use: Implications for sleep quality, academic performance, and subjective well-being. Computers in Human Behavior, 52, 450–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Li, S., Feng, N., & Cui, L. (2024). Network analysis of social anxiety and problematic mobile phone use in Chinese adolescents: A longitudinal study. Addictive Behaviors, 155, 108026. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Liu, C., Ren, L., Li, K., Yang, W., Li, Y., Rotaru, K., Wei, X., Yücel, M., & Albertella, L. (2022). Understanding the association between intolerance of uncertainty and problematic smartphone use: A network analysis. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 13, 917833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Lukoff, K., Yu, C., Kientz, J., & Hiniker, A. (2018). What makes smartphone use meaningful or meaningless? Proceedings of the ACM on Interactive, Mobile, Wearable and Ubiquitous Technologies, 2(1), 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Lyngs, U., Lukoff, K., Slovak, P., Binns, R., Slack, A., Inzlicht, M., Van Kleek, M., & Shadbolt, N. (2019, May 4–9). Self-control in cyberspace: Applying dual systems theory to a review of digital self-control tools. 2019 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (pp. 1–18), Glasgow, UK. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Mackinnon, A., Jorm, A. F., Christensen, H., Korten, A. E., Jacomb, P. A., & Rodgers, B. (1999). A short form of the positive and negative affect schedule: Evaluation of factorial validity and invariance across demographic variables in a community sample. Personality and Individual Differences, 27(3), 405–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Marciano, L., & Camerini, A. L. (2022). Duration, frequency, and time distortion: Which is the best predictor of problematic smartphone use in adolescents? A trace data study. PLoS ONE, 17(2), e0263815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. McClure, S. M., & Bickel, W. K. (2014). A dual-systems perspective on addiction: Contributions from neuroimaging and cognitive training. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1327, 62–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Meng, L., He, L., Chen, M., & Huang, Y. (2024). The compensation effect of competence frustration and its behavioral manifestations. PsyCh Journal, 13(4), 654–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Naab, T. K., & Schnauber, A. (2016). Habitual initiation of media use and a response-frequency measure for its examination. Media Psychology, 19(1), 126–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Ng, T. W. H., Sorensen, K. L., & Eby, L. T. (2006). Locus of control at work: A meta-analysis. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27(8), 1057–1087. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Osman, A., Wong, J. L., Bagge, C. L., Freedenthal, S., Gutierrez, P. M., & Lozano, G. (2012). The depression anxiety stress scales—21 (DASS-21): Further examination of dimensions, scale reliability, and correlates. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 68(12), 1322–1338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Oulasvirta, A., Rattenbury, T., Ma, L., & Raita, E. (2012). Habits make smartphone use more pervasive. Personal and Ubiquitous Computing, 16(1), 105–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Panova, T., & Lleras, A. (2016). Avoidance or boredom: Negative mental health outcomes associated with use of information and communication technologies depend on users’ motivations. Computers in Human Behavior, 58, 249–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Park, J. S., Barber, R., Kirlik, A., & Karahalios, K. (2019). A slow algorithm improves users’ assessments of the algorithm’s accuracy. Proceedings of the ACM on Human-Computer Interaction, 3, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Park, N., Kim, Y.-C., Shon, H. Y., & Shim, H. (2013). Factors influencing smartphone use and dependency in South Korea. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(4), 1763–1770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Parkes, K. R. (1984). Locus of control, cognitive appraisal, and coping in stressful episodes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(3), 655–668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Parry, D. A., Davidson, B. I., Sewall, C. J. R., Fisher, J. T., Mieczkowski, H., & Quintana, D. S. (2021). A systematic review and meta-analysis of discrepancies between logged and self-reported digital media use. Nature Human Behaviour, 5(11), 1535–1547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Petrosky, M. J., & Birkimer, J. C. (1991). The relationship among locus of control, coping styles, and psychological symptom reporting. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 47(3), 336–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Reid, D. J., & Reid, F. J. M. (2007). Text or talk? social anxiety, loneliness, and divergent preferences for cell phone use. Cyber Psychology & Behavior, 10(3), 424–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Reinecke, L., & Hofmann, W. (2016). Slacking off or winding down? an experience sampling study on the drivers and consequences of media use for recovery versus procrastination. Human Communication Research, 42(3), 441–461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Ridner, S. H. (2004). Psychological distress: Concept analysis. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 45(5), 536–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Roffarello, A. M., & De Russis, L. (2021). Understanding, discovering, and mitigating habitual smartphone use in young adults. ACM Transactions on Interactive Intelligent Systems, 11(2), 1–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Roggeveen, S., van Os, J., Bemelmans, K., van Poll, M., & Lousberg, R. (2018). Investigating associations between changes in mobile phone use and emotions using the experience sampling method: Pilot study. JMIR Formative Research, 2(1), e8499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 80(1), 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Rozgonjuk, D., Elhai, J. D., Täht, K., Vassil, K., Levine, J. C., & Asmundson, G. J. G. (2019). Non-social smartphone use mediates the relationship between intolerance of uncertainty and problematic smartphone use: Evidence from a repeated-measures study. Computers in Human Behavior, 96, 56–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Schneider, F. M., Rieger, D., & Hopp, F. R. (2023). First aid in the pocket: The psychosocial benefits of smartphones in self-threatening situations. Computers in Human Behavior, 141, 107629. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Shulman, E. P., Smith, A. R., Silva, K., Icenogle, G., Duell, N., Chein, J., & Steinberg, L. (2016). The dual systems model: Review, reappraisal, and reaffirmation. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 103–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Snijders, T. A. B., & Bosker, R. (2011). Multilevel analysis: An introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling (pp. 1–368). Sage. [Google Scholar]
  69. Stanisławski, K. (2019). The coping circumplex model: An integrative model of the structure of coping with stress. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Stiglbauer, B. (2017). Under what conditions does job control moderate the relationship between time pressure and employee well-being? Investigating the role of match and personal control beliefs. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 38(5), 730–748. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Tng, G. Y. Q., & Yang, H. (2024). Nuanced relationships between indices of smartphone use and psychological distress: Distinguishing problematic smartphone use, phone checking, and screen time. Behaviour & Information Technology, 43(5), 956–969. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Toh, W. X., Ng, W. Q., Yang, H., & Yang, S. (2021). Disentangling the effects of smartphone screen time, checking frequency, and problematic use on executive function: A structural equation modelling analysis. Current Psychology, 42(5), 4225–4242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. University of New South Wales. (2025, January 9). DASS FAQ. Available online: https://www2.psy.unsw.edu.au/dass/DASSFAQ.htm (accessed on 1 September 2024).
  74. Venkatesh, V., Brown, S. A., Maruping, L. M., & Bala, H. (2008). Predicting different conceptualizations of system use: The competing roles of behavioral intention, facilitating conditions, and behavioral expectation. Mis Quarterly, 32(3), 483–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Verplanken, B., & Orbell, S. (2003). Reflections on past behavior: A self-report index of habit strength. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33(6), 1313–1330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Wadley, G., Smith, W., Koval, P., & Gross, J. J. (2020). Digital emotion regulation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(4), 412–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Wang, Q., Bowling, N. A., & Eschleman, K. J. (2010). A meta-analytic examination of work and general locus of control. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(4), 761–768. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Wei, D., Chan, L.-S., Du, N., Hu, X., & Huang, Y.-T. (2024). Gratification and its associations with problematic internet use: A systematic review and meta-analysis using Use and Gratification theory. Addictive Behaviors, 155, 108044. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Wickord, L. C., & Quaiser-Pohl, C. (2025). The role of flow and media multitasking for problematic smartphone use and the different types of smartphone use. Computers in Human Behavior, 166, 108583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Wickord, L. C., & Quaiser-Pohl, C. M. (2022). Does the type of smartphone usage behavior influence problematic smartphone use and the related stress perception? Behavioral Science, 12(4), 99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Wolfers, L. N., Festl, R., & Utz, S. (2020). Do smartphones and social network sites become more important when experiencing stress? Results from longitudinal data. Computers in Human Behaviors, 109, 106339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Wood, W., & Rünger, D. (2016). Psychology of habit. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 289–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Xu, W., Bai, Y., Wu, Y., & Liu, Y. (2024). “Just using my phone to escape stress today.” Daily perceived stress influences problematic mobile phone use: A daily diary study. Personality and Individual Differences, 228, 112745. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Yang, J., Fu, X., Liao, X., & Li, Y. (2020). Association of problematic smartphone use with poor sleep quality, depression, and anxiety: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research, 284, 112686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Yang, X., Knights, J., Bangieva, V., & Kambhampati, V. (2023). Association between the severity of depressive symptoms and human-smartphone interactions: Longitudinal study. JMIR Formative Research, 7(1), e42935. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Ytre-Arne, B., Syvertsen, T., Moe, H., & Karlsen, F. (2020). Temporal ambivalences in smartphone use: Conflicting flows, conflicting responsibilities. New Media & Society, 22(9), 1715–1732. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Zappala-Piemme, K. E., Sturman, E. D., Brannigan, G. G., & Brannigan, M. J. (2023). Building mental toughness: A middle school intervention to increase grit, locus of control, and academic performance. Psychology in the Schools, 60(8), 2975–2990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. The moderation effect of locus of control in the relationship between psychological distress and daily smartphone use. LOC = locus of control.
Figure 1. The moderation effect of locus of control in the relationship between psychological distress and daily smartphone use. LOC = locus of control.
Behavsci 15 01121 g001
Figure 2. The moderation effect of locus of control in the relationship between psychological distress and daily average session duration. The daily average session duration is calculated by dividing the total smartphone use time by the number of smartphone use sessions. LOC = locus of control. * p < 0.05.
Figure 2. The moderation effect of locus of control in the relationship between psychological distress and daily average session duration. The daily average session duration is calculated by dividing the total smartphone use time by the number of smartphone use sessions. LOC = locus of control. * p < 0.05.
Behavsci 15 01121 g002
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the primary measures at the between-person and within-person levels.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for the primary measures at the between-person and within-person levels.
VariableMSD123456
Between-person
1. Age26.005.02
2. Gender−0.010.51−0.15
3. PD4.653.42−0.130.16
4. LOC10.733.970.12−0.020.18
5. Avg DNES2.640.53−0.33 *0.230.33 *0.24
6. Avg DSUD308.15139.710.39 *0.010.030.21−0.19
7. Avg DSUF184.0287.730.160.18−0.050.19−0.180.81 **
Within-person
1. DNES
2. DSUD 0.05
3. DSUF 0.020.77 **
Observations = 837, N = 37. Gender: 1/2 = female, −1/2 = male; PD (psychological distress) is the average of the depression, anxiety, and stress subscales; Avg indicates each participant’s average level of the variable across the study period; LOC = locus of control; DNES = daily negative emotional state; DSUD = daily smartphone use duration; DSUF = daily smartphone use frequency. * p < 0.05. ** p < 0.01.
Table 2. Multilevel linear model (Model 1) results for daily smartphone use duration and frequency.
Table 2. Multilevel linear model (Model 1) results for daily smartphone use duration and frequency.
DSUDDSUF
Predictorsβtpβtp
wp.DNES0.031.440.1510.000.220.829
bp.DNES−0.13−0.910.365−0.20−1.350.178
PD0.070.520.603−0.03−0.200.840
LOC0.161.120.2630.201.410.158
Age0.291.930.0540.070.450.650
Gender0.070.530.5930.211.520.129
Random Effects
σ2110,678.804194.87
τ0017,398.957355.40
ICC0.620.64
N3737
Observations837837
Marginal R2/
Conditional R2
0.137/0.6720.089/0.669
Gender: 1/2 = female, −1/2 = male; PD = psychological distress; LOC = locus of control; wp.DNES = within-person daily emotional state; bp.DNES = between-person daily emotional state; DSUD = daily smartphone use duration; DSUF = daily smartphone use frequency.
Table 3. Multilevel linear model (Model 2) results for daily smartphone use duration and frequency.
Table 3. Multilevel linear model (Model 2) results for daily smartphone use duration and frequency.
DSUDDSUF
Predictorsβtpβtp
PD0.090.750.4510.010.200.845
LOC0.171.400.1610.222.120.034
wp.DNES0.031.550.1220.010.370.711
bp.DNES−0.04−0.280.783−0.04−0.310.759
Age0.342.330.0200.171.260.209
Gender0.040.290.7720.151.260.209
PD:LOC0.271.790.0740.473.440.001
wp.DNES:LOC−0.02−1.010.312−0.03−1.300.194
Random Effects
σ210,678.224191.17
τ0016,215.995392.40
ICC0.600.56
N3737
Observations837837
Marginal R2/
Conditional R2
0.168/0.6700.216/0.657
Gender: 1/2 = female, −1/2 = male; PD = psychological distress; LOC = locus of control; wp.DNES = within-person daily emotional state; bp.DNES = between-person daily emotional state; DSUD = daily smartphone use duration; DSUF = daily smartphone use frequency.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Chu, Y.; Li, J.; Liu, S.; Liu, Y.; Xu, J. Differential Effects of Short- and Long-Term Negative Affect on Smartphone Usage: The Moderating Role of Locus of Control. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 1121. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15081121

AMA Style

Chu Y, Li J, Liu S, Liu Y, Xu J. Differential Effects of Short- and Long-Term Negative Affect on Smartphone Usage: The Moderating Role of Locus of Control. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(8):1121. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15081121

Chicago/Turabian Style

Chu, Yang, Jiahao Li, Shan Liu, Yanfang Liu, and Jie Xu. 2025. "Differential Effects of Short- and Long-Term Negative Affect on Smartphone Usage: The Moderating Role of Locus of Control" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 8: 1121. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15081121

APA Style

Chu, Y., Li, J., Liu, S., Liu, Y., & Xu, J. (2025). Differential Effects of Short- and Long-Term Negative Affect on Smartphone Usage: The Moderating Role of Locus of Control. Behavioral Sciences, 15(8), 1121. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15081121

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop