Monkey Do, Monkey See? The Effect of Imitation Strategies on Visuospatial Perspective-Taking and Self-Reported Social Cognitive Skills
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Procedure
2.2.1. Training
2.2.2. Computerized Visuospatial Perspective-Taking Task
2.2.3. IRI Questionnaire
2.3. Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Training
3.2. Visuospatial Perspective-Taking Task
3.3. IRI Questionnaire
4. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
1 | Beyond the somehow arbitrary nature of some terms, it may be pointed out that there is an incongruency between the so called “mirror neurons” and the fact that we are more prone to mirror than to imitate in anatomical coordinates. If we were to truly imitate others, i.e., share a common sensory and motor experience, we should rather spontaneously select anatomical reference frame over the visual reference frame used in mirroring. |
References
- Bardi, L., & Brass, M. (2016). The control of shared representations and social cognition. Shared representations (pp. 151–170). Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Bekkering, H., Wohlschlager, A., & Gattis, M. (2000). Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53(1), 153–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brass, M., & Heyes, C. (2005). Imitation: Is cognitive neuroscience solving the correspondence problem? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(10), 489–495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brass, M., Ruby, P., & Spengler, S. (2009). Inhibition of imitative behaviour and social cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 2359–2367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chiavarino, C., Apperly, I. A., & Humphreys, G. W. (2007). Exploring the functional and anatomical bases of mirror-image and anatomical imitation: The role of the frontal lobes. Neuropsychologia, 45(4), 784–795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Coll, M. P., Viding, E., Rütgen, M., Silani, G., Lamm, C., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2017). Are we really measuring empathy? Proposal for a new measurement framework. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 83, 132–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Coutté, A., Margas, N., & Heurley, L. P. (2024). Mirror game as a tool to influence interpersonal spontaneous behavior after performance. Cognitive Processing, 25(4), 655–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cracco, E., Bardi, L., Desmet, C., Genschow, O., Rigoni, D., De Coster, L., Radkova, I., Deschrijver, E., & Brass, M. (2018). Automatic imitation: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 144(5), 453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(1), 113–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Coster, L., Verschuere, B., Goubert, L., Tsakiris, M., & Brass, M. (2013). I suffer more from your pain when you act like me: Being imitated enhances affective responses to seeing someone else in pain. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 13(3), 519–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Coster, L., Wiersema, J. R., Deschrijver, E., & Brass, M. (2017). The effect of being imitated on empathy for pain in adults with high-functioning autism: Disturbed self–other distinction leads to altered empathic responding. Autism, 22(6), 712–727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Guzman, M., Bird, G., Banissy, M. J., & Catmur, C. (2016). Self–other control processes in social cognition: From imitation to empathy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1686), 20150079. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Vignemont, F. (2014). Shared body representations and the ‘Whose’ system. Neuropsychologia, 55, 128–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Epley, N., & Caruso, E. M. (2009). Perspective taking: Misstepping into others’ shoes. In Handbook of imagination and mental simulation (pp. 295–309). Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Erle, T. M., & Topolinski, S. (2017). The grounded nature of psychological perspective-taking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112, 683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fox, N. A., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Yoo, K. H., Bowman, L. C., Cannon, E. N., Vanderwert, R. E., Ferrari, P. F., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2016). Assessing human mirror activity with EEG mu rhythm: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 142(3), 291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Franz, E. A., Ford, S., & Werner, S. (2007). Brain and cognitive processes of imitation in bimanual situations: Making inferences about mirror neuron systems. Brain Research, 1145, 138–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. I. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mindreading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 493–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gallotti, M., & Frith, C. D. (2013). Social cognition in the we-mode. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(4), 160–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jeannerod, M., & Pacherie, E. (2004). Agency, simulation and self-identification. Mind & Language, 19(2), 113–146. [Google Scholar]
- Kessler, K., & Rutherford, H. (2010). The two forms of visuo-spatial perspective taking are differently embodied and subserve different spatial prepositions. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kessler, K., & Thomson, L. A. (2010). The embodied nature of spatial perspective taking: Embodied transformation versus sensorimotor interference. Cognition, 114, 72–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Keysar, B. (1994). The illusory transparency of intention: Linguistic perspective taking in text. Cognitive Psychology, 26, 165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Keysers, C., & Gazzola, V. (2009). Expanding the mirror: Vicarious activity for actions, emotions, and sensations. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 19(6), 666–671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Leys, C., Ley, C., Klein, O., Bernard, P., & Licata, L. (2013). Detecting outliers: Do not use standard deviation around the mean, use absolute deviation around the median. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(4), 764–766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lhermitte, F., Pillon, B., & Serdaru, M. (1986). Human autonomy and the frontal lobes. Part I: Imitation and utilization behavior: A neuropsychological study of 75 patients. Annals of Neurology: Official Journal of the American Neurological Association and the Child Neurology Society, 19(4), 326–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McGarry, L. M., & Russo, F. A. (2011). Mirroring in dance/movement therapy: Potential mechanisms behind empathy enhancement. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 38(3), 178–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Michelon, P., & Zacks, J. M. (2006). Two kinds of visual perspective taking. Perception & Psychophysics, 68(2), 327–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Muto, H., Matsushita, S., & Morikawa, K. (2018). Spatial perspective taking mediated by whole-body motor simulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44, 337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Niedenthal, P. M. (2007). Embodying emotion. Science, 316(5827), 1002–1005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Quesque, F., Apperly, I., Baillargeon, R., Baron-Cohen, S., Becchio, C., Bekkering, H., Bernstein, D., Bertoux, M., Bird, G., Bukowski, H., Burgmer, P., Carruthers, P., Catmur, C., Dziobek, I., Epley, N., Erle, T. M., Frith, C., Frith, U., Galang, C. M., … Gallese, V. (2024). Defining key concepts for mental state attribution. Communications Psychology, 2(1), 29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Quesque, F., & Brass, M. (2019). The role of the temporoparietal junction in self-other distinction. Brain Topography, 32(6), 943–955. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Quesque, F., Chabanat, E., & Rossetti, Y. (2018). Taking the point of view of the blind: Spontaneous level-2 perspective-taking in irrelevant conditions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 79, 356–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Quesque, F., & Coello, Y. (2015). Perceiving what you intend to do from what you do: Evidence for embodiment in social interactions. Socioaffective Neuroscience & Psychology, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Quesque, F., Foncelle, A., Barat, E., Chabanat, E., Rossetti, Y., & Van Der Henst, J. B. (2021). Sympathy for the Underdog: People are inclined to adopt the emotional perspective of powerless (versus powerful) others. Cognition and Emotion, 35, 902–917. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Quesque, F., & Rossetti, Y. (2020). What do theory of mind tasks actually measure? Theory and practice. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(2), 384–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Reniers, R. L., Corcoran, R., Drake, R., Shryane, N. M., & Völlm, B. A. (2011). The QCAE: A questionnaire of cognitive and affective empathy. Journal of Personality Assessment, 93(1), 84–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Samuel, S., Erle, T. M., Kirsch, L. P., Surtees, A., Apperly, I., Bukowski, H., Auvray, M., Catmur, C., Kessler, K., & Quesque, F. (2024). Three key questions to move towards a theoretical framework of visuospatial perspective taking. Cognition, 247, 105787. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Santiesteban, I., White, S., Cook, J., Gilbert, S. J., Heyes, C., & Bird, G. (2012). Training social cognition: From imitation to theory of mind. Cognition, 122, 228–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Saxe, R. (2005). Against simulation: The argument from error. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(4), 174–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shield, A., & Meier, R. P. (2018). Learning an embodied visual language: Four imitation strategies available to sign learners. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 811. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Simon, J. R., & Rudell, A. P. (1967). Auditory SR compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51(3), 300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Simpson, A. J., & Todd, A. R. (2017). Intergroup visual perspective-taking: Shared group membership impairs self-perspective inhibition but may facilitate perspective calculation. Cognition, 166, 371–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sowden, S., & Catmur, C. (2015). The role of the right temporoparietal junction in the control of imitation. Cereb Cortex, 25(4), 1107–1113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Todd, A. R., Hanko, K., Galinsky, A. D., & Mussweiler, T. (2011). When focusing on differences leads to similar perspectives. Psychological Science, 22, 134–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tsakiris, M. (2017). The multisensory basis of the self: From body to identity to others. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 70(4), 597–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wapner, S., & Cirillo, L. (1968). Imitation of a model’s hand movements: Age changes in transposition of left-right relations. Child Development, 39, 887–894. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- World Medical Association. (2013). Declaration of Helsinki: Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA, 310, 2191–2194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Ducret, M.; Chabanat, E.; Kambara, A.; Rossetti, Y.; Quesque, F. Monkey Do, Monkey See? The Effect of Imitation Strategies on Visuospatial Perspective-Taking and Self-Reported Social Cognitive Skills. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 1112. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15081112
Ducret M, Chabanat E, Kambara A, Rossetti Y, Quesque F. Monkey Do, Monkey See? The Effect of Imitation Strategies on Visuospatial Perspective-Taking and Self-Reported Social Cognitive Skills. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(8):1112. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15081112
Chicago/Turabian StyleDucret, Marion, Eric Chabanat, Ayumi Kambara, Yves Rossetti, and Francois Quesque. 2025. "Monkey Do, Monkey See? The Effect of Imitation Strategies on Visuospatial Perspective-Taking and Self-Reported Social Cognitive Skills" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 8: 1112. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15081112
APA StyleDucret, M., Chabanat, E., Kambara, A., Rossetti, Y., & Quesque, F. (2025). Monkey Do, Monkey See? The Effect of Imitation Strategies on Visuospatial Perspective-Taking and Self-Reported Social Cognitive Skills. Behavioral Sciences, 15(8), 1112. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15081112