Shame Withdraws, Guilt Corrects: Distinguishing Shame and Guilt in Goal Pursuit—An Experimental Study
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Theoretical Background
2.1. Goal Progress
2.2. Shame Versus Guilt
3. Study
3.1. Objective
3.2. Research Design
3.3. Procedure
“Imagine that you have a job that pays USD 4000 monthly after taxes (i.e., take-home income). Your monthly non-discretionary expenses, like rent, bills, groceries, transportation, and insurance, add up to USD 2000. This leaves USD 2000 each month to spend on discretionary items. However, a few months ago, you decided to start saving money for the future. Taking a tip from a popular money management book, you decided to set a personal savings goal for yourself. In particular, you decided that of the USD 2000 that remains after your non-discretionary expenditures, you would like to save USD 700. In other words, you would like to spend no more than USD 1300 on discretionary items. However, you have not been so successful in achieving that goal over the past months. Now, imagine that it is almost the end of November, and you are checking your expenses, and you realize that after paying all your necessary expenses, you have only USD 500 left from your paycheck for this month, which means that again you exceeded your spending limit.”
“Now complete the sentence stem “If only I were (were not) …” Complete the stem twice. Both times, describe how, if you were or were not a particular TYPE OF PERSON (e.g., with particular personality traits, emotional styles, intelligence), you wouldn’t have gotten into this situation in the first place. Thus, your sentence should take the form: “If only I were (were not) [a particular type of person].”
“Now, a friend calls you the next day to tell you that a local theater is selling a special package for USD 150. The package includes a music performance by an artist that you really like. The package also includes dinner and drinks. Your friend plans to go, but at the back of your mind, you are thinking about your expenses this month. You promise your friend that you will call back soon. You have a few minutes to think about this and then make a decision.”
4. Results
4.1. Sample Characteristics
4.2. Moderation Analysis
5. Discussion
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Bagozzi, R. P., & Dholakia, U. (1999). Goal setting and goal striving in consumer behavior. Journal of Marketing, 63(1), 19–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bandura, A., & Simon, K. M. (1977). The role of proximal intentions in self-regulation of refractory behavior. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 1, 177–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baumeister, R. F., & Heatherton, T. F. (1996). Self-regulation failure: An overview. Psychological Inquiry, 7(1), 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2016). Strength model of self-regulation as limited resource: Assessment, controversies, update. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 54, 67–127. [Google Scholar]
- Billore, S., Anisimova, T., & Vrontis, D. (2023). Self-regulation and goal-directed behavior: A systematic literature review, public policy recommendations, and research agenda. Journal of Business Research, 156, 154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review, 97(1), 19–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Choi, H. (2024). Integrating guilt and shame into the self-concept: The influence of future opportunities. Behavioral Science, 14(6), 472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duckworth, A. L., White, R. E., Matteucci, A. J., Shearer, A., & Gross, J. J. (2016). A stitch in time: Strategic self-control in high school and college students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108(3), 329–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Duhachek, A., Agrawal, N., & Han, D. (2012). Guilt versus shame: Coping, fluency, and framing in the effectiveness of responsible drinking messages. Journal of Marketing Research, 49(6), 928–941. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elliott, E. S., & Dweck, C. S. (1988). Goals: An approach to motivation and achievement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(1), 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fishbach, A., & Dhar, R. (2005). Goals as excuses or guides: The liberating effect of perceived goal progress on choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 32(3), 370–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Frijda, N. H. (1987). Emotion, cognitive structure, and action tendency. Cognition & Emotion, 1, 115–143. [Google Scholar]
- Fujita, K. (2011). On conceptualizing self-control as more than the effortful inhibition of impulses. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 15(4), 352–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Giner-Sorolla, R. (2001). Guilty pleasures and grim necessities: Affective attitudes in dilemmas of self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(2), 206–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hayes, A. F. (2012). Process. Available online: http://www.afhayes.com (accessed on 1 October 2012).
- Heath, C., Larrick, R. P., & Wu, G. (1999). Goals as reference points. Cognitive Psychology, 38, 79–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Herrald, M. M., & Tomaka, J. (2002). Patterns of emotion-specific appraisal, coping, and cardiovascular reactivity during an ongoing emotional episode. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(2), 434–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Higgins, T. E. (2002). How self-regulation creates distinct values: The case of promotion and prevention decision making. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12, 177–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hofmann, W., Friese, M., & Strack, F. (2009). Impulse and self-control from a dual-systems perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4(2), 162–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Höpfner, J., & Keith, N. (2021). Goal missed, self hit: Goal-setting, goal-failure, and their affective, motivational, and behavioral consequences. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 704790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ilies, R., & Judge, T. A. (2005). Goal regulation across time: The effect of feedback and affect. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(3), 453–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jia, L. (2019). Adaptive indulgence in self-control: A multilevel cost–benefit analysis. International Journal for the Advancement of Psychological Theory, 30(3), 140–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jones, N. P., Papadakis, A. A., Orr, C. A., & Strauman, T. J. (2013). Cognitive processes in response to goal failure: A study of ruminative thought and its affective consequences. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 32, 482–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Khan, U., Dhar, R., & Fishbach, A. (2009). Guilt as motivation: Role of guilt in choice justification. Advances in Consumer Research, 26, 27–28. [Google Scholar]
- Khan, U., Dhar, R., & Wertenbroch, K. (2005). A behavioral decision-theoretic perspective on hedonic and utilitarian choice. In S. Ratneshwar, & D. G. Mich (Eds.), Inside consumption: Frontiers of research on consumer motives, goals, and desires (pp. 144–165). Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Lawrence, J. W., Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2002). Velocity toward goal attainment in immediate experience as a determinant of affect. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 788–802. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Leach, C. W., & Cidam, A. (2015). When is shame linked to constructive approach orientation? A meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(6), 983–1002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lebeau, J. C., Gatten, H., Perry, I., Wang, Y., Sung, S., & Tenenbaum, G. (2018). Is failing the key to success? A randomized experiment investigating goal attainment effects on cognitions, emotions, and subsequent performance. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 38, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lewis, H. B. (1971). Shame and guilt in neurosis. International Universities Press. [Google Scholar]
- Lickel, B., Kushlev, K., Savalei, V., Matta, S., & Schmader, T. (2014). Shame and the motivation to change the self. Emotion, 14(6), 1049. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Loewenstein, G. (1996). Out of control: Visceral influences on behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 65(3), 272–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meade, L. B., Semenchuk, B. N., & Strachan, S. M. (2019). Is there positive in the negative? Understanding the role of guilt and shame in physical activity self-regulation. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 18, 502–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moors, A., Van de Cruys, S., & Pourtois, G. (2021). Comparison of the determinants for positive and negative affect proposed by appraisal theories, goal-directed theories, and predictive processing theories. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 39(8), 147–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mróz, J., & Sornat, W. (2022). Shame-and guilt-proneness and self-compassion as predictors of self-forgiveness. Journal of Beliefs & Values, 44(2), 188–202. [Google Scholar]
- Niedenthal, P. M., Tangney, J. P., & Gavanski, I. (1994). “If only I weren’t” versus “If only I hadn’t”: Distinguishing shame and guilt in counterfactual thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(4), 585–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pervin, L. A. (1989). Goal concepts: Themes, issues, and questions. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Goal concepts in personality and social psychology (pp. 473–479). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
- Saintives, C., & Lunardo, R. (2016). How guilt affects consumption intention: The role of rumination, emotional support and shame. Journal of Consumer Marketing, 33(1), 41–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scherer, K. R. (1984). Emotion as a multicomponent process: A model and some cross-cultural data. In P. Shaver (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 37–63). Sage. [Google Scholar]
- Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1996). Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In E. T. Higgins, & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 433–465). Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Shen, L. (2018). The evolution of shame and guilt. PLoS ONE, 13(5), e0199448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shi, C., Ren, Z., Zhao, C., Zhang, T., & Chan, S. H. W. (2021). Shame, guilt, and posttraumatic stress symptoms: A three-level meta-analysis. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 82, 102443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shi, Y., Sears, L. E., Coberley, C. R., & Pope, J. E. (2013). The association between modifiable well-being risks and productivity: A longitudinal study in pooled employer sample. Journal of Occupational and Environmental Medicine, 55, 353–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1987). Patterns of appraisal and emotion related to taking an exam. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 475–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Soman, D., & Cheema, A. (2004). When goals are counterproductive: The effects of violation of a behavioral goal on subsequent performance. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(1), 52–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stichter, M. (2020). Learning from failure: Shame and emotion regulation in virtue as skill. Ethical Theory and Moral Practice, 23, 341–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tangney, J. P. (1990). Assessing individual differences in proneness to shame and guilt: Development of the self-conscious affect and attribution inventory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(1), 102–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tangney, J. P. (1992). Situational determinants of shame and guilt in young adulthood. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 199–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tangney, J. P. (1995). Recent advances in the empirical study of shame and guilt. American Behavioral Scientist, 38, 1132–1145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58(1), 345–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tangney, J. P., Wagner, P., & Gramzow, R. (1992). Proneness to shame, proneness to guilt, and psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 101(3), 469–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Welsh, D. T., Baer, M. D., & Sessions, H. (2020). Hot pursuit: The affective consequences of organization-set versus self-set goals for emotional exhaustion and citizenship behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 105, 166–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wiener, B. (1980). May I borrow your class notes? An attributional analysis of judgments of help giving in an achievement-related context. Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 676–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zemack-Rugar, Y., Bettman, J. R., & Fitzsimons, G. (2007). The effects of nonconsciously priming emotion concepts on behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(6), 927–939. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Variable/Condition | Results | Interpretation |
---|---|---|
Gender Distribution | 68% female, 32% male | Sample skewed female, no significant effect on choice (p = 0.2) |
Age Range (Median) | 18–46 years (Median = 20) | Young sample, no significant effect on choice (p = 0.4) |
Income Range | $3000–$800,000 | Wide income range, no significant effect on choice (p = 0.7) |
Prime × Shame Proneness | β = −8.3744, p = 0.05 | Shame proneness significantly moderates the effect of emotion prime on choice |
Prime × Shame Proneness × Guilt Proneness | χ2 = 3.5376, p = 0.06 | Significant three-way interaction—suggests a joint role of shame and guilt in choice |
Low Shame/High Guilt | Predicted Probability = 0.69, p = 0.03 | High guilt and low shame promote goal-consistent behavior |
High Shame/Low Guilt | Predicted Probability = 0.23, p = 0.02 | High shame and low guilt promote goal-inconsistent behavior |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Semaan, R.W. Shame Withdraws, Guilt Corrects: Distinguishing Shame and Guilt in Goal Pursuit—An Experimental Study. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 725. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15060725
Semaan RW. Shame Withdraws, Guilt Corrects: Distinguishing Shame and Guilt in Goal Pursuit—An Experimental Study. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(6):725. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15060725
Chicago/Turabian StyleSemaan, Rania W. 2025. "Shame Withdraws, Guilt Corrects: Distinguishing Shame and Guilt in Goal Pursuit—An Experimental Study" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 6: 725. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15060725
APA StyleSemaan, R. W. (2025). Shame Withdraws, Guilt Corrects: Distinguishing Shame and Guilt in Goal Pursuit—An Experimental Study. Behavioral Sciences, 15(6), 725. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15060725