A Causal Model for Surveys of Exploratory Listening and Music Appreciation
Abstract
1. Introduction
1.1. Frameworks for Exploring Music
1.2. Causation as Listening
1.3. Quantification of Explorations
2. Modeling Approach
2.1. The Causal Theory
2.2. The Conceptual Causal Model (DAG)
2.3. The Statistical Causal Model (SEM)
model_1 <- ‘ Evaluation =~ Q47 + Q48 + Q49 Immersion =~ Q33 + Q35 + Q31 Embodiment =~ Q37 + Q39 + Q41 Emotion =~ Q43 Kind_of_experience =~ Immersion + Embodiment + Emotion Kind_of_experience ~ Quality Evaluation ~ Kind_of_experience Q1 ~ Evaluation + Q2 ### Q1 = Global_appreciation’ |
3. Selection of Questions
- Global_appreciation is estimated using a single question (Q1), scored on a scale from 1 to 10. This question reflects how eager the listener would be to listen to the music again.
- Quality is measured using six yes/no questions (Q6, Q8, Q10, Q12, Q14, Q16, detailed in Appendix A) that explore various aspects of satisfaction with musical qualities. It is then represented by a single score, which is a kind of weighted combination of the binary responses. The score is calculated as follows: for each answered question, “1” is assigned, while unanswered questions are marked with “0”. The weighted sum, called Quality, is then computed using the following formula:
- Evaluation is based on three questions (Q47, Q48, Q49), assessing the personal value of the experience described in Kind_of_experience.
- Kind_of_experience is subdivided into three components: Immersion (Q31, Q33, Q35), Embodiment (Q37, Q39, Q41) and Emotion (Q45). These questions explore the type of experience generated by the music.
- Q2 stands as a separate question about connection (“With this piece of music I feel no connection”).
4. Survey and Data
4.1. Context of the Survey
4.2. Questionnaire
4.3. Participants
4.4. Distribution of Global_Appreciation
5. Correlation Analysis
6. Statistical Analysis Using SEM
semfit1 <- lavaan::sem(model_1, data = Data, group = “Q57”, meanstructure = TRUE) |
6.1. Measurement Invariance
Base Model: semfit1 <- lavaan::sem(model_1, data = Data, group = “Q57”, meanstructure = TRUE) Nested Model 1: semfit2<- lavaan::sem(model_1, data = Data, group = “Q57”, meanstructure = TRUE, group.equal = c(“loadings”)) Nested Model 2: semfit3<- lavaan::sem(model_1, data = Data, group = “Q57”, meanstructure = TRUE, group.equal = c(“loadings”, “intercepts”)) |
####################### Model Fit Indices ########################### chisq df pvalue rmsea cfi tli srmr aic bic semfit1 902.093† 122 0.000 0.089 0.849† 0.810 0.083† 40633.042† 41042.366 semfit2 956.857 130 0.000 0.089† 0.840 0.811† 0.085 40671.807 41038.044† semfit3 1125.185 136 0.000 0.095 0.809 0.783 0.093 40828.134 41162.057 |
- ■
- Chi-square: All models show significant Chi-square values (p < 0.05), indicating that the models do not perfectly fit the data. This is common in large sample sizes, where even minor deviations from the model can lead to significant Chi-square results.
- ■
- RMSEA: All models have RMSEA values between 0.089 and 0.095, which fall within an acceptable range (typically below 0.08 for a good fit), suggesting a reasonable fit overall.
- ■
- CFI and TLI: CFI values range from 0.81 to 0.85, and TLI values range from 0.79 to 0.81, indicating a reasonable fit.
- ■
- SRMR: The model semfit1 has the lowest SRMR (0.083), indicating the best fit in terms of standardized root mean square residual.
- ■
- AIC and BIC: The model semfit1 has the lowest AIC value, suggesting it is the most parsimonious model among the three.
6.2. Analysis of Parameters
summary(semfit1, standardized = TRUE, rsquare = TRUE, ci = T) |
6.3. Analysis of Latent Variables
Immersion: | |
Liking: Std.all = 0.870 | |
Disliking: Std.all = 0.940 | |
Embodiment: | |
Liking: Std.all = 0.393 | |
Disliking: Std.all = 0.694 | |
Emotion: | |
Liking: Std.all = 0.589 | |
Disliking: Std.all = 0.299 |
6.4. Regression Analysis
Evaluation ~ Kind_of_experience | |
Liking: Std.all = 0.597 | |
Disliking: Std.all = 0.857 | |
Kind_of_experience ~ Quality | |
Liking: Std.all = 0.141 | |
Disliking: Std.all = 0.453 | |
Q1 ~ Evaluation | |
Liking: Std.all = 0.497 | |
Disliking: Std.all = 0.523 | |
Q1 ~ Q2 | |
Liking: Std.all = −0.010 (not significant) | |
Disliking: Std.all = −0.098 (significant) |
6.5. Testing Against Other Models
model_2 <- ‘ Evaluation =~ Q47 + Q48 + Q49 Immersion =~ Q33 + Q35 + Q31 Embodiment =~ Q37 + Q39 + Q41 Emotion =~ Q43 Kind_of_experience =~ Immersion + Embodiment + Emotion Evaluation ~ Kind_of_experience + Quality Q1 ~ Evaluation + Q2’ |
####################### Model Fit Indices ########################### chisq df pvalue rmsea cfi tli srmr aic bic semfit1_model_2 988.969† 122 0.000 0.094 0.832† 0.788 0.098† 40579.822† 40988.863 semfit2_model_2 1042.164 130 0.000 0.093† 0.823 0.791† 0.101 40617.017 40983.002† semfit3_model_2 1209.020 136 0.000 0.099 0.792 0.765 0.105 40771.873 41105.564 |
7. Discussion
7.1. Statistical Modeling
7.2. Modeling Exploratory Listening Behavior
7.3. Experiences
7.4. The Quality/Experience Problem
7.5. Limitations
8. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
(#tab:chapListener_datasetQ) Questionnaire with labels, question summaries and questions. L5 means Likert scale from 1 to 5 | ||
Label | Question Summary | Question |
Title | Title Open | Give the title of the musical piece |
Composer | Composer Open | Give the name of the composer |
Q1 | Global_appreciation 1 to 10 | Give your global appreciation score |
Q2 | Connection L5 | |With this piece of music I feel no connection |
Q3 | Arousal L5 | |In this piece I hear density, energy, activity, excitement |
Q4 | Valence L5 | |In this piece I hear joy, optimism, positive emotions |
Q6 | Loving Yes(1)/No(2) | |Does the piece of music have certain core qualities for you? These are characteristics that you immediately think of when you consider the piece of music. They give you a euphoric feeling, they touch you. They are also called wow-effects. Because of these core qualities, you love the piece of music. |
Q8 | Liking Yes/No | |Question 2: Besides core qualities, the piece of music may have distinctive elements for you that are certainly worth listening to. They give you a good feeling and often create a ‘click’ with the piece of music. Do you recognize these elements? |
Q10 | Indifferent Yes/No | |Question 3: Does the piece of music have certain distinctive elements for you that you don’t care for? These may be found, for example, in transitional passages. They have no positive, but also no negative effect on your musical experience. |
Q12 | Disliking Yes/No | |Question 4: Even if the piece is your first KLARA top 100 piece, it may still have certain distinctive elements that you don’t like. Do you recognize this? |
Q14 | Disturbing Yes/No | |Question 5: Does the piece of music have certain distinctive elements that bother you or even annoy you? |
Q16 | Missing Yes/No | |Question 6: It may be that the piece of music lacks certain elements that are important to you. Is this the case? |
Q18 | Sphere L5 | |Statement 1: The atmosphere I hear in the music influences my choice. |
Q20 | Structure L5 | |Statement 2: The structure/composition of the piece of music influences my choice. |
Q22 | Melody L5 | |Statement 3: The melody of this piece of music influences my choice. |
Q24 | Harmony L5 | |Statement 4: The harmony/ensemble singing in this music influences my choice. |
Q26 | Time L5 | |Statement 5: The rhythm and tempo of this piece of music influences my choice. |
Q28 | Texture Categorial | |Does the piece of music have vocals or is it instrumental? |
Q29 | Texture L5 | |The lyrics influence my choice. |
Q31 | Attention L5 | |Statement 1: Generally, this piece of music demands my full attention (for example, ‘forgetting time’, ‘full concentration on the music (and the lyrics)’, ‘completely shut off from the surroundings’). |
Q33 | Absorbtion L5 | |Statement 2: Generally, it feels as if this piece of music and I are one (for example, ‘being completely captivated by the music’, ‘feeling one with the music’, ‘feeling after listening as if I have been on a journey’). |
Q35 | Engagement L5 | |Statement 3: Generally, I feel completely engaged with this piece of music (for example, sympathy, empathy, and identification with the performer(s), composer, characters in the music, meaning of the music). |
Q37 | Moving L5 | |Statement 4: Generally, I move automatically with this piece of music (for example, tapping to the beat, swaying, conducting along). |
Q39 | Participation L5 | |Statement 5: Generally, I sing, whistle, or hum along with this piece of music. |
Q41 | Physical L5 | |Statement 6: Generally, I experience physical sensations with this piece of music (for example, goosebumps, shivers down the spine, lump in the throat, tears of emotion, heart beats faster). |
Q43 | Emotions L5 | |Statement 7: Generally, I experience certain emotions with this piece of music (for example, nostalgia, energy, happiness, sadness, inspiration). |
Q45 | Mood L5 | |Statement 8: Generally, my mood changes when listening to this piece of music. |
Q47 | Enjoying L5 | |I can intensely enjoy this piece of music. |
Q48 | Touching L5 | |This piece of music affects me in a positive way. |
Q49 | Annoying L5 | |I can get extremely annoyed by this piece of music. |
Q50 | Listening Categorial | |How long do you typically listen to music in a day? |
Q51 | Playing Categorial | |How long do you typically play music yourself in a day? |
Q52 | Level Categorial | |How would you rate your musical ability? |
Q53 | Age Categorial | |What is your age? |
Q54 | Gender Categorial | |You are |
Appendix B
Group 1 [Liking]: Latent Variables: Estimate Std.Err z-value P(>|z|) ci.lower ci.upper Std.lv Std.all Evaluation =~ Q47 1.000 1.000 1.000 0.315 0.674 Q48 0.848 0.114 7.422 0.000 0.624 1.072 0.267 0.383 Q49 −0.368 0.054 -6.756 0.000 −0.474 −0.261 −0.116 −0.337 Immersion =~ Q33 1.000 1.000 1.000 0.671 0.783 Q35 0.702 0.050 13.968 0.000 0.604 0.801 0.471 0.600 Q31 0.856 0.058 14.747 0.000 0.742 0.969 0.574 0.649 Embodiment =~ Q37 1.000 1.000 1.000 0.896 0.727 Q39 0.957 0.104 9.205 0.000 0.753 1.161 0.857 0.672 Q41 0.408 0.052 7.860 0.000 0.306 0.509 0.365 0.381 Emotion =~ Q43 1.000 1.000 1.000 0.681 1.000 Kind_of_experience =~ Immersion 1.000 1.000 1.000 0.870 0.870 Embodiment 0.602 0.094 6.412 0.000 0.418 0.786 0.393 0.393 Emotion 0.688 0.075 9.156 0.000 0.540 0.835 0.589 0.589 Regressions: Estimate Std.Err z-value P(>|z|) ci.lower ci.upper Std.lv Std.all Evaluation ~ Kind_of_exprnc 0.322 0.040 8.028 0.000 0.244 0.401 0.597 0.597 Kind_of_experience ~ Quality 0.089 0.027 3.300 0.001 0.036 0.142 0.152 0.141 Q1 ~ Evaluation 1.382 0.160 8.630 0.000 1.068 1.696 0.435 0.497 Q2 −0.015 0.046 −0.318 0.750 −0.105 0.075 −0.015 −0.010 Group 2 [Disliking]: Latent Variables: Estimate Std.Err z-value P(>|z|) ci.lower ci.upper Std.lv Std.all Evaluation =~ Q47 1.000 1.000 1.000 0.568 0.787 Q48 0.988 0.049 20.044 0.000 0.892 1.085 0.561 0.761 Q49 −0.742 0.067 −11.033 0.000 −0.874 −0.610 −0.421 −0.420 Immersion =~ Q33 1.000 1.000 1.000 0.617 0.837 Q35 1.027 0.040 25.803 0.000 0.949 1.105 0.633 0.854 Q31 0.775 0.059 13.041 0.000 0.659 0.892 0.478 0.467 Embodiment =~ Q37 1.000 1.000 1.000 0.766 0.901 Q39 0.904 0.044 20.712 0.000 0.818 0.989 0.692 0.800 Q41 0.413 0.059 6.989 0.000 0.298 0.529 0.317 0.261 Emotion =~ Q43 1.000 1.000 1.000 1.287 1.000 Kind_of_experience =~ Immersion 1.000 1.000 1.000 0.940 0.940 Embodiment 0.917 0.058 15.852 0.000 0.804 1.031 0.694 0.694 Emotion 0.664 0.086 7.766 0.000 0.497 0.832 0.299 0.299 Regressions: Estimate Std.Err z-value P(>|z|) ci.lower ci.upper Std.lv Std.all Evaluation ~ Kind_of_exprnc 0.840 0.051 16.573 0.000 0.740 0.939 0.857 0.857 Kind_of_experience ~ Quality 0.205 0.017 12.108 0.000 0.172 0.238 0.354 0.453 Q1 ~ Evaluation 1.517 0.109 13.914 0.000 1.303 1.731 0.861 0.523 Q2 −0.142 0.044 -3.190 0.001 −0.229 −0.055 −0.142 −0.098 |
1 | See Pearl et al. (2016). The causal logic of a DAG can be tested using the tool Daggity. It involves evaluating whether the assumed causal structure is consistent, that is, whether certain sets of variables are conditionally independent given other sets of variables. Our DAG is safe. No adjustment is necessary to estimate the total effect of Kind_of_Experience on Global_Appreciation, meaning that there are no confounding paths between the variable Kind_of_Experience and the outcome variable Global_Appreciation. |
References
- Alaei, R., Rule, N. O., & MacDonald, G. (2022). Individuals’ favorite songs’ lyrics reflect their attachment style. Personal Relationships, 29(4), 778–794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bader, R. (Ed.). (2018). Springer handbook of systematic musicology. Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Berlyne, D. E. (1971). Aesthetics and psychobiology. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [Google Scholar]
- Berridge, K. C., & Kringelbach, M. L. (2015). Pleasure systems in the brain. Neuron, 86(3), 646–664. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. Wiley. [Google Scholar]
- Brattico, E., & Pearce, M. (2013). The neuroaesthetics of music. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 7(1), 48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Broeckx, J. L. (1981). Muziek, ratio en affect: Over de wisselwerking van rationeel denken en affectief beleven bij voortbrengst en ontvangst van muziek. Metropolis. [Google Scholar]
- Bunge, M. (2017). Causality and modern science. Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Clemente, A., Pearce, M. T., & Nadal, M. (2022). Musical aesthetic sensitivity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 16(1), 58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Leeuw, T. (2000). Bedrijfskundig management: Primair proces, strategie en organisatie. Van Gorcum. [Google Scholar]
- Eerola, T., & Vuoskoski, J. K. (2012). A review of music and emotion studies: Approaches, emotion models, and stimuli. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 30(3), 307–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fricke, K. R., Greenberg, D. M., Rentfrow, P. J., & Herzberg, P. Y. (2021). Measuring musical preferences from listening behavior: Data from one million people and 200,000 songs. Psychology of Music, 49(3), 371–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gebauer, L., Kringelbach, M. L., & Vuust, P. (2012). Ever-changing cycles of musical pleasure: The role of dopamine and anticipation. Psychomusicology: Music, Mind, and Brain, 22(2), 152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hatten, R. S. (2003). Thematic gestures, topics, and tropes. In E. Tarasti (Ed.), Musical semiotics revisited (pp. 80–91). Hakapaino. [Google Scholar]
- Huron, D. (2006). Sweet anticipation: Music and the psychology of expectation. MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kermanshachi, S., Nipa, T. J., & Nadiri, H. (2022). Service quality assessment and enhancement using Kano model. PLoS ONE, 17(2), e0264423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Knowlton, L. W., & Phillips, C. C. (2012). The logic model guidebook: Better strategies for great results. Sage. [Google Scholar]
- Kunst, J. (1978). Making sense in music: An enquiry into the formal pragmatics of art. Communication & Cognition. [Google Scholar]
- Kunst, J., & Van den Bergh, H. (1984). The analysis of musical meaning. A theory and an experiment. Interface (Journal of New Music Research), 13(2), 75–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leman, M. (2007). Embodied music cognition and mediation technology. MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Leman, M. (2016). The expressive moment: How interaction (with music) shapes human empowerment. MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Leman, M., & Schneider, A. (1997). Origin and nature of cognitive and systematic musicology: An introduction. In M. Leman (Ed.), Music, gestalt, and computing: Studies in cognitive and systematic musicology (pp. 13–29). Springer-Verlag. [Google Scholar]
- Levitin, D. J. (2011). This is your brain on music: Understanding a human obsession. Atlantic Books Ltd. [Google Scholar]
- Meyer, L. B. (1956). Emotion and meaning in music. University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Mikulić, J., & Prebežac, D. (2011). A critical review of techniques for classifying quality attributes in the Kano model. Managing Service Quality: An International Journal, 21(1), 46–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Panksepp, J., & Bernatzky, G. (2002). Emotional sounds and the brain: The neuro-affective foundations of musical appreciation. Behavioural Processes, 60(2), 133–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pearl, J., Glymour, M., & Jewell, N. P. (2016). Causal inference in statistics: A primer. John Wiley & Sons Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Reybrouck, M., Podlipniak, P., & Welch, D. (2024). Music listening as exploratory behavior: From dispositional reactions to epistemic interactions with the sonic world. Behavioral Sciences, 14(9), 825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Révész, G. (1944). Inleiding tot de muziekpsychologie. N.V. Noord-Hollandsche Uitgevers Maatschappij. [Google Scholar]
- Robbins, T. W., & Clark, L. (2015). Behavioral addictions. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 30, 66–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rosseel, Y. (2012). Lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(2), 1–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1161–1178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sachs, M. E., Damasio, A., & Habibi, A. (2015). The pleasures of sad music: A systematic review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sauerwein, E., Bailom, F., Matzler, K., & Hinterhuber, H. H. (1996, February 19–23). The Kano model: How to delight your customers. In Proceedings of the International Working Seminar on Production Economics (Vol. 1 No. 4. , pp. 313–327), Innsbruck, Austria. [Google Scholar]
- Schäfer, T., Sedlmeier, P., Städtler, C., & Huron, D. (2013). The psychological functions of music listening. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schneider, A. (2018). Systematic musicology: A historical interdisciplinary perspective. In Springer handbook of systematic musicology (pp. 1–24). Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Seashore, C. E. (1938). Psychology of music. McGraw-Hill. [Google Scholar]
- Swarbrick, D., Martin, R., Høffding, S., Nielsen, N., & Vuoskoski, J. K. (2024). Audience musical absorption: Exploring attention and affect in the live concert setting. Music & Science, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thompson, W. F., Bullot, N. J., & Margulis, E. H. (2023). The psychological basis of music appreciation: Structure, self, source. Psychological Review, 130(1), 260–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vuust, P., Heggli, O. A., Friston, K. J., & Kringelbach, M. L. (2022). Music in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 23, 287–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wellek, A. (1963). Musikpsychologie und musiästhetik: Grundriss der systematischen musikwissenschaft. Akademische Verlagsgesellschaft. [Google Scholar]
- Zatorre, R. (2024). From perception to pleasure: The neuroscience of music and why we love it. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Zatorre, R. J., & Salimpoor, V. N. (2013). From perception to pleasure: Music and its neural substrates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110, 10430–10437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Jacobs, H.; Leman, M.; Van Dyck, E. A Causal Model for Surveys of Exploratory Listening and Music Appreciation. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 676. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15050676
Jacobs H, Leman M, Van Dyck E. A Causal Model for Surveys of Exploratory Listening and Music Appreciation. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(5):676. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15050676
Chicago/Turabian StyleJacobs, Henk, Marc Leman, and Edith Van Dyck. 2025. "A Causal Model for Surveys of Exploratory Listening and Music Appreciation" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 5: 676. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15050676
APA StyleJacobs, H., Leman, M., & Van Dyck, E. (2025). A Causal Model for Surveys of Exploratory Listening and Music Appreciation. Behavioral Sciences, 15(5), 676. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15050676