Next Article in Journal
The Roles of Proactive Personality and Career Satisfaction in the Impact of Employer Brand Attributes Congruence on Creativity
Next Article in Special Issue
Psychopathic Traits in Adult versus Adolescent Males: Measurement Invariance across the PCL-R and PCL:YV
Previous Article in Journal
The Mediating Role of Interoceptive Sensitivity in the Relationship between Physical Activity and Depression Symptoms in College Students
Previous Article in Special Issue
Do Juveniles Who Have Committed Sexual Offenses Have Higher Callous-Unemotional Traits Compared to Juveniles Who Have Committed General Offenses? A Systematic Review
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Callous–Unemotional Traits and Conduct Problems in Children: The Role of Strength and Positive Characteristics

by
Patrícia Figueiredo
1,*,
Andreia Azeredo
1,
Ricardo Barroso
2,3,4 and
Fernando Barbosa
1,4
1
Laboratory of Neuropsychophysiology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Porto, Rua Alfredo Allen, 4200-135 Porto, Portugal
2
Department of Education and Psychology, University of Trás-os-Montes and Alto Douro, 5000-801 Vila Real, Portugal
3
Center of Psychology, University of Porto, Rua Alfredo Allen, 4200-135 Porto, Portugal
4
U.North Psychology Consortium, Portugal
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2024, 14(7), 609; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14070609
Submission received: 20 May 2024 / Revised: 5 July 2024 / Accepted: 15 July 2024 / Published: 18 July 2024
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Externalizing and Antisocial Behavior Across the Lifespan)

Abstract

:
In recent decades, many researchers have focused on the development of Conduct Problems from childhood to adolescence. Understanding behavior problems also requires an understanding of well-regulated characteristics. Focusing our assessment on strengths makes it possible, on the one hand, to help children or adolescents with deficits in important areas (e.g., socio-emotional deficits) to develop emotional regulation skills and adapt their responses to different contexts. This study aims to understand the role of self-competence, self-regulation, empathy, and responsibility (strength variables) in the relationship between Callous Unemotional characteristics and Conduct Problems, with a sample of 236 children aged between 3 and 10 years (M = 7.51, SD = 1.63), through mediation analysis. In general, our findings suggest that self-regulation significantly explains the relationship between the callous dimension of the Inventory of Callous–Unemotional Traits and Conduct Problems, pointing out that this strength variable seems to act as a protective factor against the development of behavior problems. No other mediation effects were found, and these results are considered in light of some limitations.

1. Introduction

Callous–unemotional (CU) traits include characteristics such as arrogance, a lack of remorse and empathy, and being manipulative and deceitful [1,2], usually predicting a severe, aggressive, and stable pattern of behavior problems, specifically Conduct Problems [3]. There is evidence that these traits are uniquely associated with aggressive and severe behavior problems [4], with stability from late childhood to early adolescence [5,6], representing the affective component of adult psychopathy. Children who express these traits often do not feel empathy for others, lack remorse for the disruptive behaviors in which they engage, can be very manipulative, and express low levels of fearful inhibition [7].
Although Frick and White [8] suggest that CU traits show stability, these traits are not immutable [9,10]. In fact, CU traits do not follow the same developmental trajectory in all children. Previous studies provide evidence for individual variability in the development of CU traits (e.g., [11,12,13]), but few studies have assessed the factors associated with stability or changes in CU and their translation into Conduct Problems (e.g., [13,14]). This heterogeneity within CU traits might be associated with different developmental processes that can help either explain the co-occurrence or not of Conduct Problems. The association between CU traits and Conduct Problems can accumulate with important individual and contextual risk and protective factors, suggesting that in parallel with CU traits, there are factors capable of influencing the development of adaptive and maladaptive behaviors (e.g., [15,16]). In this way, further understanding of both the risk and protective factors that might contribute to the relation between CU traits and Conduct Problems in youth is needed.
Furthermore, although research in child and youth psychology has evolved over the last few decades, creating a greater emphasis on assessing individual strengths and positive characteristics, the focus on risk factors or diagnosing pathology is still very marked [17,18,19,20]. Due to the social changes that occur in childhood, as well as the experience of transition from the family environment to the school environment [21], this is a key period during which children develop socio-emotional skills that may affect their ability to learn how to interact with others and their well-being [22,23,24]. Alongside the risk factors for pathology, assessing individual positive characteristics can provide a broader understanding of individuals’ abilities that can be used effectively to reduce the likelihood of developing Conduct Problems. In recent decades, many researchers have focused on the development of aggressive, antisocial, and violent behavior [25]. Understanding Conduct Problems also requires understanding those who exhibit well-regulated emotions [26], such as self-control and proper abilities, to deal with negative emotionality are fundamental to preventing pathological behavior [27]. Indeed, a common feature of Conduct Problems is the inability to effectively regulate emotions and self-evaluations in different contexts [27,28].
Self-regulation is described as a phenomenon that begins at birth and develops into adulthood, gaining significance at all stages of an individual’s life [29,30]. This construct refers to the inhibition of behavior or self-control, whereas its absence indicates impulsivity [31]. Self-regulation, a complex framework linked to both positive and negative adjustment, encompasses components such as working memory, attention, and inhibitory control [30,32]. Inhibitory control, which involves the regulation of behavior, refers to the ability to suppress impulses to achieve goals [33,34]. This structure is important for emotional, social, and cognitive development [35]. Particularly during early childhood, self-regulation assumes an important role in controlling the magnitude of emotions [36,37], including emotions that trigger aggression or provoke sadness and anxiety [38]. Although emotions are inherently regulated, their regulation varies depending on the person and the context [39]. Children with poor emotion regulation skills are at greater risk of poor behavioral outcomes. These children have difficulty getting along with adults and peers and are at higher risk of future aggressive behavior problems, Conduct Problems, substance abuse, and psychopathy [40,41].
On the contrary, children with high emotional regulation tend to be more socially competent [41]. Even when young people show low empathy or psychopathic traits, high self-control can enable them to avoid Conduct Problems and other manifestations of antisocial behavior [42]. Most studies based on questionnaires of adolescents reveal that non-antisocial young people with high CU traits have greater self-regulation than young people who score high on both CU traits and Conduct Problems [43]. Thus, the ability to regulate behavior considering individual goals, potential actions [44], and strategic planning [45] can differentiate young people with positive personality traits with and without Conduct Problems. Moreover, social competence has been a popular topic for research because it is not only a key aspect of children’s development but also a significant indicator of their functioning in other domains, such as academic achievement [46] or externalizing and internalizing psychopathology [47]. As such, identifying mechanisms for variations in children’s social competence may inform early prevention efforts to promote their healthy development. Children who show higher levels of CU may have more difficulty in relational contexts because the deficits that these children experience are key to successful social interactions [48,49]. Although CU traits and social competence may emerge simultaneously from early ages [50,51], they are distinct albeit related constructs. While CU traits are a stable, dispositional factor [6,52], social competence may be more malleable and determined by many contextual and individual factors, including CU traits [50].
Additionally, CU traits are primarily characterized by affective deficits. In contrast, social competence encompasses a more diverse set of interpersonal skills, some of which may be influenced by individual differences in CU traits. Furthermore, studies have shown that knowing and managing emotions is important for positive social interactions and stable relationships [53]. Thus, emotional competencies are important for acquiring social skills, especially for developing prosocial behavior and empathy [54], reducing their involvement in aggressive interactions [55,56].
In recent decades, many researchers have focused on the development of Conduct Problems from childhood to adolescence. Understanding Conduct Problems also requires understanding well-regulated characteristics [26]. The ability to self-control and deal with negative emotions are fundamental characteristics for understanding both normative and pathological behaviors [57]. Focusing on the assessment of strengths makes it possible, on the one hand, to help children or adolescents with deficits in important areas (e.g., socio-emotional deficits) to develop emotional regulation skills and adapt their responses to different contexts [58]. Therefore, the main objective of this study is to understand the role of strength and positive characteristics in the relationship between CU traits and Conduct Problems. It was hypothesized that (a) the callousness and uncaring dimensions of CU traits are positively related to Conduct Problems and that (b) strength and positive characteristics, namely social competence, self-regulation, empathy, and responsibility, mediate the above relation, i.e., they can diminish the effect of CU traits, reducing the likelihood of Conduct Problems occurring.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The selection of participants for this study was conducted in two phases. First, children between 3 and 10 years of age were randomly selected in schools in the Northern region of Portugal, and informed consent was provided to the parents of the selected children. Parents who agreed to participate and completed an informed consent form were sent a questionnaire about the behavior and routines of their children. Once the consent forms and questionnaires were received from the parents, the child’s teacher completed similar questionnaires. This procedure resulted in a sample of 236 children (52% girls; 4% receiving special education support) with characteristics closely approximating the participating school districts (see Table 1 for additional information on sample demographics). All procedures were appraised and followed the ethical standards of the institutional ethics committee.

2.2. Instruments and Measures

2.2.1. Inventory of Callous–Unemotional Traits—Teacher (ICU-Teacher; [59,60]; Portuguese Version Developed by Figueiredo et al. [61,62])

The ICU teacher is a teacher-report inventory composed of 12 out of 24 original items for assessing callous–unemotional traits. It is responded to on a 4-point Likert scale, ranging from 0 (=Not at all true) to 3 (=Definitely true), leading to a minimum score of zero and a maximum of 36. Higher scores on the total scale and subscales of this instrument indicate higher levels of CU traits. The first validation study of the ICU, by Essau et al. [59], found satisfactory to adequate internal consistency values, with Cronbach’s α of 0.64 and 0.73 for the unemotional and uncaring subscales, respectively, and 0.77 for the total score. In the present study, Cronbach’s alphas ranged between 0.83 and 0.85. The Portuguese versions were developed by Figueiredo et al. [61,62] and are organized into two factors—callous and uncaring—comprising a total of 11 and 12 items, respectively, for each version.

2.2.2. Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ; Goodman et al. [63])

The SDQ is a 25-item screening instrument that can be administered to parents and teachers of children aged 4 to 17 or as a self-report in participants over 11 years old. The questionnaire covers common social, emotional, and behavioral functioning areas across five subscales: Emotional Symptoms, Conduct Problems, Hyperactivity/Inattention, Peer problems, and Prosocial Behavior. The students’ and teachers’ versions are almost identical, except that the wording is slightly different (the students assess themselves, while the teachers assess their students). For each statement, the participants (students and/or teachers) respond using a 3-point Likert scale, ranging from 0 (=Not True) to 2 (=Certainly True), so that “0” indicates no problems, and “2” indicates problems. The maximum score for each subscale is 10, and the maximum total score is 50. Although the SDQ was adapted for Portuguese by Fleitlich et al. [64], the study of psychometric proprieties was carried on by Simões (see [65]) and revealed that a five-subscale structure is adequate for the Portuguese version of the SDQ. In the present study, SDQ was filled out by parents, and only the Conduct Problems subscale was used, which revealed a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.70.

2.2.3. Social–Emotional Assets and Resilience Scale—Teacher Form (SEARS-T; [58])

The SEARS-T consists of a scale measuring children’s and adolescents’ social-emotional competencies and assets. Social and emotional assets and resilience can be broadly defined as a set of adaptive characteristics important for success at school, with peers, and in the outside world. The SEARS-T comprises 41 items on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 0 (=Never) to 3 (=Always). It is organized into four empirically derived scales, including the following: (a) self-regulation; (b) empathy; (c) social competence; and (d) responsibility. The self-regulation subscale assesses the child’s self-awareness, metacognition, intrapersonal perception, and self-management from the teacher’s perspective (e.g., disagrees without fighting or arguing). The empathy subscale reflects the teacher’s assessment of the child’s empathy with the situations and feelings of others (e.g., feeling sorry when bad things happen to others). The social competence subscale reflects how the teacher perceives the child’s ability to maintain friendships with peers, engage in effective verbal communication, and feel comfortable in peer groups (e.g., feeling comfortable talking to different people). The responsibility subscale refers to the teacher’s assessment of the child’s ability to take responsibility, behave consciously, and think before acting (e.g., making good decisions). The Portuguese version by Figueiredo et al. [66], which comprises 40 items, revealed very high internal consistency for the four scales—responsibility, empathy, self-regulation, and self-competence—with Cronbach’s α of 0.94, 0.92, 0.95 and 0.92, respectively. In the present study, Cronbach’s α range was between 0.73 and 0.95.

2.3. Statistical Analyses

All analyses were performed using SPSS Version 28.0 for Windows (IBM, 2020), using a significance level of 0.05. Initially, the assumption of normality was assessed using the Shapiro–Wilk test, and when violated, this was complemented by analysis of the asymmetry (Sk) and kurtosis (Ku) coefficients. Since the absolute values of these coefficients varied between 2 and 7 [67], parametric tests were always used. The sphericity assumption was assessed using the Mauchly test, and when this assumption was violated, the Greenhouse–Geisser correction was applied, and the epsilon value (Ɛ) was reported. Preliminary statistical analyses were performed for all variables, including descriptive statistics and Pearson correlations. The Conduct Problems subscale of SDQ was entered as the dependent variable into a mediation analysis, resorting to the PROCESS macro for SPSS [68], which is a computational tool run within SPSS to generate mediation, moderation, and conditional process analyses [68]. CU trait variables (namely callous and uncaring scores of the ICU scale) were considered independent, and strength variables (specifically self-regulation, empathy, social competence, and responsibility scores of the SEARS scale) were considered mediators. All indirect effects were examined using bootstrapping analytical methods to estimate a bias-corrected 95% confidence interval (CI) by taking 5000 bootstrapped samples [69]. Confidence intervals of the conditional indirect effect that did not contain a value of zero indicated significant mediation effects. Complete mediation was considered to occur when path c’ (the direct effect) was equal to 0, suggesting that the association between X and Y was fully accounted for by the mediators, while partial mediation was considered when path c’ was a value other than 0 but significantly less than path c [69].

3. Results

Descriptive statistics for the variables of interest are provided in Table 1. Correlations between CU traits and Conduct Problems were examined, revealing that both callous, r = 0.21, p = 0.003, and uncaring scores, r = 0.34, p < 0.001, were positively related to Conduct Problems, as was the total score of ICU, r = 0.32, p < 0.001. In addition, both CU traits and Conduct Problems were negatively associated with strength and positive characteristics, namely social competence, empathy, self-regulation, and responsibility (see Table 2). These results support the further examination of strength variables as mediators in the relationship between CU traits and Conduct Problems.

Mediation Effects of Strength and Positive Characteristics

Three mediation models were computed to examine the strength and positive characteristics as possible mediators of the relationship between CU traits, namely total CU traits, callous, uncaring, and Conduct Problems. In the model, considering the total score of the ICU traits as the independent variable (Figure 1), bootstrapping analyses revealed the total effect of CU traits on Conduct Problems, b = 0.09, p < 0.001. Empathy, responsibility, self-regulation, and social competence did not significantly mediate the relation between total CU traits and Conduct Problems (all p > 0.05). It was verified that the total ICU score predicted all strength variables (all p < 0.001), but none of the said variables significantly predicted Conduct Problems (all p > 0.05). The results from a multiple mediation analysis testing empathy, responsibility, self-regulation, and social competence as mediators in the relation between total CU traits and Conduct Problems are presented in Table 3 and Figure 1.
Taking the callous subscale score as the independent variable (Figure 2), bootstrapping analyses revealed the total effect on Conduct Problems, b = 0.11, p = 0.020. Self-regulation significantly mediated the relation between the callous subscale and Conduct Problems, such that higher scores in the callous subscale were associated with higher scores in the Conduct Problems subscale through the effects of decreased Self-regulation at BaC 95% CI [−0.162, −0.007]. Empathy, responsibility, and social competence subscales were not significant mediators in the relation between callous scores and Conduct Problems (all p > 0.05). The scores of the callous subscale predicted all strength and positive characteristics (all p < 0.05), but none of these variables significantly predicted Conduct Problems (all p > 0.05), except Self-regulation (p < 0.05). The results from a multiple mediation analysis testing empathy, responsibility, self-regulation, and social competence as mediators in the relationship between callous and Conduct Problems are presented in Table 3 and Figure 2.
With the uncaring subscale as the independent variable (Figure 3), bootstrapping analyses revealed the total effect on Conduct Problems, b = 0.16, p < 0.001. Empathy, responsibility, self-regulation, and social competence were not significant mediators in the relationship between the uncaring subscale and Conduct Problems (all p > 0.05). Individually, it was verified that the uncaring subscale predicted all strength and positive characteristics (all p < 0.001). However, none of these variables significantly predicted Conduct Problems (all p > 0.05). The results from multiple mediation analyses testing empathy, responsibility, self-regulation, and social competence as mediators between the uncaring subscale and Conduct Problems are presented in Table 3 and Figure 3.

4. Discussion

Over the last two decades, psychopathic traits in child and adolescent samples have been consistently associated with more severe and persistent patterns of Conduct Problems [11,70,71]. Given the increased risk that these traits seem to carry for the development of Conduct Problems throughout life and the importance of maximizing the potential of early prevention, it is of great interest to assess such traits as early as possible. For this reason, researchers have increasingly examined the role of callous–unemotional (CU) traits in children, gathering evidence that these traits may already be evident at preschool age [52,72,73]. According to Fanti et al. [74], it is necessary to understand the development of CU traits associated with different risk and protective factors for developing conduct problems in childhood and adolescence.
However, the factors associated with antisocial behavior have often been studied in isolation, even though they are not independent of each other and often interact with the same individual [75]. Thus, examining the sole influence of one factor will provide an insufficient explanation for Conduct Problems. Furthermore, although research in psychology has evolved over the last few decades, creating a movement towards greater emphasis on the assessment of individual strengths and positive characteristics (strength outcomes), the focus on risk factors or pathology diagnosis is still very evident [17,18,19,20]. Alongside the risk factors for pathology, assessing individual positive characteristics can provide a broader understanding of individuals’ abilities that can be used effectively to reduce the likelihood of developing disruptive behavior disorders.
Considering that previous research points to callous–unemotional (CU) traits as possible markers for a particularly severe presentation of Conduct Problems, eventually beginning in early childhood [76] and extending to childhood and adolescence, this study examines how strength characteristics could be related to CU traits, and possibly mediate the relationship between such traits and Conduct Problems in childhood.
Correlation analyses revealed the expected positive and significant association between the total CU traits score, as well as the callous and uncaring subscale scores and Conduct Problems. Additionally, correlation analyses revealed the expected significant but negative relationships between strength variables, namely social competence, self-regulation, empathy, and responsibility, with both CU traits and Conduct Problems. This preliminary analysis supported mediation analyses, which revealed that self-regulation significantly explains the relation between the callous dimension of the Inventory of Callous–Unemotional Traits and Conduct Problems; no other mediation effects were found. This indicates that self-regulation may act as a protective factor against the development of Conduct Problems. However, it is important to note that these findings are based on cross-sectional data and cannot establish causality. Longitudinal studies are necessary to investigate the developmental trajectories of CU traits and their influence on Conduct Problems.
The abovementioned finding suggests that characteristics of self-regulation as a strength variable, and more specifically, higher levels of self-regulation, may protect children with high CU traits from developing Conduct Problems. On the contrary, in line with Hadjicharalambous and Fanti’s [43] study, results support the idea that self-regulation deficits may contribute to the antisocial behavior of young people with high CU traits. Therefore, low self-regulation may interplay with low empathy and low remorse [4], leading someone to engage in antisocial behaviors. Prior works have already shown that impulsive individuals with deficits in self-control and attention are more likely to develop antisocial behavior due to their difficulties in regulating their behavior (e.g., [12]). Contrariwise, our results suggest that normative levels of self-regulation may be considered a protective factor, possibly enabling children to refrain from antisocial behavior despite their high levels of CU traits.
The results of this study should be considered alongside common methodological shortcomings in the field. First, cross-sectional research using mediation analyses cannot establish temporal precedence, so longitudinal data are needed to examine causation in these relationships. Additionally, it is necessary to consider the potential grouping of data since each teacher evaluated about 3 to 5 students. This clustering can introduce bias and lead to the non-independence of observations, which might affect the results. Future studies should account for this clustering effect, possibly using Multilevel Modeling techniques to control teacher-related variance. The way teachers approached classifying their students was not accounted for in our analyses, and systematic grouping was considered a potential source of measurement bias that could affect reliability [77]. Future studies could resort to a Multilevel Modeling approach to obtain unbiased parameter estimates and statistical inferences. To carry out a Multilevel Modeling approach, it is recommended to use cluster sizes of between 5 and 30 [78]. However, in this study, the number of cases evaluated per professor was insufficient to generate robust analyses, because, in our data, there were teachers with a minimum of one and a maximum of eight children evaluated. In addition, there was only one informant for the child’s Conduct Problems and individual characteristics (i.e., strength and positive characteristics), and it is known that a single informant may not provide a comprehensive view of Conduct Problems. This single-informant approach can lead to a one-sided perspective, potentially overlooking behaviors that occur outside the school environment. Therefore, this study must be replicated, but measures must be used from observational methods or data from multiple informants for the same variables, including, for example, assessments from the parents’ perspective. It is important to keep in mind that meta-analyses of multi-informant reports on child psychopathology (e.g., [79,80]) reveal a low correlation between parent and teacher reports (e.g., [81]). This may suggest that the information provided by parents and teachers covers different aspects of the child’s behavior, mainly because the observations are made in different environments (i.e., home vs. school). Thus, teachers’ reports are relevant and unique sources of information about children’s behavior. Teachers usually spend more time during the day with children than parents do and have the opportunity to observe them in various dynamics and interactions with other children and other adults [82]. In addition, teachers observe children in structured (e.g., classroom) and unstructured (e.g., playground) environments with their peers [83,84]. Finally, it should be noted that this study did not consider the assessment and/or analysis of confounding factors. These uncontrolled variables represent significant limitations as they may confound the observed relationships. Future studies should consider including such analyses to control potential confounders and provide a more comprehensive understanding of the variables influencing the outcomes. Examples of potential confounding factors include socioeconomic status, parental education levels, home environment, and prior exposure to interventions. Addressing these factors could enhance the robustness and validity of the findings.
Despite the above limitations, the results of this study add to those of others that contribute to a better understanding of the relationship between CU traits and Conduct Problems in preschool and school-age children. In fact, the trajectories of antisocial behavior begin to be investigated as early as the second year of life (e.g., [85]), and CU traits, measurable from the age of two, seem to play a central role in developing Conduct Problems. Identifying and assessing CU traits at an early age, as well as their associated factors, could have important implications for research into the structuring trajectories of psychopathic personalities, as well as for intervention with the potential to prevent the emergence of Conduct Problems. It should be noted that the same factors, depending on their expression (negative or positive), can act as risk or protective variables. At an individual level, social–emotional skills, specifically self-regulation, are important for the prosocial development of behavior, as they promote social skills and stable interpersonal relationships. As such, it is important to understand the potential effects of protective factors at different points in development. Thus, assessing and understanding positive characteristics can help inform strategies that address needs and challenges throughout development. As the percentage of children and young people who are considered at risk of developing conduct problems associated with emotional problems continues to rise, new ways of thinking about assessment and intervention services are needed. The public health approach and the well-known triangle of support model are recommended as a way of planning services for all children and adolescents in a given school or community population. Using this approach can help identify procedures and objectives for screening and assessment and help link the outcomes of screening and assessment to appropriate intervention services at the primary, secondary, and tertiary levels of prevention (e.g., see [86]). These strategies can involve training in social and emotional skills, including, for example, increasing emotional awareness, impulse control, and conflict management (e.g., [87,88]), encouraging parental involvement, and training in adopting methods to support the development of self-regulation.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, P.F.; methodology, P.F.; validation, P.F.; R.B. and F.B.; formal analysis, P.F.; investigation, P.F.; writing—original draft preparation, P.F. and A.A.; writing—review and editing, P.F. and F.B.; supervision, R.B. and F.B.; project administration, P.F.; funding acquisition, P.F. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Fundação para a Ciência e Tecnologia (FCT), grant number SFRH/BD/133694/2017.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board (or Ethics Committee) of the Faculty of Psychology and Educational Science of the University of Porto (code Refª 2018/11-8).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the patients to publish this paper.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because due to technical limitations. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to correspondence author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Cooke, D.; Michie, C.; Hart, S.; Clark, D. Reconstructing psychopathy: Clarifying the significance of antisocial and socially deviant behavior in the diagnosis of psychopathic personality disorder. J. Personal. Disord. 2004, 18, 337–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Farrington, D. The importance of child and adolescent psychopathy. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2005, 33, 489–497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Frick, P.J.; Stickle, T.R.; Dandreaux, D.M.; Farrell, J.M.; Kimonis, E.R. Callous–unemotional traits in predicting the severity and stability of conduct problems and delinquency. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2005, 33, 471–487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Frick, P.J.; White, S.F. Research Review: The importance of callous-unemotional traits for developmental models of aggressive and antisocial behavior. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2008, 49, 359–375. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Munoz, L.C.; Frick, P.J. The reliability, stability, and predictive utility of the self-report version of the antisocial process screening device. Scand. J. Psychol. 2007, 48, 299–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Obradovic, J.; Pardini, D.; Long, J.D.; Loeber, R. Measuring interpersonal callousness in boys from childhood to adolescence: An examination of longitudinal invariance and temporal stability. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2007, 36, 276–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Frick, P.J.; Marsee, M.A. Psychopathy and developmental pathways to antisocial behavior in youth. In The Handbook of Psychopathy; Patrick, C.J., Ed.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2018; pp. 353–375. [Google Scholar]
  8. Frick, P.J.; Cornell, A.H.; Barry, C.T.; Bodin, S.D.; Dane, H.E. Callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems in the prediction of conduct problem severity, aggression, and self-report of delinquency. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2003, 31, 457–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Lyman, D.R.; Caspi, A.; Moffitt, T.E.; Raine, A.; Loeber, R.; Stouthamer-Loeber, M. Adolescent psychopathy and the big five: Results from two samples. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2007, 33, 431–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Fontaine, N.M.G.; McCrory, E.J.P.; Boivin, M.; Moffitt, T.E.; Viding, E. Predictors and outcomes of joint trajectories of callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems in childhood. J. Abnorm. Child Psychiatry 2011, 120, 730–742. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Frick, P.; Ray, J.; Thorton, L.; Kahn, R. Can Callous–Unemotional traits enhance the understanding, diagnosis and treatment of serious conduct problems in children and adolescents? A comprehensive review. Psychol. Bull. J. 2014, 140, 1–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Pardini, D.A.; Loeber, R. Interpersonal Callousness trajectories across adolescence: Early social influences and adult outcomes. Crim. Justice Behav. 2008, 35, 173–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Fontaine, N.M.; Rijsdijk, F.V.; McCrory, E.J.; Viding, E. Etiology of different developmental trajectories of Callous-Unemotional traits. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2010, 49, 656–664. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  14. Klingzell, I.; Fanti, K.A.; Colins, O.F.; Frogner, L.; Andershed, A.K.; Andershed, H. Early childhood trajectories of conduct problems and Callous-Unemotional traits: The role of fearlessness and psychopathic personality dimensions. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2015, 27, 236–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Brazil, I.A. Considering new insights into antisociality and psychopathy. Lancet Psychiatry 2015, 2, 115–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Fanti, K.A. Individual, social, and behavioral factors associated with co-occurring conduct problems and callous-unemotional traits. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2013, 41, 811–824. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Garmezy, N. Vulnerability and resilience. In Studying Lives through time: Personality and Development; Funder, D.C., Parke, R.D., Tomlinson-Keasey, C., Widaman, K., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 1993; pp. 377–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Kirby, L.; Fraser, M. Risk and resilience in childhood. In Risk and Resilience in Childhood: An Ecological Perspective; Fraser, M., Ed.; NASW: Washington, DC, USA, 1997; pp. 10–33. [Google Scholar]
  19. Seligman, M.; Csikszentmihalyi, M. Positive psychology: An introduction. Am. Psychol. 2000, 55, 5–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Suldo, S.M.; Shaffer, E.J. Looking beyond psychopathology: The dual-factor model of mental health in youth. Sch. Psychol. Rev. 2008, 37, 52–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Vecchiotti, S. Kindergarten: An Overlooked Educational Policy Priority. Soc. Policy Rep. 2003, 17, 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Denham, S.A.; Bassett, H.H.; Zinsser, K. Early Childhood Teachers as Socializers of Young Children’s Emotional Competence. Early Child. Educ. J. 2012, 40, 137–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Heo, K.H.; Squires, J. Cultural adaptation of a parent completed social emotional screening instrument for young children: Ages and stages questionnaire-social emotional. Early Hum. Dev. 2012, 88, 151–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Yates, T.; Ostrosky, M.M.; Cheatham, G.A.; Fettig, A.; Shaffer, L.; Santos, R.M. Research Synthesis on Screening and Assessing Social and Emotional Competence. Center on the Social Emotional Foundations of Early Learning at Vanderbilt University, 2008. Available online: http://csefel.vanderbilt.edu/documents/rs_screening_assessment.pdf (accessed on 3 February 2024).
  25. Granic, I.; Patterson, G.R. Toward a comprehensive model of antisocial development: A dynamic systems approach. Psychol. Rev. 2006, 113, 101–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Salekin, R.T. Psychopathy in childhood: Why should we care about grandiose-manipulative and daring-impulsive traits? Br. J. Psychiatry 2016, 209, 189–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; APA: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  28. Kashdan, T.B.; Rottenberg, J. Psychological flexibility as a fundamental aspect of health. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2010, 30, 865–878. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. McCabe, L.A.; Cunnington, M.; Brooks-Gunn, J. The development of self-regulation in young children. In Handbook of the Self-Regulation: Research Theory and Applications; Baumeister Ve, R.F., Vohs, K.D., Eds.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2004; pp. 341–357. [Google Scholar]
  30. McClelland, M.M.; Cameron, C.E. Self-regulation in early childhood: Improving conceptual clarity and developing ecologically valid measures. Child Dev. Perspect. 2012, 6, 136–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Steinberg, L.; Albert, D.; Cauffman, E.; Banich, M.; Graham, S.; Woolard, J. Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as indexed by behavior and self-report: Evidence for a dual systems model. Dev. Psychol. 2008, 44, 1764–1778. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Skibbe, L.E.; Connor, C.M.; Morrison, F.J.; Jewkes, A.M. Schooling effects on preschoolers’ self-regulation, early literacy and language growth. Early Child. Res. Q. 2011, 26, 42–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Cuevas, K.; Rajan, V.; Bryant, L.J. Emergence of executive function in infancy. In Executive Function: Development across the Life Span; Wiebe, S.A., Karbach, J., Eds.; Routledge: London, UK, 2008; pp. 25–42. [Google Scholar]
  34. Posner, M.I.; Rothbart, M.K. Developing mechanisms of self-regulation. Dev. Psychopthol. 2009, 12, 427–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Watson, A.J.; Bell, M.A. Individual differences in ınhibitory control skills at three years of age. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2013, 38, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Carlson, S.M.; Wang, T.S. Inhibitory control and emotion regulation in preschool children. Cogn. Dev. 2007, 22, 489–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Whitebread, D.; Basilio, M. The emergence of early development of self regulation in young children. Profesorado 2012, 116, 15–33. [Google Scholar]
  38. Lewis, M.; Granic, I.; Lamm, C. Behavioral differences in aggressive children linked with neural mechanisms of emotion regulation. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2006, 1094, 164–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Cole, P.M.; Martin, S.E.; Dennis, T.A. Emotion regulation as a scientific construct: Methodological challenges and directions for child development research. Child Dev. 2004, 75, 317–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Frick, P.J.; Morris, A.S. Temperament and developmental pathways to conduct problems. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2004, 33, 54–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Eisenberg, N. Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2000, 51, 665–697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Hadjicharalambous, M.Z.; Fanti, K.A. Self regulation, cognitive capacity and risk taking: Investigating heterogeneity among adolescents with callous-unemotional traits. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2017, 49, 331–340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Wall, T.D.; Frick, P.J.; Fanti, K.A.; Kimonis, E.R.; Lordos, A. Factors differentiating callous-unemotional children with and without conduct problems. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2016, 57, 976–983. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Roelofs, A. Goal-referenced selection of verbal action: Modeling attentional control in the Stroop task. Psychol. Rev. 2003, 110, 88–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Carver, C.S.; Scheier, M.F. On the Self-Regulation of Behavior; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2001. [Google Scholar]
  46. Zhou, Q.; Chen, S.H.; Main, A. Commonalities and differences in the research on children’s effortful control and executive function: A call for an integrated model of self-regulation. Child Dev. Perspect. 2012, 6, 112–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Bornstein, M.H.; Hahn, C.-S.; Haynes, O.M. Social competence, externalizing, and internalizing behavioral adjustment from early childhood through early adolescence: Developmental cascades. Dev. Psychopathol. 2010, 22, 717–735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Roos, S.; Hodges, E.V.; Salmivalli, C. Do guilt-and shame-proneness differentially predict prosocial, aggressive, and withdrawn behaviors during early adolescence? Dev. Psychol. 2014, 50, 941–946. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Sallquist, J.; Eisenberg, N.; Spinrad, T.L.; Eggum, N.D.; Gaertner, B.M. Assessment of preschoolers’ positive empathy: Concurrent and longitudinal relations with positive emotion, social competence, and sympathy. J. Posit. Psychol. 2009, 4, 223–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Howes, C. Social competence with peers in young children: Developmental sequences. Dev. Rev. 1987, 7, 252–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Kimonis, E.R.; Frick, P.J.; Boris, N.W.; Smyke, A.T.; Cornell, A.H.; Farrell, J.M.; Zeanah, C.H. Callous-unemotional features, behavioral inhibition, and parenting: Independent predictors of aggression in a high-risk preschool sample. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2006, 15, 741–752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Dadds, M.R.; Fraser, J.; Frost, A.; Hawes, D.J. Disentangling the underlying dimensions of psychopathy and conduct problems in childhood: A community study. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 2005, 73, 400–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Hay, D.F.; Payne, A.; Chadwick, A. Peer relations in childhood. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2004, 45, 84–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Izard, C.E.; Fine, S.; Mostow, A.; Trentacosta, C.; Campbell, J. Emotional processes in normal and abnormal development and preventive intervention. Dev. Psychopathol. 2002, 14, 761–787. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Arsenio, W.F.; Cooperman, S.; Lover, A. Afective predictors of preschoolers’ aggression and peer acceptance: Direct and indirect effects. Dev. Psychopathol. 2000, 36, 438–448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. De Castro, B.O.; Bosch, J.D.; Veerman, J.W.; Koops, W. The effects of emotion regulation, attribution, and delay prompts on aggressive boys’ social problem solving. Cogn. Ther. Res. 2003, 27, 153–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. DeLisi, M.; Vaughn, M.G. Foundation for a temperament-based theory of antisocial behavior and criminal justice system involvement. J. Crim. Justice 2014, 42, 10–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Merrell, K. Social Emotional Assets and Resilience Scales; Professional Manual; PAR: Lutz, FL, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  59. Essau, C.A.; Sasagawa, S.; Frick, P.J. Callous-unemotional traits in a community sample of adolescents. Assessment 2006, 13, 454–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Frick, P.J. The Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits (Unpublished Rating Scale); University of New Orleans: New Orleans, LA, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  61. Figueiredo, P.; Moreira, D.; Ramião, E.; Barroso, R.; Barbosa, F. Psychometric properties of the Portuguese teacher-version of the inventory of Callous-Unemotional traits. Clin. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2022, 27, 135910452110701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Figueiredo, P.; Ramião, E.; Moreira, D.; Barroso, R.; Barbosa, F. Psychometric properties of the Portuguese teacher-version of the Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits in Preschoolers. Análise Psicológica 2023, 41, 127–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Goodman, R.; Meltzer, H.; Bailey, V. The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A pilot study on the validity of the self-report version. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 1998, 7, 125–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Fleitlich, B.; Cortázar, P.G.; Goodman, R. Questionário de capacidades e dificuldades (SDQ). Infanto-Rev. Neuropsiquiatr. Infância Adolesc. 2000, 8, 44–50. [Google Scholar]
  65. Marzocchi, G.M.; Capron, C.; Di Pietro, M.; Duran Tauleria, E.; Duyme, M.; Frigerio, A.; Gaspar, M.F.; Hamilton, H.; Pithon, G.; Simões, A.; et al. The use of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) in Southern European countries. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2004, 13, ii40–ii46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Figueiredo, P.; Azeredo, A.; Barroso, R.; Barbosa, F. Psychometric Properties of Teacher Report of Social-Emotional Assets and Resilience Scale in Preschoolers and Elementary School Children. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 2020, 42, 799–807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Kim, H.Y. Statistical notes for clinical researchers: Assessing normal distribution using skewness and kurtosis. Restor. Dent. Endod. 2013, 38, 52–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Hayes, A.F. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  69. Baron, R.M.; Kenny, D.A. The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1986, 51, 1173–1182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Lynam, D.R.; Caspi, A.; Moffitt, T.E.; Loeber, R.; Stouthamer-Loeber, M. Longitudinal evidence that psychopathy scores in early adolescence predict adult psychopathy. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2008, 116, 155–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Salekin, R.T. Research Review: What do we know about psychopathic traits in children? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2017, 58, 1180–1200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Ezpeleta, L.; de la Osa, N.; Granero, R.; Penelo, E.; Domènech, J.M. Inventory of Callous-Unemotional traits in a community sample of preschoolers. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2013, 42, 91–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Kimonis, E.; Goulter, N.; Hawes, D.; Wilbur, R.; Groer, M. Neuroendocrine factors distinguish juvenile psychopathy variants. Dev. Psychobiol. 2016, 59, 161–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Fanti, K.A.; Colins, O.F.; Andershed, H.; Sikki, M. Stability and change in Callous-Unemotional traits: Longitudinal associations with potential individual and contextual risk and protective factors. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 2016, 87, 62–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Flouri, E.; Tzavidis, N.; Kallis, C. Area and family effects on the psychopathology of the Millennium Cohort Study children and their older siblings. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2010, 51, 152–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Longman, T.; Hawes, D.J.; Kohlhoff, J. Callous–Unemotional Traits as Markers for Conduct Problem Severity in Early Childhood: A Meta-analysis. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2015, 47, 326–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Webb, N.; Shavelson, R. Generalizability Theory: Overview. In Encyclopedia of Statistics in Behavioral Science; Everitt, B., Howell, D., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons Ltd.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Luke, D. Multilevel Modeling (Quantitative Applications in the Social Sciences); SAGE Publications, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  79. De Los Reyes, A.; Augenstein, T.M.; Wang, M.; Thomas, S.A.; Drabick, D.A.; Burgers, D.E.; Rabinowitz, J. The validity of the multi-informant approach to assessing child and adolescent mental health. Psychol. Bull. 2015, 141, 858–900. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. De Los Reyes, A.; Kazdin, A.E. Informant discrepancies in the assessment of childhood psychopathology: A critical review, theoretical framework, and recommendations for further study. Psychol. Bull. 2005, 131, 483–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Valo, S.; Tannock, R. Diagnostic instability of DSM-IV ADHD subtypes: Effects of informant source, instrumentation, and methods for combining symptom reports. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2010, 39, 749–760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Abikoff, H.; Courtney, M.; Pelham, W.E.; Koplewicz, H.S. Teachers’ ratings of disruptive behaviors: The influence of halo effects. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 1993, 21, 519–533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Curhan, K.B.; Levine, C.S.; Markus, H.R.; Kitayama, S.; Park, J.; Karasawa, M.; Park, J.; Karasawa, M.; Kawakami, N.; Love, G.; et al. Subjective and objective hierarchies and their relations to psychological well-being: A US/Japan comparison. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 2014, 5, 855–864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Farrell, A.K.; Imami, L.; Stanton, S.C.E.; Slatcher, R.B. Affective processes as mediators of links between close relationships and physical health. Soc. Personal. Psychol. Compass 2018, 12, e12408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Monahan, K.C.; Steinberg, L.; Cauffman, E.; Mulvey, E.P. Trajectories of antisocial behavior and psychosocial maturity from adolescence to young adulthood. Dev. Psychol. 2009, 45, 1654–1668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Whitcomb, S. Behavioral, Social, and Emotional Assessment of Children and Adolescents; Routledge: London, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  87. Diamond, A.; Lee, K. Interventions Shown to Aid Executive Function Development in Children 4 to 12 Years Old. Science 2011, 333, 959–964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Tomporowski, P.D.; McCullick, B.; Pendleton, D.M.; Pesce, C. Exercise and children’s cognition: The role of exercise characteristics and a place for metacognition. J. Sport Health Sci. 2015, 4, 47–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Mediation effects of strength variables on the relationship between total CU traits and Conduct Problems.
Figure 1. Mediation effects of strength variables on the relationship between total CU traits and Conduct Problems.
Behavsci 14 00609 g001
Figure 2. Mediation effects of strength variables on the relationship between the callousness subscale and Conduct Problems.
Figure 2. Mediation effects of strength variables on the relationship between the callousness subscale and Conduct Problems.
Behavsci 14 00609 g002
Figure 3. Mediation effects of strength variables on the relationship between the uncaring subscale and Conduct Problems.
Figure 3. Mediation effects of strength variables on the relationship between the uncaring subscale and Conduct Problems.
Behavsci 14 00609 g003
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for variables of interest.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for variables of interest.
VariablesPossible RangeNMinMaxMean (SD)
Age 2363117.14 (1.99)
Education
   Kindergarten69
   Elementary School167
Total CU traits(0–33)2150278.76 (5.37)
Callousness(0–18)2150142.10 (2.89)
Uncaring(0–15)2150156.65 (3.31)
Conduct Problems(0–10)223081.77 (1.62)
Self-Regulation (0–39)21603421.49 (5.60)
Empathy(0–48)21621811. 19 (2.71)
Social Competence(0–39)21603411.20 (3.05)
Responsibility(0–30)21613018.95 (5.13)
Notes. CU = callous–unemotional; SD = standard deviation.
Table 2. Correlations among variables of interest.
Table 2. Correlations among variables of interest.
12345678
1. Total of CU traits
2. Callousness0.85 **
3. Uncaring0.89 **0.50 **––
4. Conduct Problems0.32 **0.21 **0.334 **
5. Self-Regulation −0.74 **−0.56 **−0.71 **−0.35 **
6. Empathy−0.72 **−0.59 **−0.74 **−0.30 **0.82 **
7. Social Competence−0.39 **−0.24 **−0.43 **−0.21 **0.55 **0.52 **
8. Responsibility−0.74 **−0.58 **−0.70 **−0.31 **0.84 **0.80 **0.55 **
Notes. CU = callous–unemotional; ** p < 0.001.
Table 3. Indirect effects of CU traits on Conduct Problems.
Table 3. Indirect effects of CU traits on Conduct Problems.
Independent
Variable
Mediated EffectPoint EstimateSEBCa 95% CI
Total CU traits
Empathy−0.0020.023[−0.048, 0.044]
Responsibility−0.0050.030[−0.059, 0.056]
Self-Regulation0.0570.034[−0.009, 0.124]
Social Competence0.0030.011[−0.026, 0.016]
Callousness
Empathy0.0080.030[−0.051, 0.067]
Responsibility0.0050.044[−0.076, 0.095]
Self-Regulation0.088 *0.046[0.005, 0.187]
Social Competence0.0020.013[−0.035, 0.015]
Uncaring
Empathy−0.0170.039[−0.098, 0.056]
Responsibility−0.0030.047[−0.090, 0.093]
Self-Regulation0.0890.051[−0.011, 0.191]
Social Competence0.0030.020[−0.051, 0.028]
Note. SE = standardized error; BCa 95% CI = Bias-corrected and accelerated 95% confidence interval. * p < 0.05.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Figueiredo, P.; Azeredo, A.; Barroso, R.; Barbosa, F. Callous–Unemotional Traits and Conduct Problems in Children: The Role of Strength and Positive Characteristics. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 609. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14070609

AMA Style

Figueiredo P, Azeredo A, Barroso R, Barbosa F. Callous–Unemotional Traits and Conduct Problems in Children: The Role of Strength and Positive Characteristics. Behavioral Sciences. 2024; 14(7):609. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14070609

Chicago/Turabian Style

Figueiredo, Patrícia, Andreia Azeredo, Ricardo Barroso, and Fernando Barbosa. 2024. "Callous–Unemotional Traits and Conduct Problems in Children: The Role of Strength and Positive Characteristics" Behavioral Sciences 14, no. 7: 609. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14070609

APA Style

Figueiredo, P., Azeredo, A., Barroso, R., & Barbosa, F. (2024). Callous–Unemotional Traits and Conduct Problems in Children: The Role of Strength and Positive Characteristics. Behavioral Sciences, 14(7), 609. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs14070609

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop