Next Article in Journal
Research Progress on Glycoprotein 5 of Porcine Reproductive and Respiratory Syndrome Virus
Next Article in Special Issue
Life History Parameters and Fishing Aspects of the Alien Nimble Spray Crab Percnon gibbesi in a Native Area of the Central-East Atlantic
Previous Article in Journal
Warm Season Turfgrass Equine Sports Surfaces: An Experimental Comparison of the Independence of Simple Measurements Used for Surface Characterization
Previous Article in Special Issue
Fish Community Structure and Biomass Particle-Size Spectrum in the Upper Reaches of the Jinsha River (China)
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Diversity of Underwater Vocalizations in Chinese Soft-Shelled Turtle (Pelodiscus sinensis)

Ministry of Education Key Laboratory for Ecology of Tropical Islands, Key Laboratory of Tropical Animal and Plant Ecology of Hainan Province, College of Life Sciences, Hainan Normal University, Haikou 571158, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2023, 13(5), 812; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13050812
Submission received: 4 January 2023 / Revised: 11 February 2023 / Accepted: 21 February 2023 / Published: 23 February 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Aquatic Fauna Biodiversity and Conservation)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Vocalization is important for the survival of underwater animals. Pelodiscus sinensis spends most of its life in water, and its wild population is highly vulnerable. However, its underwater vocalization, which can serve as the basis for ecological and evolutionary research, has not been explored. We identified 10 call types of the species from underwater recordings and found differences in vocalizations between individuals of different ages and sexes. The Chinese soft-shelled turtle has a high diversity of vocalizations and shows a tendency for vocalization to become more diverse with age. These features help them to adapt to complex and dark underwater environments.

Abstract

Sound communication is important for underwater species. The wild population of the Chinese soft-shelled turtle (Pelodiscus sinensis) is listed as vulnerable. However, its vocalization, which can serve as the basis for ecological and evolutionary research, has not been studied. Here, we performed underwater recordings of 23 Chinese soft-shelled turtles of different ages and sexes and identified 720 underwater calls. The turtle calls were manually divided into 10 call types according to visual and aural inspection properties. The similarity test indicated that the manual division was reliable. We described the acoustic properties of the calls and the statistical analysis showed that the peak frequency of calls was significantly different between adult females and males, and also between subadults and adults. Similar to other aquatic turtles that prefer to live in deep water, Chinese soft-shelled turtles have a high vocal diversity and many harmonic calls, indicating that this highly aquatic species developed a variety of vocalizations to enhance their underwater communication, which helped them adapt to the complex and dim underwater environment. Furthermore, the turtles showed a tendency for vocalization to become more diverse with age.

1. Introduction

As an indispensable source of information, sound is important for underwater species [1]. Vocalization plays an important role in a turtle’s life, conveying a large amount of individual information [2,3], and is associated with mating and incubation [4,5,6]. Female Testudo marginata prefer fast-rate and high-pitched calls in males [5]. Vocalization of T. hermanni can transmit information about individuals’ size and health status and is related to their mating success [2]. Moreover, the vocalizations of Chelonia mydas and Eretmochelys imbricata hatchlings in a nest are related to the synchronization of emergence [4,6].
According to previous studies, turtle vocalizations are relatively simple compared to those of other groups of species, mostly coming in the form of pulses and harmonics [7,8,9,10,11,12]. Early studies on the vocalization of turtles were mainly in the air, such as Dermochelys coriacea and Testudinidae [3,13,14,15]. However, in recent years, an increasing number of studies have focused on the underwater vocalizations of turtles. Chelonida oblonga produces 17 call types in semi-natural pools [10]. Three call types of Carettochelys insculpta [16], 11 call types of Podocnemis expansa [17], and six call types of Lepidochelys kempii [7] were recorded in the field. Cultured Mauremys sinensis and Trachemys scripta elegans can produce 10 and 12 underwater sounds, respectively [12,18], and even juvenile green turtles (C. mydas) in field can produce 11 types of underwater sounds [19].
Researchers believe that reptiles intermittently regulate the passage of air by expulsing large amounts of air and changing the size of the glottis [20]. According to the anatomy of the turtle’s throat, the structure and position of the hyoid cartilage and two bands of elastic fibers in the tortoise’s larynx suggest that these bands are capable of vibrating, and two diverticula supported by the cricoid form a resonating chamber, all of which may function as vocal cords [21]. This may explain why turtles can make underwater sounds.
The most common method for turtle call classification is to record vocalizations and manually classify them according to visual and auditory differences [10,16]. Turtles make sounds when they move underwater in tanks, such as crawling, stroking water, releasing bubbles, sucking water, scratching claws against the tank bottom, and scratching the tank wall with claws, and spectrograms of the sounds produced by these behaviors have been reported [18]. These sounds can be easily distinguished from the vocalizations of turtles [12]. The correlation coefficient test of signals is a traditional method for checking the degree of similarity between two signals [22,23]. A method that combines manual classification with a signal similarity test can decrease the subjectivity of the manual division of calls, and it performed well in a previous study for turtle call classification [12].
Chinese soft-shelled turtles (Pelodiscus sinensis) live in many countries in East and Southeast Asia [24]. In China, they are bred in large numbers on farms, but their wild population continues to decline, and it is listed as a “Vulnerable species” by the International Union for Conservation of Nature Red List of Threatened Species [25]. The physiology, ecology, and molecular biology of this species have been extensively studied [26,27,28,29], but its vocalizations have not been studied. The Chinese soft-shelled turtle inhabits inland fresh water and is a highly aquatic turtle (spends most of its life in water). These turtles prefer to live in deep water, normally 1–2 m and they dive into the mud at the bottom of the water most of the time [29]. Thus, underwater vocalizations may be crucial for communication between individuals of the Chinese soft-shelled turtle species. Therefore, vocalization research can provide the basis for the study of the behaviors and ecology of Chinese soft-shelled turtles.
In this study, passive acoustic monitoring was used to investigate the diversity of underwater vocalizations of Chinese soft-shelled turtles in a laboratory. We aimed to classify calls and describe their acoustic characteristics. Moreover, we used a similarity test to verify the objectivity of classification. We also analyzed differences in vocalizations across sex and age groups.

2. Materials & Methods

2.1. Data Collection

Twenty-three healthy, captive Chinese soft-shelled turtles were used for the recordings: four adult females, four adult males, four subadult females, four subadult males, and seven hatchlings. These turtles live in semi-natural pools in a feedlot. We recorded the five groups of turtles. In addition, eight adult male and female turtles were mixed and recorded to determine whether there were any vocalizations associated with mating. The recordings were performed at Hainan Normal University in July 2021, in a professional soundproof room, free from environmental noise.
Turtles were recorded in an inflatable plastic circular tank, which effectively reduced the sound reflection. The diameter of the tank was 1.2 m, and the water depth was 30 cm. The turtles could move freely through the water, and the hydrophone was placed in the center of the tank, 15 cm below the surface. Vocalizations of turtles were recorded using a Song Meter SM4 underwater sound recorder (Wildlife Acoustics, Inc., Maynard, MA, USA) (omnidirectional hydrophone bandwidth: 2–30,000 Hz ± 3 dB; sensitivity: −165 dB Re: 1 V/μPa; sampling rate: 44,100 Hz; gain: 16 dB). The hydrophone was calibrated by the company (Wildlife Acoustics, Inc.) before purchase. Each group was continuously recorded for 24 h, and 144 h of data were obtained.

2.2. Data Analysis

Raven Pro 1.6 software (The Cornell Lab of Ornithology, USA) was used to visualize the waveforms and spectra of the recorded data. Sounds with spectral shapes similar to published turtle sounds [10,12,16] were selected by researchers with experience in turtle vocalizations. Only calls with a high signal-to-noise ratio were selected. Before the selection of turtle calls, we identified the sounds produced by the common behaviors of turtles in water based on studies of other freshwater turtles [12]. Turtle calls were classified based on differences in auditory sensing and spectrograms [10,12]. Two calls that showed extremely similar spectral properties and aural characteristics were classified as the same type. Sennheiser IE300 earphones (Sennheiser, Hanover, Germany) were used to analyze the data. The low frequency (lower limit of a call, Hz), high frequency (upper limit of a call, Hz), peak frequency (frequency of a call at its highest energy point, Hz), signal duration (the time difference between end time and start time of a call, ms), and number of harmonics of calls were extracted using the Raven Pro 1.6 software.
We chose the Pearson correlation coefficient to calculate the similarity between calls to judge the accuracy of the manual division [15,16]. This included the similarity between calls of different call types and similarity between calls of the same call type. Pearson’s linear correlation coefficient [30] was calculated using MATLAB R2021a (MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA, USA).

2.3. Statistical Analyses

The median and interquartile range (IQR) for all call types were calculated. We evaluated the differences in two acoustic parameters between age and sex groups: peak frequency and duration. First, the parameters of the sounds for each group were estimated using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test [31] to determine if they followed a normal distribution. If the distribution was normal (p > 0.05), the differences in the parameters of the calls among groups were tested using the least significant difference test (post hoc test algorithms); if not (p < 0.05), the differences were tested using Kruskal–Wallis analysis (non-parametric tests algorithms) [32]. Second, if the differences in the parameters of the calls among the groups were significant (p < 0.05), paired comparisons were conducted between every two groups to estimate the differences. Differences were considered statistically significant at p < 0.05. These analyses were completed using IBM SPSS Statistics 26.0 (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA).

3. Results

We found a total of 720 calls, of which 143 were produced by subadult males, 117 by subadult females, 192 by adult males, 133 by adult females, 135 by mixed-sex adults, and none by juveniles (Table 1). No courtship behavior was detected during the recording of the mixed-sex adult group. Turtle calls were classified into 10 types (Table 1 and Table 2 and Figure 1). Audio samples of each type are available in Supplementary Audio A–J. Acoustical parameters for each call are shown in Supplementary Table S1.

3.1. Description of Call Types

Ten call types were identified, of which five were high-frequency calls and five were low-frequency calls. The statistics of their acoustic parameters are listed in Table 2 and their spectra are shown in Figure 1.
Type A was a high-frequency call. It is a short vertical line on the spectrum, looking like a pulse (Figure 1A) with a very short duration. It was the most common call type of the Chinese soft-shelled turtle; more than half of them were produced by subadults.
Type B was also a high-frequency call. The spectrum is composed of two parts: the front is a vertical pulse signal, followed by a point signal (Figure 1B). This call type can be found in all age and sex groups, and more than half were produced by subadult males.
Type C is a high-frequency call that has a fan-shaped spectrum (Figure 1C), and its peak frequency is stable at approximately 14 kHz; Approximately half of the calls were emitted by adult females, but very few by adult males.
Type D is a high-frequency call with an oblique vertical line on the spectrum, the top half of which is vertical and the bottom half rightward (Figure 1D). It has a wide bandwidth. More than half of the calls were emitted by subadult females but none by subadult males.
Type E is a medium-high frequency call that appears M-shaped or inverted V-shaped on the spectrogram, with one or three inflection points (Figure 1E). This call has a long duration. Half of the calls were produced by subadult males.
Type F is a low-frequency call with a long duration. It appeared as a straight line in the spectrum (Figure 1F). This type occurred in all age and sex groups, with adult females accounting for more than 30 % of the calls.
Type G is a low-frequency call with 1–11 down-sweep harmonics on the spectrogram (Figure 1G) for a long duration. The peak frequency is stable at approximately 100 Hz. More than two-thirds of calls were produced by adult males, but none by adult females.
Type H is a low-frequency call, and the spectrum diagram shows 1–7 up-sweep harmonics with a turning point (Figure 1H). More than half of these calls were produced by adult males.
Type I is a low-frequency multi-harmonic call, whose spectrum diagram is a parallel horizontal line with 1–7 harmonics, most are 2–3 harmonics (Figure 1I). Most calls of this type were produced by adults, with very few produced by subadults. This call type is the most common type of harmonic call in Chinese soft-shell turtles.
Type J is a low-frequency multi-harmonic call, and the spectrum diagram shows a curved shape with 2–12 harmonics and multiple inflection points (Figure 1J). Its peak frequency is 100 Hz. Only 10 calls of this type were found, all emitted by adults.

3.2. Similarity Calculation

All the correlation coefficients between the calls within each call type were larger than 0.6, averaging 0.73, which means that the similarity between calls in each call type was high, and the difference was small. All the correlation coefficients between the 10 call types were lower than 0.22, which means that the call similarity between each type was low, and the difference was large. Thus, the manual classification of calls by Chinese soft-shell turtles had high reliability. The specific correlation coefficients are presented in Table 3.

3.3. Differences in Vocalizations between Sexes and Age Groups

3.3.1. Peak Frequency

Three of the call types (Types A, C, and I) showed significant differences in the peak frequencies among the five groups. There is no significant differences in their peak frequencies among groups in other call types (Figure 2; Supplementary Table S2).
In Type A, the peak frequency of subadult males showed significantly higher than that in the other groups (p < 0.05). No significant differences in peak frequency were detected between subadult females, adult males, and mixed-sex adults (p > 0.05).
In Type C, subadult females showed a significantly higher peak frequency than mixed-sex adults (p < 0.05). There were no significant differences among the subadult males, adult males, and adult females (p > 0.05).
In Type I, adult females showed significantly higher peak frequencies than adult males and mixed-sex adults (p < 0.05). There were no significant differences among subadult males, subadult females, adult males, and mixed-sex adults. (p > 0.05). In addition, there were no significant differences between the adult females and subadult groups (p > 0.05).

3.3.2. Duration

Seven call types (Types A, B, C, F, G, H, and I) showed significant differences in call duration among the five groups. (Figure 3; Supplementary Table S3).
In type A, adult females and males showed significantly longer call duration than subadult males and mixed-sex adults (p < 0.05). There were no significant differences between adult females and males, between subadult males, subadult females and mixed-sex adults (p > 0.05).
In Type B, the adult female group had a significantly longer call duration than the subadult male group (p < 0.05). No significant differences were detected between adult females and adult males or between subadult females and subadult males (p > 0.05).
In Type C, adult females showed significantly longer call durations than subadult males and subadult females (p < 0.05). There were no significant differences between adult females and adult males or between subadult females and subadult males (p > 0.05). Mixed-sex adults showed no significant differences from other groups (p > 0.05).
In Type F, adult females showed a significantly longer call duration than mixed-sex adults (p < 0.05). No significant differences were detected among the other groups (p > 0.05).
In Type G, adult males had a significantly longer call duration than mixed-sex adults (p < 0.05). No significant differences were detected among the other groups (p > 0.05).
In Types H and I, adult males presented significantly longer call durations than mixed-sex adults (p < 0.05). No significant differences were detected among the other groups (p > 0.05).

4. Discussion

4.1. Vocalization Diversity

The Chinese striped-neck turtle (M. sinensis), a sympatric species of the Chinese soft-shelled turtle, can produce 10 call types (three types are harmonics) [18]. Of these call types, only one is a high-frequency pulse call type. In contrast, Chinese soft-shelled turtle can produce four high-frequency pulse call types, with much higher peak frequencies than those of Chinese striped-neck turtles. Both species vocalize down-sweep harmonics, but the peak frequency and high frequency of the Chinese striped-neck turtle’s harmonics are higher than those of the Chinese soft-shelled turtle (the harmonics of the Chinese striped-neck turtle can reach 7 kHz, while the harmonics of the Chinese soft-shelled turtle are below 2 kHz). In addition, the Chinese striped-neck turtle has a low-frequency call type that is very similar to call type F of the Chinese soft-shelled turtle (spectrogram and auditory), but the peak frequency of Chinese soft-shelled turtles is approximately twice as high as that of Chinese striped-neck turtles. Whether the very similar vocalizations between the two species are due to their similar living environments and whether they have the same communication function requires further study.
Of the 12 call types produced by the red-eared turtle (T. scripta elegans), another sympatric species of Chinese soft-shelled turtle, none resemble those produced by the Chinese soft-shelled turtle [12]. Among the harmonic calls of long-necked turtles (Chelodina oblonga), three call types are very similar to call types G, I, and J of Chinese soft-shelled turtles [10]. Although the habitats of the two species do not overlap, they are both carnivorous and often dive into the sediments of water [10,25]. This finding may be explained by their similar lifestyles.
Many tortoises, freshwater turtles, and sea turtles can vocalize, and most have been shown to produce multi-harmonic calls [2,3,6,10,11,13,33]. Chinese soft-shelled turtles can produce 10 call types underwater, four of which are multi-harmonic calls. Many turtle species can produce multi-harmonic calls, which may be determined by the similar larynx structure of turtles [34]. The structure and position of the larynx of the turtle are similar to those of the mammalian vocal cords, and the two diverticula held by the cricoid might function as low-frequency resonating chambers, improving the harmonic structure of tortoise calls [21,35]. Similar to the Chinese soft-shelled turtle, other identified turtle species with more call types and more harmonic calls are highly aquatic species that prefer to live in deep water, such as P. expansa (11 call types, six of which are harmonics) and C. oblonga (17 call types, nine of which are harmonics) [10,36]. These species might have developed a greater variety to adapt to dim underwater environments.
The matched filter hypothesis proposes that tuning of the receiver’s auditory sensitivity will evolve to closely match the dominant frequency of species-specific calls [37]. Several studies on insects, fish, birds, and anurans have verified this hypothesis [38,39]. However, because of the influences of natural and sexual selection, a mismatched relationship might arise between the sender’s signals and the receiver’s auditory sensitivity [40,41]. Existing research has revealed that the hearing range of turtles is concentrated at low-frequencies [42,43,44,45]; however, aquatic turtle species have been found to produce high-frequency calls that can reach 6000–15,000 Hz [6,11,12,46]. The Chinese soft-shelled turtle can produce high-frequency calls underwater, similar to its sympatric red-eared turtle (T. scripta elegans) in China [12]. The advantage of high-frequency calls is that they help resist interference from low-frequency noise in communication, since natural waters are full of low-frequency noise [47]. For instance, frogs (Odorrana tormota and O. graminea) and birds (Abroscopus albogularis) inhabiting aquatic environments produce calls containing ultrasonic components to avoid masking by wideband noise [48,49,50]. As there have been no studies on the hearing threshold of Chinese soft-shelled turtles, we cannot be sure whether they can hear such high calls or whether these high-frequency calls have communication functions. In some reptiles, high-frequency calls are often used to intimidate heterospecific intruders but are not heard by conspecifics [51,52,53,54].
Vocalization has been found to change with age in bats (Myotis nattereri bombinus and Rhinolophus ferrumequinum) [55,56], and this kind of change for Lonchura striata swinhoei was shown to be associated with the development of neural modulation mechanisms [57]. The differences in vocalization and auditory sensitivity between Lusitanian toadfish (Halobatrachus didactylus) juveniles and adults demonstrates that acoustic communication may be absent in their early developmental stages [58]. As Melopsitiacus undelafusp grows, its calls become more complex and higher in amplitude; as they grow, male birds have more complex calls than female birds [59]. American Coots (Fulica americana) change their call types as they grow [60]. The harmonic number of the song of the lesser horseshoe bat (R. cornutus) increases with age after birth [61]. In rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), the pitch of the “cooing” drops and the amplitude becomes more constant as they age [62]. Green sea turtle (C. mydas) hatchlings produce only four call types, whereas subadults can emit 11 call types [13]. In this study, juvenile Chinese soft-shelled turtles were not found to produce vocalizations; the subadults produced a large number of high-frequency calls, but only a very small number of low-frequency harmonic call types; and adults produce a more balanced number of different types of calls. This shows a tendency for the vocalization of Chinese soft-shelled turtles to become more diverse with age. Whether this phenomenon is related to the degree of development of the physiological function of the vocal organs requires further study.

4.2. Sex and Age Differences in Vocalization

Diverse vocalizations among individuals in an animal population are often caused by differences in the structure of the vocal organs [63,64]. A larger body size usually indicates a larger vocal organ that produces lower frequencies [65,66]. Many birds and frogs have verified that their dominant frequencies of vocalization are inversely related to body size [67,68,69]. The vocal frequency of tortoises is found inversely proportional to their size [4]. In our recordings, the Chinese soft-shelled subadults had higher peak frequencies and shorter durations in two high-frequency call types. This may be because the size of subadults is smaller than that of adults, or it may be due to age differences in the neural modulation of vocalizations [52].
However, the frequency of animal calls is not always inversely related to their body size. In several owl species, females produce higher calls than males, but females are larger than males [70]. Male baboons (Papio cynocephalus), gibbons (Hylobates lar), and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) are larger than their respective females but have higher peak frequencies than their females in some call types [71,72,73]. Jacana spinosa is larger than J. jacana but emits higher frequency calls than J. jacana [64]. Adult females of red-eared turtles (T. scripta elegans) are much larger than adult males of the same age but produce more high-frequency calls than adult males [12]. For call type I, which was the most frequent low-frequency call, the peak frequencies of adult females were significantly higher than that of adult males; however, the body sizes of the two groups were similar. For type A, there was also a difference in the peak frequency between male and female subadults of similar body size. Recent research has revealed that freshwater turtles show differences in sexually dimorphic hearing sensitivity [40], which is associated with the amount of gene expression in neural inhibitory pathways for hub genes related to the inner ear and tympanic membrane [74]. Hence, sex differences in hearing sensitivity and auditory genes may be one reason for sex differences in vocalization.
The potential function of turtle calls remains unclear, but they may be important for inter-specific communication [12]. Research has shown that individual-specific vocalizations are used for the individual recognition of sea turtles [13]. It must be noted that because these calls were recorded in an artificial environment, the sound descriptions might not completely reflect free-field recordings [10].

5. Conclusions

We identified 720 underwater calls of Chinese soft-shelled turtles in recordings and divided them into 10 call types. These results provide basic data for other bioacoustics and evolutionary studies of Chinese soft-shelled turtles. Chinese soft-shelled turtles have a high vocal diversity and many harmonic calls, indicating that they may have developed a greater variety to adapt to dim underwater environments. In addition, Chinese soft-shelled turtles show a tendency for vocalization to become more diverse with age. The peak frequency of calls in Chinese soft-shelled turtles was significantly different between adult females and males and between subadults and adults. These findings suggest that further studies on the ecology and anatomy of Chinese soft-shelled turtle should be conducted to explain the mechanisms behind these phenomena. Further behavioral and auditory studies are needed to determine the intra-specific communication function and ecological implications of Chinese soft-shelled turtle vocalizations.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ani13050812/s1, Table S1: Acoustic parameters of each call produced by Chinese soft-shelled turtles; Table S2: Difference analysis of peak frequencies for 10 call types between five groups (Kruskal-Wallis tests); Table S3: Difference analysis of call durations for 10 call types between five groups (Kruskal-Wallis tests); Audio A: Sample of call type A; Audio B: Sample of call type B; Audio C: Sample of call type C; Audio D: Sample of call type D; Audio E: Sample of call type E; Audio F: Sample of call type F; Audio G: Sample of call type G; Audio H: Sample of call type H; Audio I: Sample of call type I; Audio J: Sample of call type J.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, L.Z., H.S. and J.W.; methodology, L.Z. and J.W.; software, L.Z.; formal analysis, L.Z.; investigation, L.Z., J.L., X.Z. and N.L.; resources, L.Z., H.S. and J.W.; data curation, L.Z. and J.W.; writing—original draft preparation, L.Z.; writing—review and editing, L.Z. and J.W.; visualization, L.Z.; supervision, J.W.; project administration, L.Z. and J.W.; funding acquisition, L.Z. and J.W. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Natural Science Foundation of Hainan Province under Grant No. 2019RC175, Postdoctoral Research Project of Hainan Province under Grant No. RC2100007855 and the Academician Working Platform Project in Hainan Province.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The research was reviewed and approved by the Animal Research Ethics Committee of the Hainan Provincial Education Centre for Ecology and Environment, Hainan Normal University (HNECEE-2021-002).

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The data set of vocal parameters of the turtles used in this study can be found in Supplementary Table S1.

Acknowledgments

We thank the graduate students Yuanyu Qing and Shuo Cui at Hainan Normal University for their substantial assistance during data recording. We thank Liang Tao from Nanjing Forestry University and Haoran Ji from Hunan University for their help with the data analysis.

Conflicts of Interest

All authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Shang, Y.C. Animal Behavior, 2nd ed.; Peking University Press: Beijing, China, 2014; pp. 256–267. [Google Scholar]
  2. Sacchi, R.; Galeotti, P.; Fasola, M.; Ballasina, D. Vocalizations and courtship intensity correlate with mounting success in marginated tortoises Testudo marginata. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2003, 55, 95–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Galeotti, P.; Sacchi, R.; Fasola, M.; Ballasina, D. Do mounting vocalisations in tortoises have a communication function? A comparative analysis. Br. J. Herpetol. 2005, 15, 61–71. [Google Scholar]
  4. Ferrara, C.R.; Mortimer, J.A.; Vogt, R.C. First evidence that hatchlings of Chelonia mydas emit sounds. Copeia 2014, 2, 245–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Galeotti, P.; Sacchi, R.; Rosa, D.P.; Fasola, M. Female preference for fast-rate, high-pitched calls in Hermann’s tortoises (Testudo hermanni). Behav. Ecol. 2005, 16, 301–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Monteiro, C.C.; Carmo, H.M.; Santos, A.J.; Corso, G.; Sousa-Lima, R.S. First record of bioacoustic emission in embryos and hatchlings of hawksbill sea turtles (Eretmochelys imbricata). Chelonian Conserv. Biol. 2019, 18, 273–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Ferrara, C.R.; Vogt, R.C.; Giles, J.C.; Kuchling, G. Chelonian vocal communication. In Biocommunication of Animals; Witzany, G., Ed.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2014; pp. 261–274. [Google Scholar]
  8. Chen, Z.; Wiens, J.J. The origins of acoustic communication in vertebrates. Nat. Commun. 2020, 11, 369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Jorgewich-Cohen, G.; Townsend, S.W.; Padovese, L.R.; Klein, N.; Praschag, P.; Ferrara, C.R.; Ettmar, S.; Menezes, S.; Varani, A.P.; Serano, J.; et al. Common evolutionary origin of acoustic communication in choanate vertebrates. Nat. Commun. 2022, 13, 6089. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Giles, J.C.; Davis, J.A.; Mccauley, R.D.; Kuchling, G. Voice of the turtle: The underwater acoustic repertoire of the long-necked freshwater turtle, Chelodina oblonga. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2009, 126, 434–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Ferrara, C.R.; Vogt, R.C.; Sousa-Lima, R.S.; Lenz, A.; Morales-Mávil, J.E. Sound communication in embryos and hatchlings of Lepidochelys kempii. Chelonian Conserv. Biol. 2019, 18, 279–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Zhou, L.; Zhao, L.; Li, H.; Wang, T.; Shi, H.; Wang, J. Underwater vocalizations of Trachemys scripta elegans and their differences among sex–age groups. Front. Ecol. Evol. 2022, 10, 1022052. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Carr, A.F. Handbook of Turtles; Comstock Publishing Associates: New York, NY, USA, 1995; pp. 122–134. [Google Scholar]
  14. Lutcavage, M.E.; Lutz, P.L. Diving physiology. In The Biology of Sea Turtles; Lutz, P.L., Musick, J.A., Eds.; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 1997; pp. 277–296. [Google Scholar]
  15. Cook, S.L.; Forrest, T.G. Sounds produced by nesting leatherback sea turtles (Dermochelys coriacea). Herpetol. Rev. 2005, 36, 387–390. [Google Scholar]
  16. Ferrara, C.R.; Vogt, R.C.; Eisemberg, C.C.; Doody, J.S. First evidence of the pig-nosed turtle (Carettochelys insculpta) vocalizing underwater. Copeia 2017, 105, 29–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Ferrara, C.R.; Vogt, R.; Sousa-Lima, R.; Tardio, B.M.R.; Bernardes, V.G.D. Sound Communication and Social Behavior in an Amazonian River Turtle (Podocnemis expansa). Herpetologica 2014, 70, 149–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Zhou, L.; Lei, J.; Zhai, X.; Shi, H.; Wang, J. Chinese striped-neck turtles vocalize underwater and show differences in peak frequency among different age and sex groups. PeerJ 2022, 10, e14628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Charrier, I.; Jeantet, L.; Maucourt, L.; Régis, S.; Lecerf, N.; Benhalilou, A.; Chevallier, D. First evidence of underwater vocalizations in green sea turtles Chelonia mydas. Endanger. Species Res. 2022, 48, 31–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Gans, C.; Maderson, P.F.A. Sound producing mechanisms in recent reptiles: Review and comment. Integr. Comp. Biol. 1973, 13, 1195–1203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Sacchi, R.; Galeotti, P.; Fasola, M.; Gerzeli, G. Larynx morphology and sound production in three species of testudinidae. J. Morphol. 2004, 261, 175–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Manolakis, D.G.; Ingle, V.K.; Kogon, S.M. Statistical and Adaptive Signal Processing: Spectral Estimation, Signal Modeling, Adaptive Filtering and Array Processing (Artech House Signal Processing Library); Artech House: Norwood, UK, 2005; pp. 111–113. [Google Scholar]
  23. Zheng, J. Signals and Systems (Volume 1), 3rd ed.; Higher Education Press: Beijing, China, 2011; pp. 165–176. [Google Scholar]
  24. Wang, K.; Ren, J.; Chen, H.; Lyu, Z.; Guo, X.; Jiang, K.; Chen, J.; Li, J.; Guo, P.; Wang, Y.; et al. The updated checklists of amphibians and reptiles of China. Biodivers. Sci. 2020, 28, 189–218. [Google Scholar]
  25. IUCN Red List. Available online: https://www.iucnredlist.org/species/39620/97401140 (accessed on 22 December 2022).
  26. Yamada, K.; Nishida-Umehar, C.; Matsuda, Y. Molecular and cytogenetic characterization of site-specific repetitive DNA sequences in the Chinese soft-shelled turtle (Pelodiscus sinensis, Trionychidae). Chromosome Res. 2005, 13, 33–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Li, H.; Liu, J.; Xiong, L.; Zhang, H.; Zhou, H.; Yin, H.; Jing, W.; Li, J.; Shi, Q.; Wang, Y.; et al. Phylogenetic relationships and divergence dates of softshell turtles (Testudines: Trionychidae) inferred from complete mitochondrial genomes. J. Evol. Biol. 2017, 30, 1011–1023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Zhu, Q.; Kong, F.; Shi, H. Nesting activity of the Chinese softshell turtle, Pelodiscus sinensis, on the Yellow River, North western China. Chelonian Conserv. Biol. 2021, 20, 290–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Tang, Z.; Liu, S.; Niu, C. Effects of Extreme Light Cycle and Density on Melatonin, Appetite, and Energy Metabolism of the Soft-Shelled Turtle (Pelodiscus sinensis). Biology 2022, 11, 965. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Gibbons, J.D. Nonparametric Statistical Inference, 2nd ed.; Marcel Dekker: New York, NY, USA, 1985; pp. 47–54. [Google Scholar]
  31. Dallal, G.E.; Wilkinson, L. An analytic approximation to the distribution of Lilliefor’s test statistic for normality. Am Stat. 1986, 40, 294–296. [Google Scholar]
  32. Siegel, S. Nonparametric Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences; McGraw-Hill: New York, NY, USA, 1956; pp. 122–134. [Google Scholar]
  33. Ferrara, C.R.; Vogt, R.C.; Pappas, M. Emydoidea Blandingii (blanding’s Turtle) vocalizations. Herpetol. Rev. 2018, 49, 526–527. [Google Scholar]
  34. Russell, A.P.; Bauer, A.M. Vocalization by extant non-avian reptiles: A synthetic overview of phonation and the vocal apparatus. Anat. Rec. 2021, 304, 1478–1528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Göppert, E. Larynx and trachea. In Handbook of Comparative Anatomy of Vertebrates (Vol. 3); Bolk, L., Göppert, E., Kallius, E., Lubosch, W., Eds.; Urban and Schwarzenberg: Berlin, Germany, 1937; pp. 797–866. [Google Scholar]
  36. Ferrara, C.R.; Vogt, R.C.; Sousa-Lima, R.S. Turtle Vocalizations as the First Evidence of Posthatching Parental Care in Chelonians. J. Comp. Psychol. 2013, 127, 24–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Capranica, R.R.; Moffat, A.J.M. Neurobehavioral correlates of sound communication in anurans. In Advances in Vertebrate Neuroethology; Ewert, J.P., Ed.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1983; pp. 701–730. [Google Scholar]
  38. Gerhardt, H.C.; Huber, F. Acoustic Communication in Insects and Anurans: Common Problems and Diverse Solutions; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2002; pp. 122–130. [Google Scholar]
  39. Rogers, L.J.; Kaplan, G.T. Songs, Roars, and Rituals: Communication in Birds, Mammals, and Other Animals; Harvard University Press: Boston, MA, USA, 2000; pp. 103–112. [Google Scholar]
  40. Ryan, M.J.; Perrill, S.A.; Wilczynski, W. Auditory tuning and call frequency predict population- based mating preferences in the cricket frog, Acris crepitans. Am. Nat. 1992, 139, 1370–1383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Wilczynski, W.; Rand, A.S.; Ryan, M.J. Evolution of calls and auditory tuning in the Physalaemus pustulosus species group. Brain Behav. Evol. 2001, 58, 137–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Christensen-Dalsgaard, J.; Brandt, C.; Willis, K.L.; Christensen, C.B.; Ketten, D.; Edds-Walton, P.; Fay, R.R.; Madsen, P.T.; Carr, C.E. Specialization for underwater hearing by the tympanic middle ear of the turtle, Trachemys scripta elegans. Proc. Royal Soc. B 2012, 279, 2816–2824. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Piniak, W.E.D.; Eckert, S.A.; Harms, C.A.; Stringer, E.M. Underwater Hearing Sensitivity of the Leatherback Sea Turtle (Dermochelys coriacea): Assessing the Potential Effect of Anthropogenic Noise; U.S. Department of the Interior and Bureau of Ocean Energy Management: Herndon, VA, USA, 2012; pp. 8–13. [Google Scholar]
  44. Piniak, W.E.D.; Mann, D.A.; Eckert, S.A.; Harms, C.A. Amphibious hearing in sea turtles. In The Effects of Noise on Aquatic Life; Popper, A.N., Hawkins, A., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2012; pp. 83–87. [Google Scholar]
  45. Wang, T.; Li, H.; Cui, J.; Zhai, X.; Shi, H.; Wang, J. Auditory brainstem responses in the red-eared slider Trachemys scripta elegans (Testudoformes: Emydidae) reveal sexually dimorphic hearing sensitivity. J. Comp. Physiol. A 2019, 205, 847–854. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. McKenna, L.N.; Paladino, F.V.; Tomillo, P.S.; Robinson, N.J. Do Sea Turtles Vocalize to Synchronize Hatching or Nest Emergence? Copeia 2019, 107, 120–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Zhou, L.; Chen, X.; Duan, P.; Wang, D.; Wang, Z.; Wang, K. Spatial–temporal variations in biosonar activity of Yangtze finless porpoise in the lower reaches of the Yangtze River and its correlation with underwater noise: Are quieter non-shipping branches the remaining shelters? Aquat. Conserv. 2021, 31, 964–978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Feng, A.S.; Narins, P.M.; Xu, C.H.; Lin, W.Y.; Yu, Z.L.; Qiu, Q.; Xu, Z.; Shen, J. Ultrasonic communication in frogs. Nature 2006, 440, 333–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Narins, P.M.; Feng, A.S.; Lin, W.; Schnitzler, H.; Denzinger, A.; Suthers, R.A.; Xu, C. Old World frog and bird vocalizations contain prominent ultrasonic harmonics. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2004, 115, 910–913. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  50. Shen, J.X.; Xu, Z.M.; Feng, A.S.; Narins, P.M. Large odorous frogs (Odorrana graminea) produce ultrasonic calls. J. Comp. Physiol. A 2011, 119, 1027–1030. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Young, B.A.; Nejman, N.; Meltzer, K.; Marvin, J. The mechanics of sound production in the puff adder Bitis arietans (Serpentes: Viperidae) and the information content of the snake hiss. J. Exp. Biol. 1999, 202, 2281–2289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Young, B.A. Snake bioacoustics: Toward a richer understanding of the behavioral ecology of snakes. Q. Rev. Biol. 2003, 78, 303–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Todd, N.P.M. Estimated source intensity and active space of the American alligator (Alligator Mississippiensis) vocal display. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2007, 122, 2906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Wang, X.; Wang, D.; Wu, X.; Wang, C.; Wang, R.; Xia, T. Response Specificity to Advertisement Vocalization in the Chinese Alligator (Alligator sinensis). Ethology 2009, 115, 832–839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Yamasaki, M.; Matsumura, S. Development of sounds and mother-infant communication in the Natterer’s bat, Myotis nattereri bombinus. Bull. Akiyoshi-Dai Mus. Nat. Hist. 2004, 39, 23–36. [Google Scholar]
  56. Liu, Y.; Feng, J.; Jiang, Y.L.; Wu, L.; Sun, K.P. Vocalization development of greater horseshoe bat, Rhinolophus ferrumequinum (Rhinolophidae, Chiroptera). Folia Zool. 2007, 56, 126–136. [Google Scholar]
  57. Zuo, M.; Zeng, S.; Peng, W.; Zhang, X. Neural development of vocal behavior in striated mannikin (Lonchura striata swinhoei). Acta Zool. Sin. 2002, 48, 50–57. [Google Scholar]
  58. Vasconcelos, R.O.; Ladich, F. Development of vocalization, auditory sensitivity and acoustic communication in the Lusitanian toadfish Halobatrachus didactylus. J. Exp. Biol. 2008, 211, 502–509. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  59. Zhang, H.; Dong, Y. The Study on Acoustic Behavior in the Melopsitiacus Undelafusp of Different ages. J. Shanxi Norm. Univ. Sci. Ed. 2007, 21, 93–95. [Google Scholar]
  60. Cosens, S.E. Development of vocalizations in the American Coot. Can. J. Zool. 1981, 59, 1921–1928. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Funakoshi, K.; Nomura, E.; Matsukubo, M.; Wakita, Y. Postnatal Growth and Vocalization Development of the Lesser Horseshoe Bat, Rhinolophus cornutus, in the Kyushu District, Japan. Mamm. Study 2010, 35, 65–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Hammerschmidt, K.; Newman, J.D.; Champoux, M.; Suomi, S.J. Changes in rhesus macaque “Coo” vocalizations during Early Development. Ethology 2000, 106, 873–886. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Tobias, M.L.; Kelley, D.B. Vocalizations by a sexually dimorphic isolated larynx: Peripheral constraints on behavioral expression. J. Neurosci. 1987, 7, 3191–3197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Sathyan, R.; Engelbrecht, A.; Couldridge, V.C.K. Morphological, acoustic and genetic divergence in the bladder grasshopper Bullacris unicolor. Ecol. Evol. 2017, 29, 552–573. [Google Scholar]
  65. Narins, P.M.; Feng, A.S.; Fay, R.R. Hearing and Sound Communication in Amphibians (Vol. 28); Springer Science and Business Media: New York, NY, USA, 2006; pp. 233–236. [Google Scholar]
  66. Garcia, M.; Charlton, B.D.; Wyman, M.T.; Fitch, W.T.; Reby, D. Do Red Deer Stags (Cervus elaphus) Use Roar Fundamental Frequency (F0) to Assess Rivals? PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e83946. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Bowman, R.I. The evolution of song in Darwin’s finches. In Patterns of Evolution in Galapagos Organisms; Bowman, R.I., Berson, M., Leviton, A.E., Eds.; Association for the Advancement of Science: San Francisco, CA, USA, 1983; pp. 237–537. [Google Scholar]
  68. Ryan, M.J. The Tungara Frog; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 1985; pp. 34–43. [Google Scholar]
  69. Buck, E.J.; Monroe, T.; Zwicky, G.; Derryberry, E.P.; Lipshutz, S.E. Species and sex differences in vocalizations between sex-role reversed shorebirds, Northern Jacana (Jacana spinosa) and Wattled Jacana (J. jacana). Wilson J. Ornithol. 2020, 132, 343–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Miller, A.H. The vocal apparatus of North American owls. Condor 1934, 36, 204–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Fischer, J.; Kitchen, D.M.; Seyfarth, R.M.; Cheney, D.L. Baboon loud calls advertise male quality: Acoustic features and their relation to rank, age, and exhaustion. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2004, 56, 140–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Barelli, C.; Mundry, R.; Heistermann, M.; Hammerschmidt, K. Cues to androgens and quality in male gibbon songs. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e82748. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Ammie, K.K. Evidence for sexual dimorphism in chimpanzee vocalizations: A comparison of male and female call production and acoustic parameters. In The Chimpanzees of the Taï Forest 40 Years of Research; Boesch, C., Wittig, R., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2019; pp. 410–421. [Google Scholar]
  74. Lu, N.; Chen, B.; Qing, J.; Lei, J.; Wang, T.; Shi, H.; Wang, J. Transcriptome analyses provide insights into the auditory function in Trachemys scripta elegans. Animals 2022, 12, 2410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Spectrogram and waveform views of underwater sounds produced by Pelodiscus sinensis. All spectrograms were obtained with Raven Pro 1.5 using Hamming windows with 1024 pt fast Fourier transform. The waveforms were drawn in MATLAB R2021a. Capital letters A–J represent call types A–J.
Figure 1. Spectrogram and waveform views of underwater sounds produced by Pelodiscus sinensis. All spectrograms were obtained with Raven Pro 1.5 using Hamming windows with 1024 pt fast Fourier transform. The waveforms were drawn in MATLAB R2021a. Capital letters A–J represent call types A–J.
Animals 13 00812 g001
Figure 2. Differences analysis in peak frequencies between five groups. Capital letters A–J represent call types A–J. Boxplots with different lowercase letters indicate significant results (p < 0.05). Boxplots with the same lowercase letters indicate no significant difference (p > 0.05). When boxplots are marked with only one letter, the boxplot marked “a” is significantly higher than the boxplot marked “b” in a call type (p < 0.05). Call types without letters on the boxplots means no significant differences between groups. Groups: 1 represents subadult males, 2 represents subadult females, 3 represents adult males, 4 represents adult females and 5 represents mixed-sex adults.
Figure 2. Differences analysis in peak frequencies between five groups. Capital letters A–J represent call types A–J. Boxplots with different lowercase letters indicate significant results (p < 0.05). Boxplots with the same lowercase letters indicate no significant difference (p > 0.05). When boxplots are marked with only one letter, the boxplot marked “a” is significantly higher than the boxplot marked “b” in a call type (p < 0.05). Call types without letters on the boxplots means no significant differences between groups. Groups: 1 represents subadult males, 2 represents subadult females, 3 represents adult males, 4 represents adult females and 5 represents mixed-sex adults.
Animals 13 00812 g002
Figure 3. Differences analysis of call durations between five groups. Capital letters A–J represent call types A–J. Boxplots with different lowercase letters indicate significant results (p < 0.05). Boxplots with the same lowercase letters indicate no significant difference (p > 0.05). When boxplots are marked with only one letter, the boxplot marked “a” is significantly higher than the boxplot marked “b” in a call type (p < 0.05). Call types without letters on the boxplots means no significant differences between groups. Groups: 1 represents subadult males, 2 represents subadult females, 3 represents adult males, 4 represents adult females and 5 represents mixed-sex adults.
Figure 3. Differences analysis of call durations between five groups. Capital letters A–J represent call types A–J. Boxplots with different lowercase letters indicate significant results (p < 0.05). Boxplots with the same lowercase letters indicate no significant difference (p > 0.05). When boxplots are marked with only one letter, the boxplot marked “a” is significantly higher than the boxplot marked “b” in a call type (p < 0.05). Call types without letters on the boxplots means no significant differences between groups. Groups: 1 represents subadult males, 2 represents subadult females, 3 represents adult males, 4 represents adult females and 5 represents mixed-sex adults.
Animals 13 00812 g003
Table 1. Number of call types produced by Pelodiscus sinensis in each group.
Table 1. Number of call types produced by Pelodiscus sinensis in each group.
TypeAdult
Females
Ratio
(%)
Adult
Males
Ratio
(%)
Subadult
Females
Ratio
(%)
Subadult
Males
Ratio
(%)
Mixed-Sex
Adults
Ratio
(%)
Sum
A2711.49218.946527.666929.365322.55235
B822.2225.56513.891952.7825.5636
C2649.0611.89611.321528.3059.4353
D310.00620.001653.33 516.6730
E39.38412.50515.631650.00412.5032
F2730.681415.911618.181921.591213.6488
G 0.006977.5322.2533.371516.8589
H26.251959.3813.1313.13928.1332
I3429.575245.2210.8710.872723.48115
J330.00440.00 330.0010
Total133 192 117 143 135 720
Note: This ratio is equal to the number of calls of a call type made by a group divided by the total number of calls of that call type.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics of the acoustic parameters of all call types.
Table 2. Descriptive statistics of the acoustic parameters of all call types.
TypeLow Frequency Median ± IQR
(Hz)
High Frequency Median ± IQR
(Hz)
Peak Frequency Median ± IQR
(Hz)
Duration Median ± IQR
(ms)
Bandwidth (Hz) No. of
Harmonics
A10,656 ± 621811,567 ± 632410,745 ± 618031.57 ± 30.45689 ± 947
B13,230 ± 581115,749 ± 192914,729 ± 355395.58 ± 92.171497 ± 1975
C13,714 ± 224814,539 ± 181114,320 ± 2068122.86 ± 108.89538 ± 527
D8572 ± 632310,262 ± 50939055 ± 601349.15 ± 32.49722 ± 1098
E6506 ± 59397086 ± 58366406 ± 3726137.92 ± 102.55367 ± 333
F331 ± 303.25426 ± 334366 ± 367244.76 ± 186.7886 ± 43
G86 ± 56385 ± 228129 ± 108284.08 ± 240.57129 ± 861~11
H275 ± 162721 ± 503345 ± 210182.43 ± 264.17129 ± 1831~7
I151 ± 158424 ± 405215 ± 172327.62 ± 275.5186 ± 211~7
J84 ± 451058 ± 1039140 ± 54231.95 ± 232.79216 ± 2152~12
Note: “High frequency” indicates the upper limit of the frequency for a call type; “low frequency” indicates the lower limit of the frequency for a call type; “IQR” is the interquartile range of the call type.
Table 3. Average value of correlation coefficients for calls within each call type and between call types.
Table 3. Average value of correlation coefficients for calls within each call type and between call types.
Call TypesABCDEFGHIJ
Average value of correlation coefficients between call types A0.71
B0.16 0.77
C0.15 0.21 0.78
D0.15 0.22 0.21 0.71
E0.12 0.15 0.17 0.17 0.80
F0.14 0.18 0.20 0.20 0.16 0.77
G0.11 0.15 0.15 0.16 0.12 0.14 0.77
H0.10 0.14 0.15 0.14 0.11 0.13 0.11 0.60
I0.11 0.15 0.16 0.15 0.13 0.15 0.11 0.10 0.71
J0.14 0.18 0.19 0.19 0.15 0.17 0.14 0.12 0.14 0.63
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Zhou, L.; Lei, J.; Zhai, X.; Lu, N.; Shi, H.; Wang, J. Diversity of Underwater Vocalizations in Chinese Soft-Shelled Turtle (Pelodiscus sinensis). Animals 2023, 13, 812. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13050812

AMA Style

Zhou L, Lei J, Zhai X, Lu N, Shi H, Wang J. Diversity of Underwater Vocalizations in Chinese Soft-Shelled Turtle (Pelodiscus sinensis). Animals. 2023; 13(5):812. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13050812

Chicago/Turabian Style

Zhou, Lu, Jinhong Lei, Xiaofei Zhai, Ningning Lu, Haitao Shi, and Jichao Wang. 2023. "Diversity of Underwater Vocalizations in Chinese Soft-Shelled Turtle (Pelodiscus sinensis)" Animals 13, no. 5: 812. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13050812

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop