Next Article in Journal
A Comparative Analysis of Metabolic Profiles of Embryonic Skeletal Muscle from Lantang and Landrace Pigs
Next Article in Special Issue
Behavioral Assessment of Six Reptile Species during a Temporary Zoo Closure and Reopening
Previous Article in Journal
Dead Brood of Apis mellifera Is Removed More Effectively from Small-Cell Combs Than from Standard-Cell Combs
Previous Article in Special Issue
Use of Blood Lactate in Assessment of Manual Capture Techniques of Zoo-Housed Crocodilians
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Investigating the Behavior and Personality Structure of the Aldabra Tortoise during Human Interactions and Training Events

by
Giovanni Quintavalle Pastorino
1,*,
Vanessa Smith
1,
Massimo Faustini
2,
Eleonora Bonacina
2,
Davide Guadagnini
3,
Roberto Robbiati
3,
Alice Cavalleri
1,
James Edward Brereton
4 and
Richard Preziosi
5
1
Zoo Biology, Manchester Metropolitan University, All Saints Building, All Saints, Manchester M15 6BH, UK
2
Università degli studi di Milano, Via Festa del Perdono, 7, 20122 Milan, Italy
3
Parco faunistico Le Cornelle, Via Cornelle, 16, 24030 Valbrembo, Italy
4
Animal and Zoo Science, Higher Education, University Centre Sparsholt, Westley Lane, Sparsholt, Winchester SO21 2NF, UK
5
Higher Education, University of Plymouth, Drake Circus, Plymouth PL4 8AA, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2022, 12(4), 419; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12040419
Submission received: 2 January 2022 / Revised: 29 January 2022 / Accepted: 8 February 2022 / Published: 10 February 2022

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

There are many studies that suggest that a range of animal species have personalities, and that animals can benefit from interactions with their human caregivers. However, many of these previous studies have focused only on mammals, with fewer studies focusing on reptiles. Research was undertaken at the Faunistic Park Le Cornelle, Italy, to investigate the effects of approach tests and food interaction events on 5 male and 5 female Aldabra tortoises’ (Aldrabrachelys gigantea) behavior. There were differences in behavior between tortoises, and in response to different types of events. The tortoises also responded differently to their own keepers, vets, or unfamiliar people. The personality of the tortoises were also quantified using principal component analysis. Overall, the study revealed that individuals acted significantly differently to one another, and that while females initially appeared to display a greater number of shy behaviors, this was not consistent across the group. PCA revealed two personality dimensions in the tortoises, boldness and avoidance. Overall, the study revealed that the tortoises possessed personalities that were independent of sex or scenario. The study also revealed that many tortoises chose to interact with their keepers during training and approach tests: this suggests that human-tortoise interactions may have some enrichment value. Future research could quantify tortoise personality in other scenarios, such as in social interactions with other tortoises.

Abstract

Human–animal interaction (HAI) can be valuable for captive animals, and many zoo-housed species benefit from interactions with their keepers. There is also an increasing body of evidence that some animal species possess personalities that are temporally consistent. However, the majority of zoo research, particularly on personality and HAI, traditionally has focused on mammals, and there are comparatively fewer studies on reptiles. Research was undertaken at the Faunistic Park Le Cornelle, Italy, to investigate the effects of approach tests and food interaction events on 5 male and 5 female Aldabra tortoise (Aldrabrachelys gigantea) behavior. During human–tortoise interactions, continuous focal sampling of behavior took place. The behavioral differences between individual tortoises, interaction type (approach or training) and person involved in the interaction (keeper, vet, or observer) were analysed using general linear mixed-effects models. The personality components of the tortoises were also quantified using principal component analysis. Overall, the study revealed that individuals acted significantly differently to one another, and that while females initially appeared to display a greater number of shy behaviors, this was not consistent across the group. PCA revealed two personality dimensions in the tortoises: boldness and avoidance. Overall, the study revealed that the tortoises possessed personalities that were independent of sex or scenario. The study also revealed that many tortoises chose to interact with their keepers during training and approach tests: this suggests that human–tortoise interactions may have some enrichment value. Future research could quantify tortoise personality in other scenarios such as in social interactions with other tortoises.

1. Introduction

Captive animals regularly come into contact with keeping staff or visitors. For humans, interaction with other species such as companion animals, may have benefits in terms of health, stress relief and immune function [1,2,3,4,5,6]. For animals, particularly non-domesticated species such as those housed in zoos, the relationship is often complex [2,4]. Some species may perceive any human presence to be a threat, similar to that of predators in their natural state. Others may consider the presence of visitors and keepers to be enriching [2,3,5]. Animals that consider human presence to be enriching, or at least non-threatening, are likely to cope better in a captive environment [4].
The study of animal personality has gained momentum in the zoo community in recent years, given its value in terms of animal compatibility and evolutionary function [3,4,5]. Personality in this case refers to responses to stimuli that are temporally stable and similar across different scenarios [4]. Personality in humans is well studied (for example, the five factor model), but the personality dimensions identified in humans cannot be directly applied to animals [5,6]. Research on animal personality has focused on mammalian species, particularly in the families of Primates [4,7,8], Suidae [9], Canidae [10,11] and Felidae [12,13], though also in fish [14]. Personality research has value in helping animal keepers to better understand the compatibility of animals in breeding programs, in applied research on the evolutionary development of personality [15,16], and in predicting conservation translocation success [17,18,19,20].
Traditionally, reptiles have received less research focus than mammals in terms of personality research [20], and in research in general [21]. However, reptiles are well represented in zoological collections [22], so there remains a research gap in the field of reptilian behavioral science. Historically, ectothermy was a barrier to behavioral research, as reptiles are generally inactive until they reach their preferred optimal temperature [22,23,24,25,26,27]. However, once sufficiently heated, reptiles have been shown to be capable of problem solving and social learning, equivalent to that of mammals [15].
While limited, there is some research available on tortoise personality [28]. Several studies have focused on the potential implications of personality on reintroduction outcomes and survival, particularly in the desert tortoise (Gopherus agassizii) [20,29]. In these studies, individuals that were more timid had a higher probability of survival in environments containing predators [29]. In the Hermann’s tortoise (Testudo hermanni), the personality dimensions of aggression and boldness were associated with darker shelled individuals [30,31]. At current, personality has been assessed for only a handful of tortoise species, and it is not clear if the personality dimensions that have been identified are consistent across the taxon.
The Aldabra giant tortoise (Aldabrachelys (Geochelone) gigantea) is one of the world’s largest tortoise species, with some individuals reaching weights of more than 250 kg [32]. Endemic to the Aldabra Atoll, the species is described as vulnerable by the International Union for the Conservation of Nature [33], and has been used in conservation translocations to the Seychelles to replace extinct tortoise species [34]. In the wild, A. gigantea act as a keystone species, grazing and transporting the seeds of a wide range of native shrubs, grasses, and leaves [35]. There is a well-marked daily cycle of activity, with feeding being limited to the early morning and late evening [34]. Wild social groupings and population densities are variable, and tortoises may be found on their own or may congregate into small herds depending on food availability [32,36]. Agonistic behavior is reported to be virtually absent among wild groups [32]. The species is of conservation concern due to historic collection of tortoises as a food source and competition with goats for grazing [36,37]. The zoological community has recognized the threats to the wild population and have bred this species in captivity. For example, a review of Species360’s Zoological Information Management System (ZIMS) revealed over 800 Aldabra giant tortoises in captivity at the time of writing [38].
Given the large number of individuals in captivity, along with the potential conservation value of captive specimens, the Aldabra giant tortoise is a good candidate for behavioral research [25,39]. Research may also allow A. gigantea to act as a model species, promoting further personality studies on a range of Chelonia species. Given the limited published research available on human–animal interactions for reptiles [25,40], this species has potential as a useful study subject.
The aim of this study was to investigate the personality structure of zoo-housed Aldabra giant tortoises during food interaction events. Observations of behavioral responses during these events were used to identify whether individual tortoises were consistent in their responses to animal care staff during training events.

2. Methods

2.1. Study Subjects

Before the study was undertaken, the project was ethically reviewed and approved by the Ethical Review Committee at Manchester Metropolitan University. The study was carried out on 5 male and 5 female Aldabra tortoises, which were housed at the Faunistic Park Le Cornelle in Valbremo, Italy. A picture identification guide was produced before the study, allowing individuals to be identified based on morphological features. Individuals were also referred to by names they were given, throughout the study (Table 1). All tortoises were mature adults, each with an estimated age between 30–60 years. The tortoises were housed in an indoor exhibit 840.38 m2, which was open to the public from 10:00 until 17:00 (Figure 1). Tall barriers prevented the tortoises from interacting with the public. Research was undertaken in the indoor exhibit where the temperature was kept between 20–27 °C to mimic the tortoises’ natural climate [25].

2.2. Data Collection

Between 10 and 16 approach tests were carried out on each tortoise (with variance due to tortoise interest in the trials), with four different people undertaking the tests. These consisted of two keepers (known to the tortoises), a vet and an unknown observer. The slow movements of tortoises, combined with time restrictions, limited the number of replicates that could take place. Each test began with a person standing 5 m away from one of the tortoises and slowly walking towards them, stopping beside the individual. After a period of 30 sec, the person would crouch down and rub the tortoise shell, head, and legs. Similar interactions have been conducted by other researchers on the Aldabra tortoise [25], and other tortoise species [26,40].
Food interaction events were also performed with each tortoise. Between two and five training events were conducted per animal, with variance in the number of trials occurring because of differences in engagement levels between tortoises. Food interaction events consisted of one of the keepers approaching and presenting the individual with a piece of food speared onto the end of a stick. Gradually, as the tortoise became more comfortable, the keeper would move further away from the tortoise, encouraging them to step forward and take the piece of food. The food items presented to them included grapes (Vitis vinifera), apple (Malus domestica), sweet pepper (Capsicum annuum) and plum (Prunus domestica): these items were selected because they were considered attractive to the tortoises. Previous research has shown that tortoises show a preference for red, yellow, and orange food items [40,41]. The food items used in food interaction events were not given as part of the normal diet of the tortoises which encouraged them to cooperate. Tortoises were free to walk away from the social interaction at any point during the approach tests and food interaction events.
All approach tests and food interaction events were video recorded by the same researcher, who kept still and was partly concealed by the exhibit perimeter. This individual conducted all observations and conducted analysis of all videos.
Prior to the study, an ethogram of the Aldabra tortoises at Faunistic Park Le Cornelle was constructed for use in a pilot study, adapted from Ruby and Niblick’s [42] ethogram. After initially reviewing the video data, the ethogram was adapted, taking the most relevant social and defensive behaviors from the original ethogram (Table 2). Behaviors were categorized as either “fearful” or “bold” in response to human interaction.
Each video was analysed using Behavioral Observation Research Interactive Software (BORIS) [43]. Continuous focal sampling was used to record all the behaviors exhibited by each individual during approach tests and food interaction events. Total video length for each individual varied greatly, for both approach tests and food interaction events. Therefore, the analysed data were exported to Microsoft Excel where the durations of state behaviors were converted into percentages of time and events were converted into counts per minute.

2.3. Data Analysis

All data were uploaded into a Microsoft Excel 2013™ spreadsheet. Statistical analyses were performed using Minitab version 17. To analyze the behavioral impact of the approach tests and food interaction events, general linear mixed-effects models (GLMM) were run. Both the approach tests and food interaction events were combined into one single set of GLMMs. In these models, the behavior was included as the outcome, and the session type (food interaction events or approach), the individual tortoise and the person present (keeper, vet, or unknown observer) were used as predictors. The sex of the tortoise was initially used as a predictor, but due to its high collinearity with the individual, it was discarded.
Principal component analysis (PCA) was then used to illustrate how individuals differed from one another in their response to human interaction, both during approach tests and food interaction events [44,45]. PCA is used to determine which variables are commonly correlated, and to reduce a range of variables into a smaller number, known as components. For the PCA, all behaviors were input into the analysis (excluding food bite and food sniff, which were both zero inflated). Individual differences in behavioral response to approach tests and food interaction events were tested for normality. As all were identified as being not normally distributed, the behaviors were examined using Kruskal–Wallis tests and further visualized using PCA.

3. Results

3.1. Behavior

An initial graph was produced to show the difference between the behavior of individual tortoises during the approach tests (Figure 2). Two behaviors, food bite and food sniff, were removed as they were not relevant to the approach tests. Graphs were produced to illustrate differences between individual tortoises (Figure 3). As in the approach tests, individual tortoises showed considerable differences between each other in terms of their behavior during food interaction events.
GLMM modelling for the approach tests and food interaction events combined, found that at least one predictor was significant for each behavior outcome, and often all three predictors were significant (Table 3). All models were significant. However, some models explained the variance in behavior better than others: R2 values varied from 8.94% (head jerk) to 41.42% (head withdrawn). The individual tortoise was a significant predictor of all behaviors except sitting, standing, and stepping forward.

3.2. Personality

PCA of individual tortoise responses to approach tests and food interaction events were conducted (Figure 4 and Figure 5). Behaviors were inputted into the PCA to identify which behaviors occurred most commonly together.
In the PCA for approach tests, a total of 11 components were identified. As PC1 and PC2 explained the majority of the variance, they were selected and the remaining components were discarded. PC1 and PC2 explained 30.2% and 15.9% of the variance respectively (Figure 4). In total, 46.1% of the total variance was explained using the PCA (Table 4 and Table 5). For PC1, the behaviors that showed the most positive loading were ’Step away’, ‘High stand’, ‘Head defensive’, ‘Defensive posture’ and ‘Neck extension’. These behaviors appear to be involved in avoidance or defense. ‘Throat pump’, ‘High stand’ and ‘Step forward’ were negatively loaded on PC1. Higher values of PC1 therefore indicate individuals that are more averse to interaction: to take this into account, PC1 was labelled as ‘Avoidance’.
Higher values of PC2 were indicative of more confident individuals. ‘Throat pump’, ‘High stand’ and ‘Step forward’ were all negatively loaded on PC2. The behaviors ‘Defensive posture’, ‘Head defensive’ and ‘Step away’, by contrast, were negatively loaded on PC2. Pirimide, Bucco and Confy appeared to be the most bold, with Assy, Blu and Piccolo Liscia being the most fearful during approach tests. PC2 was labelled as ‘boldness’.
For the food interaction events (Figure 5), 10 Principal Components were identified. PC1 and PC2 explained 28.4% and 20.2% of the variance respectively: 48.7% of variance was explained in total (Table 6 and Table 7). As a result, the first two components were utilized. The behaviors were loaded in a similar fashion to those identified in the approach tests, though in this case, PC1 was positively loaded with bold behaviors (e.g., neck extension, step forward, standing) and PC2 was associated with avoidance behaviors (head jerk, head defensive, head withdrawn). As a result, this time PC1 was labelled as ‘boldness’ and PC2 was labelled as ‘avoidance’.

4. Discussion

Overall, the study revealed that Aldabra tortoises reacted differently to the approach tests and training events. While the behavioral differences initially appeared to be related to sex, the differences were better explained by differences between individual tortoises. Two personality dimensions: boldness and avoidance, were identified using PCA.

4.1. Tortoise Behavior and Personality

On first inspection, there appeared to be considerable sex related differences in behavior during training and approach tests. Namely, male tortoises appeared to spend more time engaged in high stands, food sniffing, neck extensions and throat pumping. All of the above behaviors tend to suggest a more confident individual. For example, extension of the neck leaves the animal in a more vulnerable position; a state avoided in shyer individuals [42]. In contrast, females were more likely to spend time sitting, withdrawing their heads, or adopting a defensive posture. These behaviors generally suggest a more defensive or shy temperament.
However, closer inspection of the data and statistical analysis showed this not to be the case: whilst on average female tortoises engaged in more defensive behaviors, this was not consistent across all individuals in the group. For example, there were some females that rarely took up the ‘head defensive’ posture, and some males that spend long periods of time engaged in this behavior. This suggests that analysis at the individual level is a better method of assessing temperament [26,27].
The first personality dimension identified in the study was boldness. Boldness was associated with behaviors such as extension of the neck, high stand, and step forward. Boldness has also been identified as a personality dimension in other chelonians, such as the Hermann’s [31] and desert tortoise [17], and eastern box turtles (Terrapene carolina) [46]. Boldness has a survival context for wild tortoises: in some studies, desert tortoises were more likely to be predated if they expressed a bold personality, as they were more likely to explore and come across coyotes (Canis latrans) [20,28]. In contrast, boldness may have a survival advantage in environments where predators are scarce, as bolder animals may have more opportunities to find food and mates than their shy counterparts [20], or maintain a higher body temperature [45]. In the wild, there are few natural predators of adult Aldabra tortoises [32], so in theory a bolder temperament may come with advantages. However, bold individuals may also be more likely to explore new, unsuitable terrain. A mix of both bold and shy individuals in the population may therefore be beneficial.
The PCA also identified one other personality dimension, which was labelled here as avoidance. Throat pumping was positively loaded on this dimension, whereas avoidance behaviors such as ‘step away’ and ‘high stand’ were negatively loaded. Behaviors such as biting, and ramming were not observed in the study because individuals were not observed interacting with one another. However, behaviors such as throat pumping associated with confidence and lack of avoidance in tortoise species [42]. Boldness and avoidance are not mutually exclusive: a tortoise could be both shy and averse to interactions, or non-avoiding and bold. In this study, an individual that showed a low aggression score tended to avoid interaction with keepers, veterinarians and observers.
Aggression has an environment-dependent survival advantage. In environments where other tortoises are non-aggressive, an aggressive individual may be more successful in competing for food resources or mates. In contrast, an aggressive individual in an aggressive tortoise community is more likely to engage in fights that could result in injury [28]. Previous studies in Hermann’s tortoises have showed that aggression was correlated with darker shell colors [30]. Whilst no such research has been conducted on Aldabra tortoise’s, it appears that aggression/avoidance is a personality dimension for both species.
It is beyond the scope of this study to identify the origin of each tortoise personality. Previous research in reptiles suggests that the personality of some species may be affected by egg incubation temperature [47]. Rearing and the environment of the tortoise may also influence the personality [20]. As they are a long-lived species, the individual life history of each tortoise observed may have influenced their behavior during interactions with people.
There is some practical value in assessing the personality of captive Aldabra tortoises. Understanding individual temperaments may allow keepers to better predict how individuals will interact when provided with novel enrichment types. Similarly, personality assessment might be used when planning to move animals for the purpose of breeding: bold, aggressive individuals could pose a challenge when group-housed alongside shy, non-aggressive individuals. Social dynamics vary between tortoise populations and species; social dynamics therefore need to be assessed within individual populations.

4.2. Human–Tortoise Interaction

The discovery that some tortoises may choose to interact with humans, and find the process enriching, is still a relatively novel finding [26,27,40]. During this study, individual tortoises had the ability to walk away from human interactions, yet some individuals spent long periods of time with keepers and appeared to enjoy the interaction. Tortoises were also able to differentiate between people: the person present was a significant predictor of defensive posture, food sniff and high stand. It is possible, therefore, that tortoises were able to recognize either the person (or the clothing) of their keepers versus the vet or unknown observer. It should be noted, however, that as only one vet and keeper were available, it is not known whether the tortoises recognized the person or the clothing. Generally, defensive behaviors were observed more when the vet was present: this may be a learned response as a result of previous unpleasant interactions during veterinary care or treatment. Behaviors such as the high stand and food sniff occurred more when keepers were present. This suggests that tortoises may both be able to identify individuals (or their clothing) and remember the results of prior interactions.

4.3. Future Directions

Whilst the sample size for this study was good, all tortoises were housed in the same collection. Potential husbandry effects on tortoise behavior could be accounted for in a multi-zoo study [48]. Similarly, temperature is known to affect tortoise behavior [24,28]. Whilst the temperature within the enclosure was considered suitable for Aldabra tortoises, individual temperature assessments could be used to determine whether tortoises were more likely to interact with training when their body temperatures were higher [27].
Personality assessments in animals have been conducted for a range of species, including primates, carnivores, birds, and to a lesser extent, reptiles [30,49]. Two general methodologies exist for assessing animal temperament: the first involves observing the animal under a range of scenarios to determine whether their behavior is temporally consistent. The second technique involves asking caretakers to assess each animal’s temperament [3]. When used together, keeper responses can be correlated against observed behavior to investigate how effective keepers are in assessing animal personality. This study used only behavioral observations: future research could include keeper questionnaires in order to validate personality elements.
Future studies could also extend this research question to determine whether tortoises are able to discriminate between individuals, or whether they are only able to identify individuals based on more generic features (such as clothing). It was beyond the scope of this study to identify whether the tortoises could identify individuals if they were not dressed in their normal clothing (e.g., keeper, vet). This could be incorporated into a future study.
One final direction is to investigate the social interactions between individual Aldabra tortoises. It was beyond the scope of this study to investigate intra-specific interactions as tortoises were trained individually. Given the plasticity in social grouping of Aldabra tortoises in the wild, it is possible that many affiliative interactions exist, which would not be picked up by this study’s methodology [36,37]. Future studies could incorporate a social network component to better understand Aldabra tortoise sociality.

5. Conclusions

Over the course of this study two key messages were identified. Firstly, two personality dimensions were identified during the study: boldness and avoidance of interactions. Whilst this study only considered one group of Aldabra tortoises in one zoo, the findings are similar to those for Hermann’s tortoises and gopher tortoises in both wild and captive settings. Extrapolating from these results, it is possible that these personality dimensions are conserved across the Chelonia taxonomic group: further research on more tortoise species would be beneficial.
Secondly, whilst there was variance in behavior between individuals, some Aldabra tortoises chose to interact with people, even when there was no reward involved. Given these results, it is possible that keeper interaction is a viable enrichment tool for those working with Aldabra tortoises.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, G.Q.P. and A.C.; methodology, V.S.; software, M.F. and R.P.; validation, V.S., R.R., M.F. and E.B.; formal analysis, J.E.B.; investigation, V.S.; resources, G.Q.P.; data curation, V.S. and D.G.; writing—original draft preparation, J.E.B. and G.Q.P.; writing—review and editing, J.E.B. and G.Q.P.; supervision, G.Q.P. and R.P.; project administration, R.P. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of Manchester Metropolitan University (protocol code MMU15 and 16 July 2019).

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Data can be requested through correspondence with the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to permissions from the zoological collection.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank the keepers at Cornelle Zoological Park for their assistance and guidance throughout the project.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. McCune, S.; Kruger, K.A.; Griffin, J.A.; Esposito, L.; Freund, L.S.; Hurley, K.J.; Bures, R. Evolution of research into the mutual benefits of human-animal interaction. Anim. Front. 2014, 4, 49–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Rose, P.E.; Scales, J.S.; Brereton, J.E. Why the “Visitor Effect” Is Complicated. Unraveling Individual Animal, Visitor Number, and Climatic Influences on Behavior, Space Use and Interactions With Keepers—A Case Study on Captive Hornbills. Front. Vet. Sci. 2020, 7, 236–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Quintavalle Pastorino, G.; Christodoulides, Y.; Curone, G.; Pearce-Kelly, P.; Faustini, M.; Albertini, M.; Preziosi, R.; Mazzola, S.M. Behavioural Profiles of Brown and Sloth Bears in Captivity. Animals 2017, 7, 39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Itoh, K. Personality research with non-human primates: Theoretical formulation and methods. Primates 2002, 43, 249–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. McAdams, D.P. The Five-Factor Model In Personality: A Critical Appraisal. J. Personal. 1992, 60, 329–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Michelangeli, M.; Chapple, D.G.; Goulet, C.T.; Bertram, M.G.; Wong, B.B.M. Behavioral syndromes vary among geographically distinct populations in a reptile. Behav. Ecol. 2019, 30, 393–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. King, J.E.; Figueredo, A.J. The Five-Factor Model plus Dominance in Chimpanzee Personality. J. Res. Personal. 1997, 31, 257–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Weiss, A.; King, J.E.; Perkins, L. Personality and subjective well-being in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus and Pongo abelii). J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2006, 90, 501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. O’Malley, C.I.; Turner, S.P.; D’Eath, R.B.; Steibel, J.P.; Bates, R.O.; Ernst, C.W.; Siegford, J.M. Animal personality in the management and welfare of pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2019, 218, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Barnard, S.; Wells, D.L.; Hepper, P.G.; Milligan, A.D. Association between lateral bias and personality traits in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). J. Comp. Psychol. 2017, 131, 246–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Svartberg, K.; Forkman, B. Personality traits in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2002, 79, 133–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Quintavalle Pastorino, G.Q.; Viau, A.; Curone, G.; Pearce-Kelly, P.; Faustini, M.; Vigo, D.; Mazzola, S.M.; Preziosi, R. Role of Personality in Behavioral Responses to New Environments in Captive Asiatic Lions (Panthera leo persica). Vet. Med. Int. 2017, 2017, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Quintavalle Pastorino, G.; Paini, F.; Williams, C.L.; Faustini, M.; Mazzola, S.M. Personality and Sociality in Captive Tigers (Panthera tigris). Annu. Res. Rev. Biol. 2017, 21, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Harcourt, J.L.; Sweetman, G.; Johnstone, R.A.; Manica, A. Personality counts: The effect of boldness on shoal choice in three-spined sticklebacks. Anim. Behav. 2009, 77, 1501–1505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Wilkinson, A.; Kuenstner, K.; Mueller, J.; Huber, L. Social learning in a non-social reptile (Geochelone carbonaria). Biol. Lett. 2010, 6, 614–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Wilkinson, A.; Sebanz, N.; Mandl, I.; Huber, L. No evidence of contagious yawning in the red-footed tortoise Geochelone carbonaria. Curr. Zool. 2011, 57, 477–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Germano, J.M.; Nafus, M.G.; Perry, J.A.; Hall, D.B.; Swaisgood, R.R. Predicting translocation outcomes with personality for desert tortoises. Behav. Ecol. 2017, 28, 1075–1084. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Waters, R.M.; Bowers, B.B.; Burghardt, G.M. Personality and individuality in reptile behavior. In Personality in Nonhuman Animals; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2017; pp. 153–184. [Google Scholar]
  19. Learmonth, M.J. The matter of non-avian reptile sentience, and why it “matters” to them: A conceptual, ethical and scientific review. Animals 2020, 10, 901. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Steele, M.A. Personality in Head-Started Desert Tortoises: A Potentially Useful Tool for the Conservation of a Threatened Species. Master’s Thesis, University of California, Davis, CA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  21. Melfi, V.A. There are big gaps in our knowledge, and thus approach, to zoo animal welfare: A case for evidence-based zoo animal management. Zoo Biol. 2009, 28, 574–588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Brereton, S.R.; Brereton, J.E. Sixty years of collection planning: What species do zoos and aquariums keep? Int. Zoo Yearb. 2020, 54, 131–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Burghardt, G.M. Environmental enrichment and cognitive complexity in reptiles and amphibians: Concepts, review, and implications for captive populations. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2013, 147, 286–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Terespolsky, A.; Brereton, J.E. Investigating the Thermal Biology and Behaviour of Captive Radiated Tortoises. J. Vet. Med. Anim. Sci. 2021, 4, 1–6. [Google Scholar]
  25. Falcón, W.; Baxter, R.P.; Furrer, S.; Bauert, M.; Hatt, J.M.; Schaepman-Strub, G.; Ozgul, A.; Bunbury, N.; Clauss, M.; Hansen, D.M. Patterns of activity and body temperature of Aldabra giant tortoises in relation to environmental temperature. Ecol. Evol. 2018, 8, 2108–2121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Learmonth, M.J.; Sherwen, S.; Hemsworth, P.H. Assessing preferences of two zoo-housed Aldabran giant tortoises (Aldabrachelys gigantea) for three stimuli using a novel preference test. Zoo Biol. 2021, 40, 98–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Learmonth, M.J.; Sherwen, S.; Hemsworth, P. Assessing choice ability and preferences of five Leopard Tortoises (Stigmochelys pardalis) for three stimuli through a novel two-phase preference test. J. Zoo Aquar. Res. 2021, 9, 94–101. [Google Scholar]
  28. Doody, J.S.; Burghardt, G.M.; Dinets, V. Breaking the Social-Non-social Dichotomy: A Role for Reptiles in Vertebrate Social Behavior Research? Ethology 2013, 119, 95–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Nafus, M.G.; Germano, J.M.; Swaisgood, R.R. Cues from a common predator cause survival-linked behavioral adjustments in Mojave Desert tortoises (Gopherus agassizii). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2017, 71, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Mafli, A.; Wakamatsu, K.; Roulin, A. Melanin-based coloration predicts aggressiveness and boldness in captive eastern Hermann’s tortoises. Anim. Behav. 2011, 81, 859–863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Golubović, A.; Bonnet, X.; Djordjević, S.; Djurakic, M.; Tomović, L. Variations in righting behaviour across Hermann’s tortoise populations. J. Zool. 2013, 291, 69–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Grubb, P. The growth, ecology and population structure of giant tortoises on Aldabra. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 1971, 260, 327–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. International Union for Conservation of Nature and Natural Resources (IUCN). Geochelone gigantea. Available online: https://www.iucnredlist.org/species/9010/12949962#habitat-ecology (accessed on 20 May 2021).
  34. Walton, R.; Baxter, R.; Bunbury, N.; Hansen, D.; Fleischer-Dogley, F.; Greenwood, S.; Schaepman-Strub, G. In the land of giants: Habitat use and selection of the Aldabra giant tortoise on Aldabra Atoll. Biodivers. Conserv. 2019, 28, 3183–3198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Hnatiuk, S.H. Plant dispersal by the Aldabran giant tortoise, Geochelone gigantea (Schweigger). Oecologia 1978, 36, 345–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Bourn, D.; Gibson, C.; Augeri, D.; Wilson, C.J.; Church, J.; Hay, S.I. The rise and fall of the Aldabran giant tortoise population. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 1999, 266, 1091–1100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. Devaux, B. La Tortue Géante des Seychelles, une Survivante—Giant Tortoise of Seychelles, a Survivor: Dipsochelys elephantina (Duméril et Bibron, 1835); Chelonii; SOPTOM: Gonfaron, France, 2007; Volume 5, pp. 1–16. [Google Scholar]
  38. Species360. ZIMS. Available online: https://zims.species360.org/Main.aspx (accessed on 20 December 2021).
  39. Gutnick, T.; Weissenbacher, A.; Kuba, M.J. The underestimated giants: Operant conditioning, visual discrimination and long-term memory in giant tortoises. Anim. Cogn. 2020, 23, 159–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Mehrkam, L.R.; Dorey, N.R. Is preference a predictor of enrichment efficacy in Galapagos tortoises (Chelonoidis nigra)? Zoo Biol. 2014, 33, 275–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Spiezio, C.; Leonardi, C.; Regaiolli, B. Assessing colour preference in Aldabra giant tortoises (Geochelone gigantea). Behav. Process. 2017, 145, 60–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Ruby, D.E.; Niblick, H.A. A Behavioral Inventory of the Desert Tortoise: Development of an Ethogram. Herpetol. Monogr. 1994, 8, 88–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Friard, O.; Gamba, M. BORIS: A free, versatile open-source event-logging software for video/audio coding and live observations. Methods Ecol. Evol. 2016, 7, 1325–1330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Stevens, J.P. Applied Multivariate Statistics for the Social Sciences, 5th ed.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Tabachnick, B.G.; Fidell, L.S. Using Multivariate Statistics: Pearson New International Edition, 6th ed.; Pearson: Harlow, UK, 2014; ISBN 9781292034546. [Google Scholar]
  46. Kashon, E.A.F.; Carlson, B.E. Consistently bolder turtles maintain higher body temperatures in the field but may experience greater predation risk. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2018, 72, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Siviter, H.; Deeming, D.C.; Rosenberger, J.; Burman, O.H.; Moszuti, S.A.; Wilkinson, A. The impact of egg incubation temperature on the personality of oviparous reptiles. Anim. Cogn. 2017, 20, 109–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Shora, J.A.; Myhill, M.N.G.; Brereton, J.E. Should zoo foods be coati chopped. J. Zoo Aquar. Res 2018, 6, 22–25. [Google Scholar]
  49. Haddon, C.; Burman, O.H.; Assheton, P.; Wilkinson, A. Love in Cold Blood: Are Reptile Owners Emotionally Attached to Their Pets? Anthrozoös 2021, 34, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Enclosure design for the tortoise group.
Figure 1. Enclosure design for the tortoise group.
Animals 12 00419 g001
Figure 2. Difference in Aldabra tortoise behavior during approach tests (+/−standard error).
Figure 2. Difference in Aldabra tortoise behavior during approach tests (+/−standard error).
Animals 12 00419 g002
Figure 3. Difference in individual Aldabra tortoises’ behavior during food interaction events (+/−standard error).
Figure 3. Difference in individual Aldabra tortoises’ behavior during food interaction events (+/−standard error).
Animals 12 00419 g003
Figure 4. Output of PCA of Aldabra tortoise behavior during approach tests. A total of two components, labelled ‘boldness’ and ‘avoidance’, were identified.
Figure 4. Output of PCA of Aldabra tortoise behavior during approach tests. A total of two components, labelled ‘boldness’ and ‘avoidance’, were identified.
Animals 12 00419 g004
Figure 5. Output of PCA of Aldabra tortoise behavior during food interaction events. The two principal components, labelled ‘boldness’ and ‘avoidance’, were similar to those identified in approach tests.
Figure 5. Output of PCA of Aldabra tortoise behavior during food interaction events. The two principal components, labelled ‘boldness’ and ‘avoidance’, were similar to those identified in approach tests.
Animals 12 00419 g005
Table 1. Aldabra tortoise individuals.
Table 1. Aldabra tortoise individuals.
Tortoise IDSexDescription
AssyFemaleMedium, smooth, grey carapace
BluMaleMedium, smooth grey carapace
BuccoMaleLarge, rough, red carapace
ConfyMaleLarge, rough, red carapace
OndulinaFemaleMedium, smooth, grey carapace
Piccolo LisciaFemaleSmall, smooth, grey carapace (smallest individual)
Piccolo PuntaFemaleSmall, smooth, grey carapace
PirimideMaleLarge, rough, red carapace (largest individual)
Red LightMaleLarge, rough, red carapace
SolcaFemaleMedium, smooth, grey carapace
Table 2. Aldabra tortoise ethogram. Adapted from Ruby and Niblick [42]. States are shown using s and events as e.
Table 2. Aldabra tortoise ethogram. Adapted from Ruby and Niblick [42]. States are shown using s and events as e.
BehaviorDescription
Defensive posture sHead and forelegs tucked in tightly, with back legs extended pushing front of body downwards. Only recorded during approach tests.
Head defensive eHead fully withdrawn into shell with limbs still exposed.
Head jerk eSudden withdrawal of the head, either by shortening the neck or retreating head into shell.
Head withdrawn sHead withdrawn but still visible. Neck retracted and not exposed.
Sitting sBody resting on ground with all limbs exposed.
Step away eTortoise steps away from person present.
Neck extension sNeck fully extended, reaching forward.
Standing sBody raised on all four limbs but close to the ground.
High stand sLegs fully extended with body raised fully off of the ground.
Step forward eTortoise steps towards the person present.
Throat pump sClear, steady pulsation of the neck, more obvious when neck is extended.
Food sniff eNose exploration of the food item, close to or direct contact. Only recorded during training sessions
Food bite eTortoise bites the piece of fruit offered on a stick. Only recorded during training sessions
Table 3. Outputs of General Linear Models (GLMM) on tortoise behavior during food interaction events. For the predictor of ‘person present’, the SE coefficients are displayed as pairwise comparisons against animal keeper 1. K1 = Keeper 1, K2 = Keeper 2, V = Vet, U = Unknown person. * indicates a staistically significant variable.
Table 3. Outputs of General Linear Models (GLMM) on tortoise behavior during food interaction events. For the predictor of ‘person present’, the SE coefficients are displayed as pairwise comparisons against animal keeper 1. K1 = Keeper 1, K2 = Keeper 2, V = Vet, U = Unknown person. * indicates a staistically significant variable.
BehaviorR2 (p)PredictorDFSE Coefficientp
Defensive posture27.38% (p < 0.001 *)Session type11.580.009 *
Person present3K1-K2: 1.63,
K1-V: 1.65,
K1-U: 1.58
0.038 *
Individual tortoise92.30<0.001 *
Food bite36.68% (p < 0.001 *)Session typeNANANA
Person present3K1-K2: 0.10,
K1-V: 0.10,
K1-U: 0.09
<0.001 *
Individual tortoise90.1660.031 *
Food sniff18.66% (p < 0.001 *)Session typeNANANA
Person present3K1-K2: 1.31,
K1-V: 1.26,
K1-U: 1.34
<0.001 *
Individual tortoise92.180.007 *
Head defensive39.28% (p < 0.001 *)Session type14.40<0.001 *
Person present3K1-K2: 3.99,
K1-V: 3.85,
K1-U: 4.07
0.027 *
Individual tortoise96.63<0.001 *
Head jerk8.94% (p = 0.010 *)Session type10.280.332
Person present3K1-K2: 0.24,
K1-V: 0.24,
K1-U: 0.23
0.138
Individual tortoise90.3350.008 *
Head withdrawn41.42% (p < 0.001 *)Session type15.910.269
Person present3K1-K2: 6.07,
K1-V: 5.91,
K1-U: 6.17
0.017 *
Individual tortoise98.58<0.001 *
High stand16.90% (p < 0.001 *)Session type14.640.329
Person present3K1-K2: 4.77,
K1-V: 4.65,
K1-U: 4.85
0.005 *
Individual tortoise96.740.001 *
Neck extension36.55% (p < 0.001 *)Session type12.670.001 *
Person present3K1-K2: 2.75,
K1-V: 2.68,
K1-U: 2.86
0.201
Individual tortoise93.880.001 *
Sitting 16.33% (p < 0.001 *)Session type18.490.001 *
Person present3K1-K2: 8.73,
K1-V: 8.51,
K1-U: 8.78
0.144
Individual tortoise912.300.276
Standing12.32% (p < 0.001 *)Session type18.420.004 *
Person present3K1-K2: 8.66,
K1-V: 8.44,
K1-U: 8.80
0.155
Individual tortoise912.200.151
Step away27.00% (p < 0.001 *)Session type10.2740.303
Person present3K1-K2: 0.281,
K1-V: 0.274,
K1-U: 0.286
0.587
Individual tortoise90.3970.001 *
Step forward28.25% (p < 0.001 *)Session type10.1780.001 *
Person present3K1-K2: 0.183,
K1-V: 0.186,
K1-U: 0.178
0.628
Individual tortoise90.2580.152
Throat pump29.89% (p < 0.001 *)Session type14.810.036 *
Person present3K1-K2: 4.95,
K1-V: 4.82,
K1-U: 5.03
0.086
Individual tortoise96.990.001 *
Table 4. Output of principal component analysis for approach tests. A total of 11 Principal Components were identified.
Table 4. Output of principal component analysis for approach tests. A total of 11 Principal Components were identified.
PC1PC2PC3PC4PC5PC6PC7PC8PC9PC10PC11
Eigenvalue3.31731.75161.24011.02410.93100.73660.59690.52250.48350.36540.0311
Proportion0.3020.1590.1130.0930.0850.0670.0540.0480.0440.0330.003
Cumulative0.3020.4610.5740.6670.7510.8180.8720.9200.9640.9971.000
Table 5. Eigenvector values for PCA and PC2 for approach tests.
Table 5. Eigenvector values for PCA and PC2 for approach tests.
VariablePC1 (Avoidance)PC2 (Boldness)
Sitting0.4310.387
Head defensive0.28−0.163
Defensive posture0.23−0.162
Head withdrawn0.228−0.336
Step away0.12−0.498
Head jerk0.0360.211
Step forward−0.2410.11
Throat pump−0.2770.456
Neck extension−0.3640.062
High stand−0.398−0.125
Standing−0.436−0.391
Table 6. Output of principal component analysis for food interaction events. A total of ten principal components were identified.
Table 6. Output of principal component analysis for food interaction events. A total of ten principal components were identified.
PC1PC2PC3PC4PC5PC6PC7PC8PC9PC10
Eigenvalue2.84442.02261.10361.07760.84300.76850.52630.49490.25260.0664
Proportion0.2840.2020.1100.1080.0840.0770.0530.0490.0250.007
Cumulative0.2840.4870.5970.7050.7890.8660.9190.9680.9931.000
Table 7. Eigenvector values for PCA and PC2 for food interaction events.
Table 7. Eigenvector values for PCA and PC2 for food interaction events.
VariablePC1 (Boldness)PC2 (Avoidance)
Standing0.4890.204
Step forward0.4140.102
Neck extension0.315−0.306
High stand0.1960.173
Step away0.1190.23
Head jerk−0.0980.571
Throat pump−0.106−0.406
Head defensive−0.2220.361
Head withdrawn−0.3520.329
Sitting−0.493−0.206
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Quintavalle Pastorino, G.; Smith, V.; Faustini, M.; Bonacina, E.; Guadagnini, D.; Robbiati, R.; Cavalleri, A.; Brereton, J.E.; Preziosi, R. Investigating the Behavior and Personality Structure of the Aldabra Tortoise during Human Interactions and Training Events. Animals 2022, 12, 419. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12040419

AMA Style

Quintavalle Pastorino G, Smith V, Faustini M, Bonacina E, Guadagnini D, Robbiati R, Cavalleri A, Brereton JE, Preziosi R. Investigating the Behavior and Personality Structure of the Aldabra Tortoise during Human Interactions and Training Events. Animals. 2022; 12(4):419. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12040419

Chicago/Turabian Style

Quintavalle Pastorino, Giovanni, Vanessa Smith, Massimo Faustini, Eleonora Bonacina, Davide Guadagnini, Roberto Robbiati, Alice Cavalleri, James Edward Brereton, and Richard Preziosi. 2022. "Investigating the Behavior and Personality Structure of the Aldabra Tortoise during Human Interactions and Training Events" Animals 12, no. 4: 419. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12040419

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop