What’s in a Click? The Efficacy of Conditioned Reinforcement in Applied Animal Training: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis
Abstract
:Simple Summary
Abstract
1. Introduction
“The best way to reinforce the behavior with the necessary speed is to use a ‘conditioned’ reinforcer. This is a signal which the animal is conditioned to associate with food. The animal is always given food immediately after the signal, and the signal itself then becomes the reinforcer… As soon as the dog moves, sound the cricket and give food.”
2. Methods
“clicker training” or “secondary reinforcers” or “secondary reinforcement” or “conditioned reinforcers” or “conditioned reinforcement” or “bridging stimulus” or “event marker” and “dogs” or “cats” or “parrots” or “cows” or “pigs” or “goats” or “horses” or “fish” or “sheep” or “primates” or “pinnipeds” or “cetaceans” and not “humans”
2.1. Literature Search and Study Selection Process
2.2. Assessment of Studies and Variable Coding
2.3. Data Extraction and Meta-Statistics
Heterogeneity Assessment
3. Results
3.1. Systematic Review
3.1.1. Search Results and Study Characteristics
3.1.2. Main Findings of the Systematic Review
Unconditioned Reinforcers (SR+) and Preference Assessments
Schedules of Reinforcement
Interstimulus-Intervals (ISI), Delays between Responses and Conditioned Reinforcers (R − Sr+), and Number of Pairings
Positioning of the Experimenters and Location of the Unconditioned Reinforcers (SR+)
3.1.3. Meta-Statistical Results
Effects of Clicker Training
Heterogeneity Assessment
Moderator Analyses
4. Discussion
4.1. Systematic Review
4.2. Meta-Analysis
4.3. Limitations
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Diamond, J. Evolution, consequences, and future of plant and animal domestication. Nature 2002, 418, 700–707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cooper, J.O.; Heron, T.E.; Heward, W.L. Applied Behavior Analysis, 2nd ed.; Prentice Hall: Upper Saddle River, NJ, USA, 2007; pp. 176–224. [Google Scholar]
- Pryor, K.; Ramirez, K. Modern Animal Training A Transformative Technology. In The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Operant and Classical Conditioning, 1st ed.; Mcsweeney, E.S., Murphy, F.K., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons: Chichester, UK, 2014; pp. 456–487. [Google Scholar]
- Fernandez, E.J. An 8-Step Program: Shaping and Fixed-Time Food Delivery Effects on Several Approximations and Undesired Responses in Goats. Master’s Thesis, University of North Texas, Denton, TX, USA, May 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Gillis, T.E.; Janes, A.C.; Kaufman, M.J. Positive reinforcement training in squirrel monkeys using clicker training. Am. J. Primatol. 2012, 74, 712–720. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Grant, R.A.; Warrior, J.R. Clicker training increases exploratory behaviour and time spent at the front of the enclosure in shelter cats: Implications for welfare and adoption rates. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2019, 211, 77–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mirwan, H.B.; Mason, G.J.; Kevan, P.G. Complex operant learning by worker bumblebees (Bombus impatiens): Detour behaviour and use of colours as discriminative stimuli. Insectes Sociaux 2015, 62, 365–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pfaller-Sadovsky, N.; Medina, L.G.; Hurtado-Parrado, C. It is mine! Using clicker training as a treatment of object guarding in 4 companion dogs (Canis lupus familiaris). J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2017, 22, 57–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slater, C.; Dymond, S. Using differential reinforcement to improve equine welfare: Shaping appropriate truck loading and feet handling. Behav. Process. 2011, 86, 329–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hendriksen, P.; Elmgreen, K.; Ladewig, J. Trailer-loading of horses: Is there a difference between positive and negative reinforcement concerning effectiveness and stress-related signs? J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2011, 6, 261–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ziv, G. The effects of using aversive training methods in dogs—A review. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2017, 19, 50–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ramirez, K. Basic Operant Conditioning. In Animal Training: Successful Animal Management through Positive Reinforcement, 1st ed.; Shedd Aquarium: Chicago, IL, USA, 1999; pp. 65–111. [Google Scholar]
- Pavlov, I.P.; Gantt, W.H. Lectures on Conditioned Reflexes: Twenty-Five Years of Objective Study of the Higher Nervous Activity (Behaviour) of Animals; Gantt, W.H., Ed.; Liverwright Publishing Corporation: New York, NY, USA, 1928. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Skinner, B.F. The Behavior of Organisms; B.F. Skinner Foundation: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1938. [Google Scholar]
- Pierce, W.D.; Cheney, C.D. Behavior Analysis and Learning, 6th ed.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2017; pp. 1–613. [Google Scholar]
- Skinner, B.F. How to Teach Animals. Sci. Am. 1951, 185, 26–29. Available online: https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1952-04212-001 (accessed on 8 July 2020). [CrossRef]
- Roddy, J. Harvard-trained dog. LOOK, 20 May 1952; pp. 17–20. [Google Scholar]
- Burch, M.R.; Bailey, J.S. How Dogs Learn; Wiley Publishing, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 1999; pp. 1–177. [Google Scholar]
- Pryor, K. History of Clicker Training I. 2013. Available online: https://www.clickertraining.com/node/153 (accessed on 9 March 2020).
- Chiandetti, C.; Avella, S.; Fongaro, E.; Cerri, F. Can clicker training facilitate conditioning in dogs? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016, 184, 109–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Feng, L.C.; Hodgens, N.H.; Woodhead, J.K.; Howell, T.J.; Bennett, P.C. Is clicker training (clicker + food) better than food-only training for novice companion dogs and their owners? Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2018, 204, 81–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferguson, D.L.; Rosales-Ruiz, J. Loading the problem loader: The effects of target training and shaping on trailer-loading behavior of horses. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 2001, 34, 409–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Rybova, V. Target Training as a Differential Reinforcement Intervention for Separation-Induced Challenging Behaviour in Horses. Master’s Thesis, University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Lansade, L.; Calandreau, L.A. conditioned reinforcer did not help to maintain an operant conditioning in the absence of a primary reinforcer in horses. Behav. Process. 2018, 146, 61–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- McCall, C.A.; Burgin, S.E. Equine utilization of secondary reinforcment during response extinction and acquisition. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2002, 78, 253–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, S.M.; Davis, E.S. Clicker increases resistance to extinction but does not decrease training time of a simple operant task in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 110, 318–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wennmacher, P.L. Effects of Click + Continuous Food versus Click + Intermittent Food on the Maintenance of Dog Behavior. Master’s Thesis, University of North Texas, Denton, TX, USA, May 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Willson, E.K.; Stratton, R.B.; Bolwell, C.F.; Stafford, K.J. Comparison of positive reinforcement training in cats: A pilot study. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2017, 21, 64–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wood, L. Clicker Bridging Stimulus Efficacy; Hunter College: New York, NY, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Fernandez, E.J.; Dorey, N.R. An examination of shaping with an African Crested Porcupine (Hystrix cristata). J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnston, J.M.; Pennypacker, H.S. Strategies and Tactics of Behavioral Research, 3rd ed.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- D’Onofrio, J.M. Measuring The Efficiency of Clicker Training for Service Dogs. Master’s Thesis, Pennsylvania State University, Pennsylvania, PA, USA, December 2015. Available online: https://etda.libraries.psu.edu/files/final_submissions/11557 (accessed on 8 July 2020).
- Häderer, I.K.; Michiels, N.K. Successful operant conditioning of marine fish in their natural habitat. Copeia 2016, 104, 380–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Williams, J.L.; Friend, T.H.; Nevill, C.H.; Archer, G. The efficacy of a secondary reinforcer (clicker) during acquisition and extinction of an operant task in horses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2004, 88, 331–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dozier, C.L.; Iwata, B.A.; Thomason-Sassi, J.; Worsdell, A.S.; Wilson, D.M. A comparison of two pairing procedures to establish praise as a reinforcer. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 2012, 45, 721–735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Williams, B.A. Conditioned Reinforcement: Experimental and Theoretical Issues. Behav. Anal. 1994, 17, 261–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Williams, B.A. Conditioned reinforcement: Neglected or outmoded explanatory construct? Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1994, 1, 457–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Martin, S.; Friedman, S.G. Blazing Clickers. Available online: http://www.behaviorworks.org/files/journals/Blazing%20Clickers.pdf (accessed on 23 September 2020).
- Fernandez, E.J. Click or Treat: A trick or two in the zoo. Am. Anim. Train. Mag. 2001, 2, 41–44. [Google Scholar]
- Dorey, N.R.; Cox, D.J. Function matters: A review of terminological differences in applied and basic clicker training research. PeerJ 2018, 6, e5621. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Feng, L.C.; Howell, T.J.; Bennett, P.C. How clicker training works: Comparing Reinforcing, Marking, and Bridging Hypotheses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016, 181, 34–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Petticrew, M.; Roberts, H. Systematic Reviews in the Social Sciences: A Practical Guide; Blackwell Publishing: Malden, MA, USA, 2006; pp. 1–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Liberati, A.; Altman, D.G.; Tetzlaff, J.; Mulrow, C.; Gøtzsche, P.C.; Ioannidis, J.P.A.; Clarke, M.; Devereaux, P.J.; Kleijnen, J.; Moher, D. The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: Explanation and elaboration. PLoS Med. 2009, 6, e1000100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moher, D.; Liberati, A.; Tetzlaff, J.; Altman, D.G. The PRISMA Group. Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses: The PRISMA Statement. PLoS Med. 2009, 6, e1000097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sandieson, R.W.; Kirkpatrick, L.C.; Sandieson, R.M.; Zimmerman, W. Harnessing the power of education research databases with the pearl-harvesting methodological framework for information retrieval. J. Spec. Educ. 2010, 44, 161–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morville, P.; Rosenfeld, L. Information Architecture for the World Wide Web, 3rd ed.; O’Reilly Media, Inc.: Sebastopol, CA, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Sandieson, R.W. Pathfinding in the Research Forest: The Pearl Harvesting Method for Effective Information Retrieval. Educ. Train. Dev. Disabil. 2006, 41, 401–409. Available online: https://www.jstor.org/stable/23879666 (accessed on 8 July 2020).
- McGreevy, P.D.; Boakes, R. Learning Theory and Positive Reinforcement. In Carrots and Sticks: Principles of Animal Training; McGreevy, P.D., Boakes, R., Eds.; Sydney University Press: Sydney, Australia, 2011; pp. 24–64. [Google Scholar]
- Lipsey, M.W.; Wilson, D.B. Practical Meta-Analysis, 1st ed.; Sage Publications Ltd.: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2001; pp. 1–247. [Google Scholar]
- Martinez de Andino, E.V.; McDonnell, S.M. Evaluation of operant learning in young foals using target training. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2017, 193, 67–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Langbein, J.; Siebert, K.; Nuernberg, G.; Manteuffel, G. The impact of acoustical secondary reinforcement during shape discrimination learning of dwarf goats (Capra hircus). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 103, 35–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Logan, L.R.; Hickman, R.R.; Harris, S.R.; Heriza, C.B. Single-subject research design: Recommendations for levels of evidence and quality rating. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2008, 50, 99–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bormann, I. DigitizeIt: Digitizer Software-Digitize Scanned Graph or Chart into (x,y)-Data. Available online: https://www.digitizeit.de (accessed on 20 May 2019).
- Parker, R.I.; Vannest, K.J.; Davis, J.L.; Sauber, S.B. Combining nonoverlap and trend for Single-Case Research: Tau-U. Behav. Ther. 2011, 42, 284–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Parker, R.I.; Vannest, K.J.; Davis, J.L. Effect size in single-case research: A review of nine nonoverlap techniques. Behav. Modif. 2011, 35, 303–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Vannest, K.J.; Ninci, J. Evaluating intervention effects in single-case research designs. J. Couns. Dev. 2015, 93, 403–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morin, K.L.; Ganz, J.B.; Vannest, K.J.; Haas, A.N.; Nagro, S.A.; Peltier, C.J.; Ura, S.K. A Systematic review of Single-Case Research on video analysis as professional development for special educators. J. Spec. Educ. 2019, 53, 3–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vannest, K.J.; Parker, R.I.; Gonen, O.; Adiguzel, T. Single-Case Research: Web-Based Calculators for SCR Analysis (Version 2.0) [Web-Based Application]; Texas A&M University: College Station, TX, USA, 2016; Available online: https://www.singlecaseresearch.org (accessed on 10 September 2019).
- Biostat, Inc. Comprehensive Meta-Analysis©, version 3.0; Computer Software; Biostat, Inc.: Englewood, NJ, USA, 2019; Available online: https://www.metaanalysis.com/index.php?cart=BMNJ1648138 (accessed on 12 June 2019).
- Borenstein, M.; Hedges, L.V.; Higgins, J.P.; Rothstein, H.R. Regression in Meta-Analysis. 2015. Available online: https://www.meta-analysis.com/downloads/MRManual.pdf (accessed on 9 January 2020).
- Borenstein, M.; Hedges, L.V.; Higgins, J.P.; Rothstein, H.R. Introduction to Meta-Analysis; John Wiley & Sons: Chichester, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Higgins, J.P.; Green, S. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions; The Cochrane Collaboration: London, UK, 2011; Available online: http://handbook-5-1.cochrane.org (accessed on 25 January 2020).
- Littell, J.H.; Corcoran, J.; Pillai, V. Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 1–193. [Google Scholar]
- Borenstein, M.; Higgins, J.P.T.; Hedges, L.V.; Rothstein, H.R. Basics of meta-analysis: I2 is not an absolute measure of heterogeneity. Res. Synth. Methods 2017, 8, 5–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Higgins, J.P.T.; Thompson, S.G. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat. Med. 2002, 21, 1539–1558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Batt, L.; Batt, M.; Baguley, J.; McGreevy, P. The effects of structured sessions for juvenile training and socialization on guide dog success and puppy-raiser participation. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2008, 3, 199–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blandina, A.G. To Click or Not to Click: The Impact of Positive Reinforcement Methods on the Acquisition of Behavior. Bachelor’s Thesis, University of Florida, Gainesville, FL, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Fernström, A.-L.; Fredlund, H.; Spangberg, M.; Westlund, K. Positive reinforcement training in Rhesus Macaques—Training progress as a result of training frequency. Am. J. Primatol. 2009, 71, 373–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fjellanger, R.; Andersen, E.K.; McLean, I.G. A Training Program for Filter-Search Mine Detection Dogs. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 2002, 15, 278–287. Available online: https://escholarship.org/uc/item/9xw413g4 (accessed on 8 July 2020).
- Flannery, B. Relational discrimination learning in horses. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1997, 54, 267–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Flynn, K.K. Conditioned Reinforcement with an Equine Subject. Master’s Thesis, North Texas State University, Denton, TX, USA, May 1980. [Google Scholar]
- Fugazza, C.; Miklosi, Á. Social learning in dog training: The effectiveness of the Do as I do method compared to shaping/clicker training. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2015, 171, 146–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guerrero-Flores, H.; Apresa-Garcia, T.; Garay-Villar, O.; Sanchez-Perez, A.; Flores-Villegas, D.; Bandera-Calderon, A.; Garcia-Palacios, R.; Rojas-Sanchez, T.; Romero-Morelos, P.; Sanches-Albor, V.; et al. A non-invasive tool for detecting cervical cancer odor by trained scent dogs. BMC Cancer 2017, 17, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Meyer, I.; Ladewig, J. The relationship between number of training sessions per week and learning in dogs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2008, 111, 311–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Strychalski, J.; Gugołek, A.; Konstantynowicz, M. Clicker Training Efficiency in Shaping the Desired Behaviour in the Following Dog Breeds: Boxer, Chow Chow and Yorkshire Terrier. Pol. J. Nat. Sci. 2015, 30, 235–243. Available online: http://www.uwm.edu.pl/polish-journal/sites/default/files/issues/articles/strychalski_et_al_2015_0.pdf (accessed on 8 July 2020).
- Thorn, J.M.; Templeton, J.J.; Van Winkle, K.M.M.; Castillo, R.R. Conditioning shelter dogs to sit. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2006, 9, 25–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Whistance, L.K.; Sinclair, L.A.; Arney, D.R.; Phillips, C.J.C. Trainability of eliminative behaviour in dairy heifers using a secondary reinforcer. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2009, 117, 128–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Willis, C.; Church, S.M.; Guest, C.M.; Cook, W.A.; McCarthy, N.; Bransbury, A.J.; Church, M.R.T.; Church, J.C.T. Olfactory detection of human bladder cancer by dogs: Proof of principle study. BMJ 2004, 329, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fernandez, E.J.; Dorey, N.J.; Rosales-Ruiz, J. A two-choice preference assessment with five cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2004, 7, 163–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chance, P. Learning and Behavior; Wadsworth Thomson Learning: Belmont, CA, USA, 2013; pp. 1–442. [Google Scholar]
- Guest, T. How to Practice Clicker Mechanics. 2012. Available online: https://www.clickertraining.com/how-to-practice-clicker-mechanics (accessed on 5 May 2020).
- Hurtado-Parrado, C.; López-López, W. Single-Case Research Methods: History and Suitability for a Psychological Science in Need of Alternatives. Integr. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 2015, 49, 323–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pfaller-Sadovsky, N.; Arnott, G.; Hurtado-Parrado, C. Using principles from applied behaviour analysis to address an undesired behaviour: Functional analysis and treatment of jumping up in companion dogs. Animals 2019, 9, 1091. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Alligood, C.A.; Dorey, N.R.; Mehrkam, L.R.; Leighty, K.A. Applying behavior-analytic methodology to the science and practice of environmental enrichment in zoos and aquariums. Zoo Biol. 2017, 36, 175–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Farhoody, P. Behavior Analysis. The Science of Training. Vet. Clin. N. Am.-Exot. Anim. Pract. 2012, 15, 361–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fernandez, E.J. Training petting zoo sheep to act like petting zoo sheep: An empirical evaluation of response-independent schedules and shaping with negative reinforcement. Animals 2020, 10, 1122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feuerbacher, E.N.; Muir, K.L. Using owner return as a reinforcer to operantly treat separation-related problem behavior in dogs. Animals 2020, 10, 1110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Protopopova, A.; Kisten, D.; Wynne, C. Evaluating a humane alternative to the bark collar: Automated differential reinforcement of not barking in a home-alone setting. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 2016, 49, 735–744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Poling, A.; Weetjens, B.; Cox, C.; Beyene, N.; Durgin, A.; Mahoney, A. Tuberculosis detection by Giant African Pouched Rats. Behav. Anal. 2011, 34, 47–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schlinger, H.D. The Importance of Analysis in Applied Behavior Analysis. Behav. Anal. Res. Pract. 2017, 17, 334–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mayir, B.; Bilecik, T.; Çakir, T.; Doǧan, U.; Gündüz, U.R.; Aslaner, A.; Oruç, M.T. Analysis of the publishing rate and the number of citations of general surgery dissertations. Turk. J. Surg. 2017, 33, 33–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Evans, S.C.; Amaro, C.M.; Herbert, R.; Blossom, J.B.; Roberts, M.C. “Are you gonna publish that?” Peer-reviewed publication outcomes of doctoral dissertations in psychology. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0192219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Branch, M.N.; Pennypacker, H.S. Generality And Generalization of Research Findings. In APA Handbook of Behavior Analysis; Madden, G.J., Ed.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013; Volume 1, pp. 151–175. [Google Scholar]
- Johnston, J.M. On the relation between generalization and generality. Behav. Anal. 1979, 2, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dorey, N.R.; Blandina, A.; Udell, M.A.R. Clicker training does not enhance learning in mixed-breed shelter puppies (Canis familiaris). J. Vet. Behav. 2020, 39, 57–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fernandez, E.J.; Rosales-Ruiz, J. A comparison of fixed-time food schedules and shaping involving a clicker for halter behavior in a petting zoo goat. Psychol. Rec. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martin, A.L.; Franklin, A.N.; Perlman, J.E.; Bloomsmith, M.A. Systematic assessment of food item preference and reinforcer effectiveness: Enhancements in training laboratory-housed rhesus macaques. Behav. Process. 2018, 157, 445–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Protopopova, A.; Brandifino, M.; Wynne, C.D.L. Preference assessments and structured potential adopter-dog interactions increase adoptions. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2016, 176, 87–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vicars, S.M.; Miguel, C.F.; Sobie, J.L. Assessing preference and reinforcer effectiveness in dogs. Behav. Process. 2014, 103, 75–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vitale-Shreve, K.R.; Mehrkam, L.R.; Udell, M.A.R. Social interaction, food, scent or toys? A formal assessment of domestic pet and shelter cat (Felis silvestris catus) preferences. Behav. Process. 2017, 141, 322–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gaalema, D.E.; Perdue, B.M.; Kelling, A.S. Food preference, keeper ratings, and reinforcer effectiveness in exotic animals: The value of systematic testing. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2011, 14, 33–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gallistel, C.R.; Craig, A.R.; Shahan, T.A. Contingency, contiguity, and causality in conditioning: Applying Information Theory and Weber’s Law to the assignment of credit problem. Psychol. Rev. 2019, 126, 761–773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McGreevy, P.D. Learning. In Equine Behavior: A Guide for Veterinarians and Equine Scientists, 2nd ed.; McGreevy, P.D., Ed.; Saunders Elsevier: Sydney, Australia, 2012; pp. 83–117. [Google Scholar]
- Mills, D.S. Training And Learning Protocols. In BSAVA Manual of Canine and Feline Behaviour, 3rd ed.; Horwitz, D.F., Mills, D.S., Eds.; British Small Animal Veterinary Association: Quedgeley, UK, 2010; pp. 49–64. [Google Scholar]
- Ramirez, K. Is a Clicker Necessary? 2017. Available online: https://www.clickertraining.com/is-a-clicker-necessary (accessed on 5 May 2020).
- Pryor, K.W. Charging the Clicker. 2006. Available online: https://www.clickertraining.com/node/824 (accessed on 5 May 2020).
- Ryan, T.; Mortensen, K. Outwitting Dogs: Revolutionary Techniques for Dog Training That Work! The Lyons Press: Guildford, CT, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Feng, L.C.; Howell, T.J.; Bennett, P.C. Comparing trainers’ reports of clicker use to the use of clickers in applied research studies: Methodological differences may explain conflicting results. Pet Behav. Sci. 2017, 3, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pryor, K.W. Getting Started: Clicker Training for Dogs; Sunshine Books, Inc.: Waltham, MA, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Feng, L.C.; Howell, T.J.; Bennett, P.C. Practices and perceptions of clicker use in dog training: A survey-based investigation of dog owners and industry professionals. J. Vet. Behav. Clin. Appl. Res. 2018, 23, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pear, J.J. The Science of Learning, 2nd ed.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2016; pp. 1–489. [Google Scholar]
- Silva, F.J.; Silva, K.M.; Pear, J.J. Sign- versus goal-tracking: Effects of conditioned-stimulus- to-unconditioned-stimulus distance. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 1992, 57, 17–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Ledford, J.R.; Barton, E.E.; Severini, K.E.; Zimmerman, K.N. A primer on Single-Case Research Designs: Contemporary use and analysis. Am. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2019, 124, 35–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pfaller-Sadovsky, N.; Medina, L.; Dillenburger, K.; Hurtado-Parrado, C. We don’t train in vain: A systematic review and meta-analysis of human and canine caregiver training. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2019, 23, 1–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hammond, D.; Gast, D.L. Descriptive Analysis of Single Subject Research Designs: 1983–2007. Educ. Train. Autism Dev. Disabil. 2010, 45, 187–202. Available online: https://www.jstor.org/stable/23879806 (accessed on 8 July 2020).
- Ledford, J.R.; King, S.; Harbin, E.R.; Zimmerman, K.N. Antecedent Social Skills Interventions for Individuals with ASD: What Works, for Whom, and Under What Conditions? Focus Autism Other Dev. Disabil. 2018, 33, 3–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pustejovsky, J.E.; Hedges, L.V. Design-comparable effect sizes in Multiple Baseline Designs: A general modeling framework. J. Educ. Behav. Stat. 2014, 39, 368–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Scruggs, T.E.; Mastopieri, M.A.; Casto, G. The quantitative synthesis of Single-Subject Research: Methodology and validation. Remedial Spec. Educ. 1987, 8, 24–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burns, M.K.; Zaslofsky, A.F.; Kanive, R.; Parker, D.C. Meta-Analysis of Incremental Rehearsal Using Phi Coefficients to Compare Single-Case and Group Designs. J. Behav. Educ. 2012, 21, 185–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feuerbacher, E.N.; Wynne, C.D.L. Shut up and pet me! Domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) prefer petting to vocal praise in concurrent and single-alternative choice procedures. Behav. Process. 2015, 110, 47–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Feuerbacher, E.N.; Wynne, C.D.L. Most domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) prefer food to petting: Population, context, and schedule effects in concurrent choice. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 2014, 101, 385–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hake, D.F. The basic-applied continuum and the possible evolution of human operant social and verbal research. Behav. Anal. 1982, 5, 21–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McIlvane, W.J. Translational behavior analysis: From laboratory science in stimulus control to intervention with persons with neurodevelopmental disabilities. Behav. Anal. 2009, 32, 273–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dube, W.V. Translational Research in Behavior Analysis. In APA Handbook of Behavior Analysis; Madden, G.J., Dube, W.V., Hackenberg, T.D., Hanley, G.P., Lattal, K.A., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013; pp. 65–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Details on SR+ Delivery | Definitions |
---|---|
Food by hand | Food was brought to the animals’ muzzles by hand (e.g., dogs, and horses) or was presented by hand in a manner that the animals could reach for the SR+ with a forelimb (e.g., rhesus macaques or squirrel monkeys). |
Food remotely | Food was delivered through an automated feeder which was remotely controlled by the experimenter. |
Food container | Food was made available to the animal in a container (e.g., feedbox for horses or a food bowl for dogs). |
Food proximity | The food was consistently delivered in the same place in close proximity to the experimenter (e.g., after the animal performed the task, the experimenter delivered the conditioned reinforcer and placed the food near to him/her on the floor [20]). |
Scratching | Scratching (e.g., chest neck or rump) was used as an alternative SR+ to food for foals because they were still nursing during the experiment [50]. |
Water | Water was available as an SR+ in a computer-controlled learning device located in a separate compartment of the home pen (stable) of the goats. The animals had 24 h access to the device, but only one goat could enter it at a time [51]. |
No SR+ | Only the Sr+ (conditioned reinforcer) was presented or reinforcement was entirely withheld (i.e., extinction). |
Not clearly stated | Type of SR+ and mode of delivery were not clearly stated. |
Doc ID | Study (Year) | Country | Publication Type | Learner Species | Learner Sex | Learner Age | Type of Conditioned Reinforcer | Setting, Training Location | Type of Study | Learner Target Behavior | Dependent Variables | Independent Variables |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1/1 | Batt et al. (2008) [66] | Australia | Peer-reviewed paper | Dog (Canis familiaris) | >50% male intact | ≤4 months | Clicker | Community hall | Group design | Attention, leave it, stand, loose-leash walking | Re-adoption of subsequent puppies | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
1/2 | Batt et al. (2008) [66] | Australia | Peer-reviewed paper | Dog (Canis familiaris) | >50% male intact | ≤4 months | Not applicable | Community hall | Group design | Attention, leave it, stand, loose-leash walking | Re-adoption of subsequent puppies | Control (treatment as usual) |
2/1 | Blandina (2010) [67] | USA | Bachelor thesis | Dog (Canis familiaris) | Not clearly stated | ≤6 months | Clicker | Shelter room | Group design | Stay | Duration | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
2/2 | Blandina (2010) [67] | USA | Bachelor thesis | Dog (Canis familiaris) | Not clearly stated | ≤6 months | Spoken word | Shelter room | Group design | Stay | Duration | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
2/3 | Blandina (2010) [67] | USA | Bachelor thesis | Dog (Canis familiaris) | Not clearly stated | ≤6 months | Not applicable | Shelter room | Group design | Stay | Duration | Food only |
3/1 | Chiandetti et al. (2016) [20] | Italy | Peer-reviewed paper | Dog (Canis familiaris) | >50% female intact | 1–5 years | Clicker | Home | Group design | Open box by pushing up the lid with nose | Number of attempts over time | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
3/2 | Chiandetti et al. (2016) [20] | Italy | Peer-reviewed paper | Dog (Canis familiaris) | >50% female intact | 1–5 years | Spoken word | Home | Group design | Open box by pushing up the lid with nose | Number of attempts over time | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
3/3 | Chiandetti et al. (2016) [20] | Italy | Peer-reviewed paper | Dog (Canis familiaris) | >50% female intact | 1–5 years | Not applicable | Home | Group design | Open box by pushing up the lid with nose | Number of attempts over time | Food only |
4/1 | D’Onofrio (2015) [32] | USA | Master’s thesis | Dog (Canis familiaris) | 50% female | 1–9 years | Clicker | Home | Single-case method | Retrieve medicine bag and pick up wallet | Frequency | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
5/1 | Feng et al. (2018) [21] | Australia | Peer-reviewed paper | Dog (Canis familiaris) | >50% male neutered | 1–5 years | Clicker | Home | Group design | Spin, hand target, object target, on your mat | Frequency | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
5/3 | Feng et al. (2018) [21] | Australia | Peer-reviewed paper | Dog (Canis familiaris) | >50% male neutered | 1–5 years | Not applicable | Home | Group design | Spin, hand target, object target, on your mat | Frequency | Control (waiting list) |
6/1 | Ferguson et al. (2001) [22] | USA | Peer-reviewed paper | Horse (Equus caballus) | Female intact | 5–18 years | Clicker | Outdoor enclosure | Single-case method | Nose touch to a cloth potholder | Latency | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
6/2 | Ferguson et al. (2001) [22] | USA | Peer-reviewed paper | Horse (Equus caballus) | Female intact | 5–18 years | Clicker | Outdoor enclosure | Single-case method | Nose touch to a cloth potholder | Parts of horse entering | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
7/1 | Fernandez (2003) [4] | USA | Master’s thesis | Goat (Capra hircus) | Male neutered | <12 months | Clicker | Outdoor enclosure | Single-case method | Slipping head into halter | Shaping steps completed | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
8/1 | Fernström et al. (2009) [68] | Sweden | Peer-reviewed paper | Rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) | Female intact | 1–5 years | Clicker | Indoor enclosure | Group design | Target, cooperative behavior, box and injections | Number of sessions to completion | Treatment intensities |
9/1 | Fjellanger et al. (2002) [69] | Norway | Peer-reviewed paper | Dog (Canis familiaris) | >50% male intact | 1–5 years | Whistle | Training facility | Case study | Scent discrimination (explosives) | Proportion correct responses | Conditioned Reinforcement and INT food |
10/1 | Flannery (1997) [70] | USA | Peer-reviewed paper | Horse (Equus caballus) | Not clearly stated | 12–24 years | Clicker | Stable | Case study | Touching labeled stimulus cards (discrimination task) | Percent correct trials | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
10/2 | Flannery (1997) [70] | USA | Peer-reviewed paper | Horse (Equus caballus) | Not clearly stated | 12–24 years | Clicker | Stable | Case study | Higher-order discrimination task | Percent correct trials | Conditioned Reinforcement and INT food |
11/1 | Flynn (1980) [71] | USA | Master’s thesis | Horse (Equus caballus) | Female intact | >15 years | Clicker | Stable | Group design | Walk around cone, open mouth, stepping diagonally, still for 60 sec, nodding | Count correct responses | Conditioned Reinforcement and INT food |
12/1 | Fugazza et al. (2015) [72] | Italy | Peer-reviewed paper | Dog (Canis familiaris) | Not clearly stated | 1–12 years | Clicker | Training facility | Group design | Open a sliding door, jumping in the air | Latency | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
12/2 | Fugazza et al. (2015) [72] | Italy | Peer-reviewed paper | Dog (Canis familiaris) | Not clearly stated | 1–12 years | Not applicable | Training facility | Group design | Open a sliding door, jumping in the air | Latency | Modeling the response |
13/1 | Gillis et al. (2012) [5] | USA | Peer-reviewed paper | Squirrel monkey (Saimiri boliviensis) | Male intact | 1–5 years | Clicker | Laboratory | Case study | Target and duration training, glove desensitization, chain and pole, injection training | Time to criterion | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
14/1 | Grant et al. (2019) [6] | UK | Peer-reviewed paper | Cat (Cattus syslvestris) | >50% male neutered | Not clearly stated | Clicker | Shelter room | Group design | Duration exploratory behavior | Duration | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
15/1 | Guerrero-Flores et al. (2017) [73] | Mexico | Peer-reviewed paper | Dog (Canis familiaris) | Male intact | 1–5 years | Clicker | Laboratory | Case study | Indicate target odor by sitting in front of sample | Frequency | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
16/1 | Häderer et al. (2016) [33] | France | Peer-reviewed paper | Fish (Tripterygion tripteronotum) | Mixed | Not clearly stated | Clicker | Natural environment/home range | Group design | Target black/white chip with muzzle | Count correct responses | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
16/2 | Häderer et al. (2016) [33] | France | Peer-reviewed paper | Fish (Tripterygion tripteronotum) | Mixed | Not clearly stated | Not applicable | Natural environment/home range | Group design | Target black/white chip with muzzle | Count correct responses | Food only |
17/1 | Hendriksen et al. (2011) [10] | Denmark | Peer-reviewed paper | Horse (Equus caballus) | Mixed | 7–20 years | Clicker | Stable | Group design | Stepping into trailer on cue | Time to criterion | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
17/2 | Hendriksen et al. (2011) [10] | Denmark | Peer-reviewed paper | Horse (Equus caballus) | Mixed | 7–20 years | Not applicable | Stable | Group design | Stepping into trailer on cue | Time to criterion | Negative reinforcement |
18/1 | Langbein et al. (2007) [51] | Germany | Peer-reviewed paper | Goat (Capra hircus) | Male intact | ≤12 months | Beep | Stable | Group design | Shape discrimination | Number of sessions to completion | Conditioned Reinforcement and water |
18/2 | Langbein et al. (2007) [51] | Germany | Peer-reviewed paper | Goat (Capra hircus) | Male intact | ≤12 months | Not applicable | Stable | Group design | Shape discrimination | Number of sessions to completion | Control (water only) |
19/1 | Lansade et al. (2018) [24] | France | Peer-reviewed paper | Horse (Equus caballus) | >50% female intact | ≤12 months | Spoken word | Stable | Group design | Nose touch cone | Count correct responses | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
19/2 | Lansade et al. (2018) [24] | France | Peer-reviewed paper | Horse (Equus caballus) | >50% female intact | ≤12 months | Not applicable | Stable | Group design | Nose touch cone | Count correct responses | Food only |
20/1 | McCall et al. (2002) [25] | USA | Peer-reviewed paper | Horse (Equus caballus) | Not clearly stated | 6 months–15 years | Buzzing sound | Stable | Group design | Pushing a lever | Mean training time | Conditioned Reinforcement and food (Phase I) |
20/2 | McCall et al. (2002) [25] | USA | Peer-reviewed paper | Horse (Equus caballus) | Not clearly stated | 6 months–15 years | Not applicable | Stable | Group design | Pushing a lever | Mean training time | Food only (Phase I) |
21/1 | Meyer et al. (2008) [74] | Denmark | Peer-reviewed paper | Dog (Canis familiaris) | 50% female | 1–5 years | Clicker | Laboratory | Group design | Paw target mouse pad | Number of sessions to completion | Treatment intensities |
21/2 | Meyer et al. (2008) [74] | Denmark | Peer-reviewed paper | Dog (Canis familiaris) | 50% female | 1–5 years | Clicker | Laboratory | Group design | Paw target mouse pad | Number of sessions to completion | Treatment intensities |
22/1 | Pfaller-Sadovsky et al. (2017) [8] | Austria | Peer-reviewed paper | Dog (Canis familiaris) | 50% female | 1–9 years | Clicker | Home | Single-case method | Delivering PI to hand | Percent trials | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
23/1 | Smith et al. (2008) [26] | USA | Peer-reviewed paper | Dog (Canis familiaris) | Mixed | 1–9 years | Clicker | Home | Group design | Nose touch cone | Latency | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
23/2 | Smith et al. (2008) [26] | USA | Peer-reviewed paper | Dog (Canis familiaris) | Mixed | 1–9 years | Clicker | Home | Group design | Nose touch cone | Latency | Conditioned Reinforcement and INT food |
23/3 | Smith et al. (2008) [26] | USA | Peer-reviewed paper | Dog (Canis familiaris) | Mixed | 1–9 years | Not applicable | Home | Group design | Nose touch cone | Latency | Control (food only) |
24/1 | Strychalski et al. (2015) [75] | Poland | Peer-reviewed paper | Dog (Canis familiaris) | 50% female | >4 years | Clicker | Home | Group design | Around cones | Number of sessions to completion | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
25/1 | Thorn et al. (2006) [76] | USA | Peer-reviewed paper | Dog (Canis familiaris) | 50% female | Not clearly stated | Spoken word | Shelter room | Case study | Sit (Experiment 1) | Latency | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
25/2 | Thorn et al. (2006) [76] | USA | Peer-reviewed paper | Dog (Canis familiaris) | 50% female | Not clearly stated | Spoken word | Outdoor enclosure | Group design | Sit (Experiment 2) | Latency | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
25/3 | Thorn et al. (2006) [76] | USA | Peer-reviewed paper | Dog (Canis familiaris) | 50% female | Not clearly stated | Clicker | Outdoor enclosure | Group design | Sit (Experiment 2) | Latency | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
26/1 | Wennmacher (2007) [27] | USA | Master’s thesis | Dog (Canis familiaris) | Mixed | 1–5 years | Clicker | Home | Single-case method | Spin, bow | Count correct responses | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
26/2 | Wennmacher (2007) [27] | USA | Master’s thesis | Dog (Canis familiaris) | Mixed | 1–5 years | Clicker | Home | Single-case method | Spin, bow | Count correct responses | Conditioned Reinforcement and INT food |
27/1 | Whistance et al. (2009) [77] | UK | Peer-reviewed paper | Cattle (Bos taurus) | Female intact | 12–24 years | Clicker | Stable | Case study | Eliminate in designated area | Count correct responses | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
28/1 | Williams et al. (2004) [34] | USA | Peer-reviewed paper | Horse (Equus caballus) | >50% male neutered | >4 years | Clicker | Stable | Group design | Nose touch cone | Number of sessions to completion | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
28/2 | Williams et al. (2004) [34] | USA | Peer-reviewed paper | Horse (Equus caballus) | >50% male neutered | >4 years | Clicker | Stable | Group design | Nose touch cone | Number of sessions to completion | Conditioned Reinforcement and INT food |
28/3 | Williams et al. (2004) [34] | USA | Peer-reviewed paper | Horse (Equus caballus) | >50% male neutered | >4 years | Not applicable | Stable | Group design | Nose touch cone | Number of sessions to completion | Food only |
29/1 | Willis et al. (2004) [78] | UK | Peer-reviewed paper | Dog (Canis familiaris) | Mixed | Not clearly stated | Clicker | Training facility | Case study | Scent discrimination (healthy/ill) | Count correct responses | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
30/1 | Willson et al. (2017) [28] | New Zealand | Peer-reviewed paper | Cat (Cattus syslvestris) | >50% male neutered | 6 months–15 years | Beep | Shelter room | Group design | Nose touching a target | Shaping steps completed | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
30/2 | Willson et al. (2017) [28] | New Zealand | Peer-reviewed paper | Cat (Cattus syslvestris) | >50% male neutered | 6 months–15 years | Not applicable | Shelter room | Group design | Nose touching a target | Shaping steps completed | Food only |
30/3 | Willson et al. (2017) [28] | New Zealand | Peer-reviewed paper | Cat (Cattus syslvestris) | >50% male neutered | 6 months–15 years | Beep | Shelter room | Group design | Nose touching a target | Number of attempts over time | Conditioned Reinforcement only |
30/4 | Willson et al. (2017) [28] | New Zealand | Peer-reviewed paper | Cat (Cattus syslvestris) | >50% male neutered | 6 months–15 years | Beep | Shelter room | Group design | Nose touching a target | Number of attempts over time | Conditioned Reinforcement only |
31/1 | Wood (2007) [29] | USA | Master’s thesis | Dog (Canis familiaris) | >50% male neutered | 1–5 years | Clicker | Shelter room | Group design | Nose touching a freestanding target | Required Rs to reach criterion | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
31/2 | Wood (2007) [29] | USA | Master’s thesis | Dog (Canis familiaris) | >50% male neutered | 1–5 years | Spoken word | Shelter room | Group design | Nose touching a freestanding target | Required Rs to reach criterion | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
32/1 | Rybova (2018) [23] | New Zealand | Master’s thesis | Horse (Equus caballus) | 50% female | 7–28 years | Clicker | Stable | Single-case method | Nose-touch target stick | Count correct responses | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
33/1 | Martinez de Andino et al. (2017) [50] | USA | Peer-reviewed paper | Horse (Equus caballus) | 50% female | ≤6 months | Spoken word | Outdoor enclosure | Group design | Touching a floor target with nose (i.e., stone) | Latency | Conditioned Reinforcement and tactile |
33/2 | Martinez de Andino et al. (2017) [50] | USA | Peer-reviewed paper | Horse (Equus caballus) | 50% female | ≤6 months | Spoken word | Outdoor enclosure | Group design | Touching a floor target with nose (i.e., stone) | Count correct responses | Conditioned Reinforcement and tactile |
33/3 | Martinez de Andino et al. (2017) [50] | USA | Peer-reviewed paper | Horse (Equus caballus) | 50% female | ≤6 months | Spoken word | Outdoor enclosure | Group design | Touching a floor target with nose (i.e., stone) | Correct response to verbal prompt rate | Conditioned Reinforcement and tactile |
33/4 | Martinez de Andino et al. (2017) [50] | USA | Peer-reviewed paper | Horse (Equus caballus) | 50% female | ≤6 months | Spoken word | Outdoor enclosure | Group design | Touching a floor target with nose (i.e., stone) | Latency | Conditioned Reinforcement and tactile |
33/5 | Martinez de Andino et al. (2017) [50] | USA | Peer-reviewed paper | Horse (Equus caballus) | 50% female | ≤6 months | Spoken word | Outdoor enclosure | Group design | Touching a floor target with nose (i.e., stone) | Count correct responses | Conditioned Reinforcement and tactile |
33/6 | Martinez de Andino et al. (2017) [50] | USA | Peer-reviewed paper | Horse (Equus caballus) | 50% female | ≤6 months | Spoken word | Outdoor enclosure | Group design | Touching a floor target with nose (i.e., stone) | Correct response to verbal prompt rate | Conditioned Reinforcement and tactile |
34/1 | Slater et al. (2011) [9] | UK | Peer-reviewed paper | Horse (Equus caballus) | Male neutered | 5–18 years | Clicker | Outdoor enclosure | Single-case method | Loading trailer | Loading steps completed | Conditioned Reinforcement and food |
34/2 | Slater et al. (2011) [9] | UK | Peer-reviewed paper | Horse (Equus caballus) | Male neutered | 5–18 years | Clicker | Stable | Single-case method | Lifting hoof | Duration | Conditioned Reinforcement and INT food |
Learner Species | Type of Sr+ | Food | Scratching | Water | No SR+ | Not clearly Stated | Total | ||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
By Hand | Remotely | Container | Proximity | ||||||||||||||||
Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | ||
Cat (Cattus syslvestris) | Beep | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% |
Clicker | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | |
Cattle (Bos taurus) | Clicker | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% |
Dog (Canis familiaris) | Clicker | 11 | 32% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 14 | 41% |
Spoken word | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | |
Whistle | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | |
Fish (Tripterygion tripteronotum) | Clicker | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% |
Goat (Capra hircus) | Beep | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% |
Clicker | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | |
Horse (Equus caballus) | Buzzing sound | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% |
Clicker | 5 | 15% | 0 | 0% | 2 | 6% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 7 | 21% | |
Spoken word | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 2 | 6% | |
Rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) | Clicker | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% |
Squirrel monkey (Saimiri boliviensis) | Clicker | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% |
Total | 22 | 65% | 1 | 3% | 5 | 15% | 2 | 6% | 1 | 3% | 1 | 3% | 1 | 3% | 1 | 3% | 34 | 100% |
Type of SR+ | Preference Assessment | Count | % | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Food | Food by hand | No | 20 | 59% |
Yes | 1 | 3% | ||
Not clearly stated | 1 | 3% | ||
Food remotely | No | 1 | 3% | |
Food container | No | 5 | 15% | |
Food proximity | No | 1 | 3% | |
Not clearly stated | 1 | 3% | ||
Scratching | Yes | 1 | 3% | |
Water | Not applicable | 1 | 3% | |
No SR+ | Not applicable | 1 | 3% | |
Not clearly stated | Not clearly stated | 1 | 3% | |
Total | 34 | 100% |
CS-US Pairing Information | Interstimulus Interval ISI | Count | % |
---|---|---|---|
>20 pairings (no further info) | 30 s–3 min | 1 | 3% |
Not clearly stated | 1 | 3% | |
≤20 pairings/day | Approximately 1 s (trace) | 2 | 6% |
Approximately 10 s | 1 | 3% | |
No delay (simultaneous) | 1 | 3% | |
Not clearly stated | 5 | 15% | |
20–40 pairings/day | Immediately | 1 | 3% |
No delay (simultaneous) | 1 | 3% | |
Not clearly stated | 2 | 6% | |
60 pairings/day | Not clearly stated | 1 | 3% |
Pairings implemented (no further info) | Immediately | 1 | 3% |
No delay (simultaneous) | 1 | 3% | |
Not clearly stated | 3 | 9% | |
No pairing sessions | Not applicable | 10 | 29% |
Not clearly stated | Not clearly stated | 3 | 9% |
Total | 34 | 100% |
Delay R → Sr+ | Beep | Buzzing Sound | Clicker | Spoken Word | Whistle | Total | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | |
Approximately 1 s | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% |
Immediately | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 3 | 9% | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 4 | 12% |
Immediately after | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% |
Simultaneous | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 3 | 9% | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 4 | 12% |
Shortly after | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% |
Not clearly stated | 2 | 6% | 1 | 3% | 18 | 53% | 1 | 3% | 1 | 3% | 23 | 68% |
Total | 2 | 6% | 1 | 3% | 27 | 79% | 3 | 9% | 1 | 3% | 34 | 100% |
Interval Sr+ → SR+ | Food | Scratching | Water | No SR+ | Not Clearly Stated | Total | ||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Food by Hand | Food Remotely | Food Container | Food Proximity | |||||||||||||||
Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | |
Approximately 1 s | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 2 | 6% |
Following | 2 | 6% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 3 | 9% |
Immediately | 3 | 9% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 5 | 15% |
Shortly after | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% |
No delay (simultaneous) | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 2 | 6% |
Not clearly stated | 15 | 44% | 1 | 3% | 2 | 6% | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 20 | 59% |
Not applicable * | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% |
Total | 22 | 65% | 1 | 3% | 5 | 15% | 2 | 6% | 1 | 3% | 1 | 3% | 1 | 3% | 1 | 3% | 34 | 100% |
Learner Species | In Front, SR+ Attached | In Front, SR+ Proximity | In Front, No SR+ | Outside View, SR+ Proximity | Peripheral View, SR+ Attached | Peripheral View, SR+ Proximity | Not Applicable | Not Clearly Stated | Total | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | Count | % | |
Cat (Cattus syslvestris) | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 2 | 6% | 2 | 6% |
Cattle (Bos taurus) | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 1 | 3% |
Dog (Canis familiaris) | 8 | 24% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 8 | 24% | 16 | 47% |
Fish (Tripterygion tripteronotum) | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% |
Goat (Capra hircus) | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 1 | 3% | 2 | 6% |
Horse (Equus caballus) | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 1 | 3% | 1 | 3% | 1 | 3% | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 5 | 15% | 10 | 29% |
Rhesus monkey (Macaca mulatta) | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 1 | 3% |
Squirrel monkey (Saimiri boliviensis) | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 0 | 0% | 1 | 3% |
Total | 8 | 24% | 3 | 9% | 1 | 3% | 1 | 3% | 1 | 3% | 1 | 3% | 1 | 3% | 18 | 53% | 34 | 100% |
Author (Year) | Species | #Subjects | Design | Reinforcers | Tau-U (CI95%) | Contrasts | Effect | Design Quality |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Ferguson and Rosales-Ruiz (2001) [21] | Horse | 5 | MBL | Click and food | 0.95 (0.78, 1) | 15 | Large | Strong |
Fernandez * (2003) [4] | Goat | 3 | CCD | Click and food | 0.51 (0.3, 0.71) | 6 | Small | Strong |
Pfaller-Sadovsky et al. (2017) [8] | Dog | 4 | MBL | Click and food | 0.77 (0.49, 1) | 7 | Medium | Strong |
Rybova (2018) * [30] | Horse | 4 | MBL | Click and food | 0.90 (0.59, 1) | 8 | Medium | Moderate |
Wennmacher * (2007) [25] | Dog | 2 | RVD | Click and food versus Click-click food | 0.48 (0.33, 0.61) | 12 | Small | Moderate |
Slater and Dymond (2011) [9] | Horse | 5 | MBL | Click and food | 0.98 (0.73, 1) | 4 | Large | Strong |
Moderator Variables | Number of Covariates | Regression ES and (SE) | Q | p-Values | R2 (%) |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Learner species | 3 | 20.57 | 0.000 * | 1 | |
Dogs | 0.573 (0.067) | ||||
Goats | 0.574 (0.11) | ||||
Horses | 0.94 (0.09) | ||||
Pairing sessions | 4 | 1.42 | 0.70 | 0 | |
>20 pairing sessions | 0.94 (0.23) | ||||
20–40 pairings/day | 0.76 (0.28) | ||||
60 pairings/day | 0.90 (0.35) | ||||
No pairings | 0.63 (0.29) | ||||
Position of trainer with Sr+ | 3 | 1.28 | 0.53 | 0 | |
In front of animal w/Sr+ attached | 0.63 (0.16) | ||||
Not clearly stated | 0.82 (0.20) | ||||
Within peripheral view w/Sr+ attached | 0.90 (0.29) | ||||
Study design | 3 | 26.86 | 0.0001 * | 1 | |
MBL | 0.92 (0.06) | ||||
Changing criterion | 0.34 (0.10) | ||||
Reversal/withdrawal | 0.44 (0.09) | ||||
Target behavior by | 4 | 2.59 | 0.46 | 12 | |
Capturing w/movement restrictions | 0.57 (0.20) | ||||
Capturing w/o movement restrictions | 0.94 (0.28) | ||||
Shaping w/prompts | 0.67 (0.25) | ||||
Shaping w/o prompts | 0.88 (0.26) | ||||
Trainer type | 3 | 0.11 | 0.94 | 0 | |
Experimenter | 0.80 (0.15) | ||||
Mixed | 0.72 (0.25) | ||||
Owner | 0.78 (0.32) |
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Pfaller-Sadovsky, N.; Hurtado-Parrado, C.; Cardillo, D.; Medina, L.G.; Friedman, S.G. What’s in a Click? The Efficacy of Conditioned Reinforcement in Applied Animal Training: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Animals 2020, 10, 1757. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10101757
Pfaller-Sadovsky N, Hurtado-Parrado C, Cardillo D, Medina LG, Friedman SG. What’s in a Click? The Efficacy of Conditioned Reinforcement in Applied Animal Training: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Animals. 2020; 10(10):1757. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10101757
Chicago/Turabian StylePfaller-Sadovsky, Nicole, Camilo Hurtado-Parrado, Daniela Cardillo, Lucia G. Medina, and Susan G. Friedman. 2020. "What’s in a Click? The Efficacy of Conditioned Reinforcement in Applied Animal Training: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis" Animals 10, no. 10: 1757. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani10101757