4.1. Implications of Findings
This study explored the cultural differences in the impact of the variables cited by Joiner’s interpersonal theory of suicide (
Joiner 2005) on suicide risk and depression symptoms in young adults from Spain and Japan. The fact that there were such slight differences in the categorical variables in the Spanish sample and non-significant differences in the Japanese sample would reinforce the validity of the comparison between both countries.
Whereas ‘perceived burdensomeness’ stood out as the most relevant variable in the models of both samples, the other independent variables showed some difference by explaining part of the variability in the dependent variable and improving the model although more modestly. The relevance of ‘perceived burdensomeness’ in suicide risk was already highlighted in high-risk populations (
Lieberman et al. 2023). The overall scores of the Japanese sample on this variable were more than double those of the Spanish sample, showing a significant difference. Although certain cultural values may protect against the ‘perceived burdensomeness’, like enduring challenges (
Taku and Arai 2023), this did not prevent this interpersonal dimension from remaining higher in Japanese young adults, perhaps due to structural, social, or normative pressure factors. To verify H1 in the Japanese sample about the effect of the measures of the independent variables over the ‘suicide risk’, we performed a simple regression model with the C-SSRS, which showed similar results in explaining the model with the main influence of INQ-PB and ACSS-FAD. The same analysis with PHQ-9 results showed better outcomes of the model only including the INQ-PB, with positively statistically significant higher beta values related to ‘depression symptoms’. On the other hand, in the Spanish sample, beta values of the interaction were not significant for the dependent variables. So, the association hypothesis would be partially fulfilled only in the Japanese population. These results find support in
Nakamura et al.’s (
2022) findings, where Japanese cultural characteristics such as self-reliance, a lower tendency to seek professional support, and limited access to psychological resources can intensify the perceptions of being a burden on acquaintances or the ability to take one’s own action to resolve problems. Although the interaction variable did not provide major differences, in the stepwise regression, models each of the independent variables explained part of the variability in both samples. Answering to what extent the association of these three independent variables explain differences in suicide risk in both samples, we found similar results both in the first simple Pearson correlations and in the linear regression model to those found in comparative studies with different cultural samples (
Suh et al. 2017). Despite relationships between some pairs of these three variables (e.g., INQ-TB and INQ-PB), the three-way interaction was also non-significant.
The H2 was not fully met when we compared the standardized beta coefficients of the ANOVA between Spain and Japanese samples. We found in the Spanish sample higher statistically significant beta values than in the Japanese sample, both with the PHQ-9 (B = 0.971 vs. B = 0.620) as a measure for the dependent variable and with the C-SSRS (B = 0.502 vs. B = 0.372), concluding a stronger relationship between the three independent variables and ‘suicide risk’ in the Spanish group. These results make sense since the ITS has been applied mostly in Western cultures, although its validation in Eastern cultures is increasingly widespread (
Lai 2019;
Suh et al. 2017;
Zhang et al. 2013).However, ‘thwarted belongingness’ appeared as an important predictor (B = 0.184,
p < 0.05) in model 2 of the Japanese sample for the dependent variable measured by the PHQ-9. In Japan, ‘perceived burdensomeness’ and ‘thwarted belongingness’ have been linked to the hikikomori phenomenon, which reflects extreme social withdrawal, suggesting that cultural factors may intensify suicide risk when comparing these variables with countries like the United States (
Taku and Arai 2023). In the Spanish sample, the independent variable ‘thwarted belongingness’ only appeared in the third model using the C-SSRS. From the greatest to least extent, the most relevant factor in the Spanish sample to predict ‘suicide risk’ was ‘perceived burdensomeness’, followed by ‘acquired capability for suicide’, and in last place ‘thwarted belongingness’. In a Spanish study on ITS with university students, ‘thwarted belongingness’ and ‘perceived burdensomeness’ were found as direct predictors of suicidal ideation, but indirect pathways were also seen through the variable ‘hopelessness’ (
Pérez Rodríguez et al. 2023) in adolescents; equally strong results were found for the variable ‘perceived burdensomeness’, which was relevant in the predictive model, but ‘thwarted belongingness’ also correlated significantly with suicide risk (
Caro-Cañizares et al. 2024). Although general levels of ‘thwarted belongingness’ were slightly higher in the Spanish sample, this difference did not translate into a statistically robust relationship with ‘suicide risk’. ‘Thwarted belongingness’ replaced ‘acquired capability’ in the importance it had in contributing to the variability in the Japanese sample, which could be explained given the collectivist nature of Japanese society. Nevertheless, studies in other countries also considered collectivist, such as China, did not find the same weight in this variable (
Zhang et al. 2013), fitting better with the results of other studies that have compared samples from collectivist and individualist cultures (Korea vs. U.S.) (
Suh et al. 2017), where no significant differences were found in the ways that ‘thwarted belongingness’, ‘acquired capability’, and ‘perceived burdensomeness’ contribute to explain suicide behavior.
The H3 was not fulfilled since in the regression model in the Spanish sample ACSS-FAD appeared as a relevant predictor, with the dependent variable’s measure C-SSRS and having higher values: B = 0.292 in model 2 and B = 0.280 in model 3 (
p < 0.001) vs. B = 0.150 (
p = 0.038) in Japanese. This differs from the findings of studies that sought to examine the role of ‘acquired capability for suicide’ as an essential component of moving from suicidal ideation to suicide attempt, studying this problem first from the perspective of a traditional Asian culture, such as Korean (
Park et al. 2017). Nevertheless, the Spanish results are consistent with previous research conducted in clinical Spanish population that indicated the ‘acquired capacity for suicide’ as a relevant factor in predicting suicide risk together with mentalizing, significantly mediating the relationship between suicidal ideation and the lethality of the suicide attempt, as well as self-harm behaviors (
Andreo-Jover et al. 2024). Even mentalizing is not exclusive to individualistic societies; in these cultures, there are often more social and educational values that promote it. However, H4 was partially fulfilled, as the ‘acquired capability for suicide’ emerged as the second-best predictor of suicide risk in the regression models with C-SSRS, but with the dependent variable measured by the PHQ-9, in the Japanese sample, the ACSS-FAD was excluded due to a non-significant negative beta value (beta = −0.30,
p = 0.622), and in the Spanish sample, it explained little variability (beta = 0.036).
The results of Levene’s test indicated no significant differences in age distribution between the two samples as expected, since we chose a specific age range (18–40) for young adults, minimizing potential confounding effects related to this variable when comparing the samples’ results. Regarding sex, we only observed significant differences in ‘capability for suicide’ and ‘thwarted belongingness’ in the Spanish sample and only in this first variable in the Japanese sample. Highlighting men in these constructs suggests that men may perceive themselves as more capable of engaging in suicidal behavior and experience higher levels of ‘thwarted belongingness’ compared to women, which aligns with previous research (
Witte et al. 2012). The construct of ‘thwarted belongingness’, which is related to ‘loneliness’, could be explained in the Spanish sample based on the results of the Barometer Report on Unwanted Loneliness in Spain
Fundación ONCE and Fundación AXA (
2024), which highlights that 20% of the national population suffers from unwanted loneliness, with a four times higher percentage of people showing suicidal behavior.
As for the other categorical variables we examined, significant differences were only found in the Spanish sample. The univariate general linear model revealed that the PHQ-9 and C-SSRS values in the Spanish sample were significantly related to occupation, playing a key role in explaining variance in depression scores, with a moderate effect size that indicated a relatively strong relationship between occupation and depression. These findings suggest that employment is associated with lower levels of depression, while individuals who are unemployed or balancing work and study reported higher levels of depressive symptoms, consistent with
World Health Organization (
2024b) findings on mental health that describe decent work as aiding integration and improving social functioning. This could be due to various factors, such as stability compared to the potential stress and financial insecurity associated with unemployment (
Sinyor et al. 2024) or managing both work and study simultaneously.
Regarding the marital status variable, significant differences were also observed in PHQ-9 results between single individuals and married individuals, indicating that marital status may influence mental health, fitting in with the literature indicating that married individuals report lower risk of suicidal thoughts (
Stephenson et al. 2023).
The findings from the Spanish and Japanese samples revealed noteworthy similarities and differences in the prevalence of depression based on PHQ-9 scores: 35.26% of the Spanish sample reported experiencing mild depressive symptoms compared to 30.5% of the Japanese sample who reported moderate symptoms, suggesting that while the prevalence of depressive symptoms in both populations is notable, the severity of depression may vary between the two groups, so the comparison can reflect relative differences. The greater prevalence of moderate depressive symptoms in the Japanese sample could reflect a different cultural understanding of depression or varying thresholds for what is considered ‘moderate’ (
Kitanaka 2012). The higher levels of depression may reflect a general increase in the prevalence of major depressive disorders in that country, especially among young people, in the post-COVID-19 pandemic context. Recent data based on real-life health records showed that the prevalence of depression in people under 20 years of age in Japan increased from 3.7% to 4.8% between 2019 and 2020 (
Demiya et al. 2022), suggesting an upward trend, as well as in Spain, where according to official data, in 2022, more than 2.4 million cases of depression were treated in primary care (
Statista 2024b).
When comparing suicide risk findings using the C-SSRS, the Spanish sample showed a higher proportion of participants reporting no risk, whereas the Japanese sample exhibited more individuals with elevated levels of risk, which aligns with the national suicide rates of both countries (
World Population Review 2025). This suggests that the magnitude of these indicators cannot be attributed to sample size but may reflect cultural, social, or structural differences specific to the Japanese context. As Japan has a population approximately three times that of Spain, the number of suicides is more than five times higher (
Instituto Nacional de Estadística 2023;
Statista 2024a), demonstrating a disproportionate burden of this phenomenon in Japanese society.
In terms of the ‘perceived burdensomeness’ subscale of the INQ, the Japanese sample had a significantly higher percentage (57.6%) of participants scoring above the threshold compared to the Spanish sample (29.6%). Recent research demonstrated that ‘perceived burdensomeness’ is a significant predictor of a pre-suicidal cognitive–affective state, mediating the effect of emotional dysregulation (
Lidia et al. 2025). This difference could be indicative of cultural variations in how individuals in each country view their personal impact on others, with Japanese individuals potentially more likely to internalize feelings of burden due to social and cultural norms (
Gross 1998). On the other hand, the subscale of ‘thwarted belongingness’ showed similar percentages between the two groups although with significant differences, showing the feeling of not belonging or being isolated may be a cross-cultural experience for both populations albeit to a slightly different extent (
Badman et al. 2022;
Fundación ONCE and Fundación AXA 2024). Lastly, in the ACSS-FAD, the Japanese sample had a lower mean compared to the Spanish sample, contrary to what was expected given the attitude towards this problem in Japanese society (
Otsuka et al. 2020).