Next Article in Journal
Effects of Simulated Heat Waves on Life History Traits of a Host Feeding Parasitoid
Next Article in Special Issue
Acetylcholine and Its Receptors in Honeybees: Involvement in Development and Impairments by Neonicotinoids
Previous Article in Journal
Age and Method of Inoculation Influence the Infection of Worker Honey Bees (Apis mellifera) by Nosema ceranae
Previous Article in Special Issue
A Multiscale Review of Behavioral Variation in Collective Foraging Behavior in Honey Bees
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Review

Spatial Vision and Visually Guided Behavior in Apidae

1
Lund Vision Group, Department of Biology, Lund University, Sölvegatan 35, 22362 Lund, Sweden
2
IISER TVM Centre for Research and Education in Ecology and Evolution (ICREEE), School of Biology, Indian Institute of Science Education and Research, Maruthamala PO, Vithura, Thiruvananthapuram, Kerala 695551, India
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Insects 2019, 10(12), 418; https://doi.org/10.3390/insects10120418
Submission received: 14 September 2019 / Revised: 5 November 2019 / Accepted: 21 November 2019 / Published: 22 November 2019
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Honeybee Neurobiology and Behavior)

Abstract

:
The family Apidae, which is amongst the largest bee families, are important pollinators globally and have been well studied for their visual adaptations and visually guided behaviors. This review is a synthesis of what is known about their eyes and visual capabilities. There are many species-specific differences, however, the relationship between body size, eye size, resolution, and sensitivity shows common patterns. Salient differences between castes and sexes are evident in important visually guided behaviors such as nest defense and mate search. We highlight that Apis mellifera and Bombus terrestris are popular bee models employed in the majority of studies that have contributed immensely to our understanding vision in bees. However, other species, specifically the tropical and many non-social Apidae, merit further investigation for a better understanding of the influence of ecological conditions on the evolution of bee vision.

1. Introduction

Apidae is the largest bee family (over 5900 species) and, besides Halictidae, has the largest number of social species including the approximately 500 species of stingless bees (subfamily Apinae; tribe Meliponini) as well as the 8 species of honeybees (genus Apis) and approximately 270 species of bumblebees (genus Bombus; tribe Bombini) [1,2]. Although social bees have attracted more attention than the even larger number of solitary bees, most studies on visual behavior and adaptations have focused on just two species, the European honeybee Apis mellifera, and the buff-tailed bumblebee Bombus terrestris. These two species, and more recently some species of stingless bees, have served as models for color vision (which we do not discuss here, for reviews see [3,4]), while other basic properties of their visual systems, such as spatial resolution, contrast sensitivity, and absolute sensitivity to light, have been studied to a lesser extent. Specifically, very few comparative studies have been performed. In this review, we aim to summarize the current knowledge on spatial vision for social Apidae, relate it to their flight activity and their visually guided behaviors, and compare it to what is known from some other bees.
Bees use a pair of large compound eyes and three small lens eyes, the ocelli, for visual tasks. The compound eyes of bees have a small dorsal region specialized for the perception of polarized light, called the dorsal rim. In this review, we do not discuss polarization vision, a topic excellently reviewed by others [5,6,7]. Behavioral tasks, such as finding flowers or mate detection, require high spatial resolution and are served by the main part of the compound eye [8], while others, likely served by ocelli or the dorsal rim region of the compound eyes, function with low spatial resolution. Low-resolution tasks include, for instance, phototaxis and the use of the polarization pattern of the sky as a compass for navigation (e.g., [7,9]). While most bees are active during daytime, few species have extended their activity into the night, which requires high absolute sensitivity of their eyes. Males may have different demands than females, and thus, dimorphic eyes occur amongst bees [10,11,12,13,14]. We will review properties of the eyes and vision first, and then discuss visually guided behaviors.

2. Visual Fields, Sensitivity, and Resolution of the Apposition Compound Eyes of Bees

In the typical apposition eyes of bees, each ommatidium, with its own facet lens and 8 or 9 photoreceptors, is equivalent to a pixel in the image. Obviously, a larger eye can have more ommatidia, ommatidia with larger facets, or a combination of both, compared to a smaller eye [8]. Given the same visual field (but see [15]), this results in higher resolution, higher sensitivity, or a combination of both. Because each pixel is served by its own optical apparatus, sensitivity and resolution can vary widely within an eye, leading to acute and bright zones, the most extreme forms of which are found in the eyes of male bees [10,11,12,13,14]. When comparing different species, we mostly focus on the eye regions with highest resolution and sensitivity (Table 1).

2.1. Sensitivity

The sensitivity S of photoreceptors to broad-spectrum light is limited by four parameters: the area of the corneal facet lens (π/4 D)2, where D is the facet diameter, the focal length f, and the length l and distal diameter d of the photo-sensitive structure, the rhabdom, as summarized by Warrant and Nilsson [23]:
S = (π/4)2 D2 (d/f)2 (k l/(2.3 + k l)
where k is the absorption coefficient of the rhabdom that is wavelength-dependent but—assuming a broad spectrum of light—can be taken as a constant (0.0067/μm, see [24]). Larger facets, and wider and longer rhabdoms, as well as a short focal length increase sensitivity.

2.2. Resolution

Spatial resolution is limited by two parameters, one of them being the angle separating the optical axes of two ommatidia, the interommatidial angle. Baumgärtner [18] determined interommatidial angles in the honeybee eye using careful sections and measuring minimum angles between adjacent facet lenses of 0.9° in the vertical and 1.6° in the horizontal plane (but see [25]). Inter-facet angles have been used to approximate optical axes of ommatidia in recent micro-CT studies on bumblebees [26]. This works when the optical axes of ommatidia are normal to the corneal surface, which is the case in central eye regions, where resolution is highest. When optical axes are skewed, as in the periphery of bee eyes, inter-facet angles underestimate interommatidial angles, and with them the extension of the visual field (see [27] for a discussion of the problem and Figure 1D in [20] for an example). Using an optical method, interommatidial angles have been observed in female carpenter bees for facet rows of different orientations [21]. Minimal angles between horizontally running x facet rows, and the vertical to oblique y and z facet rows are, respectively, 0.90°, 1.82°, and 1.28° in Xylocopa leucothorax; 0.82°, 1.82°, and 1.28° in X. tenuiscapa; and 0.67°, 1.12°, and 0.81° in X. tranquebarica.
The difference between horizontal and vertical interommatidial angles is similar amongst bees; it results from the oval shape of bee eyes and has been taken in consideration in models of bee vision (e.g., [28]). However, resolution is finally limited by the acceptance angle of receptors in each ommatidium, which can be approximated as:
Δϕ = d/f
or, most accurately, be determined electrophysiologically. Rigosi et al. [17] measured mean acceptance angles of worker honeybee photoreceptors in the equatorial eye region as 2.2° in the horizontal and 2.3° in the vertical direction, and minimal acceptance angles down to 1.6°. If interommatidial angles are smaller than acceptance angles, the eye oversamples, which will not lead to higher resolution, but can potentially allow for higher sensitivity, if neural mechanisms of spatial pooling are in place [24,29,30].

2.3. Sensitivity, Resolution and Visual Field Size Depend on Body Size

In social bees, the eyes, and both the number of ommatidia in each eye and the ommatidial diameters, are larger in larger species (Figure 1A–C). Thus, both spatial resolution and sensitivity are higher in larger than in smaller species of bees, but crepuscular or nocturnal activity has an additional influence on facet diameters (Figure 2; see also [18,21,31,32,33]).
While honeybee and stingless bee workers of the same species are very similar in size and, thus, have similar eyes and visual capabilities, bumblebees [19] and many solitary bees (e.g., [34]) can have quite dramatic variations in body and eye sizes between individuals. According to Taylor et al. [26], eye size varies even more with body size, intra- than interspecifically, at least among bumblebees. This makes very small individuals of a bumblebee hive far less suited as foragers, compared to their larger nest mates.
Besides resolution and sensitivity, the visual fields of bee eyes can also differ with body and eye size. Generally, the two compound eyes of bees have large visual fields, with a binocular region frontally and dorsally, and only a small blind region below and behind the animal. The European honeybee has a 50–60° (25–30° to the left and right of the frontal axis) binocular field in the eye region which looks forward in flight [35]. Large carpenter bees also have 40–50° binocular overlap at the eye horizon [21]. A detailed study of visual fields, however, has only been undertaken in bumblebees. Taylor et al. [26] show that larger individuals of Bombus terrestris may have larger visual fields and binocular overlap than smaller individuals, even though their method likely underestimates visual field size (see their Figure 1D). However, as this is the only study so far, more comparative work is required to better understand the behavioral and ecological relevance of differences in visual field sizes.

2.4. Sexual Dimorphism of Eyes

In social bees, in which workers, queens and males (drones) have rather different repertoires of visually guided behaviors, the eyes can differ distinctly. In females (queens and workers), resolution is highest in a fronto-horizontal region, in which they face flowers, the nest entrance, and other surfaces on which they land. The degree of sexual dimorphism depends partly on the mating system of a species. In bumblebee and carpenter bee species, which rely to a large degree on chemical cues, male and female eyes are more similar than in species that use visual mate detection [13,14,36].
Where sexual dimorphism is found, the eye region of highest spatial resolution in the drone eye, the acute zone, is often found in the dorsal part of the eye [10,11,12,13,14]. In A. mellifera drones, the upper third of the eye builds a large upwards-looking acute zone with larger facet diameters (30–40 μm), smaller inter-ommatidial angles (1–2°), and larger, and thus more sensitive, rhabdoms (2–3 μm2), than the remaining two-thirds of the eye (20–30 μm; 2–4°; 0.8–2 μm2) [10,11]. In A. mellifera drones, which have twice as many ommatidia in each eye than workers, the visual field is also expanded compared to females, because the two compound eyes meet at the dorsal border, and the lateral extension of each eye is larger (2.5 mm versus 1 mm). Enlarged eyes with dorsal acute zones are also found in the drones of other honeybee species, Apis florea, A. dorsata, A. cerana, and A. andreniformis (Table 2; and see [12]). The drones of the two open-nesting Asian species, A. florea and A. dorsata, have the most extreme adaptations for mate detection, with A. dorsata possessing enlarged ommatidia, and A. florea having smaller but more facets, an indication for spatial pooling. In carpenter bees, sexual dimorphism of eye size is correlated with mating strategy [36]. Males, which defend resources, tend to have dorsal acute zones with more and larger facets and smaller interommatidial angles than females, as described in X. tenuiscapa [14], while no obvious sexual dimorphism is seen in species with other mating strategies. A similar pattern can be seen in bumblebees: males of species, which adopt a perching strategy, for instance B. confusus, B. melaleucus, and B. niveatus, have larger eyes and facets than workers, while those using a patrolling strategy have similarly sized eyes ([13]; and see Table 2). Further studies of sexual dimorphism in bees will reveal which adaptations of the eyes are related to general properties of the habitat and which are related to the demands posed by sex-specific behaviors.

3. Ocelli and Their Function

Fronto-dorsally on the head, between the pair of large compound eyes, bees possess three small lens eyes, called ocelli [5,6,9]. Independent of their specific arrangement and position in different species, and partly as a result of different head angles during flight [6,15], all bee ocelli have similar, large visual fields covering the entire dorsal and frontal part of the world, even though hairs occlude part of the visual field in furry bees like Apis and Bombus [15]. Their large lenses (Figure 1D and Figure 2B) make ocelli more sensitive than the single ommatidia of the compound eyes. Consequently, honeybees [37] and bumblebees [38] with blinded ocelli start and stop foraging flights at about five times higher light intensities than bees with intact ocelli. That high absolute sensitivity of ocelli may be generally important is strengthened by the observation that nocturnal bee species have extremely large ocellar lenses, up to 1 mm in diameter (Xylocopa tranquebarica; [21]).
Bee ocelli have irregularly shaped and often astigmatic lenses, whose focal plane usually lies behind the retina, resulting in relatively poor spatial resolution (e.g., [15,39]). The retina is bi-sectioned: the ventral retina is thin and views the—usually bright—sky above the bee, while the dorsal retina, which looks at the horizon of the flying bee, is usually thicker [6]. A prominent equatorial fovea-like indentation of the retinae is common, in which distal receptor endings are furthest away from the lens surface, and receptor densities are highest. The rhabdoms, each formed by rhabdomeres with short microvilli of two adjacent photoreceptors, build elongated, non-twisting plates, making the cells strongly polarization-sensitive [5,6,40].
Despite similarities across bees [6], there is also variation in structure, and likely function. Orchid bee ocelli are exceptional in having the dorsal retina within the focal zone of the lens, allowing for higher spatial resolution of the scene on front. In addition, microvilli within each ocellus are parallel, but oriented at 60° to those of the other two ocelli, making the ventral retinae a perfect polarization analyzer for the sky above the bee [41]. Honeybee ocelli have two types of photoreceptors, maximally sensitive to 500 nm and 360 nm [40]; the UV-green contrast is suitable to detect the sky-terrestrial boundary (e.g., [42]). The nocturnal halictid bee Megalopta genalis only has ocellar receptors with peak sensitivity at 500 nm and no polarization sensitivity, both likely adaptations increasing absolute sensitivity [5,43]. The nocturnal carpenter bee X. tranquebarica is the only described case in which the dorsal retina of the ocelli has evolved a tapetum (specifically, a tracheal tapetum) to enhance absolute sensitivity [21].
Ocelli can be sexually dimorphic (Table 2). In drones of most honeybee species, where they compete for space with the enlarged compound eyes, ocelli are slightly larger than in worker bees and have smaller visual fields, both increasing sensitivity [5]. The exception are giant honeybees Apis dorsata, in which the facultatively nocturnal workers have equally large ocelli as the drones [12]. In stingless bees, ocelli are understudied, but in one investigated species, males have significantly larger ocelli than both workers and queens ([10] and see Table 2). In B. terrestris, there are no signs of sexual dimorphism [15].
Bee ocelli likely serve several functions, including horizon detection and flight control by the dorsal retina [9] and evaluation of the polarized skylight by comparison of signals from the ventral retinae of all three ocelli ([38,40] but see [43]). These tasks are best served by highly sensitive receptors, large visual fields, and low spatial resolution. Probably related to their higher sensitivity compared to the compound eyes, ocelli allow bees to expand flight activity into dimmer light intensities [37,38]. Across bee species, ocellar diameters are well correlated with the light regime under which bees fly (Figure 2B; [33,44,45]). The fact that the ocellar interneurons of bees show a lower degree of convergence than those of some other insects [9], together with retinal adaptations [6], strongly indicates additional functions that require better spatial resolution. This is most obvious in the orchid bee [41], but more detailed studies of additional bee species are required for a broader understanding of ocellar function across bees. Specifically, comparisons of more bees foraging or performing other visually guided behaviours in different habitats and time windows may reveal further adaptations and the plasticity of ocellar function.

4. Visually Guided Behaviors in Bees

Almost all behaviors that bees perform outside the hive require some spatial resolution and are restricted by light intensity and the sensitivity of compound eyes and ocelli [31,32,33,43,44]. We limit this review to behavioral tasks guided by the main part of the eye and exclude the dorsal rim that serves the important task of extracting compass information from the polarization pattern of the sky, for which we refer the reader to the excellent review by Zeil et al. [7].

4.1. Nest Defense

Social Apidae are known to guard their nest entrance from intruders. In the stingless bee Tetragonisca angustula, guard bees hover in front of the nest in stable positions to protect the flight path from intruders. Positioning is under visual control, thus, when presented with an expanding pattern of stripes or a rotating spiral at the nest front, bees flew away from the nest, whereas, with a contracting pattern, they flew closer to it [45,46]. Identification of intruders, however, is also chemically mediated [47]. The giant honeybee Apis dorsata, which inhabits large open combs in high trees, rocks, and buildings, also uses visual cues in nest defense. Guard bees sitting on the comb approach and attack objects (potential intruders) that move towards the hive. In experiments with paper disks, Koeniger et al. [48] showed that the guard bees react much more strongly to objects moving upwards than to objects moving downwards, likely because the latter are perceived as harmless objects, such as falling leaves.

4.2. Use of Landmarks for Navigation and Homing

When a bee leaves the hive for the first time, she performs a learning flight, which allows her to find her way back to the nest (e.g., [49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56]). Even during later foraging periods, she updates this information whenever local features change in the vicinity of the nest. These flights have been synonymously referred to as orientation flights, learning flights, or turn back and look behavior (e.g., [49,50,51,52,53]. This behavior, which has been largely documented in A. mellifera [50,51,52,53] and B. terrestris [49,54,56,57], helps a forager learn features around the nest or a feeding location when viewed from different positions. Orientation flights are typically quite restricted to a narrow range around the goal point and as a bee gains experience, she flies faster during these flights; honeybee workers extend the range of these flights to cover increasing distances over time [53].
While performing these flights, a bee typically exits the nest or feeding location, turns around to face it while backing away in a series of successive arcs that are approximately centered on the point of interest i.e., the nest or feeding location. With experience, the bee will fly out in a straight line and will perform orientation flights only when local landmarks around the point of interest are disturbed. The principal function of these flights is to learn to recognize salient locations; thus, studies have compared outbound and return flights for correspondence in ground-nesting bees and in bumblebees (e.g., [54,55,56]). In the bumblebee Bombus terrestris, loops are components of learning flights that are more common when the bee is outbound than during return flights. On the other hand, zigzag flight paths are more prominent during return flights. A common feature to both loops and zigzags is that bees often face the nest such that it is held within the fronto-lateral visual field [56]. These findings suggest that bees can store and recall nest-centered views during learning and return flights. Interestingly, male bumblebees were found to perform learning flights only when leaving a food source and not the natal nest, suggesting inter-sexual differences in the value of the nest to males and females [57].

4.3. Foraging: Flower Detection

The best-studied function of the visual system of bees is the spatial resolution for circular or flower-shaped single targets. In the context of flower visits, experiments have asked from which distance—or at which visual angle—bees can detect and discriminate such targets. While the focus often was on color discrimination [3,4], a lot has been learned about spatial resolution using behavioral methods (Table 3; e.g., [19,28,58,59,60,61]). As eye size, ommatidial diameter and the number of ommatidia in each eye are correlated with body size (see above and Figure 1A–C), the small stingless bee Tetragonula carbonaria can detect flowers from the background only when they have almost a 10° visual extension, meaning they can detect a flower of diameter 1 cm at a distance of 6 cm only [20]. A European honeybee can detect the same flower when it fills 3° or 5° of their visual field, thus from a distance of 12 to 18 cm (e.g., [28,59,60]), and a medium-sized bumblebee can detect this flower from a distance of 30 cm, when it subtends 1.8° (see Table 3; [62] but see [19] for the effect of body and eye size variation). Spatial resolution of the large carpenter bees is likely even higher but has not been determined behaviorally [63].
Spatial contrast sensitivity, which relates contrast sensitivity and spatial resolution, has been behaviorally estimated only rarely in bees, by finding contrast sensitivity thresholds for gratings of different spatial frequencies [58,66].

4.4. Flight Ranges and Flight Control

It can be expected that in bees using visual cues for navigation, flight range may at least partially depend on visual abilities. Since flying bees can seldom be tracked in nature (but see [69,70]), foraging distances have been estimated using release-homing experiments with marked individuals or by training bees to feeders at set distances and recording the distance at which bees cease to visit the feeder (Table 4). Four species of neotropical stingless bees were able to find artificial feeders placed between 320 and 680 m from the hive and also to return to the nests when released 600–800 m away from nests [71]. Alternatively, in honeybees, the distance travelled can be inferred from waggle dances [72]. When more than one method was deployed for a bee species, the estimated foraging distances differed somewhat (Table 4). Foraging distances are affected by landscape structure, as has been shown by Steffan-Dewenter and Kuhn [73], who compared European honeybees in grasslands, forests, arable land, and built up areas in Germany. The mean foraging distance estimated from waggle dances was 1.5 km [73].
Meta-analyses suggest a positive non-linear correlation between body size and foraging distances [83,85]. Nevertheless, apart from Apis mellifera, foraging distances are not well known in most Apidae species. The few studies that have examined Asian honeybee species, for instance, suggest considerable within- and between- site variation in foraging distances [72,86].
Bees, like many flying animals, use optic flow to measure distance flown on their way to and from food sources [87]. Optic flow is measured as the amount of image motion in the lateral visual field of the bee’s eye while flying towards a goal. Environments with many contrasting structures generate greater optic flow than those with fewer visual contrasts, suggesting that the bee’s perception of the distance flown varies in relation to the landscape [87,88]. This was investigated in honeybees and bumblebees flying through experimental tunnels, in which patterns on the walls of provided optic cues (e.g., [68,89,90]. Bees estimate flight distance using only the green receptor contrast of the patterns, independent of chromatic contrast and brightness contrast [91], and because translational optic flow depends on distance, the estimate depends on the distance of bees to the contrasting patterns (e.g., [92]). Bees also use optic flow to control flight speed and flight height [93], and to avoid flying too close to obstacles, by balancing the optic flow on both sides (e.g., [64,89]). In tunnel experiments, this tendency is obvious as the ‘centering response’: bees fly in the middle of the tunnel with symmetric optic flow, but close to one of the walls if it provides no optic flow cues [64,68]. Tunnel experiments also indicated that the stingless bee Melipona panamanica uses optic flow to gauge distance as well as the height of food sources [94]. The nocturnal sweat bee, Megalopta genalis, also uses optic flow in experimental tunnels [95]. This nocturnal bee flew five times slower than day-active bumblebees, and increased groundspeed when motion cues were reduced in the tunnel, though it did not their affect centering response.

4.5. Male Mating Flights: Detection of Females

Besides homing (at least in some species), and finding and returning to flowers, the only important—and most demanding—visual task that a male bee has to perform is finding a mate. Males seek out mates at sites where females emerge, at food resources and at non-resource locations such as landmarks and flyways [96]. Studies on mating behavior of bees are sparse, however, whether or not this task involves visual cues can often be guessed from sexually dimorphic eyes (e.g., [36]).
The high resolution and contrast sensitivity of receptors in the dorsal acute zone of drone honeybees Apis mellifera carnica allows a queen in flight to be detected against the sky even when she darkens just a small part of the visual field of a single ommatidium by about 6% [97]. Honeybee drones use the upper frontal part of the visual field to chase queens and they fixate the queen with the lower part of their frontal visual field where the ommatidial diameter is maximum [98].
More generally, the male eyes suggest an influence of mating flight timings, body size limitations, and the relative roles of vision and olfaction in mating behavior of various bee species. Drones of different honeybee species perform mating flights at different, barely overlapping times of the day, with the large A. dorsata being the only species using the dimmer time window after sunset (18:30–19:00 h in Thailand, during February) [99].
Mating behavior also varied in three sympatric species of carpenter bees that are diurnal (Xylocopa tenuiscapa and X. leucothorax) or nocturnal (X. tranquebarica) [14]. X. tenuiscapa perched outside nests early in the season and patrolled or perched along flyways and close to flowering plants later in the season. X. leucothorax patrolled along flyways, while the nocturnal X. tranquebarica patrolled close to flowering bushes. Perching X. tenuiscapa males can detect females flying at a distance of 20 m, which darkens the visual field of a single ommatidium by only 2%, suggesting that these carpenter bee males have similar or higher contrast sensitivity than honeybee drones [14,97].

5. Light Intensity and the Sensitivity of the Eyes

Most species of bees are diurnal, limited to the day for foraging and other activities performed outside the nest. This limitation could be due to lower temperatures or low light intensities at night or both. While temperature differences between day and night can be marked in temperate and arctic climates and in dry habitats, they are much smaller and less likely to limit bee flight in humid and tropical areas [90]. Thus, bees are generally more limited by light intensities than low temperatures [31,32,33].
Therefore, it is not surprising that the lens diameter of both types of bee eyes—the facet lenses of the compound eyes, and the ocellar lenses—and thus their sensitivity, dictate the lowest light intensity in which a species can be active ([31,33,44] and see Figure 2). In a recent study on the stingless bee Scaptotrigona depilis [100], for instance, the relationship between flight activity and light intensity was highly significant, while the relationship with temperature was not. Similarly, Heard and Hendrikz [101] reported that, in the Australian species Tetragonula carbonaria, commencement of flight activity is determined by temperature during the colder months of the year, but by light intensity during the warmer months, and that light intensity always determined the end of flight activity in the afternoon.
Streinzer et al. [33] proved this to be true for a larger sample of stingless bee species, and Somanathan et al. [18] showed that among the four studied species of honeybees, Apis dorsata has the most sensitive eyes. A. dorsata is facultatively nocturnal—by contrast to the closely related species A. laboriosa [104]—and can forage during full moon nights [102], and is the only species in which drones perform mating flights during dusk [99]. However, the most intriguing example of nocturnality known among Apidae studied so far is the carpenter bee X. tranquebarica, which is able to forage and return to the nest on new moon nights [21,105], and can even use color under these conditions [106]. Studies on more species may reveal additional species which can change their activity window to the dimmer times of the day, likely escaping competition and improving foraging efficiency.
A move to the nocturnal niche is possible for smaller species of bees (see, for instance [30,31,44] for examples of crepuscular or nocturnal bees and wasps with relatively small body size). This is mostly due to adaptations in the neural processing of visual information, which allow for spatial and temporal pooling of visual signals, both of which increase sensitivity [22,24,29,30].

6. Concluding Remarks

Social bees of the family Apidae, the most important pollinators of wild flowers and crops, rely on vision for many crucial behavioral tasks. A considerable body of research on the two European temperate species, Apis mellifera and Bombus terrestris, has accumulated over the years; the few studies on other bumblebees—including B. impatiens—and the large diversity of tropical Apidae indicate interesting differences, even though their compound eyes and ocelli all follow the same building plan. Most importantly, eye size, and with it, both absolute sensitivity and spatial resolution, scale with body size. The investment in eyes and ocelli, and subtler differences in the compromise between high spatial resolution and high absolute sensitivity to light, differ between both sexes and between bees active in different light levels. We expect that more adaptations, as well as differences in visual field size, will be found once more species are investigated. Such studies may reveal interesting and hitherto unreported adaptations to behavioral tasks and habitats, such as forests or open meadows. From the perspective of pollination biology, the relationship between body and eye size, and the foraging range of different bee species, is another topic that requires further investigation, as it may facilitate optimal pollinator augmentation.
We have focused on social Apidae, partly because their eyes and vision have been studied most thoroughly. We conclude that far too little is known about the eyes and visual abilities of the majority of bee species and suggest that comparative studies, both between temperate and tropical Apidae, and between solitary and social species, are required for a deeper understanding of how different ecological conditions have shaped the evolution of bee vision, behavior, and pollination services.

Author Contributions

Both authors jointly planned and wrote the text, prepared the figures and compiled the tables.

Funding

No specific funding was received for this particular review.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Michener, C.D. The Bees of the World; The Johns Hopkins University Press: Baltimore, MD, USA; Londres, Argentina, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  2. Ascher, J.S.; Pickering, J. Discover Life Bee Species Guide and World Checklist (Hymenoptera: Apoidea: Anthophila). Available online: http://www.discoverlife.org/mp/20q?guide=Apoidea_species (accessed on 29 October 2019).
  3. Chittka, L.; Vorobyev, M.; Shmida, A.; Menzel, M. Bee colour vision—The optimal system for the discrimination of flower colours with three types of photoreceptor types? In Sensory Systems of Arthropods; Wiese, K., Gribakin, F.G., Popov, A.V., Renninger, G., Eds.; Birkhäuser: Basel, Switzerland, 1993; pp. 211–218. [Google Scholar]
  4. Hempel de Ibarra, N.; Vorobyev, M.R.; Menzel, R. Mechanisms, functions and ecology of colour vision in the honeybee. J. Comp. Physiol. B 2014, 200, 411–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Ribi, W.A.; Warrant, E.J.; Zeil, J. The organization of honeybee ocelli: Regional specialization and rhabdom arrangements. Arthropod Struct. Dev. 2011, 40, 509–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Ribi, W.A.; Zeil, J. Diversity and common themes in the organization of ocelli in Hymenoptera, Odonata and Diptera. J. Comp. Physiol. A 2018, 204, 505–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Zeil, J.; Ribi, W.; Narendra, A. Polarisation vision in ants, bees and wasps. In Polarized Light and Polarization Vision in Animal Sciences; Horvath, G., Ed.; Springer: Heidelberg, Germany, 2014; pp. 41–60. [Google Scholar]
  8. Land, M.F. Visual acuity in insects. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 1997, 42, 147–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Mizunami, M. Functional diversity of neural organization in insect ocellar systems. Vis. Res. 1995, 35, 443–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Ribi, W.A.; Engels, E.; Engels, W. Sex and caste specific eye structures in stingless bees and honey bees (Hymenoptera: Trigonidae, Apidae). Entomol. Gener. 1989, 14, 233–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Menzel, J.G.; Wunderer, H.; Stavenga, D.G. Functional morphology of the divided compound eye of the honeybee drone (Apis mellifera). Tissue Cell Res. 1991, 23, 525–535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Streinzer, M.; Brockmann, A.; Nagaraja, N.; Spaethe, J. Sex and caste-specific variation in compound eye morphology of five honeybee species. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e57702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Streinzer, M.; Spaethe, J. Functional morphology of the visual system and mating strategies in bumblebees (Hymenoptera, Apidae, Bombus). Zool. J. Linn. Soc. 2014, 170, 735–747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Somanathan, H.; Borges, R.M.; Warrant, E.J.; Kelber, A. Visual adaptations for mate detection in the male carpenter bee Xylocopa tenuiscapa. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0168452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Wilby, D.; Aarts, T.; Tichit, P.; Bodey, A.; Rau, C.; Taylor, G.; Baird, E. Using micro-CT techniques to explore the role of sex and hair in the functional morphology of bumblebee (Bombus terrestris) ocelli. Vis. Res. 2019, 158, 100–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Baumgärtner, H. Der Formensinn und die Sehschärfe der Bienen. Z. vgl. Physiol. 1928, 7, 56–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Rigosi, E.; Wiederman, S.D.; O’Carroll, D.C. Visual acuity of the honey bee retina and the limits for feature detection. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 45972. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Somanathan, H.; Warrant, E.J.; Borges, R.M.; Wallén, R.; Kelber, A. Resolution and sensitivity of the eyes of the Asian honeybees Apis florea, Apis cerana and Apis dorsata. J. Exp. Biol. 2009, 212, 2448–2453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Spaethe, J.; Chittka, L. Interindividual variation of eye optics and single object resolution in bumblebees. J. Exp. Biol. 2003, 206, 3447–3453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Dyer, A.G.; Streinzer, M.; Garcia, J. Flower detection and acuity of the Australian native stingless bee Tetragonula carbonaria Sm. J. Comp. Physiol. A 2016, 202, 629–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Somanathan, H.; Kelber, A.; Borges, R.M.; Wallén, R.; Warrant, E.J. Visual ecology of Indian carpenter bees II: Adaptations of eyes and ocelli to nocturnal and diurnal lifestyles. J. Comp. Physiol. A 2009, 195, 571–583. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Greiner, B.; Ribi, W.A.; Wcislo, W.T.; Warrant, E.J. Neural organisation in the first optic ganglion of the nocturnal bee Megalopta genalis. Cell Tiss. Res. 2004, 318, 429–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Warrant, E.J.; Nilsson, D.-E. Absorption of white light in photoreceptors. Vis. Res. 1998, 38, 195–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Warrant, E.; Porombka, T.; Kirchner, W.H. Neural image enhancement allows honeybees to see at night. Proc. R. Soc. B 1996, 263, 1521–1526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Seidl, R. Die Sehfelder und Ommatidien-Divergenzwinkel der drei Kasten der Honigbiene. Verh. Dtsch. Zool. Ges. 1980, 367. [Google Scholar]
  26. Taylor, G.J.; Tichit, P.; Schmidt, M.D.; Bodey, A.J.; Rau, C.; Baird, E. Bumblebee visual allometry results in locally improved resolution and globally improved sensitivity. eLife 2019, 8, e40613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Stavenga, D.G. Pseudopupils of compound eyes. In Handbook of Sensory Physiology; Autrum, H., Ed.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany; New York, NY, USA, 1979; Volume VII/6A, pp. 357–439. [Google Scholar]
  28. Giurfa, M.; Vorobyev, M.; Kevan, P.; Menzel, R. Detection of coloured stimuli by honeybees: Minimum visual angles and receptor specific contrasts. J. Comp. Physiol. A 1996, 178, 699–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Theobald, J.C.; Greiner, B.; Wcislo, W.T.; Warrant, E.J. Visual summation in night-flying sweat bees: A theoretical study. Vision Res. 2006, 46, 2298–2309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Warrant, E.J. Seeing in the dark: Vision and visual behaviour in nocturnal bees and wasps. J. Exp. Biol. 2008, 22, 1737–1746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Kelber, A.; Warrant, E.J.; Pfaff, M.; Wallén, R.; Theobald, J.C.; Wcislo, W.; Raguso, R. Light intensity limits foraging activity in nocturnal and crepuscular bees. Behav. Ecol. 2006, 17, 63–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Kapustjanskij, A.; Streinzer, M.; Paulus, H.F.; Spaethe, J. Bigger is better: Implications of body size for flight ability under different light conditions and the evolution of alloethism in bumblebees. Funct. Ecol. 2007, 21, 1130–1136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Streinzer, M.; Huber, W.; Spaethe, J. Body size limits dim-light foraging activity in stingless bees (Apidae: Meliponini). J. Comp. Physiol. A 2016, 202, 643–655. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Roulston, T.H.; Cane, J.H. The effect of diet breadth and nesting ecology on body size variation in bees (Apiformes). J. Kans. Entomol. Soc. 2000, 73, 129–142. [Google Scholar]
  35. Seidl, R.; Kaiser, W. Visual field size, binocular domain and the ommatidial array of the compound eyes in worker honey bees. J. Comp. Physiol. A 1981, 143, 17–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Leys, R.; Hogendoorn, K. Correlated evolution of mating behaviour and morphology in large carpenter bees (Xylocopa). Apidologie 2008, 39, 119–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Schricker, B. Die Orientierung der Biene in der Dämmerung, zugleich ein Beitrag zur Frage der Ocellenfunktion bei Bienen. Z. Vgl. Physiol. 1965, 49, 420–458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Wellington, W.G. Bumblebee ocelli and navigation at dusk. Science 1974, 183, 550–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Hung, Y.-S.; Ibbotson, M.R. Ocellar structure and neural innervation in the honeybee. Front. Neuroanat. 2014, 8, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  40. Ogawa, Y.; Ribi, W.; Zeil, J.; Hemmi, J.M. Regional differences in the preferred e-vector orientation of honeybee ocellar photoreceptors. J. Exp. Biol. 2017, 220, 1701–1708. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Taylor, G.J.; Ribi, W.; Bech, M.; Bodey, A.J.; Rau, C.; Steuwer, A.; Warrant, E.J.; Baird, E. The dual function of orchid bee ocelli as revealed by X-ray microtomography. Curr. Biol. 2016, 26, 1319–1324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Möller, R. Insects could exploit UV-green contrast for landmark navigation. J. Theor. Biol. 2002, 214, 619–631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Berry, R.P.; Wcislo, W.T.; Warrant, E.J. Ocellar adaptations for dim light vision in a nocturnal bee. J. Exp. Biol. 2011, 214, 1283–1293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Kerfoot, W.B. Correlation between ocellar size and foraging activities of bees (Hymenoptera; Apoidea). Am. Nat. 1967, 101, 65–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Kelber, A.; Zeil, J. A robust procedure for visual stabilisation of hovering flight position in guard bees of Trigona (Tetragonisca) angustula (Apidae: Meliponini). J. Comp. Physiol. A 1990, 167, 569–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Kelber, A.; Zeil, J. Tetragonisca guard bees take expanding and contracting patterns as unintended displacement in space. J. Comp. Physiol. A 1997, 181, 257–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Wittmann, D.; Radtke, R.; Zeil, J.; Lübke, G.; Francke, W. Robber bees (Lestrimelitta limao) and their host chemical and visual cues in nest defense by Trigona (Tetragonisca) angustula (Apidae: Meliponinae). J. Chem. Ecol. 1990, 16, 631–641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Koeniger, N.; Kurze, C.; Phiancharoen, M.; Koeniger, G. “Up” or “down” that makes the difference. How giant honeybees (Apis dorsata) see the world. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0185325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  49. Wagner, W. Psychobiologische Untersuchungen an Hummeln. Zoologica 1907, 19, 1–239. [Google Scholar]
  50. Becker, L. Untersuchungen über das Heimfindevermögen der Bienen. J. Comp. Physiol. A Neuroethol. Sens. Neural Behav. Physiol. 1958, 41, 1–25. [Google Scholar]
  51. Vollbehr, J. Zur Orientierung junger Honigbienen bei ihrem ersten Orientierungsflug. Zool. Jb. Allg. Zool. Physiol. 1975, 79, 33–69. [Google Scholar]
  52. Lehrer, M. Bees which turn back and look. Naturwissenschaften 1991, 78, 274–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Capaldi, E.A.; Smith, A.D.; Osborne, J.L.; Fahrbach, S.E.; Farris, S.M.; Reynolds, D.R.; Edwards, A.S.; Martin, A.; Robinson, G.E.; Poppy, G.M.; et al. Ontogeny of orientation flight in the honeybee revealed by harmonic radar. Nature 2000, 403, 537–540. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Robert, T.; Frasnelli, E.; Hempel de Ibarra, N.; Collett, T.S. Variations on a theme: Bumblebee learning flights from the nest and from flowers. J. Exp. Biol. 2018, 221, jeb172601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Zeil, J.; Kelber, A.; Voss, R. Structure and function of learning flights in ground-nesting bees and wasps. J. Exp. Biol. 1996, 199, 245–252. [Google Scholar]
  56. Philippides, A.; Hempel de Ibarra, N.; Riabinina, O.; Collett, T.S. Bumblebee calligraphy: The design and control of flight motifs in the learning and return flights of Bombus terrestris. J. Exp. Biol. 2013, 216, 1093–1104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  57. Robert, T.; Frasnelli, E.; Collett, T.S.; Hempel de Ibarra, N. Male bumblebees perform learning flights on leaving a flower but not when leaving their nest. J. Exp. Biol. 2017, 220, 930–937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  58. Srinivasan, M.V.; Lehrer, M. Spatial acuity of honeybee vision and its spectral properties. J. Comp. Physiol. A 1988, 162, 159–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Lehrer, M.; Bischof, S. Detection of model flowers by honeybees: The role of chromatic and achromatic contrast. Naturwissenschaften 1995, 82, 145–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Hempel de Ibarra, N.; Giurfa, M.; Vorobyev, M. Detection of coloured patterns by honeybees through chromatic and achromatic cues. J. Comp. Physiol. A 2001, 187, 215–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Zhang, L.-Z.; Zhang, S.-W.; Wang, Z.-L.; Yan, W.-Y.; Zeng, Z.-J. Cross-modal interaction between visual and olfactory learning in Apis cerana. J. Comp. Physiol. A 2014, 200, 899–909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Wertlen, A.M.; Niggebrügge, C.; Vorobyev, M.; Hempel de Ibarra, N. Detection of patches of coloured discs by bees. J. Exp. Biol. 2008, 211, 2101–2104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Somanathan, H.; Saryan, P.; Balamurali, G.S. Foraging strategies and physiological adaptations in large carpenter bees. J. Comp. Physiol. A 2019, 205, 387–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Chakravarthi., A.; Rajus, S.; Kelber, A.; Dacke, M.; Baird, E. Differences in spatial resolution and contrast sensitivity of flight control in the honeybees Apis cerana and Apis mellifera. J. Exp. Biol. 2018, 221, 184267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Hecht, S.; Wolf, E. The visual acuity of the honey bee. J. Gen. Physiol. 1929, 12, 727–760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Chakravarthi, A.; Baird, E.; Dacke, M.; Kelber, A. Spatial vision in Bombus terrestris. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2016, 10, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  67. Macuda, T.; Gegear, R.J.; Laverty, T.M.; Timney, B. Behavioural assessment of visual acuity in bumblebees (Bombus impatiens). J. Exp. Biol. 2001, 204, 559–564. [Google Scholar]
  68. Dyhr, J.P.; Higgins, C.M. The spatial frequency tuning of optic-flow dependent behaviors in the bumblebee Bombus impatiens. J. Exp. Biol. 2010, 213, 1643–1650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  69. Riley, J.R.; Smith, A.D.; Reynolds, D.R.; Edwards, A.S.; Osborne, J.L.; Williams, I.H.; Poppy, G.M. Tracking bees with harmonic radar. Nature 1996, 379, 29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Wikelski, M.; Moxley, J.; Eaton-Mordas, A.; Lopez-Uribe, M.M.; Holland, R.; Moskowitz, D.; Roubik, D.W.; Kays, R. Large-range movements of neotropical orchid bees observed via radio telemetry. PLoS ONE 2010, 5, e10738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  71. Van Nieuwstadt, M.G.L.; Iraheta, C.R. Relation between size and foraging range in stingless bees (Apidae, Meliponinae). Apidologie 1996, 27, 219–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Dyer, F.C.; Seeley, T.D. Dance dialects and foraging range in three Asian honey bee species. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1991, 28, 227–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Steffan-Dewenter, I.; Kuhn, A. Honeybee foraging in differentially structured landscapes. Proc. R. Soc. B 2003, 270, 569–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  74. Abrol, D.P. Foraging range of subtropical bees, Megachile flavipes, Megachile nana (Hymenoptera: Megachilidae) and Apis florea (Hymenoptera: Apidae). J. Indian Inst. Sci. 1988, 68, 43–47. [Google Scholar]
  75. Dhaliwal, H.S.; Sharma, P.L. Foraging range of the Indian honeybee. J. Apicult. Res. 1974, 13, 137–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Visscher, P.K.; Seeley, T.D. Foraging strategy of honeybee colonies in a temperate deciduous forest. Ecology 1982, 63, 1790–1801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Beekman, M.; Ratnieks, F.L.W. Long-range foraging by the honey-bee, Apis mellifera L. Funct. Ecol. 2000, 14, 490–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  78. Walther-Hellwig, K.; Frankl, R. Foraging distances of Bombus muscorum, Bombus lapidarius, and Bombus terrestris (Hymenoptera, Apidae). J. Insect Behav. 2000, 13, 239–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Osborne, J.L.; Martin, A.P.; Carreck, N.L.; Swain, J.L.; Knight, M.E.; Goulson, D.; Hale, R.J.; Sanderson, R.A. Bumblebee flight distances in relation to the forage landscape. J. Anim. Ecol. 2008, 77, 406–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Osborne, J.L.; Clark, S.J.; Morris, R.J.; Williams, I.H.; Riley, J.R.; Smith, A.D.; Reynolds, D.R.; Edwards, A.S. A landscape-scale study of bumble bee foraging range and constancy, using harmonic radar. J. Appl. Ecol. 1999, 36, 519–533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  81. Pasquet, R.S.; Peltier, A.; Hufford, M.B.; Oudin, E.; Saulnier, J.; Paul, L.; Knudsen, J.T.; Herren, H.R.; Gepts, P. Long-distance pollen flow assessment through evaluation of pollinator foraging range suggests transgene escape distances. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2008, 105, 13456–13461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  82. Molitor, A. Zur vergleichenden Psychobiologie der akuleaten Hymenopteren auf experimenteller Grundlage. Biol. Gen. 1937, 13, 294–333. [Google Scholar]
  83. Araújo, E.D.; Costa, M.; Chaud-Netto, J.; Fowler, H.G. Body size and flight distance in stingless bees (Hymenoptera: Meliponini): Inference of flight range and possible ecological implications. Braz. J. Biol. 2004, 64, 563–568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Roubik, D.W.; Aluja, M. Flight ranges of Melipona and Trigona in tropical forest. J. Kansas Entom. Soc. 1983, 56, 217–222. [Google Scholar]
  85. Greenleaf, S.S.; Williams, N.M.; Winfree, R.; Kremen, C. Bee foraging ranges and their relationship to body size. Oecologia 2007, 153, 589–596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Punchihewa, R.W.K.; Koeniger, N.; Kevan, P.G.; Gadawski, R.M. Observations on the dance communication and natural foraging ranges of Apis cerana, Apis dorsata and Apis florea in Sri Lanka. J. Apicul. Res. 1985, 24, 168–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Esch, H.E.; Zhang, S.; Srinivasan, M.V.; Tautz, J. Honeybee dances communicate distances measured by optic flow. Nature 2001, 411, 581. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  88. Tautz, J.; Zhang, S.; Spaethe, J.; Brockmann, A.; Si, A.; Srinivasan, M. Honeybee odometry: Performance in varying natural terrain. PLoS Biol. 2004, 2, e211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  89. Srinivasan, M.V.; Zhang, S.; Lehrer, M.; Collett, T. Honeybee navigation en route to the goal: Visual flight control and odometry. J. Exp. Biol. 1996, 199, 237–244. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  90. Srinivasan, M.V. Going with the flow: A brief history of the study of the honeybee’s navigational ‘odometer’. J. Comp. Physiol. A 2014, 200, 563–573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Chittka, L.; Tautz, J. The spectral input to honeybee visual odometry. J. Exp. Biol. 2003, 206, 2393–2397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  92. Linander, N.; Baird, E.; Dacke, M. Bumblebee flight performance in environments of different proximity. J. Comp. Physiol. A 2016, 202, 97–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Baird, E.; Srinivasan, M.V.; Zhang, S.-W.; Lamont, R.; Cowling, A. Visual control of flight speed and height in the honeybee. Anim. Animat. 2006, 9, 40–51. [Google Scholar]
  94. Eckles, M.A.; Roubik, D.W.; Nieh, J.C. A stingless bee can use visual odometry to estimate both height and distance. J. Exp. Biol. 2012, 215, 3155–3160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  95. Baird, E.; Kreiss, E.; Wcislo, W.; Warrant, E.; Dacke, M. Nocturnal insects use optic flow for flight control. Biol. Lett. 2011, 7, 499–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  96. Paxton, R.J. Male mating behaviour and mating systems of bees: An overview. Apidologie 2005, 36, 145–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Vallet, A.M.; Coles, J.A. The perception of small objects by the drone honeybee. J. Comp. Physiol. A 1993, 172, 183–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Praagh, J.V.; Ribi, W.; Wehrhahn, C.; Wittmann, D. Drone bees fixate the queen with the dorsal frontal part of their compound eyes. J. Comp. Physiol. A 1980, 136, 263–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Rinderer, T.E.; Oldroyd, B.P.; Wongsiri, S.; Sylvester, H.A.; de Guzman, L.I.; Potichot, S.; Sheppard, W.S.; Buchmann, S.L. Time of drone flight in 4 honey bee species in south-eastern Thailand. J. Apicult. Res. 1993, 32, 27–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. De Figueiredo-Mecca, G.; Bego, L.R.; do Nascimento, F.S. Foraging behavior of Scaptotrigona depilis (Hymenoptera, Apidae, Meliponini) and its relationship with temporal and abiotic factors. Sociobiology 2013, 60, 267–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  101. Heard, T.A.; Hendrikz, J.K. Factors influencing flight activity of colonies of the stingless bee Trigona carbonaria (Hymenoptera, Apidae). Aust. J. Zool. 1993, 41, 343–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Dyer, F.C. Nocturnal orientation by the Asian honey bee, Apis dorsata. Anim. Behav. 1985, 33, 769–774. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Borges, R.M.; Somanathan, H.; Kelber, A. Patterns and processes in nocturnal and crepuscular pollination services. Quart. Rev. Biol. 2016, 91, 389–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. Kirchner, W.H.; Dreller, C.; Grasser, A.; Baidya, D. The silent dances of the Himalayan honeybee, Apis laboriosa. Apidologie 1996, 27, 331–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Somanathan, H.; Borges, R.M.; Warrant, E.J.; Kelber, A. Visual ecology of Indian carpenter bees I: Light intensities and flight activity. J. Comp. Physiol. A 2008, 194, 97–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Somanathan, H.; Borges, R.M.; Warrant, E.J.; Kelber, A. Nocturnal bees learn landmark colours in starlight. Curr. Biol. 2008, 18, R996–R997. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Figure 1. The dorso-ventral length of the compound eye (A), its maximal facet diameter (B), and number of ommatidia (C), as well as the diameter of the median ocellus (D) in differently sized female bees. Intertegular width is taken as an independent measure for body size. Data from [12,13,18,21,33]. Eye length was not measured in bumblebees. See [33] for statistical analyses of the relationship between body size and eye size.
Figure 1. The dorso-ventral length of the compound eye (A), its maximal facet diameter (B), and number of ommatidia (C), as well as the diameter of the median ocellus (D) in differently sized female bees. Intertegular width is taken as an independent measure for body size. Data from [12,13,18,21,33]. Eye length was not measured in bumblebees. See [33] for statistical analyses of the relationship between body size and eye size.
Insects 10 00418 g001
Figure 2. Ommatidial facet size (A) and median ocellus diameter (B) in bees flying at different light intensities: bee species with larger facets and larger ocelli fly at dimmer light intensities than species with smaller facets and smaller ocelli. Inset in (B) also applies to (A). Data from [18,21,33,102]. See [103] for a comparison of luminance and illuminance values.
Figure 2. Ommatidial facet size (A) and median ocellus diameter (B) in bees flying at different light intensities: bee species with larger facets and larger ocelli fly at dimmer light intensities than species with smaller facets and smaller ocelli. Inset in (B) also applies to (A). Data from [18,21,33,102]. See [103] for a comparison of luminance and illuminance values.
Insects 10 00418 g002
Table 1. Ommatidial values in the eye regions of highest spatial resolution and resulting optical sensitivity of female bees. (a) anatomical estimation, (e) electrophysiological measure, (c) corneal angles, (po) pseudopupil othrodromic illumination, (pa) pseudopupil antidromic illumination, (h) horizontal, (v) vertical (or close to vertical, taking average of y and z rows). For additional information and methods descriptions please see the text and references.
Table 1. Ommatidial values in the eye regions of highest spatial resolution and resulting optical sensitivity of female bees. (a) anatomical estimation, (e) electrophysiological measure, (c) corneal angles, (po) pseudopupil othrodromic illumination, (pa) pseudopupil antidromic illumination, (h) horizontal, (v) vertical (or close to vertical, taking average of y and z rows). For additional information and methods descriptions please see the text and references.
SpeciesIntertegular Width (mm)Interommatial AngleAcceptance Angle (°)Sensitivity S (m2 sr)References
Apis mellifera3.20.9° v/1.6° h (c)1.7 (a), 1.6 (e)0.11[16,17,18]
Apis cerana3.0 1.2 (a)0.07[18]
Apis florea2.2 1.1 (a)0.03[18]
Apis dorsata3.9 1.8 (a)0.21[18]
Bombus terrestrissmall 3.0
large 4.2
1.2 v/2.9 h (pa)
0.9 v/2.1 h (pa)
[19]
Tetragonula carbonaria 1.3 v/2.3 h (pa) [20]
Xylocopa leucothorax7.50.9 v/1.5 h (po)0.8 (a)0.1[21]
Xylocopa tenuiscapa8.80.8 v/1.5 h (po)1.1 (a)0.3[21]
Xylocopa tranquebarica7.10.7 v/1.0 h (po)2.7 (a)2.7[21]
Megalopta genalis2.81.4 (po)5.62.7[22]
Table 2. Sexually dimorphic eyes and ocelli are common amongst Apidae. For additional details please see the references.
Table 2. Sexually dimorphic eyes and ocelli are common amongst Apidae. For additional details please see the references.
SpeciesSex/CasteMaximal Facet Diameter (m)Number of Facets/EyeMinimal Interommatial AngleMedian Ocellus Diameter (mm)References
Apis melliferaqueen26.14460 0.30[10,11,12,16,25]
worker25.253751.60.28
drone40.199931.00.34
Apis ceranaqueen25.93582 0.27[12]
worker25.44921 0.25
drone35.87994 0.30
Apis floreaqueen24.94036 0.27[12]
worker22.14394 0.20
drone38.09434 0.32
Apis dorsataqueen34.74479 0.38[12]
worker30.85974 0.40
drone46.38383 0.40
Bombus pratorumqueen30.15805 0.30[13]
worker27.14301 0.23
male28.54492 0.25
Bombus terrestrisqueen29.37691 0.38[13]
worker25.15656 0.28
male27.45624 0.31
Bombus melaleucusqueen36.98528 0.39[13]
worker29.55659 0.30
male39.38299 0.36
Bombus niveatusqueen28.88617 0.42[13]
worker26.87230 0.33
male36.48051 0.34
Bombus wurfleniiqueen32.66960 0.34[13]
worker27.95213 0.27
male28.65604 0.30
Bombus lapidariusqueen29.96765 0.38[13]
worker25.94800 0.30
male29.35214 0.30
Bombus hortorumqueen30.27010 0.31[13]
worker28.45170 0.25
male28.05232 0.25
Bombus pascuorumqueen32.26426 0.33[13]
worker28.05803 0.25
male29.15666 0.27
Bombus soroeensisqueen28.86042 0.31[13]
worker26.04250 0.24
male27.84968 0.26
Bombus confususqueen29.77569 0.35[13]
worker26.75870 0.25
male39.27821 0.33
Bombus mendaxqueen28.16868 0.35[13]
worker24.55375 0.25
male34.07032 0.28
Scaptotrigona posticaqueen193800 0.24[10]
worker213900 0.22
drone214500 0.29
queen193500 0.24
Xylocopa tenuiscapafemale37.315,9941.00.50[14,21]
male48.015,7510.70.60
Xylocopa leucothoraxfemale34.212,716 0.40[14,21]
male35.011,331 0.40
Xylocopa tranquebaricafemale38.718,804 0.95[14,21]
male40.015,511 0.90
Table 3. Behaviorally determined thresholds of spatial resolution in bees. (a) values for point objects are calculated taking the visual angle obtained by the object as half the resolvable wavelength, allowing for direct comparison with the other data sets. Thus, bees can detect objects subtending half the diameter indicated here.
Table 3. Behaviorally determined thresholds of spatial resolution in bees. (a) values for point objects are calculated taking the visual angle obtained by the object as half the resolvable wavelength, allowing for direct comparison with the other data sets. Thus, bees can detect objects subtending half the diameter indicated here.
SpeciesStimulusBehavioral Response TestedMinimum Spatial Wavelength (deg)Minimum Spatial Frequency (Cycle deg−1)Reference
Apis ceranaSine wave stationary gratingsObject discrimination2.8–3.80.26–0.36[61]
Sine wave gratingsFlight control, centering response1.2–8.30.12–0.8[64]
Apis melliferaGratings, bright lightObject discrimination40.25[24]
Gratings, dim lightObject discrimination8.30.12[24]
Square objectObject discrimination5.7 (a)0.18[16]
Square wave gratingsOptomotor response, walking bees2.10.48[65]
Square wave gratingObject discrimination40.25[58]
Point objectObject detection6–11 (a)0.09–0.17[28,59,60,62]
Sine wave gratingsFlight control, centering response5.5–80.12–0.18[64]
Bombus terrestrisPoint objectObject discrimination3.6–14 (a)0.27–0.07[19,62]
Sine wave gratingsObject discrimination4.80.21[66]
Sine wave gratingsFlight control, centering response4.80.21[64]
Bombus impatiensSine wave gratingsObject discrimination2.8–2.90.35–0.36[67]
Sine wave gratingsFlight control, centering response7.10.14[68]
Tetragonula carbonariaPoint objectObject detection18.8 (a)0.053[20]
Table 4. Foraging distances estimated for different Apidae species. For more information on methods, please see the references.
Table 4. Foraging distances estimated for different Apidae species. For more information on methods, please see the references.
SpeciesMethod Used to Infer Foraging RangesAverage Foraging Distance (m)Maximal Distance Estimate (m)Reference
Honeybees
Apis floreaMark-recapture150–250750[74]
Dances268>800[72]
Apis ceranaDances1951200[72]
Feeder6501423[75]
Apis dorsataDances8631000[72]
Apis melliferaDances 10,000[72]
Dances2300approx. 8000[76]
Dances157010,000[73]
Dances5500>10,000[77]
Bumblebees
Bombus muscorumMark-recapture55125[78]
Bombus lapidariusMark-recapture2601500[78]
Bombus terrestrisMark-recapture6631750[78]
Mark-recapture 1500[79]
Harmonic radar 630[80]
Carpenter bees
Xylocopa flavorufaRadio-transmitter 6040[81]
Xylocopa violaceaMark-recapture 1200[82]
Stingless bees
Plebeia droryana 540[83]
Melipona compressipes 2470[83]
Trigona spinipes 840[83]
Melipona quadrifasciata 2000[83]
Melipona fasciataRelease 2085[84]
Trigona capitataRelease 1547[84]
Trigona corvinarelease/feeder 590/320[71]
Tetragonisca angustularelease/feeder 662/680[71]
Nanotrigona testaceicornisrelease/feeder 484/120[71]
Paramona cff cupirarelease/feeder 622/520[71]

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Kelber, A.; Somanathan, H. Spatial Vision and Visually Guided Behavior in Apidae. Insects 2019, 10, 418. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects10120418

AMA Style

Kelber A, Somanathan H. Spatial Vision and Visually Guided Behavior in Apidae. Insects. 2019; 10(12):418. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects10120418

Chicago/Turabian Style

Kelber, Almut, and Hema Somanathan. 2019. "Spatial Vision and Visually Guided Behavior in Apidae" Insects 10, no. 12: 418. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects10120418

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop