Next Article in Journal
Distributed and Asynchronous Population-Based Optimization Applied to the Optimal Design of Fuzzy Controllers
Next Article in Special Issue
Why Are Most Humans Right-Handed? The Modified Fighting Hypothesis
Previous Article in Journal
Solving Integral Equations via Hybrid Interpolative ℛℐ-Type Contractions in 𝔟-Metric Spaces
Previous Article in Special Issue
Lateral Bias in Visual Working Memory
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

No Sex Differences in the Attentional Bias for the Right Side of Human Bodies

1
Department of Psychological Sciences, Health and Territorial, University of Chieti, Via dei Vestini 29, 66100 Chieti, Italy
2
Department of Medicine and Aging Sciences, Behavioral Imaging and Neural Dynamics (BIND) Center, University “G. d’Annunzio” of Chieti-Pescara, Via dei Vestini 29, 66100 Chieti, Italy
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Symmetry 2023, 15(2), 466; https://doi.org/10.3390/sym15020466
Submission received: 23 October 2022 / Revised: 13 January 2023 / Accepted: 6 February 2023 / Published: 9 February 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Individual Differences in Behavioral and Neural Lateralization)

Abstract

:
Ambiguous silhouettes representing human individuals which perform unimanual actions are interpreted more often as right-handed. Such a preference might reflect a perceptual frequency effect, due to the fact that most social interactions occur with right-handers. As a consequence, observers would preferentially attend to the region in which others’ dominant hand usually falls, thus increasing the efficiency in monitoring both aggressive and communicative acts. Given that men can be more dangerous compared with women, the right-hand bias should be larger when observing male rather than female individuals, and given that aggressive interactions involve men more frequently than women, it should be larger in male rather than female observers. However, previous studies did not specifically test whether: (i) male—compared with female—observers pay more attention to the right side of others (regardless of the observed individuals’ sex), or (ii) observers (regardless of their sex) pay more attention to the right side of male—compared with female—individuals. Therefore, in the present study we used ambiguous human silhouettes rotating about their vertical axis with one arm extended in order to determine whether the rightward bias is larger for male rather than female figures and/or in male rather than female participants. According to our data, the bias toward the right side of human bodies was not significantly associated with either the figure’s or the participant’s sex.

1. Introduction

Several studies indicate a perceptual and attentional bias toward the right side of others’ bodies in both right- and left-handers. For example, people tend to imagine right-handed individuals [1,2,3,4], and to perceive ambiguous silhouettes representing human individuals which perform unimanual actions as right-limbed [5,6,7,8,9,10,11]. Moreover, a perceptual frequency effect [12] has been proposed to explain the advantage that left-handed people exhibit in interactive sports, namely those which involve a physical contest with one or more opponents [13,14,15]. For instance, an advantage of left-handedness has been reported for various combat sports [16], including boxing [17,18,19], karate and taekwondo [20], wrestling [21], mixed martial arts [19,22,23], and fencing [24,25]. Consistent findings have also been observed in non-combat sports such as basketball [26], baseball [27], cricket [28], and tennis [29,30,31,32,33,34,35]. Some authors accounted for the left-handers’ advantage in sports with the so-called innate superiority hypothesis, which claims that left-handers are endowed with greater motor, spatial, and visual abilities compared with right-handers. However, other authors reject such an explanation and suggest some kind of tactical advantage (see [7] for a more detailed discussion). In particular, a common explanation is that the benefit of left-handed players is due to their opponents having little experience with playing against left-handed individuals, namely the negative frequency-dependent selection hypothesis [12]. Indeed, given that left-handers represent only about 10% of the general population [36,37], people usually play against right-handers, with the consequence that left-handers could be particularly advantaged in interactive sports because they utilize unfamiliar playing strategies and patterns of attack [14,38]. This could cause some problems because the required motor responses are underpracticed, with the consequence that the defensive reactions would be less automatic and effective [30]. Similarly, usual offensive strategies may fail against left-handers (e.g., as regards tennis, the backhand is generally less effective than the forehand, so that, when playing against a left-handed opponent, a player should accustom themselves to this uncommon situation [33]). As already proposed [1,2,3,4,5,6,7,9,10,11,39,40], the bias to perceive right-handed actions might be a result of the preference to attend to the region in which others’ dominant hand usually falls. Whereas such a tendency could entail an increased efficiency in monitoring both aggressive and communicative acts, given that the right rather than the left limb is preferred for both types of behavior, the drawback would be a reduced monitoring of the region in which the left hand falls. Thus, the left-handers’ advantage in fighting [12] and sports (for a review, see [15]) might arise from a reduced ability in the discrimination of left- rather than right-handed movements. The present study is theoretically grounded in the negative frequency-dependent selection hypothesis rather than in the innate superiority hypothesis, and we believe that the bias to perceive ambiguous human silhouettes as right-limbed observed in previous studies [5,6,7,8,9,10,11] can be accounted for by the frequent interaction with right-handed and right-footed individuals. It should be remarked that this interpretation is in agreement with the finding that the visual presentation of right- and left-handed actions intensifies and attenuates, respectively, the higher predictability of the outcome of right- rather than left-handed actions [41].
In previous research using ambiguous stimuli, we reported some findings suggestive of a stronger preference for perceiving right-limbed movements in male rather than female observers ([6], Experiment 1 in [7]). From an evolutionary perspective, such findings might be accounted for by the higher incidence of physical fights among male rather than female individuals [42], which could have resulted in a larger bias toward the right side of men rather than women, as well as in a greater advantage of male left-handers compared with female left-handers in interactive sports. In line with this reasoning, tennis research suggests that the left-handers’ advantage might be modulated by sex, because it seems to be larger for male compared with female players [15,29,34,43,44,45]. For example, Breznik [29] found that male left-handed professional tennis players are more successful in competitions than their female counterparts. In this regard, the male bias observed in several studies—according to which point-light walkers are perceived more frequently as male than as female [46,47,48]—is not astonishing, because misinterpreting a man for a woman can be more costly than misinterpreting a woman for a man, with men usually representing more threatening stimuli compared with women. The facing bias reported for human biological motion (according to which point-light walkers without explicit depth cues are interpreted more frequently as facing toward the viewer than as facing away from the viewer; [46,49], see also [5,7,8]) seems also to be linked to the perceived sex of the figure. Indeed, such a bias is larger when the figure is perceived as male rather than female [46,50], and even larger in male observers than in female observers [46]. In summary, given that men might be more dangerous compared with women, the right-hand bias should be larger when observing male rather than female individuals, and given that aggressive interactions involve men more frequently than women, it should be larger in male rather than female observers. Such hypotheses would be in agreement with the aforementioned finding that the advantage of left-handed players is larger for males rather than females [15,29,34,43,44,45]. However, since previous research has examined males playing against males and females playing against females, they do not allow the discrimination of whether male observers (compared with female observers) pay more attention to the right side of others (regardless of the observed individuals’ sex), or whether observers (regardless of their sex) pay more attention to the right side of male rather than female individuals. On the other hand, testing both female and male participants with both female and male stimuli would allow the discrimination between the two possible explanations. In line with these considerations, the aim of our study was to demonstrate whether the rightward bias is larger for male rather than female silhouettes and/or in male rather than female participants.

2. Materials and Method

Following the recommendations proposed by Simmons, Nelson, and Simonsohn [51], we provide detailed information on how we defined the sample size, as well as on manipulations, measures, and data exclusions carried out in the present study.

2.1. Participants

Sixty-four participants (32 females and 32 males; age: 18–30 years) took part in the study. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision. For each combination of response arrow spinning direction (clockwise, CW, or counterclockwise, CCW), color (red or green), and position (above or below; see Procedure section), we scheduled the recruitment of eight female participants and eight male participants.

2.2. Stimuli

As stimuli, we used 256 animations obtained from two original animations, created with the software Poser Pro 2012 (Smith Micro Software Inc., Pittsburgh, PA, USA), one representing the silhouette of a human female and the other of a human male (the two silhouettes visually differed by their physical appearance, and more specifically by their sexually dimorphic traits, such as different waist-to-hip ratios and the presence of breasts in the female one). The silhouette stood on its legs with one arm extended (Figure 1) and rotated about its vertical axis while maintaining a static posture. The silhouette was depicted in black against a white background and no clear-cut depth cue was available, so that the animation was ambiguous and could be perceived as rotating either CW or CCW. The two original animations were decomposed into their 32 constituent frames using Adobe Photoshop CS6 (Adobe, San Jose, CA, USA). Then, we created 64 different versions for each animation. Each version consisted of a whole spin of the silhouette, and was obtained by rearranging the 32 frames on the basis of starting frame and order (i.e., from the 1st frame to the 32nd and vice versa, from the 2nd frame to the 1st and vice versa, and so on). This order manipulation counterbalanced the association between extended arm and spinning direction, given that in the original order (which can be defined as “palmward” with reference to the hand movement) the CW rotation was congruent with an extended left arm, whereas in the inverted order (which can be defined as “backward” with reference to the hand movement) the CW rotation was congruent with an extended right arm, and vice versa for the CCW rotation. Although the Poser parameters (such as camera elevation and distance) were set so as to remove—as far as possible—potential perspective cues (such as relative height and relative size), a second set of 128 animations was created by mirroring, horizontally, each frame. This manipulation counterbalanced the effects of any remaining uncontrolled depth cue on the extended arm. Therefore, the final set of stimuli consisted of 256 animations, which represented each possible combination of the figure’s sex (female or male), starting frame, type of rotation (palmward or backward movement), and mirroring (the whole set of frames constituting the different stimuli is available in the Open Science Framework repository at: https://osf.io/djx5v, accessed on 7 February 2023). The component frames of each stimulus measured around 10.1° vertically and, on average, around 3.7° horizontally at a viewing distance of 57 cm.

2.3. Procedure

The experiment was run using SuperLab 4.0 (Cedrus Corporation, San Pedro, CA, USA) on a Windows (Microsoft Corporation, Redmond, WA, USA) notebook with an Intel (Intel Corporation, Santa Clara, CA, USA) processor and a 15.6-inch monitor. Participants, seated comfortably in a quiet room with their eyes about 57 cm from the computer screen, were instructed to place their hands palm-down on the table and not to cross their legs, arms, or even fingers throughout the experiment, which consisted of two 128-trial blocks, one with the female silhouette and the other with the male silhouette as stimuli. After the first block, participants were allowed to take a short break before the second block (the order of block presentation was counterbalanced across participants). In each trial, a black fixation cross, presented for 500 ms in the center of a white screen, was followed by one of the previously described stimuli, presented centrally for 2300 ms, and then by a pair of colored arrows, one slightly above and one slightly below the center of the screen, which represented the two possible spinning directions of the silhouette (see Figure 2). Participants were required to gaze at the fixation point and to indicate the perceived spinning direction of the silhouette by responding with the word “ROSSO” (the Italian word for “RED”) or “VERDE” (“GREEN”) on the basis of the arrow corresponding to their percept. The experimenter, seated behind the participant, recorded the participant’s response by pressing the key “R” or “V” on a keyboard connected to the computer, and then the next trial started. In each block, stimuli were shown in a random sequence.
The starting frame of each stimulus lasted 750 ms, whereas each of the following 31 frames lasted 50 ms (this stratagem aimed to reduce the possible carry-over of responses from trial to trial; e.g., see [52]). Moreover, we instructed the participants to provide their responses by selecting one of the two colored arrows, which represented the two spinning directions (CW or CCW), rather than by asking them to provide simple vocal responses, because labeling as CW or CCW a rotation about an axis approximately parallel to their own body axis can be rather difficult for participants. Before the experiment, participants were familiarized with the response modality by administering them a pretest in which they used the two response arrows to indicate the spinning direction of a black human silhouette which included a clear-cut perspective clue, such as the relative size of the hands in different positions. This stratagem allowed the exclusion from the study of any individual unable to carry out the task. Finally, participants’ hand preference was assessed by administering them the Italian version of the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [53]. The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Psychology of the Department of Psychological Sciences, Health and Territory of the University of Chieti (protocol code 21003). All participants provided written informed consent.

2.4. Data Analysis

Fifty-eight participants (with a positive laterality score on the Italian version of the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory [53] [range: 0.15/1.00; M = 0.65 ± 0.09 SEM]) were classified as right-handers, and six participants (with a negative laterality score [range: 0.80/0.19; M = 0.54 ± 0.28 SEM]) were classified as left-handers.
We aimed to examine whether the rightward bias for male stimuli overcomes the rightward bias for female stimuli. First of all, we discarded from the data analysis two male subjects that gave the same response—‘ROSSO’ or ‘VERDE’—to all the 256 trials. Then, we disregarded six female participants and four male participants who scored more than two standard deviations above or below the mean of their ‘Sex’ group in the number of figures perceived as right-handed for any combination of the figure’s sex (female or male) and type of rotation (palmward or backward). After testing whether the number of figures perceived as right-handed was larger than the number of figures perceived as left-handed, we performed a repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) on the number of figures perceived as right-handed, using type of rotation (palmward or backward) and figure’s sex (female or male) as within-subjects factors, and participant’s sex (female or male) as the between-subjects factor. Because of the limited number of left-handers, we did not include handedness as an independent variable in the analysis of variance performed, but we correlated the laterality score with the number of figures perceived as right-handed.

3. Results

The number of figures perceived as right-handed (M = 139.54 [54.51%]) was larger than the number of figures perceived as left-handed (M = 116.46 [45.49%]; t51 = 5.54; p < 0.001). The ANOVA showed no significant effect (Table 1).
As shown in Figure 3, both sex groups perceived a significantly larger number of right-handed figures (female participants: t25 = 4.60; p < 0.001; male participants: t25 = 3.22; p < 0.005). As shown in Figure 4, participants perceived a significantly larger number of right-handed figures for both female and male stimuli (female stimuli: t51 = 4.36; p < 0.001; male stimuli: t51 = 5.26; p < 0.001). Moreover, no significant correlation was found between the number of figures perceived as right-handed and the laterality score (r52 = −0.06; ns).

4. Discussion

The present study corroborated the existence of a bias toward the right side of observed bodies, as already shown in several previous studies [1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9,10,11]. Indeed, regardless of the figure’s sex (female or male) and the participant’s sex (female or male), participants showed a preference to perceive the silhouette spinning consistently with a right- rather than a left-hand action. Therefore, the predictions that the bias for the right hand would have been stronger (1) in the male rather than the female participants or (2) when observing male rather than female individuals were not confirmed. Actually, in our previous research with ambiguous stimuli we found only circumstantial clues to a stronger preference for perceiving right-limbed movements in male rather than female observers, and they might be instances of type I errors ([6], Experiment 1 in [7]). The present data are thus not in line with those from sport studies indicating that the left-handers’ advantage is larger for male than for female players [15,29,34,43,44,45]. However, it should be noticed that the large prevalence of right-handed actions in the real world (about 90%) is reflected in a slight prevalence of ambiguous human bodies perceived as right-limbed (in most cases, about 53%) [5,6,7,8,9,10,11]. Therefore, it is plausible that, if any sex difference exists in attentional and perceptual asymmetries toward the right side of others, it may not be strong enough to modulate accordingly the tendency to perceive ambiguous human silhouettes as right-handed, having to act on an extremely narrow range. Another possibility is that the relatively low level of interaction implied in the stimuli (i.e., an isolated individual rotating about their vertical axis with one arm extended) might have prevented the emergence of the hypothesized sex differences. On the other hand, the present results further support the generalizability of the bias to interpret ambiguous human bodies as right-handed rather than left-handed. In fact, whereas previous studies showed that the right-limb bias is observed both for static [8,9,10,11] and dynamic [5,6,7,10] stimuli, and for both hand [6,7,8,9,10,11] and foot [5,6,7] actions, the finding reported here demonstrates how the ubiquity of the bias extends to human bodies with different features, such as the sexually dimorphic traits of female and male bodies. However, other individual differences might modulate the bias toward the right side of human bodies. For instance, testing different age groups might determine whether such a bias is innate (in which case it should be comparable in younger and older individuals) or acquired (in which case it should increase with age, as occurs for asymmetries in face perception; for a review, see [54]). Moreover, it could be examined whether the bias toward the right side of human bodies is reduced in individuals with autistic traits. Indeed, it should be noticed that (1) individuals with autism exhibit impaired configural processing [55] and (2) inversion—which impairs the configural processing of human bodies (e.g., [56,57])—extinguished the preference to perceive right-handed actions in ambiguous human bodies [10]. Asymmetries in face perception also seem to be absent [58] or delayed [59] in individuals with autism. Therefore, an interesting question is whether the bias toward the right side of human bodies is related to perceptual asymmetries for faces, as well as to any factors affecting the latter (e.g., maternal cradling preferences, anxiety, emotional context; see [39] for a more detailed discussion). In this regard, we point out that the long-standing observation (e.g., [60,61]) of a perceptual bias in favor of the right side of human faces (which falls in the observer’s left visual field) cannot be accounted for by asymmetric characteristics of the faces themselves, especially in light of the fact that the left hemiface is usually more expressive than the right hemiface (e.g., see [62] for a more detailed discussion). On the contrary, we have proposed [39,40] that the continuous interaction with right-handed individuals might foster the emergence of a rightward bias not only for bodies but also for faces (and hence the hypothesized associations). Future studies specifically addressing these issues are warranted.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.L. and D.M.; methodology, C.L., C.F. and D.M.; formal analysis, C.L. and D.M.; investigation, C.L. and C.F.; resources, M.B. and L.T.; writing—original draft preparation, C.L. and D.M.; writing—review and editing, C.F., M.B. and L.T.; visualization, C.L.; supervision, D.M. and L.T.; project administration, D.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Psychology of the Department of Psychological Sciences, Health and Territory of the University of Chieti (protocol code 21003).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

All stimuli and datasets generated and analyzed during the current study are available in the Open Science Framework repository at: https://osf.io/36d7y/?view_only= (accessed on 7 February 2023).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Marzoli, D.; Menditto, S.; Lucafò, C.; Tommasi, L. Imagining Others’ Handedness: Visual and Motor Processes in the Attribution of the Dominant Hand to an Imagined Agent. Exp. Brain Res. 2013, 229, 37–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Marzoli, D.; Lucafò, C.; Rescigno, C.; Mussini, E.; Padulo, C.; Prete, G.; D’Anselmo, A.; Malatesta, G.; Tommasi, L. Sex-Specific Effects of Posture on the Attribution of Handedness to an Imagined Agent. Exp. Brain Res. 2017, 235, 1163–1171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Marzoli, D.; Mitaritonna, A.; Moretto, F.; Carluccio, P.; Tommasi, L. The Handedness of Imagined Bodies in Action and the Role of Perspective Taking. Brain Cogn. 2011, 75, 51–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Marzoli, D.; Palumbo, R.; Di Domenico, A.; Penolazzi, B.; Garganese, P.; Tommasi, L. The Relation between Self-Reported Empathy and Motor Identification with Imagined Agents. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e14595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Lucafò, C.; Marzoli, D.; Prete, G.; Tommasi, L. Laterality Effects in the Spinning Dancer Illusion: The Viewing-from-above Bias Is Only Part of the Story. Br. J. Psychol. Lond. Engl. 1953 2016, 107, 698–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Lucafò, C.; Marzoli, D.; Padulo, C.; Troiano, S.; Pelosi Zazzerini, L.; Malatesta, G.; Amodeo, I.; Tommasi, L. Hemifield-Specific Rotational Biases during the Observation of Ambiguous Human Silhouettes. Symmetry 2021, 13, 1349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Lucafò, C.; Marzoli, D.; Zdybek, P.; Malatesta, G.; Smerilli, F.; Ferrara, C.; Tommasi, L. The Bias toward the Right Side of Others Is Stronger for Hands than for Feet. Symmetry 2021, 13, 146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Marzoli, D.; Lucafò, C.; Pagliara, A.; Cappuccio, R.; Brancucci, A.; Tommasi, L. Both Right- and Left-Handers Show a Bias to Attend Others’ Right Arm. Exp. Brain Res. 2015, 233, 415–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Marzoli, D.; Pagliara, A.; Prete, G.; Malatesta, G.; Lucafò, C.; Padulo, C.; Brancucci, A.; Tommasi, L. Lateralized Embodiment of Ambiguous Human Silhouettes: Data on Sex Differences. Data Brief 2019, 25, 104009. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Marzoli, D.; Lucafò, C.; Padulo, C.; Prete, G.; Giacinto, L.; Tommasi, L. Inversion Reveals Perceptual Asymmetries in the Configural Processing of Human Body. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2017, 11, 126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Marzoli, D.; Pagliara, A.; Prete, G.; Malatesta, G.; Lucafò, C.; Padulo, C.; Brancucci, A.; Tommasi, L. Hemispheric Asymmetries in the Processing of Body Sides: A Study with Ambiguous Human Silhouettes. Neurosci. Lett. 2017, 656, 114–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Faurie, C.; Raymond, M. Handedness, Homicide and Negative Frequency-Dependent Selection. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2005, 272, 25–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Grouios, G.; Tsorbatzoudis, H.; Alexandris, K.; Barkoukis, V. Do Left-Handed Competitors Have an Innate Superiority in Sports? Percept. Mot. Skills 2000, 90, 1273–1282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Grouios, G.; Koidou, E.; Tsormpatzoudis, C.; Alexandris, K. Handedness in Sport. J. Hum. Mov. Stud. 2002, 43, 347–361. [Google Scholar]
  15. Raymond, M.; Pontier, D.; Dufour, A.B.; Møller, A.P. Frequency-Dependent Maintenance of Left Handedness in Humans. Proc. Biol. Sci. 1996, 263, 1627–1633. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Groothuis, T.G.G.; McManus, I.C.; Schaafsma, S.M.; Geuze, R.H. The Fighting Hypothesis in Combat: How Well Does the Fighting Hypothesis Explain Human Left-Handed Minorities? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2013, 1288, 100–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Gursoy, R. Effects of Left- or Right-Hand Preference on the Success of Boxers in Turkey. Br. J. Sports Med. 2009, 43, 142–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Loffing, F.; Hagemann, N. Pushing through Evolution? Incidence and Fight Records of Left-Oriented Fighters in Professional Boxing History. Laterality Asymmetries Body Brain Cogn. 2015, 20, 270–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Richardson, T.; Gilman, R.T. Left-Handedness Is Associated with Greater Fighting Success in Humans. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 15402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Cingoz, Y.E.; Gursoy, R.; Ozan, M.; Hazar, K.; Dalli, M. Research on the Relation between Hand Preference and Success in Karate and Taekwondo Sports with Regards to Gender. Adv. Phys. Educ. 2018, 8, 308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Ziyagil, M.A.; Gursoy, R.; Dane, S.; Yuksel, R. Left-Handed Wrestlers Are More Successful. Percept. Mot. Skills 2010, 111, 65–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Dochtermann, N.A.; Gienger, C.M.; Zappettini, S. Born to Win? Maybe, but Perhaps Only against Inferior Competition. Anim. Behav. 2014, 96, e1–e3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Pollet, T.V.; Stulp, G.; Groothuis, T.G. Born to Win? Testing the Fighting Hypothesis in Realistic Fights: Left-Handedness in the Ultimate Fighting Championship. Anim. Behav. 2013, 86, 839–843. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Bisiacchi, P.S.; Ripoll, H.; Stein, J.; Simonet, P.; Azemar, G. Left-Handedness in Fencers: An Attentional Advantage? Percept. Mot. Skills 1985, 61, 507–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Harris, L.J. In Fencing, What Gives Left-Handers the Edge? Views from the Present and the Distant Past. Laterality 2010, 15, 15–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Lawler, T.P.; Lawler, F.H. Left-Handedness in Professional Basketball: Prevalence, Performance, and Survival. Percept. Mot. Skills 2011, 113, 815–824. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Goldstein, S.R.; Young, C.A. “ Evolutionary” Stable Strategy of Handedness in Major League Baseball. J. Comp. Psychol. 1996, 110, 164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Brooks, R.; Bussière, L.F.; Jennions, M.D.; Hunt, J. Sinister Strategies Succeed at the Cricket World Cup. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2004, 271 (Suppl. 3), S64–S66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Breznik, K. On the Gender Effects of Handedness in Professional Tennis. J. Sports Sci. Med. 2013, 12, 346–353. [Google Scholar]
  30. Hagemann, N. The Advantage of Being Left-Handed in Interactive Sports. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2009, 71, 1641–1648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Holtzen, D.W. Handedness and Professional Tennis. Int. J. Neurosci. 2000, 105, 101–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Loffing, F.; Hagemann, N.; Strauss, B. The Serve in Professional Men’s Tennis: Effects of Players’ Handedness. Int. J. Perform. Anal. Sport 2009, 9, 255–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Loffing, F.; Hagemann, N.; Strauss, B. Automated Processes in Tennis: Do Left-Handed Players Benefit from the Tactical Preferences of Their Opponents? J. Sports Sci. 2010, 28, 435–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Loffing, F.; Hagemann, N.; Strauss, B. Left-Handedness in Professional and Amateur Tennis. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e49325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Wood, C.J.; Aggleton, J.P. Handedness in “fast Ball” Sports: Do Left-Handers Have an Innate Advantage? Br. J. Psychol. Lond. Engl. 1953 1989, 80, 227–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Coren, S. The Left-Hander Syndrome: The Causes and Consequences of Left-Handedness; Free Press: New York, NY, USA, 1992; p. 308. ISBN 978-0-02-906682-9. [Google Scholar]
  37. Papadatou-Pastou, M.; Ntolka, E.; Schmitz, J.; Martin, M.; Munafò, M.R.; Ocklenburg, S.; Paracchini, S. Human Handedness: A Meta-Analysis. Psychol. Bull. 2020, 146, 481–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Grouios, G. Motoric Dominance and Sporting Excellence: Training versus Heredity. Percept. Mot. Skills 2004, 98, 53–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Marzoli, D.; Prete, G.; Tommasi, L. Perceptual Asymmetries and Handedness: A Neglected Link? Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Marzoli, D.; D’Anselmo, A.; Malatesta, G.; Lucafò, C.; Prete, G.; Tommasi, L. The Intricate Web of Asymmetric Processing of Social Stimuli in Humans. Symmetry 2022, 14, 1096. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Schorer, J.; Loffing, F.; Hagemann, N.; Baker, J. Human Handedness in Interactive Situations: Negative Perceptual Frequency Effects Can Be Reversed! J. Sports Sci. 2012, 30, 507–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Daly, M.; Wilson, M. Killing the Competition: Female/Female and Male/Male Homicide. Hum. Nat. Hawthorne N 1990, 1, 81–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Baker, J.; Kungl, A.-M.; Pabst, J.; Strauß, B.; Büsch, D.; Schorer, J. Your Fate Is in Your Hands? Handedness, Digit Ratio (2D:4D), and Selection to a National Talent Development System. Laterality 2013, 18, 710–718. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Barrenetxea-Garcia, J.; Torres-Unda, J.; Esain, I.; Gil, S.M. Relative Age Effect and Left-Handedness in World Class Water Polo Male and Female Players. Laterality 2019, 24, 259–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Connor, J.D.; Mann, D.L.; Gomez, M.-A.; Leicht, A.S.; Doma, K. Performance Advantages of Left-Handed Cricket Batting Talent. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Schouten, B.; Troje, N.F.; Brooks, A.; van der Zwan, R.; Verfaillie, K. The Facing Bias in Biological Motion Perception: Effects of Stimulus Gender and Observer Sex. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2010, 72, 1256–1260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Troje, N.F.; Sadr, J.; Geyer, H.; Nakayama, K. Adaptation Aftereffects in the Perception of Gender from Biological Motion. J. Vis. 2006, 6, 850–857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. van der Zwan, R.; Machatch, C.; Kozlowski, D.; Troje, N.F.; Blanke, O.; Brooks, A. Gender Bending: Auditory Cues Affect Visual Judgements of Gender in Biological Motion Displays. Exp. Brain Res. 2009, 198, 373–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Vanrie, J.; Dekeyser, M.; Verfaillie, K. Bistability and Biasing Effects in the Perception of Ambiguous Point-Light Walkers. Perception 2004, 33, 547–560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Brooks, A.; Schouten, B.; Troje, N.F.; Verfaillie, K.; Blanke, O.; van der Zwan, R. Correlated Changes in Perceptions of the Gender and Orientation of Ambiguous Biological Motion Figures. Curr. Biol. CB 2008, 18, R728–R729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Simmons, J.P.; Nelson, L.D.; Simonsohn, U. A 21 Word Solution. Dialogue Off. Newsl. Soc. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2012, 26, 4–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Liu, C.-H.; Tzeng, O.J.L.; Hung, D.L.; Tseng, P.; Juan, C.-H. Investigation of Bistable Perception with the “Silhouette Spinner”: Sit Still, Spin the Dancer with Your Will. Vision Res. 2012, 60, 34–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Salmaso, D.; Longoni, A.M. Problems in the Assessment of Hand Preference. Cortex J. Devoted Study Nerv. Syst. Behav. 1985, 21, 533–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Watling, D.; Woekman, L.; Bourne, V.J. Emotion Lateralisation: Developments throughout the Lifespan. Laterality 2012, 17, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  55. Behrmann, M.; Avidan, G.; Leonard, G.L.; Kimchi, R.; Luna, B.; Humphreys, K.; Minshew, N. Configural Processing in Autism and Its Relationship to Face Processing. Neuropsychologia 2006, 44, 110–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Reed, C.L.; Stone, V.E.; Bozova, S.; Tanaka, J. The Body-Inversion Effect. Psychol. Sci. 2003, 14, 302–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Reed, C.L.; Stone, V.E.; Grubb, J.D.; McGoldrick, J.E. Turning Configural Processing Upside down: Part and Whole Body Postures. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2006, 32, 73–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Dundas, E.M.; Best, C.A.; Minshew, N.J.; Strauss, M.S. A Lack of Left Visual Field Bias When Individuals with Autism Process Faces. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2012, 42, 1104–1111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Taylor, S.; Workman, L.; Yeomans, H. Abnormal Patterns of Cerebral Lateralisation as Revealed by the Universal Chimeric Faces Task in Individuals with Autistic Disorder. Laterality 2012, 17, 428–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Gilbert, C.; Bakan, P. Visual Asymmetry in Perception of Faces. Neuropsychologia 1973, 11, 355–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Wolff, W. The Experimental Study of Forms of Expression. Character Personal. Q. Psychodiagnostic Allied Stud. 1933, 2, 168–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Malatesta, G.; Marzoli, D.; Tommasi, L. Keep a Left Profile, Baby! The Left-Cradling Bias Is Associated with a Preference for Left-Facing Profiles of Human Babies. Symmetry 2020, 12, 911. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Examples of male (left) and female (right) stimuli.
Figure 1. Examples of male (left) and female (right) stimuli.
Symmetry 15 00466 g001
Figure 2. Examples of timeline.
Figure 2. Examples of timeline.
Symmetry 15 00466 g002
Figure 3. Mean number (and SEM) of actions perceived as right- and left-handed by female and male participants.
Figure 3. Mean number (and SEM) of actions perceived as right- and left-handed by female and male participants.
Symmetry 15 00466 g003
Figure 4. Mean number (and SEM) of actions perceived as right- and left-handed for female and male stimuli.
Figure 4. Mean number (and SEM) of actions perceived as right- and left-handed for female and male stimuli.
Symmetry 15 00466 g004
Table 1. ANOVA results and corresponding effect size estimates.
Table 1. ANOVA results and corresponding effect size estimates.
FactorsdfFSig.ηp2
participant’s sex1,501.5010.2260.029
figure’s sex1,500.5560.4590.011
figure’s sex X participant’s sex1,502.3220.1340.044
type of rotation1,502.3420.1320.045
type of rotation X participant’s sex1,500.0420.8390.001
figure’s sex X type of rotation1,500.4400.5100.009
figure’s sex X type of rotation X participant’s sex1,503.5810.0640.067
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Lucafò, C.; Marzoli, D.; Ferrara, C.; Bertollo, M.; Tommasi, L. No Sex Differences in the Attentional Bias for the Right Side of Human Bodies. Symmetry 2023, 15, 466. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym15020466

AMA Style

Lucafò C, Marzoli D, Ferrara C, Bertollo M, Tommasi L. No Sex Differences in the Attentional Bias for the Right Side of Human Bodies. Symmetry. 2023; 15(2):466. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym15020466

Chicago/Turabian Style

Lucafò, Chiara, Daniele Marzoli, Cosimo Ferrara, Maurizio Bertollo, and Luca Tommasi. 2023. "No Sex Differences in the Attentional Bias for the Right Side of Human Bodies" Symmetry 15, no. 2: 466. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym15020466

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop