Next Article in Journal
Fuzzy Weighted Clustering Method for Numerical Attributes of Communication Big Data Based on Cloud Computing
Previous Article in Journal
Particles with Negative Energies in Nonrelativistic and Relativistic Cases
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Navigators’ Errors in a Ship Collision via Simulation Experiment in South Korea

1
Mokpo National Maritime University, Mokpo, Jeollanam-do 58628, Korea
2
Division of Navigation Sciences, Korea Maritime and Ocean University, 727, Busan 49112, Korea
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Symmetry 2020, 12(4), 529; https://doi.org/10.3390/sym12040529
Submission received: 28 February 2020 / Revised: 18 March 2020 / Accepted: 18 March 2020 / Published: 3 April 2020
(This article belongs to the Section Computer)

Abstract

:
A very significant number of marine accidents occur because of human errors. This study aimed to prevent ship collisions by identifying types of navigators’ errors. Based on Reason’s classification theory, the possible human errors are classified into skill-based slips (SBSs) (errors caused by the lack of skills), rule-based mistakes (RBMs) (errors caused by the misapplication of rules), and knowledge-based mistakes (KBMs) (errors caused by the lack of navigator’s knowledge). For this study, a scenario-based experiment using a ship-handling simulator was conducted with 50 recruited student navigators. The results revealed two primary human errors of accidents, namely lack of knowledge and misapplication of rules. The results suggest that a collision can be minimized when a navigator has sufficient knowledge of an appropriate course of action and a deep understanding of safety rules. Accidents cannot be prevented by identifying errors, but steps can be taken to narrow the knowledge gap. Based on the results, we proposed a simulation training on navigator error in an unfamiliar situation. The results are expected to reduce errors in the maritime sector using a human-centric work system.

1. Introduction

The magnitude of marine accidents is considerable; therefore, their possible causes must be assessed [1]. Human errors must be considered the greatest risk [2] because they are related to a majority of marine accidents (75%–96%) [3]. Several researchers have investigated methods to reduce human errors, which requires a clear understanding of human behavior [4]. The US Navy’s aircraft carriers, nuclear power plants, and organizations such as air traffic control centers have experienced difficulties in reducing human errors [5]. For example, research has been conducted to divide workers’ behavior based on skills, rules, and knowledge to reduce human errors in nuclear power plants [6].
In the marine industry, the navigator’s error causing a ship collision occurs during bridgework [7]. Bridgework includes rudder and wheel controls for maneuvering ships, circumference, avoiding ships, reflecting plans to destinations, and locating ships. In South Korea, 79% of maritime accidents that occurred over the last five years were related to navigators’ errors [8]. Navigators’ errors occur because of insufficient preparation for departure, poor management of voyage plan, negligence of evaluating ship location, inappropriate maneuvering, negligence of lookout, insufficient preparation and response to severe weather conditions and anchoring and mooring, misapplication of a navigation rule, negligence of duty, and non-compliance with safe working regulations on board. Similar to the case of aviation, human factor analysis and classification systems have been used to investigate human errors in the marine industry [9]. In the maritime sector, studies have been conducted to classify situational awareness of seafarers via surveys [10] and classified bridgework as a criterion for situational awareness [11,12]. Recently, because of the use of augmented reality, there has been an improvement in avoiding collisions [13] and human behavior in intelligent systems [14]. However, because human errors have not been sufficiently investigated, studying human errors via education and training is important [4,15]. To avoid any unforeseen accidents, navigators’ errors can be corrected using a ship-handling simulator by enhancing their relevant skills, rules, and knowledge required [16]. Therefore, Reason’s human error classification was applied herein [17,18]. This study aimed to identify navigators’ errors via ship-handling simulations. Using a simulator is an improved alternative method of training with relatively low cost and high safety [19,20]. Using a ship-handling simulator, Gould et al. tested human performance [21], but they did not intensively analyze human errors; therefore, in this study, we evaluated the navigator’s errors. Evaluating navigators’ errors can be used for human–machine evaluation studies and interface manipulation that are currently expanding into the human-oriented design of the maritime sector. Furthermore, specific training methods to reduce navigators’ errors can be proposed for the safety of the vessel, which should include safety via human–technical interactions. The results of the simulation experiment can be used to detail the current comprehensive range of training and assess the bridgework, thus identifying the relationship between the navigational equipment interface and the navigators’ work.

2. Classification of Navigators’ Errors

Psychologists Reason and Norman developed a general classification of human errors, which were divided into two major categories, i.e., slips and mistakes [18,22,23]. According to them, slips occur because of skill-based errors or skill-based slips (SBSs), whereas mistakes are classified into rule-based mistakes (RBMs) and knowledge-based mistakes (KBMs). Slips are attributed to subconscious actions being set aside en route; they occur when the goal is correct, but the actions are not properly performed, i.e., the execution is flawed. For example, SBS can occur when a navigator manipulates the radar interface to a wrong target under the danger of collision in the open sea during bridgework [23]. For this study, the probability of SBS is between 1/200 and 1/20,000 [24].
Mistakes are attributed to inappropriate goals and plans. A person makes a poor decision, misclassifies a situation, or fails to consider relevant factors [22]. Rule-based mistakes (RBMs) perform actions consistent with their intentions but fail to achieve their intended results because of the incorrect application of rules [18]. Herein, the diagnosis of the situation is appropriate but the course of action is erroneous, i.e., the wrong rule is applied [22]. RBM is related to familiar situations or learned situations. For example, to avoid a collision, a veering behavior with the “port-to-port” principle from the Convention on the International Regulations for the Purposes of Collision at Sea should be applied [25]. Thus, RBM includes mistakes that can occur in action based on the established procedures that the navigator makes a decision in a familiar situation. For instance, while sailing on a designated course of the coastal voyage during bridgework, the navigator identified a vessel in immediate danger of collision. Note that the navigator must sail the ship in the route as per international rules, but RBM occurs when the navigator does not comply with the international navigation rules. The error probability for this study is between 1/20 and 1/2,000 [24].
Finally, owing to insufficient knowledge, the knowledge-based mistake (KBM) includes performing intended actions but not achieving the intended outcome [18]. The misdiagnosis of the problem is attributed to insufficient knowledge [22]. To avoid human errors, navigators must have clear action plans using conscious analytical processes and stored knowledge [26]. KBM occurs when the navigator has to navigate under a sudden change of visibility or weather conditions and needs to develop an action plan for collision avoiadance [26]. At this point, KBM occurs when the navigator prepares an inappropriate action plan because of the lack of knowledge [6]. The error probability for these types of behaviors is between 1/2 and 1/200 [24]. Therefore, as shown in Figure 1, this study used the human error classification developed by Reason. Herein, we investigated a navigator’s human errors based on the method used to investigate human error caused by a road traffic driver [27].

3. Materials and Methods

Generally, a ship-handling simulator is used in the industry because it creates accurate and realistic scenarios to train students; therefore, to measure the participants’ behavior according to Reason’s human error classification, we used a ship-handling simulator. The participants executed the ship collision scenario via a ship-handling simulator and their errors were assessed via checklists, which comprised items to evaluate navigator errors according to the human error classification. The participants’ errors according to the checklists were compared with video recording and participants’ written action plans to avoid a collision.

3.1. Participants

All participants were student navigators. For this study, 50 adults (45 men and 5 women) with an average age of 22.6 years (range between 22 and 25 years old) participated. These participants had taken maritime training courses for three and a half years at International Maritime Organization (IMO)-accredited maritime educational institutions. They had a third-class deck officer license and were in a merchant vessel as an intern for more than a year. All participants had at least 80 hours of experience with simulation training.

3.2. Materials

Using a real environment experiment is difficult because of both time and cost. Therefore, the proposed simulator was used as an alternative [19,21]. The equipment used herein was a ship-handling simulator from Kongsberg, Norway; it was installed by Kongsberg Korea as an experimental equipment at Mokpo Maritime University. Its configuration is shown in Figure 2. For this study, a telegraph to control the speed of the ship, a wheel to change the direction of the ship, a radar for navigational aid, and an electronic chart display and information system (ECDIS) were included. We implemented a large-screen virtual reality training session and displayed navigation information on speed and rate of turn. The visual monitor was displayed at 160° ahead, thus allowing the user to control the visible background. Moreover, a video recorder was installed to record the operation of the interface, such as participants’ behaviors and navigation devices, to reevaluate participants’ behaviors using the recorded screen in the event of any doubt on their behavior evaluation. Mokpo Port in South Korea was selected as a simulation site because most participants have visited it at least ten times; therefore, they were familiar with the place. For this study, all the dynamic data of the ship during navigation were stored by the instructor.

3.3. Procedure and Scenarios

For this study, the autopilot mode was used; therefore, the bridge team comprised a navigator solo watch. Because the ship-handling simulator was separately installed, the experiment was repeated for three consecutive weeks owing to the number of participants and individually observing the progress over three weeks. Furthermore, the bridge team normally consists of skillful captain and navigator, but in this experiment, a solo watch was used to measure the errors of navigators. The participants were trained on the core of the experiment and scenario. Then, the familiarization process with the ship-handling simulator was conducted and they became familiar with the maneuvering and various interfaces of the simulators. Because familiarization with ship-handling simulators was important for measuring SBS [6], 15 min were allowed for familiarization with the interface, radar, ECDIS, and navigation equipment despite their prior 80-hour simulation experience. After the familiarization process was completed, the experiment was conducted by applying three collision scenarios, as shown in Table 1 and Figure 3. The participants were asked to write their action plan to avoid a collision for each scenario. Because planning is a crucial factor for measuring their KBMs [28], the plan had to be prepared in detail. The results of the written action plan were then used to evaluate the KBMs.
To identify the navigator errors more clearly in the research design stage, only the environments related to the scenarios were adjusted and designed. The first scenario was the crossing of multiple vessels in the ship’s route. The second scenario was the sudden approach of multiple fishing vessels under limited visibility, whereas the last scenario was the situation where a not-under-command ship made an unexpected turn that was highly likely to lead to a collision. After the experiment was completed, the action plans to avoid a collision, which was submitted by participants, were evaluated by a group of experts. The experts comprised eight people, including captains, shipping company representatives, coast guards, navigators, professors, and researchers. The action plans written by the participants were assessed in various ways, e.g., whether or not they could be applied to the actual voyage exposed to other risks. Figure 4 shows the participants’ handling of the ship in the simulator.

3.4. Navigator Error Measurement Checklist

As shown in Table 2, the checklists comprised three steps; they were developed using three major distinctions listed in Table 3, Table 4 and Table 5. Human error classification, such as SBS, RBM, and KBM, was repeatedly evaluated for each scenario. SBS, RBM, and KBM were evaluated in steps 1, 2, and 3, respectively. Finally, the result for collision was verified. Evaluation in steps was based on the decision-making model presented by Embrey [29]. The checklist was constructed as per the stage of the study. Responses were evaluated as Yes/No, with Yes indicating an appropriate action (binary code one) and No for a non-appropriate action (code zero). For example, if the number of responses with Yes (code 1) was five for SBS in the checklist, it was rated as five points. If the number of responses with Yes was four for RBM, it was rated as four points. The number of checklists for the SBS and the RBM was five per scenario, whereas the number for KBM was one because knowledge is a behavior that includes planning for unfamiliar situations [17]. This indicates that one KBM can be measured with one scenario.
Furthermore, the participants were asked to describe their plans to avoid a collision using the evaluation sheet as the plan had to be evaluated after the experiment was completed. Therefore, the experiment was tested for three scenarios for one participant and 150 checklists were measured for all 50 participants. The progress of all the participants was recorded via video-based equipment. The characteristics of these checklists were then summarized as follows. The checklists were separately performed for the three scenarios. In step 1, the SBS with five checklists was evaluated. In step 2, the RBM with five checklists was evaluated. In step three, the KBM with one checklist was assessed. After the scenario was implemented, the cases of collision and non-collision were recorded. Because the experiment was to measure the behavior before the collision, the ship in the simulation stopped if a participant led to a collision in the experiment. In such a case, the instructor intervened in the simulation to prevent any subsequent errors and then resumed the experiment.

4. Results

Results were derived via the checklist for 150 cases, wherein 50 participants were evaluated based on three collision scenarios using a ship-handling simulator. The results were then analyzed by calculating frequencies and percentages. Moreover, Pearson’s correlation analysis was used to analyze the correlation between navigator errors and ship collisions.

4.1. Frequency Calculation for Navigator Errors

The results of the navigators’ error checklist were analyzed after being divided into two types, namely collision and non-collision events. Collisions occurred for 64 cases out of 150 cases (43%), and their frequency calculation results are listed in Table 6. The frequency calculation results for 86 cases (57%) without any collision are listed in Table 7. First, the frequency calculation results for collisions listed in Table 6 indicated the frequency of Yes and No for appropriateness of the action in three scenarios and expressed them with percentages. A total of 64 frequencies were then measured, i.e., 30, 6, and 28 cases in scenario one, two, and three, respectively. Yes was twice as much as No in step one. In step two, Yes was measured to be more than 15% of No. However, in step three, No was the majority of the evaluation (96.87%). The result of step three is noticeable because collisions occurred at a rate of 96.87% when navigators made KBMs.
Second, the frequency of Yes and No for the three scenarios wherein no collision occurred is listed in Table 7. A total of 86 frequencies were measured, i.e., 19, 44, and 23 cases in scenario one, two, and three, respectively. In step one, Yes was thrice as much as No; in step two, Yes was twice as high as No; and in step three, Yes was 81.40%. For non-collision cases, the frequency of Yes is much higher than that of No in all the three steps. In particular, 81.40% of cases did not result in collisions in the absence of KBM in step three. These results suggest that there are significant differences in human errors for both collision and non-collision.
The results are summarized as follows. First, among the total of 62 collision cases, 96.87% of them were related to KBM. However, the other two types of errors were also responsible for collisions; 33.12% and 43.44% of collisions occurred when SBSs and RBMs were made.

4.2. Correlation Analysis Between Navigator Errors and Ship Collision

To test the relation between navigator errors and ship collision, Pearson’s correlation analysis was conducted. The data used for correlation analysis were divided into 1 and 0 by experimenters. Binary code, which is the experimental result of the checklists from 50 participants, was used. The correlations between the four variables, namely SBS, RBM, KBM, and collision, were analyzed using the percentage of Yes for the questions in the checklists. The results are listed in Table 8.
The correlation coefficient between SBS and RBM was 0.620, showing a statistically significant positive correlation. A significant and positive correlation was then identified between SBS and KBM with the correlation coefficient of 0.430, whereas the correlation coefficient between SBS and collision was 0.308, also showing a significant and positive correlation. The correlation between SBS and KBM (r = 0.568) was significant, and the correlation between RBM and collision was significant and positive with a correlation coefficient of 0.545. Among the three types of human errors, the correlation coefficient between KBM and collision was 0.686 and KBM was most highly correlated to collision.

5. Discussion

KBM was most closely related to ship collisions. The correlation analysis to understand the behavior of navigators causing the ship collision showed strong relationships between KBM and collision. Although training to improve relevant skills, rules, and knowledge is important for the reduction of ship collisions, providing training for sufficient knowledge of navigation is more necessary compared to training for skills and rules. Because KBM has been shown to be mostly related to ship collisions, a method to reduce errors is required [30]. Various scenarios can be implemented via ship maneuvering simulators to reduce the navigator’s knowledge-based errors through education and training, thereby increasing the safety of ships. For participants who did not cause a collision, the error in the operation of the radar interface was common, whereas the error of the wheel operation rarely occurred. The results of these studies were similar to previous studies [8], which suggest that student navigators made errors although they had been well familiarized with handling navigational equipment interfaces. Accidents cannot be prevented by identifying errors, but steps can be taken to narrow the knowledge gap. Therefore, further research is required on the training program that enhances novice navigators’ ability to handle navigation equipment interfaces. Moreover, ship collision is attributed to the misapplication of rules [9]. However, as per the results of this study, KBM and SBS are more strongly related to ship collision accidents than RBM. Therefore, detailed education and training to reduce KBM and SBS via simulation is necessary for the safety of vessels. However, here again, KBM was measured only once, so the importance can be overemphasized due to the lack of symmetry of the parameters compared to RBM and SBS. This part needs further study. We propose policy implications based on the findings of this study. Measuring the navigator errors on a real ship is tedious and expensive [16]. Therefore, alternatives to control human errors using such a virtual reality experiment are desirable. For example, it can provide effective ways of training for student navigators, who are not ready for independently controlling a real ship, by showing their human errors.
A few limitations of the present study deserve a brief discussion. Participants were student navigators; therefore, they were more likely to make errors compared to navigators who work independently. Thus, a large number of collisions in the experiment may be related to the use of student navigators. Furthermore, errors may occur owing to the difference between the virtual environment and the actual one. Additionally, since the captain forming the bridge team was excluded in this experiment, there is a difference from the probability that an accident may actually occur. Next, there are technical limitations in implementing all real environments through the ship-handling simulator; it is thus desirable to consider developing and using other virtual reality equipment [19,31]. Moreover, the precision and diversity of experimental scenarios are important, but the present scenarios were more directly related to KBMs compared to SBSs and RBMs.

6. Conclusions

The results of the experiment demonstrated that among all the collision cases, a navigator’s insufficient knowledge is responsible for 96.87% of collisions. Moreover, a strong correlation among collision, RBM, and KBM exists. A collision can be minimized when a navigator has sufficient knowledge of an appropriate course of action, the skills to navigate, and an understanding and application of navigation rules. The results of this experiment demonstrated that navigator errors can be classified into the SBSs, the RBMs, and the KBMs, and that the RBMs and the KBMs are strongly related to ship collision. Based on the experimental results, we proposed a method of education and training of navigators using simulation to enhance the safety of ship navigation. Therefore, navigators should be trained repeatedly in scenarios of unfamiliar situations using simulators to improve their ability to respond to risk. In the future, researchers must continue to examine how to evaluate navigator errors to reduce them. In addition, to minimize navigator errors, human-centered automation methods should be studied, and the relationship between the sensor and the ship-controlling machine should be included.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.-B.Y. and C.-k.L.; Investigation, J.-B.Y.; methodology, D.-J.P. and H.-S.Y.; validation, D.-J.P.; data curation, H.-S.Y. and D.-J.P.; Formal analysis, D.-J.P. and H.-S.Y.; writing—original draft preparation, D.-J.P.; writing—review and editing, J.-B.Y. and C.-k.L.; supervision and funding acquisition, J.-B.Y. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The contents of this paper are the results of the research project funded by the Ministry of Oceans and Fisheries of Korea (fundamental research on maritime accident prevention—phase 2).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Montewka, J.; Krata, P.; Goerlandt, F.; Mazaheri, A.; Kujala, P. Marine traffic risk modeling—An innovative approach and a case study. Proc. Inst. Mech. Eng. Part O J. Risk Reliab. 2011, 225, 307–322. [Google Scholar]
  2. Rothblum, A.M. Human error and marine safety. In National Safety Council Congress and Expo; U.S. Coast Guard Research & Development Center: Orlando, FL, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  3. Hanzu-Pazara, R.; Barsan, E.; Arsenie, P.; Chiotoroiu, L.; Raicu, G. Reducing of maritime accidents caused by human factors using simulators in training process. J. Marit. Res. 2008, 5, 3–18. [Google Scholar]
  4. Cacciabue, P.C. Human error risk management for engineering systems: A methodology for design, safety assessment, accident investigation and training. Reliab. Eng. Syst. Saf. 2004, 83, 229–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Reason, J. Human error: Models and management. BMJ 2000, 320, 768–770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Lin, C.J.; Shiang, W.J.; Chuang, C.Y.; Liou, J.L. Applying the skill-rule-knowledge framework to understanding operators’ behaviors and workload in advanced main control rooms. Nucl. Eng. Des. 2014, 270, 176–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Lin, B. Behavior of ship officers in maneuvering to prevent a collision. J. Mar. Sci. Technol. 2006, 14, 225–230. [Google Scholar]
  8. KMST. Available online: http://data.kmst.go.kr/kmst/verdict/writtenverdict/selectwrittenverdict.do (accessed on 5 May 2019).
  9. Celik, M.; Cebi, S. Analytical HFACS for investigating human errors in shipping accidents. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2009, 41, 66–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Cordon, J.R.; Mestre, J.M.; Walliser, J. Human factors in seafaring: The role of situation awareness. Saf. Sci. 2017, 93, 256–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Chauvin, C.; Clostermann, J.P.; Hoc, J.M. Impact of training programs on decision-making and situation awareness of trainee watch officers. Saf. Sci. 2009, 47, 1222–1231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Sandhåland, H.; Oltedal, H.; Eid, J. Situation awareness in bridge operations—A study of collisions between attendant vessels and offshore facilities in the North Sea. Saf. Sci. 2015, 79, 277–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Procee, S.; Borst, C.; Van Paassen, M.M.; Mulder, M.; Bertram, V. Using Augmented Reality to Improve Collision Avoidance and Resolution. In Proceedings of the COMPIT’18: 17th International Conference on Computer and IT Applications in the Maritime Industries, Pavone, Italy, 14–16 May 2018. [Google Scholar]
  14. Illankoon, P.; Tretten, P.; Kumar, U. Modelling human cognition of abnormal machine behaviour. Hum. Intell. Syst. Integr. 2019, 1, 3–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Yim, J.B.; Park, D.J.; Youn, I.H. Development of Navigator Behavior Models for the Evaluation of Collision Avoidance Behavior in the Collision-Prone Navigation Environment. App. Sci. 2019, 9, 3114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Yim, J.B. A study on the analysis and identification of seafarers’ skill-rule-knowledge inherent in maritime accidents. J. Korean Soc. Mar. Environ. Saf. 2017, 23, 224–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Rasmussen, J. Skills, rules, and knowledge: Signals, signs, and symbols, and other distinctions in human performance models. IEEE Trans. Syst. Man. Cybern. 1983, 3, 257–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Reason, J. Human Error; Cambridge University Press: London, UK, 1990. [Google Scholar]
  19. Kozak, J.J.; Hancock, P.A.; Arthur, E.J.; Chrysler, S.T. Transfer of training from virtual reality. Ergonomics 1993, 36, 777–784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Stone, R. Virtual reality for interactive training: An industrial practitioner’s viewpoint. Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud. 2001, 55, 699–711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Gould, K.S.; Røed, B.K.; Saus, E.R.; Koefoed, V.F.; Bridger, R.S.; Moen, B.E. Effects of navigation method on workload and performance in simulated high-speed ship navigation. Appl. Ergon. 2009, 40, 103–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Norman, D.A. The Design of Everyday Things: Revised and Expanded Edition; Constellation: London, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  23. Lin, C.J.; Yenn, T.C.; Yang, C.W. Optimizing human–system interface automation design based on a skill-rule-knowledge framework. Nucl. Eng. Des. 2010, 240, 1897–1905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Hogenboom, I.; Kristensen, A.S. Comparison of Human Reliability Analysis Method. Master’s Thesis, Aalborg University Esbjerg, Esbjerg, Denmark, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  25. IMO. Convention on the International Regulations for Preventing Collisions at Sea, [with Amendments Adopted from December 2009]; International Maritime Organization: London, UK, 1972. [Google Scholar]
  26. Drivalou, S.; Marmaras, N. Supporting skill-, rule-, and knowledge-based behaviour through an ecological interface: An industry-scale application. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 2009, 39, 947–965. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Reason, J.; Manstead, A.; Stradling, S.; Baxter, J.; Campbell, K. Errors and violations on the roads: A real distinction. Ergonomics 1990, 33, 1315–1332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Wentink, M.; Stassen, L.P.S.; Alwayn, I.; Hosman, R.J.A.W.; Stassen, H.G. Rasmussen’s model of human behavior in laparoscopy training. Surg. Endosc. Other Interv. Tech. 2003, 17, 1241–1246. [Google Scholar]
  29. Embrey, D. Understanding human behaviour and error. Hum. Reliab. Assoc. 2005, 1, 1–10. [Google Scholar]
  30. KMST. Statistical Year Book for Maritime Accidents; Korean Maritime Safety Tribunals, Korea: Sejong, Korea, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  31. Youn, I.H.; Park, D.J.; Yim, J.B. Analysis of lookout activity in a simulated environment to investigate maritime accidents caused by human error. App. Sci. 2019, 9, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Classification of human errors (Reason, 1990).
Figure 1. Classification of human errors (Reason, 1990).
Symmetry 12 00529 g001
Figure 2. Configuration of Kongsberg’s typical ship-handling simulator.
Figure 2. Configuration of Kongsberg’s typical ship-handling simulator.
Symmetry 12 00529 g002
Figure 3. A ship-handling simulator is used to perform three scenarios for measuring navigator errors. It is an instruction program from Kongsberg and the ship is controlled by participants.
Figure 3. A ship-handling simulator is used to perform three scenarios for measuring navigator errors. It is an instruction program from Kongsberg and the ship is controlled by participants.
Symmetry 12 00529 g003
Figure 4. Participant handling the ship in the simulator from the video being recorded.
Figure 4. Participant handling the ship in the simulator from the video being recorded.
Symmetry 12 00529 g004
Table 1. Measurement of the navigator errors using a ship-handling simulator in the collision scenario.
Table 1. Measurement of the navigator errors using a ship-handling simulator in the collision scenario.
No.Selected ScenariosDuration
1The situation where multiple ships cross the ship’s route10 min
2The situation of meeting with many fishing boats under restricted visibility8 min
3In a head-on situation, sudden turn of a not-under-command vessel8 min
Table 2. Number of measurement items and questions in the checklist.
Table 2. Number of measurement items and questions in the checklist.
ObservationNumber of Questions in Each Checklist
StepFactorChecklist 1Checklist 2Checklist 3Sum
1SBS55515
2RBM55515
3KBM1113
Sum11111133
ResultCollision1113
Table 3. Checklist to observe the participants’ continuous behaviors (questions on the appropriateness of the action plan for the collision situation of multiple ships).
Table 3. Checklist to observe the participants’ continuous behaviors (questions on the appropriateness of the action plan for the collision situation of multiple ships).
StepNumberHuman Error ClassificationQuestions in ChecklistsResponse =
Yes/No (1/0)
Step 11SBSDid the participant check the course and speed of other ships on the radar?
2SBSWas the rudder operation appropriate?
3SBSWas the telegraph operation appropriate?
4SBSWas the visual lookout appropriate?
5SBSWas the radar lookout appropriate?
Step 21RBMDid the participant take the avoidance action following the procedure applied to the crossing situation?
2RBMDid the participant take the collision-avoidance action according to international regulations for preventing a collision at sea?
3RBMDid the participant follow the route?
4RBMDid the participant use navigation information such as the bow crossing range?
5RBMWere the acceptable ranges such as the closest point approach appropriate?
Step 31KBMWas the action plan set in a situation where multiple ships cross the ship’s route appropriate?
Table 4. Checklist 2 for observing continuous behaviors (multiple fishing vessels and encounters with limited visibility).
Table 4. Checklist 2 for observing continuous behaviors (multiple fishing vessels and encounters with limited visibility).
StepNumberHuman Error ClassificationQuestions in ChecklistsResponse =
Yes/No (1/0)
Steps 1 and 2 are similar to those listed in Table 3
Step 31KBMWas the plan set in the situation of meeting with many fishing boats under restricted visibility appropriate?
Table 5. Checklist 3 for observing the participants’ continuous behavior (inquiry about the plan of not-under-command in a head-on situation).
Table 5. Checklist 3 for observing the participants’ continuous behavior (inquiry about the plan of not-under-command in a head-on situation).
StepNumberHuman Error ClassificationQuestions in ChecklistsResponse =
Yes/No (1/0)
Steps 1 and 2 are similar to those listed in Table 3
Step 31KBMWas the plan set in a head-on situation, the sudden turn of the not-under-command vessel appropriate?
Table 6. Frequency calculation when a collision occurs (n = 64).
Table 6. Frequency calculation when a collision occurs (n = 64).
Step
Scenario 1 (n = 30)Scenario 2 (n = 6)Scenario 3 (n = 28)Sum (n = 64)
Yes (1)No (0)Yes (1)No (0)Yes (1)No (0)Yes (1)No (0)
1Fr.95552199842214106
%63.3336.6770.0030.0070.0030.0066.8833.12
2Fr.816914168654181139
%54.0046.0046.6753.3361.4338.5756.5643.44
3Fr.03006226262
%0.00100.000.00100.007.1492.863.1396.87
SumFr.1761543531186122397307
%53.3346.6753.0346.9760.3939.6156.3943.61
Fr.: Frequency; %: percentile, n: number of cases.
Table 7. Frequency calculation when a collision does not occur (n = 86).
Table 7. Frequency calculation when a collision does not occur (n = 86).
Step Scenario 1 (n = 19)Scenario 2 (n = 44)Scenario 3 (n = 23)Sum (n = 86)
Yes (1)No (0)Yes (1)No (0)Yes (1)No (0)Yes (1)No (0)
1Fr.6926149719025308122
%72.6327.3767.7332.2778.2621.7471.6328.37
2Fr.6728130908827285145
%70.5329.4759.0940.9176.5223.4866.2833.72
3Fr.16333112127016
%84.2115.7975.0025.0091.308.7081.4018.60
SumFr.1525731217219954663283
%72.7327.2764.4635.5478.6621.3470.0829.92
Fr.: Frequency; %: percentile, n: number of cases.
Table 8. Pearson’s correlation analysis among SBSs, RBMs, and KBMs and ship collision variables.
Table 8. Pearson’s correlation analysis among SBSs, RBMs, and KBMs and ship collision variables.
SBSRBMKBMCollision
SBS1.000
RBM0.620**1.000
KBM0.430**0.568**1.000
Collision0.308**0.545**0.686**1.000
**p < 0.01

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Park, D.-J.; Yim, J.-B.; Yang, H.-S.; Lee, C.-k. Navigators’ Errors in a Ship Collision via Simulation Experiment in South Korea. Symmetry 2020, 12, 529. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym12040529

AMA Style

Park D-J, Yim J-B, Yang H-S, Lee C-k. Navigators’ Errors in a Ship Collision via Simulation Experiment in South Korea. Symmetry. 2020; 12(4):529. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym12040529

Chicago/Turabian Style

Park, Deuk-Jin, Jeong-Bin Yim, Hyeong-Sun Yang, and Chun-ki Lee. 2020. "Navigators’ Errors in a Ship Collision via Simulation Experiment in South Korea" Symmetry 12, no. 4: 529. https://doi.org/10.3390/sym12040529

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop