To Taste or Not to Taste: A Narrative Review of the Effectiveness of Taste and Non-Taste Exposures on the Dietary Intake of Head Start Children
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Methods
3. Results
3.1. Taste Exposures
3.2. Non-Taste Exposures
3.3. Mixed Method Exposures
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
References
- U.S. Department of Agriculture and U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. Dietary Guidelines for Americans, 2020–2025, 9th ed.; 2020. Available online: https://www.dietaryguidelines.gov/ (accessed on 29 March 2025).
- Wambogo, E.A.; Ansai, N.; Ahluwalia, N.; Ogden, C.L. Fruit and Vegetable Consumption Among Children and Adolescents in the United States, 2015–2018 Key Findings Data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. 2015. Available online: https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/products/index.htm (accessed on 29 March 2025).
- Samuel, T.M.; Musa-Veloso, K.; Ho, M.; Venditti, C.; Shahkhalili-Dulloo, Y.A. Narrative Review of Childhood Picky Eating and Its Relationship to Food Intakes, Nutritional Status, and Growth. Nutrients 2018, 10, 1992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tenenbaum, A.; Lakhan, R.; Thianthai, C.; Chao, H.C. Association of Picky Eating with Growth, Nutritional Status, Development, Physical Activity, and Health in Preschool Children. Front. Pediatr. 2018, 6, 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Office of Head Start. Available online: https://acf.gov/ohs/about/head-start (accessed on 29 March 2025).
- Code of Federal Regulations. Available online: https://www.ecfr.gov/current/title-45/subtitle-B/chapter-XIII/subchapter-B/part-1302 (accessed on 29 March 2025).
- Rubio, B.; Rigal, N. Parental concerns and attributions of food pickiness and its consequences for the parent–child relationship: A qualitative analysis. J. Child Health Care 2017, 21, 404–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caputi, M.; Dulay, K.M.; Bulgarelli, D.; Houston-Price, C.; Cerrato, G.; Fanelli, M.; Masento, N.; Molina, P. See & Eat! Using E-books to Promote Vegetable Eating Among Preschoolers: Findings From an Italian Sample. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 712416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, C.L.; Perrin, E.M.; Peterson, K.E.; Brophy Herb, H.; Horodynski, M.; Contreras, D.; Miller, A.; Appulgliese, D.; Ball, S.; Lumeng, J. Association of Picky Eating With Weight Status and Dietary Quality Among Low-Income Preschoolers. Acad. Pediatr. 2018, 18, 334–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zajonc, R.B. Attitudinal Effects of Mere Exposure. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1968, 9 Pt 2, 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hetherington, M.M.; Schwartz, C.; Madrelle, J.; Croden, F.; Nekitsing, C.; Vereijken, C.; Weenan, H. A step-by-step introduction to vegetables at the beginning of complementary feeding. The effects of early and repeated exposure. Appetite 2015, 84, 280–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Karagiannaki, K.; Ritz, C.; Jensen, L.G.H.; TØrsleff, E.H.; MØller, P.; Hausner, H.; Olsen, A. Optimising repeated exposure: Determining optimal exposure frequency for introducing a novel vegetable among children. Foods 2021, 10, 913. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Robinson, E.; Henderson, J.; Keenan, G.S.; Kersbergen, I. When a portion becomes a norm: Exposure to a smaller vs. larger portion of food affects later food intake. Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 75, 113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dixon, J.; Hegde, A.V.; Goodell, L.S.; Arnold, N.; Swindle, T.; Dev, D.; Méndez, L.; McMillan, V.; Lee, T.; Stage, V.; et al. Integration of Food-based Learning with Science in the Preschool Classroom: Implementation Gaps and Opportunities. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2023, 55, 266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roberts, A.P.; Cross, L.; Hale, A.; Houston-Price, C. VeggieSense: A non-taste multisensory exposure technique for increasing vegetable acceptance in young children. Appetite 2022, 168, 105784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Anundson, K.; Sisson, S.B.; Anderson, M.; Horm, D.; Soto, J.; Hoffman, L. Staff Food-Related Behaviors and Children’s Tastes of Food Groups during Lunch at Child Care in Oklahoma. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet. 2018, 118, 1399–1407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dazeley, P.; Houston-Price, C. Exposure to foods’ non-taste sensory properties. A nursery intervention to increase children’s willingness to try fruit and vegetables. Appetite 2015, 84, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Izumi, B.T.; Eckhardt, C.L.; Hallman, J.A.; Herro, K.; Barberis, D.A. Harvest for Healthy Kids Pilot Study: Associations between Exposure to a Farm-to-Preschool Intervention and Willingness to Try and Liking of Target Fruits and Vegetables among Low-Income Children in Head Start. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet. 2015, 115, 2003–2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, S.L.; Ryan, S.M.; Kroehl, M.; Moding, K.J.; Boles, R.E.; Bellows, L.L. A longitudinal intervention to improve young children’s liking and consumption of new foods: Findings from the Colorado LEAP study. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2019, 16, 49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Masento, N.A.; Dulay, K.M.; Roberts, A.P.; Harvey, K.; Messer, D.; Houston-Price, C. See and Eat! The impact of repeated exposure to vegetable ebooks on young children’s vegetable acceptance. Appetite 2023, 182, 106447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Monsur, M.; Mansur, M.; Trina, N.A.; Cosco, N. Hands-On Gardening in Childcare Centers to Advance Preschool-Age Children’s Fruit and Vegetable Liking in Semi-Arid Climate Zone. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2024, 21, 1485. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nekitsing, C.; Blundell-Birtill, P.; Cockroft, J.E.; Hetherington, M.M. Taste Exposure Increases Intake and Nutrition Education Increases Willingness to Try an Unfamiliar Vegetable in Preschool Children: A Cluster Randomized Trial. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet. 2019, 119, 2004–2013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmitt, S.A.; Snyder, F.; Korucu, I.; Bryant, L.M.; Finders, J.K. Pilot Intervention Enhances Preschoolers’ Self-Regulation and Food Liking. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2020, 52, 1035–1042. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, E.; Sutarso, T.; Kaye, G.L. Access With Education Improves Fruit and Vegetable Intake in Preschool Children. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2020, 52, 145–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Swindle, T.; McBride, N.M.; Staley, A.; Phillips, C.; Rutledge, J.; Martin, J.; Whiteside-Mansell, L. Pester Power: Examining Children’s Influence as an Active Intervention Ingredient. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2020, 52, 801–807. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Whiteside-Mansell, L.; Swindle, T.M. Evaluation of Together We Inspire Smart Eating: Pre-school fruit and vegetable consumption. Health Educ. Res. 2019, 34, 62–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Criteria | Defined |
---|---|
Inclusion | Preschool-aged children, intervention with food or a likeness, willingness to try, amounts of food consumed, likability of food, written in English |
Exclusion | Marketing, disease, foodborne illness, children in elementary school or above, infants, non-English manuscripts |
Filters | Children between the age of 2–12, articles published in the last 10 years |
Participant Characteristics | Children aged 12 months to 5 years, Head Start participants preferred and supplemented with external populations when Head Start research was limited |
Reference | Study Design, Exposure Type | Population | Intervention or Comparison | Methods | Results |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
[16] Anundson et al., 2018 | Cross-sectional assessing staff impact on taste exposure | Three- to five-year-old children enrolled in 25 Oklahoma childcare centers (n = 201), including two Head Start and 11 tribally affiliated centers | Classroom observations were conducted to assess associations between staff’s food behaviors and children’s tasting. Behaviors were categorized as internal satiety cues, modeling, external satiety cues, and general. The behaviors were assessed with children’s tasting of food to determine the influence of staff behavior on tasting. | Researchers collected data via observation. Staff behaviors were measured with the Environmental and Policy Assessment Observation Instrument. The Dietary Observation for Child Care method was chosen to assess child tasting through plate waste. | Tasting with staff checking in with children before finishing a meal: |
| |||||
Tasting with staff sitting at the table without eating the same food: | |||||
| |||||
Tasting with staff sitting with children and eating the same food: | |||||
| |||||
Tasting with staff using food to reward: | |||||
| |||||
Tasting with staff using food to control: | |||||
| |||||
Tasting with staff encouraging to try a bite: | |||||
| |||||
Tasting with staff using healthy food talk: | |||||
| |||||
[8] Caputi et al., 2021 | Within subjects, pretest-posttest quantitative design assessing non-taste exposure | Italian parents with children between 18 and 48 months (n = 61 children) with access to a tablet or willing to use a tablet from the researchers | Families received a link for an app with customizable e-books about vegetables from farm to fork to read with their child daily. Parents were encouraged, but not required to offer the vegetables in the book to their child during the two-week timeframe. | An emailed pretest and posttest survey gathered demographic information, vegetable choice, parent rating of willingness to try, portion size, food frequency, liking, and book engagement (posttest only). Parents rated statements from the Children’s Eating Behavior Questionnaire: Food Fussiness Subscale and subcomponents of the Family Mealtime Goals Questionnaire. | Main effects and effect size: |
| |||||
Main effect of vegetable: | |||||
| |||||
Interaction of vegetable and time: | |||||
| |||||
[17] Dazeley and Houston Price, 2015 | Quasi-experimental, between-subjects design assessing non-taste exposure | Children between the ages of 12 and 36 months attending nurseries in the United Kingdom (n = 92) | Children were randomly assigned to the control (n = 37) or experimental group (n = 55). The experimental group was assigned to one of two sets of four foods to explore using sight, sound, smell, or touch during a four-week exposure phase while the control group had regular services. | Data were collected in a test phase by trained nursery staff to increase children’s comfort. Children were individually given food using a forced choice procedure with food from sets A and B, in a series of four plates. The order of plates was counterbalanced using a Latin square and placement of food was counterbalanced within participants. The order participants touched and tasted (put food to mouth) was recorded and verified with video. |
|
[14] Dixon et al., 2023 | Descriptive qualitative study with a phenomenological approach of teachers’ perspective of exposures | Head Start teachers (n = 35, 94% female, average age 40.8) from various geographic areas of North Carolina | Semi-structured, 45–60 min phone interviews were conducted and recorded with participants to increase understanding of teachers’ perspectives on food-based learning for nutrition curriculum and training development. | Demographic questionnaires were used following the US Office of Management and Budget protocols. Interviewers completed training and bracketing prior and interviews were transcribed. Researchers summarized responses at the end of the call and in a post-interview email for confirmation. Coding was completed by two independent researchers with Moustakas’ structured method. | 11 primary themes: (1) Inside mealtime environment, (2) outside mealtime environment, (3) teacher’s use of unhealthy foods, (4) uncertainty on how to integrate food-based learning into science, (5) feelings of helplessness, (6) food waste, (7) policy, (8) COVID-19, (9) motivators for food-based learning, (10) perceptions of successful food-based learning, (11) make the connection between FBL and science to promote kindergarten readiness |
[18] Izumi et al., 2015 | Quasi-experimental pretest-posttest design (non-randomization of the control group) assessing taste and mixed exposures | Head Start children in five centers operated by the Mount Hood Community College Head Start program in Portland, Oregon with data analysis conducted on those with pre- and post-intervention data (n = 226) | The eight-month intervention with nine target fruits and vegetables was conducted to assess willingness to try and food liking. Interventions were taste only, adding target foods to menus twice weekly (low-intervention), or taste combined with nutrition education using the Harvest for Healthy Kids curriculum (high-intervention) with mealtime conversation starters, books, cooking, crafts, tasting, and newsletters. | Demographic information was collected from Head Start enrollment data. Trained researchers assessed each child’s willingness to try and food liking. Willingness to try was recorded dichotomously as 1, tried the sample, or 0, refused. Liking was assessed based on the method developed by Birch using a nonverbal hedonic scale with a smiling face indicating “I like it”, a neutral face depicting “It’s okay”, and a frowning face showing “I don’t like it yet”. | Low intervention (taste exposure) willingness to try, compared to control: |
| |||||
High intervention (mixed exposure) willingness to try, compared to control: | |||||
| |||||
Low intervention (taste exposure) liking, compared to control: | |||||
| |||||
High intervention (mixed exposure) liking, compared to control: | |||||
| |||||
No statistical differences between intervention willingness to try or liking. | |||||
[19] Johnson et al., 2019 | Longitudinal, quasi-experimental design (Colorado Longitudinal Eating and Physical Activity (LEAP) study) for mixed exposures | Children in Colorado preschool centers, including Head Start, in two geographically and economically diverse areas who were advancing to Kindergarten next year (n = 250), data from those with special dietary needs and developmental disabilities were excluded from analysis | A 12-week program, Food Friends—Fun with New Foods®, was implemented in intervention classrooms by trained teachers with classroom-based exposures of nine foods with additional materials sent home to caregivers. Targeted foods included garbanzo beans, grapefruit, Gouda cheese, couscous, spinach, salmon, beets, pineapple, and jicama. Jicama was one target food with tasting opportunities once a week for the first eight weeks, the second was edamame, assessed without taste exposures. The control groups did not implement the program and had regularly scheduled lessons. In the two years of follow-up, monthly booster lessons were implemented during kindergarten and first grade, with support materials sent home. | A demographic questionnaire was sent home and completed by parents. Height and weight were measured by trained researchers using a digital scale and portable stadiometer. To evaluate program fidelity, a teacher survey was completed on activity preference and child engagement. Liking was measured with the preference assessment developed by Sullivan and Birch, using a nonverbal hedonic scale to rate as “Yummy”, “Just Okay”, and “Yucky”. Consumption was measured based on pre-and post-meal weight of food. | Intervention jicama consumption: |
| |||||
Comparison jicama consumption: | |||||
| |||||
Intervention edamame consumption: | |||||
| |||||
Comparison edamame consumption: | |||||
| |||||
Significant difference in the jicama consumption over time between intervention and control: p < 0.005 | |||||
Intervention group consumption of those rating jicama as “yummy” at baseline: | |||||
| |||||
Intervention Liking: | |||||
| |||||
Control Liking: | |||||
| |||||
Significant change over time for both groups, p = 0.0002; Liking differences not statistically significant between groups, p = 0.1980 | |||||
[20] Masento et al., 2023 | Within subjects, pretest-posttest quantitative design for non-taste exposure | United Kingdom families with children between the ages of 18 and 48 months and access to a tablet device (n = 36) | Parents received links to the See & Eat books on the Our Story 2 application with the customizable story focusing on the vegetable journey from farm to fork. Parents were asked to select two vegetables that their child does not typically eat for assessment. | Demographics were collected via a preintervention questionnaire. This questionnaire also contained questions on parent feeding style and child eating behavior. After the target and control vegetables were selected, parents rated their child’s willingness to try, intake, and liking on Likert scales, some from adapted versions of the Child Food Frequency Questionnaire and Fruit and Vegetable Familiarity and Liking Questionnaire. | Main effect of condition: |
| |||||
Main effect of time: | |||||
| |||||
Interaction effects between the condition and time: | |||||
| |||||
[21] Monsur et al., 2024 | Randomized two-group, pre-post experimental design, non-taste exposure | Three- to five-year-old children from eight Texas childcare centers participated with parent consent (n = 149) | Raised garden beds and a garden activity book were implemented in experimental centers with six vegetables (cucumbers, green beans, bell peppers, tomatoes, yellow squash, and zucchini) and five fruits (blackberries, blueberries, cantaloupe, strawberries, and watermelon) planted. The garden activity guide included planting, maintenance, and harvesting. | In an individual interview at pre- and post-intervention, children’s knowledge and liking of fruits and vegetables were measured. Liking was assessed on a five-point face scale, ranging from super yucky to super yummy, and knowledge was assessed with food identification. The interviewer placed single fruits and vegetables on a table for identification, and the session was recorded. A garden activity chart was completed for each child’s participation in the activities. | Fruit liking: |
| |||||
Vegetable liking: | |||||
| |||||
Fruit and vegetable liking | |||||
| |||||
[22] Nekitsing et al., 2019 | Cluster randomized control trial using a 2 × 2 factorial parallel design assessing taste, non-taste, and mixed exposures | Two- to five-year-old children attending 11 preschools in the United Kingdom | Mooli was selected as the intervention target vegetable. A 2 × 2 factorial parallel design was used for cluster, stratified randomization and the four groups received weekly taste exposure, nutrition education, nutrition education with taste exposure, or no intervention for 10 weeks. The Phunky Foods nutrition education program was used; however, mooli was not part of this program. | Mooli intake was measured in grams (g) at week 1, week 12, and at two follow-up points, weeks 24 and 36. Prepared and weighed snack bags of mooli were given at snack time and researchers measured post-consumption weight. | No Intervention: |
| |||||
Taste Exposure: | |||||
| |||||
Nutrition Education (non-taste): | |||||
| |||||
Nutrition Education with Taste (Mixed): | |||||
| |||||
Odds of being an eater (willing to try): | |||||
| |||||
[15] Roberts et al., 2022 | Experimental, randomized control study, non-taste exposure | Children from nurseries in south-east England between the ages of 36 to 59 months (n = 110) | The study assessed intake and willingness to try after combinations of visual, touch, and smell exposures of six vegetables (broccoli, fennel, leek, parsnip, radish, and swede). Visuals were controlled using a covering of black material, and a mesh cloth with a lid was used to control smell. Matching games with food and colored posters were used. The control received six toys using the same methods. Exposures were assessed as visual only, smell-visual, and smell-visual-tactile. | All children received two pieces of each food. Willingness to try was measured by awarding a point for each food touched to their tongue or lips, with a total of six possible (one for each vegetable). Intake was measured on a two-point scale, with one point awarded for each piece of the vegetable eaten (two pieces of each food), for a total of 12 points possible. | Willingness to try: |
| |||||
Intake: | |||||
| |||||
[7] Rubio and Rigal, 2017 | Qualitative study using focus groups of caregivers. | French parents of children aged 18 to 36 months in childcare centers (n = 38) with self-reported low or moderate income. | A total of six focus groups were completed to understand children’s food pickiness, including onset and affected foods, parent perceptions, and strategies for pickiness. | Focus groups included a moderator leading the discussion and an assistant recording and taking notes. The sessions were 65 to 80 min, ending when no additional information emerged. A discussion guide was developed and tested using a pilot group. A researcher and assistant independently completed a thematic analysis, reaching agreement on themes. | 6 Themes: (1) changes in food behaviors, (2) type of food affected by changes, (3) manifestations of pickiness, (4) attributions of toddler’s pickiness, (5) consequences of food pickiness, (6) parental strategies to overcome pickiness |
[23] Schmitt et al., 2020 | Quasi -experimental, pilot study, mixed exposures | Children in two Head Start centers (n = 39) with the majority male (n = 25) and Caucasian (n = 31) | A five-week intervention with 15 short lessons was implemented to assess self-regulation and liking of fruits and vegetables with pre-and post-intervention tests. Lessons included classroom-based activities, such as mindfulness, memory or physical activity games, and food preparation, many using real fruits and vegetables. Children in the comparison received regular services. | The Head–Toes–Knees–Shoulders Task (HTKS) measured behavioral regulation. The National Institutes of Health (NIH) Toolbox Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention Test and Toolbox Dimensional Change Card Sort Test measured executive function. Children rated food as “Yucky”, “Just Okay”, and “Yummy” to assess food liking. Teacher surveys and researcher observation measured program fidelity. |
|
[24] Smith et al., 2020 | Experimental, randomized controlled design, mixed exposures | Head Start children at a rural Ohio center without asthma or diabetes (n = 209) | In three randomly assigned and demographically similar groups, researchers studied if nutrition education and access impacted consumption after an eight-week intervention. A comparison classroom received regular services, treatment A classroom received high-carotenoid fruits and vegetables weekly, and treatment B classroom received weekly food and nutrition education from the Harvest for Healthy Kids program. | Blood carotenoid levels were assessed before and after the intervention using a Pharmanex BioPhotonic S3 Scanner using Resonance Ramen Spectroscopy. This uses light for measurement and has been validated with serum levels. | Comparison: |
| |||||
Treatment A (high-carotenoid fruits and vegetables weekly, taste exposure): | |||||
| |||||
Treatment B (weekly food and nutrition education, mixed exposure): | |||||
| |||||
Difference in carotenoid scores of treatments = F2, 206 = 12.967, p < 0.001, Cohen D = 71 | |||||
[25] Swindle et al., 2020 | Non-randomized, within-subjects, quasi-experimental design for home-effect of mixed exposures | Parents of enrolled children at seven Head Start centers in Louisiana and Arkansas | This study assessed the Together, We Inspire Smart Eating (WISE) program to determine if classroom interventions impact parental decisions at home through pester power. Windy Wise is the barn owl mascot of the program, encouraging and teaching children about food and nutrition. The curriculum provides a variety of opportunities for sensory explorations of food, including taste and non-taste exposures, and caregiver materials. | Caregivers were interviewed by trained researchers pre- and post-intervention to evaluate consumption and willingness to try WISE foods and nutrient-poor foods. Parents were asked to complete an adapted food frequency questionnaire from the Family Map Inventory tool used in some Head Start programs. Parenting feeding practices were measured using an adapted version of the Fruit and Vegetable Parenting Practices Questionnaire. Child acceptance of new foods, including willingness to try, was measured using aspects of the Toddler-Parent Mealtime Behavior Questionnaire. Questions were developed to measure pester power post-implementation. |
|
[26] Whiteside-Mansell & Swindle, 2019 | Quasi-experimental pretest-posttest design, mixed exposures | Parents of children enrolled in 15 Head Start centers in a rural, southern state (n = 526) | The WISE program was assessed by comparing parent-reported consumption in a pre/post format. Educators were trained in food preparation experiences and given ideas for integrating nutrition into educational activities. Additional information was shared via Facebook. | Family map inventories, a tool used in some Head Start programs, were completed in semi-structured pre- and post-intervention interviews with parents. The inventory included an adapted food frequency questionnaire focusing on the 10 WISE foods (apples, tomatoes, sweet potatoes, strawberries, carrots, bell peppers, spinach, greens, green beans, and blueberries) and other foods consumed in the home. | Weekly consumption of dark green, orange, and yellow vegetables: |
| |||||
Weekly consumption of fruits like apples, oranges, bananas, grapes, peaches: | |||||
| |||||
Weekly consumption of sugary sweets like cakes and candy, or sugary drinks: | |||||
| |||||
Monthly consumption of WISE vegetables: | |||||
| |||||
Monthly consumption of WISE fruits: | |||||
|
Taste | Non-Taste | Mixed | |
Willingness to Try | ↑ [18], ↔ [22] | ↑ [15], ↔ [17], ↑ [8], ↑ [20] | ↑ [18] |
Consumption | ↑ [22], ↔ [24] | ↑ [15], ↔ [22], ↑ [8], ↑ [20] | ↑ [24], ↑ [19], ↑ [26], ↑ [25] |
Like | ↔ [18] | ↑ [20], ↑ [21] | ↑ [18], ↑ [19], ↑ [23] |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Johnson, A.R.; Johnson, N.R. To Taste or Not to Taste: A Narrative Review of the Effectiveness of Taste and Non-Taste Exposures on the Dietary Intake of Head Start Children. Nutrients 2025, 17, 1817. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu17111817
Johnson AR, Johnson NR. To Taste or Not to Taste: A Narrative Review of the Effectiveness of Taste and Non-Taste Exposures on the Dietary Intake of Head Start Children. Nutrients. 2025; 17(11):1817. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu17111817
Chicago/Turabian StyleJohnson, Anna R., and Nathaniel Richard Johnson. 2025. "To Taste or Not to Taste: A Narrative Review of the Effectiveness of Taste and Non-Taste Exposures on the Dietary Intake of Head Start Children" Nutrients 17, no. 11: 1817. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu17111817
APA StyleJohnson, A. R., & Johnson, N. R. (2025). To Taste or Not to Taste: A Narrative Review of the Effectiveness of Taste and Non-Taste Exposures on the Dietary Intake of Head Start Children. Nutrients, 17(11), 1817. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu17111817