Next Article in Journal
High-Throughput CAMP Assay (HiTCA): A Novel Tool for Evaluating the Vitamin D-Dependent Antimicrobial Response
Previous Article in Journal
The Effects of Probiotics/Synbiotics on Glucose and Lipid Metabolism in Women with Gestational Diabetes Mellitus: A Meta-Analysis of Randomized Controlled Trials
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Development of Appetite: Tracking and Age-Related Differences in Appetitive Traits in Childhood

1
Division of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry & Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD 21287, USA
2
Advanced Baby Imaging Lab, Brown University & Hasbro Children’s Hospital, Rhode Island Hospital, 593 Eddy Street, Providence, RI 02903, USA
3
Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation, 500 5th Ave North, Seattle, WA 98109, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Membership of the RESONANCE Consortium is provided in the Acknowledgments.
Nutrients 2023, 15(6), 1377; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15061377
Submission received: 24 January 2023 / Revised: 8 March 2023 / Accepted: 9 March 2023 / Published: 12 March 2023
(This article belongs to the Section Pediatric Nutrition)

Abstract

:
Appetitive traits are associated with body weight. Increased understanding of how appetitive traits evolve from early life could advance research on obesity risk and inform intervention development. We report on tracking and age-related differences in appetitive traits in childhood within the RESONANCE cohort. Parents of RESONANCE children aged 6.02 ± 2.99 years completed the Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire (CEBQ). Pearson correlations of appetitive traits and age were tested for all participants contributing at least one observation, using each participant’s first observation (N = 335). Children’s first and second observations of the CEBQ (n = 127) were used to test tracking (paired correlations) and age-related differences (paired t-tests) within individuals. CEBQ correlations with age suggested that satiety responsiveness, slowness in eating, emotional undereating, and desire to drink decreased with age (r = −0.111 to r = −0.269, all p < 0.05), while emotional overeating increased with age (r = 0.207, p < 0.001). Food fussiness demonstrated a quadratic relationship with age. Paired t-tests further supported an increase in emotional overeating with age (M: 1.55 vs. 1.69, p = 0.005). All CEBQ subscales demonstrated moderate to high tracking (r = 0.533 to r = 0.760, p < 0.001). Our initial findings within the RESONANCE cohort suggest that food avoidant traits are negatively related with age, while emotional overeating increases with age, and that appetitive traits track through childhood.

1. Introduction

Appetitive traits are dispositions toward food that differ between individuals [1] and show associations with body weight in childhood [2]. A key proposition of the behavioral susceptibility theory of child obesity [3] is that even as children age and start to consume different types of food (e.g., milk to table foods) and eat in different contexts (e.g., with peers, at school), appetitive traits persist. That is, if a child shows a more avid appetite and a greater interest in food in infancy, similar food approach tendencies are expected as the child grows up. However, although tracking of body weight through childhood is well-established [4], relatively few studies have directly examined the question of whether child appetite tracks, i.e., whether children maintain their relative levels of appetite in relation to their peers across development. Moreover, few studies, including those examining tracking, have described developmental changes in appetitive traits, i.e., the ways in which appetitive traits change with age across the population. Answering these questions is important because an understanding of normative appetite development is essential to interpret the meaning of observed individual differences. For example, if a child or group of children has different appetitive trait scores to a comparison group or the same individual/s at a different age, are these levels consistent or inconsistent with what one might expect based on extant data? Clarity on normative developmental trajectories of child appetite could also inform interventions to prevent or treat obesity by addressing eating behavior. For example, a deeper understanding of tracking and developmental trends could aid in the identification of critical windows for population intervention. In addition, personalized information for parents about their child’s appetite and expected trajectories could be used to develop tailored advice and support for responsive feeding practices. Starting these interventions early is essential, since a recent study found that eating behaviors in childhood have long-lasting influences on not only diet and weight status, but also eating behaviors in adults [5]. Appetitive traits in adults, in turn, have been found to be associated with body weight [6,7]. For example, one recent study of a UK cohort using the Adult Eating Behavior Questionnaire (AEBQ) demonstrated that food responsiveness, enjoyment of food, and emotional overeating were positively associated with BMI, while satiety responsiveness, emotional undereating, and slowness in eating were negatively associated with BMI [6]. Similar findings were obtained in a study of an Australian sample, which found that emotional overeating was positively associated with BMI, while satiety responsiveness and slowness in eating were negatively associated with BMI [7].
Two early studies from the UK including 322 children at ages 4 and 11 years [8] and 31 children at ages 2 and 5 years, respectively [9], used the Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire (CEBQ) [10] to investigate the stability (i.e., tracking within individuals; same rank order over time) and discontinuity (i.e., age-related differences within individuals) of appetitive traits during childhood. Both studies found evidence for stability across all subscales (with the exception of enjoyment of food in the latter study) but differed in their findings relating to (dis-)continuity. Ashcroft et al. [8] found that all measured scales changed from age 4 to 11 years (i.e., decrease in satiety responsiveness, slowness in eating, food fussiness, and emotional undereating and increase in food responsiveness, enjoyment of food, and emotional overeating; desire to drink was not assessed), while Farrow et al. [9] found a decrease in desire to drink from 2 to 5 years but no changes in food responsiveness, emotional overeating, enjoyment of food, satiety responsiveness, slowness in eating, emotional undereating, or food fussiness. Similar findings have more recently emerged from prospective or cross-lag studies with 800 to 3800 children from Europe, such that tracking has been observed for all scales across the different time lags (e.g., 7 to 10 years [11], 4 to 10 years [12], and 6 to 8 to 10 years [13]). We are aware of fewer studies that both report and statistically compare paired mean scores across different child ages. Costa et al. [11], similar to Ashcroft, found that most traits changed between 7 and 10 years of age (i.e., increase in food responsiveness, enjoyment of food, and emotional overeating; decrease in satiety responsiveness, slowness in eating, and emotional undereating, and also in desire to drink), with no change in food fussiness. The aim of the current study was to build on the above findings by investigating how appetitive characteristics evolve during childhood via the RESONANCE study, a US cohort including children aged 2 to 15 years. Due to the wide age range of the cohort, we were able to explore cross-sectional relationships of all Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire scales with age through early to later development, in addition to conducting analyses of change and tracking within subjects over a relatively narrow timespan.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Sample

The data used in the present study came from RESONANCE, a large ongoing cohort study of socioeconomically diverse mother–child dyads beginning in infancy with a focus on early brain development [14,15]. RESONANCE is part of the NIH-funded ECHO program (http://echochildren.org (accessed on 24 January 2023)). Children (983 currently active) are followed longitudinally, with study visits every 6 (biannually up to age 24 months) or 12 months (annually after age 24 months). Participants were recruited either during pregnancy or when children were between the ages of birth and 5 years old. A variety of methods was used, including flyers; Facebook and social media; radio advertisements; community events; and in-person information sessions at school, daycares, hospitals, and community centers. Infants and children with known risk factors for learning and/or psychiatric disorders were excluded (e.g., birth prior to 32 weeks’ gestation or birthweight < 1500 g, non-singleton or complicated pregnancy, neurological trauma in child, psychiatric history in a parent or sibling) [16]. For the current study, all participants with at least one assessment of appetitive characteristics in early childhood were included. For the longitudinal data analysis, participants with two or more observations were selected. For the present paper, only the first and second observations (i.e., assessments of appetitive traits) for the Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire were evaluated. Written consent was obtained from parents or legal guardians in accordance with ethics approval from the host institution’s Institutional Review Board (IRB no.: 1500991).

2.2. Measurement Tools

2.2.1. Child Eating Behavior

To assess appetitive traits in children consuming solid foods, parents of children aged 2 years and older completed the Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire (CEBQ) [10]. The Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire consists of 35 items measured on a Likert-type scale (1 = “never” to 5 = “always”) and comprises eight subscales assessing food approach behaviors, including food responsiveness (FR, example item 1: “Even if my child is full s/he finds room to eat his/her favorite food”, example item 2: “If given the chance, my child would always have food in his/her mouth”), enjoyment of food (EF, example item 1: “My child looks forward to mealtimes”, example item 2: “My child is interested in food”), emotional overeating (EOE, example item 1: “My child eats more when worried”, example item 2: “My child eats more when s/he has nothing else to do”), and desire to drink (DD, example item 1: “If given the chance, my child would drink continuously throughout the day”, example item 2: “If given the chance, my child would always be having a drink”), and food avoidant behaviors, including satiety responsiveness (SR, example item 1: “My child leaves food on his/her plate at the end of a meal”, example item 2: “My child cannot eat a meal if s/he has had a snack just before”), slowness in eating (SE, example item 1: “My child takes more than 30 min to finish a meal”, example item 2: “My child eats more and more slowly during the course of a meal), food fussiness (FUS, example item 1: “My child is difficult to please with meals”, example item 2: “My child decides that s/he doesn’t like a food, even without tasking it”), and emotional undereating (EUE, example item 1: “My child eats less when angry”, example item 2: “My child eats less when s/he is tired”). Scores were averaged for each subscale, and mean subscale scores were used in the analyses.

2.2.2. Sample Characteristics

Mothers reported demographic characteristics for themselves and their children, including maternal age, education (grouped as (partial) high school, partial college or specialized training, college graduate, or graduate training (masters, PhD)), pre-pregnancy BMI, and subjective social status (MacArthur Scale of Subjective Social Status, range from 1 to 10, with higher scores indicating higher subjective social status) [17], as well as child sex, race, and ethnicity. Children’s weights and heights were measured during the lab visits. Child BMI z-scores were calculated using the WHO Anthro version 3.2.2. [18], WHO Anthro Plus, and macros [19].

2.3. Data Analysis

Analyses were conducted using cross-sectional as well as longitudinal data. For the cross-sectional analysis, all mothers who had at least one observation on the Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire were included, using the first observation available. Cross-sectional Pearson correlations between appetitive trait scores and child age were examined first. For those Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire subscales where no significant Pearson (linear) correlation was seen with age, in a second step models were fit that tested for quadratic relationships. Given the wide age range in the current sample, non-linear relationships between age and child development might have been expected. For the longitudinal analysis, participants’ first and second observations on the Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire were selected. First, paired t-tests were run to determine age-related differences within individuals. Cohen’s d was used to determine effect sizes [20]. Next, Pearson correlation coefficients were used to examine tracking within individuals, i.e., to test whether children kept their relative rankings amongst the group from first to second observations (i.e., rank-order stability across assessments). To adjust for the child’s age at the first observation and also for the time lag between observations 1 and 2, two additional partial Pearson correlations were conducted: one adjusting for child age, the other adjusting for child age and time lag. Analyses were conducted using SPSS 27 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA).

3. Results

In total, 335 parents completed the Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire at least once while their children were between the ages of 2.0 and 15.2 years. Of these, 127 participants provided two observations, with an average time lag between measurements of 13.29 ± 3.80 months. Sample characteristics of the 335 participants are shown in Table 1.

3.1. Cross-Sectional Results

Cross-sectional Pearson correlations between appetitive trait scores and child ages are presented in Table 2. Correlations for Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire subscales indicated decreases in satiety responsiveness (r = −0.160, p = 0.004), slowness in eating (r = −0.266, p < 0.001), and desire to drink (r = −0.227, p < 0.001) with age and increases in emotional overeating (r = 0.203, p < 0.001) with age. Quadratic relationships were examined for food responsiveness, enjoyment of food, and food fussiness, since these showed non-significant linear relationships with child age. The former two were not significant (p = 0.213 and p = 0.660), while food fussiness showed a quadratic relationship (upside-down U-shape) with age, with a peak of fussiness around the age of 6 years (beta = −0.568, p = 0.017).

3.2. Longitudinal Results

Table 3 shows the mean scores of the Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire subscales at observation 1 and observation 2, as well as the results of the paired t-tests for age-related changes within individuals. Paired t-tests for Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire subscales indicated an increase in emotional overeating with age from the first to the second observation (M1st = 1.55 vs. M2nd = 1.69, p = 0.005). No significant mean change in appetitive traits between the first and second observations was seen for food responsiveness, enjoyment of food, desire to drink, satiety responsiveness, slowness in eating, food fussiness, or emotional undereating.
Table 4 outlines the results of paired correlations for tracking within individuals. All eight Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire subscales showed significant positive correlations across observations: food responsiveness (r = 0.644, p < 0.001), enjoyment of food (r = 0.708, p < 0.001), emotional overeating (r = 0.562, p < 0.001), desire to drink (r = 0.533, p < 0.001), satiety responsiveness (r = 0.726, p < 0.001), slowness in eating (r = 0.685, p < 0.001), food fussiness (r = 0.760, p < 0.001), and emotional undereating (r = 0.576, p < 0.001). The same pattern of positive correlations was seen when adjusting for child age at the first Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire observation or adjusting for child age at the first Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire observation and the time lag between the first and second Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire observations.

4. Discussion

The aim of this study was to investigate tracking and age-related differences in appetitive traits as assessed more narrowly in early to middle childhood, extending some of the previous studies conducted in Europe to children in the US. Cross-sectionally, we found that six out of the eight subscales assessed during early to middle childhood, were correlated with child age (i.e., emotional overeating, emotional undereating, desire to drink, satiety responsiveness, and slowness in eating), including one (food fussiness) showing a quadratic relationship and highlighting the importance of examining non-linear relationships. Longitudinally, we saw little evidence for age-related change within individuals, but we found that appetitive traits tracked within individuals. On the whole, our results demonstrated a decrease in food avoidant behaviors with age and an increase in emotional overeating with age, which is consistent with population survey and cohort data suggesting increasing likelihood of obesity development as children grow older [21].
Our cross-sectional observation that food avoidant traits were negatively associated with age was dependent on the appetitive dimension. The strongest age difference was for slowness in eating, which showed a negative correlation with age. However, significant negative correlations were also seen for satiety responsiveness and emotional undereating. A potential parsimonious explanation for these combined findings is that, while very young children are able to regulate their energy intake [22,23], this ability erodes over time due to environmental influences, including controlling parent feeding practices (such as parents’ restriction of children’s food intake) [24]. However, it is also possible, particularly within the infancy period, that eating speed increases along with motor dexterity and that as children get older they are increasingly involved in structured activities, such as childcare and school, that constrain schedules and effectively dictate how much time they spend eating, while exposure to social influences may lessen food avoidant behaviors. The decrease in emotional undereating could potentially have an additional set of determinants. For example, as children age, their emotion regulation may improve [25], making them less vulnerable to acute stress from extreme negative emotions, which may be more likely to decrease appetite than less severe emotions [26]. For food fussiness, a quadratic relationship with age was found, such that levels of food fussiness increased to about age six years before they started to decline. This finding is in line with those previously reported in a life-course analysis of pooled data from Ireland (N = 3246), where food neophobia (i.e., reluctance to eat novel foods) increased with age from 1 to 6 years before decreasing until early adulthood [27].
Our observation that emotional overeating increased with age was apparent in both our cross-sectional correlations with age and our within-subject paired t-tests using longitudinal data. While many appetitive characteristics have previously demonstrated strong genetic influence, emotional overeating has been found to show relatively high levels of environmental influence [28,29]. It is therefore possible that emotional overeating increases along with increases in potential environmental triggers. For example, as they progress through development, children are increasingly exposed to diverse palatable energy-dense foods as well as social situations that could promote emotional overeating. Although Farrow et al. [9] did not find a significant difference in emotional overeating between the ages of 2 and 5 years, our results are in line with several other previous studies that found that emotional overeating increases with age [8,30,31,32].
The continuity in appetitive characteristics that we observed in the majority of our within-subject tests (i.e., no significant mean differences in seven of the eight Child Eating Behavior subscales from the first to the second observation) was consistent with the results of Farrow and colleagues [9]. While Ashcroft et al. [8] and da Costa et al. [11] found discontinuity in all subscales they assessed, both our group’s and Farrow et al.’s results were consistent with continuity across observations (with one exception each: desire to drink (Farrow) and emotional overeating (present study)). Our study extends the latter’s findings by observing continuity for appetitive traits through to middle childhood. The different results between studies are likely due to there being less developmental change occurring from 2 to 5 years [9] or across 13 months (present study) than across a 7-year timespan from early childhood (age 4) to middle childhood (age 11) [8].
All eight appetitive characteristics showed tracking through childhood. That is, children who scored relatively high for these traits at their first observation in childhood (mean age: 6 years) maintained high scores relative to their peers at their second observation (approximately 13 months later). The Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire findings are largely in line with previous studies indicating strong positive associations across observations, even with varying time lags [8,9,33,34,35,36,37]. The stronger correlations observed in our study compared with the study by Ashcroft et al. [8] are likely due to the smaller time lag between our observations. Together with previous findings, our current results suggest that, like temperament [38], appetitive characteristics track through childhood.

Strengths and Limitations

The findings of our study need to be considered in the light of its strengths and weaknesses. A feature of our study was that ages and time lags between observations varied across participants. While this variability could be viewed as a disadvantage, we note that our results hold even when controlling for this variation, promoting the generalizability of our results across ages and time lags. As shown in Table 1, more than half of the children had their first Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire observation before the age of 6 years. Another potential limitation was that children’s appetitive traits were reported by mothers using questionnaires, potentially leading to shared observer and shared method bias. However, any bias due to maternal report was likely consistent across both observations, and the Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire has been validated against behavioral tests [39]. Nevertheless, replication using repeated behavioral measures of appetitive characteristics obtained across a variety of settings would increase confidence in our results. It should also be noted that generalizability to other US families might be limited by the homogenous characteristics of the study sample, since the majority of the participants in our study (nearly 75%) identified as White. Finally, correlation coefficients between appetitive traits and age were small in this study, explaining only a limited amount of variance. Additionally, the Cohen’s d value for the mean change in emotional overeating was < 0.3, and the clinical significance of these results is unclear.

5. Conclusions

To conclude, we found in the current study that appetitive characteristics measured in both early and middle childhood are largely stable and continuous. Future research examining the stability and continuity of appetitive characteristics across different developmental stages, starting in infancy and incorporating several assessment timepoints, will be essential to more fully understand the development of appetite and how it is temporarily or more chronically impacted by environmental (e.g., parent feeding) or other influences (for a relevant conceptual model, see [38]). For example, individual differences in child characteristics at any one observation might be the result of individual variation in the rate of brain maturation or in the timing of genetic influences. Since personality traits are more stable in older adults than in children or young adults [38], it would also be of interest to examine development across the whole life course. Nonetheless, our existing results have implications. In particular, our robust finding of increased emotional overeating with age may suggest a target behavior that could be addressed in early life to limit obesity risk. For example, a recent study demonstrated that a responsive parenting intervention in early life decreased emotional overeating in children, and the effect of the intervention on emotional overeating was mediated by parents’ use of food to soothe the child, suggesting a potential parental behavior that could be targeted [40]. An increased understanding of appetite development could also provide foundations for further investigation of biological contributions to appetite development and inform the development of interventions for parents that aim to promote parent–child food-related interactions that represent the best “fit” between parental behaviors and the child’s unfolding appetitive tendencies.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, E.J. and S.C.; methodology, E.J. and S.C.; formal analysis, E.J.; investigation, J.B., and RESONANCE Consortium; data curation, J.B.; writing—original draft preparation, E.J.; writing—review and editing, G.T. and S.C.; supervision, V.D., S.D., and S.C.; project administration, J.B., V.D., and S.D.; funding acquisition, V.D., S.D., and S.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This project was funded by NIH grants R01DK113286 and UG3OD023313 and BMGF grant INV-006627.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Brown University (IRB no.: 1500991, most recent approval date: 9 October 2019).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data will be made available by the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all RESONANCE families for generously providing their time to participate in our study. Members of the RESONANCE Consortium include: Sean C. L. Deoni, Bill & Melinda Gates Foundation, Seattle, WA, USA; Viren D’Sa, Department of Pediatrics, Warren Alpert Medical School at Brown University, Providence, RI, USA; Muriel Bruchhage, Department of Pediatrics, Warren Alpert Medical School at Brown University, Providence, RI, USA; Alexandra Volpe, Department of Pediatrics, Warren Alpert Medical School at Brown University, Providence, RI, USA; Jennifer Beauchemin, Department of Pediatrics, Warren Alpert Medical School at Brown University, Providence, RI, USA; Caroline Wallace, Department of Pediatrics, Warren Alpert Medical School at Brown University, Providence, RI, USA; John Rogers, Department of Pediatrics, Warren Alpert Medical School at Brown University, Providence, RI, USA; Rosa Cano, Department of Pediatrics, Warren Alpert Medical School at Brown University, Providence, RI, USA; Jessica Fernandes, Department of Pediatrics, Warren Alpert Medical School at Brown University, Providence, RI, USA; Elizabeth Walsh, Department of Pediatrics, Warren Alpert Medical School at Brown University, Providence, RI, USA; Brittany Rhodes, Department of Pediatrics, Warren Alpert Medical School at Brown University, Providence, RI, USA; Matthew Huentelman, The Translational Genomics Research Institute, Neurogenomics Division, Phoenix, AZ, USA; Candace Lewis, The Translational Genomics Research Institute, Neurogenomics Division, Phoenix, AZ, USA; Matthew D. De Both, The Translational Genomics Research Institute, Neurogenomics Division, Phoenix, AZ, USA; Marcus A. Naymik, The Translational Genomics Research Institute, Neurogenomics Division, Phoenix, AZ, USA; Susan Carnell, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA; Elena Jansen, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA; Jennifer R. Sadler, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA; Gita Thapaliya, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, USA; Vanja Klepac-Ceraj, Department of Biological Sciences, Wellesley College, Wellesley, MA, USA; Kevin Bonham, Department of Biological Sciences, Wellesley College, Wellesley, MA, USA; Monique LeBourgeois, Department of Integrative Physiology, University of Colorado, Boulder, CO, USA; Hans Georg Mueller, Department of Statistics, University of California, Davis, CA, USA; Jane-Ling Wang, Department of Statistics, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, USA; Changbo Zhu, Department of Statistics, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, USA; Yaqing Chen, Department of Statistics, University of California, Davis, Davis, CA, USA; Joseph Braun, School of Public Health, Brown University, RI, USA.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funding sources had no involvement in the study design; in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the article for publication.

References

  1. Carnell, S.; Wardle, J. Appetite and adiposity in children: Evidence for a behavioral susceptibility theory of obesity. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2008, 88, 22–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Kininmonth, A.; Smith, A.; Carnell, S.; Steinsbekk, S.; Fildes, A.; Llewellyn, C. The association between childhood adiposity and appetite assessed using the Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire and Baby Eating Behavior Questionnaire: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Obes. Rev. 2021, 22, e13169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Llewellyn, C.H.; Fildes, A. Behavioural Susceptibility Theory: Professor Jane Wardle and the Role of Appetite in Genetic Risk of Obesity. Curr. Obes. Rep. 2017, 6, 38–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Singh, A.S.; Mulder, C.; Twisk, J.W.R.; Van Mechelen, W.; Chinapaw, M.J.M. Tracking of childhood overweight into adulthood: A systematic review of tDShe literature. Obes. Rev. 2008, 9, 474–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Dubois, L.; Bédard, B.; Goulet, D.; Prud’homme, D.; Tremblay, R.E.; Boivin, M. Eating behaviors, dietary patterns and weight status in emerging adulthood and longitudinal associations with eating behaviors in early childhood. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2022, 19, 139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Hunot, C.; Fildes, A.; Croker, H.; Llewellyn, C.H.; Wardle, J.; Beeken, R.J. Appetitive traits and relationships with BMI in adults: Development of the Adult Eating Behaviour Questionnaire. Appetite 2016, 105, 356–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Mallan, K.M.; Fildes, A.; de la Piedad Garcia, X.; Drzezdzon, J.; Sampson, M.; Llewellyn, C. Appetitive traits associated with higher and lower body mass index: Evaluating the validity of the adult eating behaviour questionnaire in an Australian sample. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2017, 14, 130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Ashcroft, J.; Semmler, C.; Carnell, S.; van Jaarsveld, C.H.M.; Wardle, J. Continuity and stability of eating behaviour traits in children. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2008, 62, 985–990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Farrow, C.; Blissett, J. Stability and continuity of parentally reported child eating behaviours and feeding practices from 2 to 5 years of age. Appetite 2012, 58, 151–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Wardle, J.; Guthrie, C. Development of the children’s eating behaviour questionnaire. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2001, 42, 963–970. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. da Costa, M.P.; Severo, M.; Oliveira, A.; Lopes, C.; Hetherington, M.; Vilela, S. Longitudinal bidirectional relationship between children’s appetite and diet quality: A prospective cohort study. Appetite 2022, 169, 105801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Derks, I.P.M.; Sijbrands, E.J.G.; Wake, M.; Qureshi, F.; van der Ende, M.; Hillegers, M.H.J.; Jaddoe, V.W.V.; Tiemeier, H.; Jansen, P.W. Eating behavior and body composition across childhood: A prospective cohort study. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2018, 15, 96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Steinsbekk, S.; Llewellyn, C.H.; Fildes, A.; Wichstrøm, L. Body composition impacts appetite regulation in middle childhood. A prospective study of Norwegian community children. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2017, 14, 70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Deoni, S.C.L.; Beauchemin, J.; Volpe, A.; Dâ Sa, V.; Resonance Consortium. Impact of the COVID-19 Pandemic on Early Child Cognitive Development: Initial Findings in a Longitudinal Observational Study of Child Health. MedRxiv, 2021; preprint. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Deoni, S.C.L.; Dean, D.C., 3rd; O’Muircheartaigh, J.; Dirks, H.; Jerskey, B.A. Investigating white matter development in infancy and early childhood using myelin water faction and relaxation time mapping. Neuroimage 2012, 63, 1038–1053. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Bruchhage, M.M.K.; Ngo, G.-C.; Schneider, N.; D’Sa, V.; Deoni, S.C.L. Functional connectivity correlates of infant and early childhood cognitive development. Brain Struct. Funct. 2020, 225, 669–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Adler, N.E.; Epel, E.S.; Castellazzo, G.; Ickovics, J.R. Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: Preliminary data in healthy. White women. Health Psychol. 2000, 19, 586–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. World Health Organization. WHO Anthro for Personal Computer; Version 3.2.2, 2011; Software for Assessing Growth and Development of the World’s Children; WHO: Geneva, Switzerland, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  19. World Health Organization. WHO AnthroPlus for Personal Computers Manual: Software for Assessing Growth of the World’s Children and Adolescents; WHO: Geneva, Switzerland, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  20. Cohen, J. A power primer. Psychol. Bull. 1992, 112, 155–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Livingstone, B. Epidemiology of childhood obesity in Europe. Eur. J. Pediatr. 2000, 159, S14–S34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Birch, L.L.; Deysher, M. Caloric compensation and sensory specific satiety: Evidence for self regulation of food intake by young children. Appetite 1986, 7, 323–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Birch, L.L.; Johnson, S.L.; Andresen, G.; Peters, J.C.; Schulte, M.C. The Variability of Young Children’s Energy Intake. N. Engl. J. Med. 1991, 324, 232–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Fisher, J.O.; Birch, L.L. Eating in the absence of hunger and overweight in girls from 5 to 7 y of age. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2002, 76, 226–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Zeman, J.; Cassano, M.; Perry-Parrish, C.; Stegall, S. Emotion Regulation in Children and Adolescents. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 2006, 27, 155–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Torres, S.J.; Nowson, C.A. Relationship between stress, eating behavior, obesity. Nutrition 2007, 23, 887–894. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Hazley, D.; Stack, M.; Walton, J.; McNulty, B.A.M.; Kearney, J. Food neophobia across the life course: Pooling data from five national cross-sectional surveys in Ireland. Appetite 2022, 171, 105941. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Herle, M.; Fildes, A.; Llewellyn, C. Emotional eating is learned not inherited in children, regardless of obesity risk. Pediatr. Obes. 2018, 13, 628–631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Herle, M.; Fildes, A.; Rijsdijk, F.; Steinsbekk, S.; Llewellyn, C. The Home Environment Shapes Emotional Eating. Child Dev. 2018, 89, 1423–1434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Braden, A.; Rhee, K.; Peterson, C.B.; Rydell, S.A.; Zucker, N.; Boutelle, K. Associations between child emotional eating and general parenting style, feeding practices, and parent psychopathology. Appetite 2014, 80, 35–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Derks, I.P.M.; Bolhuis, K.; Sijbrands, E.J.G.; Gaillard, R.; Hillegers, M.H.J.; Jansen, P.W. Predictors and patterns of eating behaviors across childhood: Results from The Generation R study. Appetite 2019, 141, 104295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Steinsbekk, S.; Barker, E.D.; Llewellyn, C.; Fildes, A.; Wichstrøm, L. Emotional Feeding and Emotional Eating: Reciprocal Processes and the Influence of Negative Affectivity. Child Dev. 2018, 89, 1234–1246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Jansen, E.; Williams, K.E.; Mallan, K.M.; Nicholson, J.M.; Daniels, L.A. Bidirectional associations between mothers’ feeding practices and child eating behaviours. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2018, 15, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Warkentin, S.; Fildes, A.; Oliveira, A. Appetitive behaviors and body composition in school-age years: Bi-directional analyses in a population-based birth cohort. Appetite 2022, 168, 105770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Berge, J.M.; Miller, J.; Veblen-Mortenson, S.; Kunin-Batson, A.; Sherwood, N.E.; French, S.A. A Bidirectional Analysis of Feeding Practices and Eating Behaviors in Parent/Child Dyads from Low-Income and Minority Households. J. Pediatr. 2020, 221, 93–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Costa, A.; Severo, M.; Vilela, S.; Fildes, A.; Oliveira, A. Bidirectional relationships between appetitive behaviours and body mass index in childhood: A cross-lagged analysis in the Generation XXI birth cohort. Eur. J. Nutr. 2021, 60, 239–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Steinsbekk, S.; Belsky, J.; Wichstrøm, L. Parental Feeding and Child Eating: An Investigation of Reciprocal Effects. Child Dev. 2016, 87, 1538–1549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  38. Fraley, R.C.; Roberts, B.W. Patterns of Continuity: A Dynamic Model for Conceptualizing the Stability of Individual Differences in Psychological Constructs Across the Life Course. Psychol. Rev. 2005, 112, 60–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Carnell, S.; Wardle, J. Measuring behavioural susceptibility to obesity: Validation of the child eating behaviour questionnaire. Appetite 2007, 48, 104–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Harris, H.A.; Anzman-Frasca, S.; Marini, M.E.; Paul, I.M.; Birch, L.L.; Savage, J.S. Effect of a Responsive Parenting Intervention on Child Emotional Overeating is Mediated by Reduced Maternal Use of Food to Soothe: The Insight RCT. Pediatr. Obes. 2020, 15, e12645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Sample characteristics of participants completing the Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire.
Table 1. Sample characteristics of participants completing the Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire.
Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire (N = 335)
nMean
(or N)
SD
(or %)
Range
Mothers
Maternal age33036.646.4321.08 to 57.42
Maternal education312
(Partial) high school 3210.3
Partial college or specialized training 7524.0
College graduate 8627.6
Graduate training (master’s, PhD) 11938.1
MacArthur Scale of Subjective Social Status [17]3135.511.592 to 10
Maternal pre-pregnancy BMI30926.66.8816.6 to 53.0
Children
BMI z-score (WHO reference data)2750.401.32−4.71 to 5.14
Sex (female)33514342.6
Race323
Asian 41.2
Black or African American 185.6
More than 1 race 5918.3
White 24274.9
Ethnicity328
Hispanic/Latino 5416.5
Non-Hispanic/Latino 27483.5
Age in years at 1st observation (all participants)3356.042.982.00 to 15.24
2–3 years 10531.3
4–5 years 7923.6
6–7 years 6619.7
8–9 years 4112.2
10+ years 4413.1
Age in years at 1st observation (participants with 2 observations)1275.862.682.01 to 12.45
Age in years at 2nd observation1276.972.692.53 to 13.46
Time lag between 1st and 2nd observations (in months)12713.293.804.87 to 32.74
Table 2. Pearson correlations between child appetitive traits assessed with the Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire and child age.
Table 2. Pearson correlations between child appetitive traits assessed with the Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire and child age.
Child Eating Behavior QuestionnaireChild Age
rp
FR−0.0550.319
EF−0.0730.187
EOE0.203<0.001
DD−0.227<0.001
SR−0.1600.004
SE−0.266<0.001
FUS−0.0140.797
EUE−0.1190.031
Abbreviations: FR = food responsiveness, EF = enjoyment of food, EOE = emotional overeating, DD = desire to drink, SR = satiety responsiveness, SE = slowness in eating, FUS = food fussiness, EUE = emotional undereating.
Table 3. Paired t-tests to determine age-related changes within individuals for Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire subscales.
Table 3. Paired t-tests to determine age-related changes within individuals for Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire subscales.
All Participants
(1st Observation)
Observation 1Observation 2Paired t-Test
Cronbach’s AlphaMeanSDMeanSDMeanSDMean Change (SD)
Ob2–Ob1
t-Scorep-ValueCohen’s d
Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire N = 337n = 127
FR0.7622.340.762.300.732.260.72−0.04 (0.61)−0.6980.486−0.062
EF0.8623.740.733.740.743.700.73−0.04 (0.56)−0.7990.426−0.072
EOE0.7441.590.571.550.541.690.630.14 (0.55)2.8290.0050.251
DD0.8412.711.022.571.012.541.00−0.03 (0.97)−0.3380.736−0.030
SR0.7422.870.642.940.612.970.610.03 (0.45)0.7910.4310.071
SE0.7702.840.792.930.802.960.810.04 (0.64)0.6260.5320.056
FUS0.9152.900.912.890.842.890.90−0.01 (0.61)−0.1530.879−0.014
EUE0.8092.510.872.560.812.570.880.01 (0.78)0.1720.8640.015
Abbreviations: FR = food responsiveness, EF = enjoyment of food, EOE = emotional overeating, DD = desire to drink, SR = satiety responsiveness, SE = slowness in eating, FUS = food fussiness, EUE = emotional undereating.
Table 4. Paired correlations between observations 1 and 2 to determine tracking within individuals for Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire subscales.
Table 4. Paired correlations between observations 1 and 2 to determine tracking within individuals for Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire subscales.
Child Eating Behavior Questionnaire (n = 127)
Pearson CorrelationPartial Correlation with Age at 1st ObservationPartial Correlation with Age at 1st Observation and Time Lag
rp-Valuerp-Valuerp-Value
FR0.644<0.0010.644<0.0010.648<0.001
EF0.708<0.0010.708<0.0010.702<0.001
EOE0.562<0.0010.550<0.0010.555<0.001
DD0.533<0.0010.524<0.0010.518<0.001
SR0.726<0.0010.724<0.0010.722<0.001
SE0.685<0.0010.679<0.0010.689<0.001
FUS0.760<0.0010.763<0.0010.767<0.001
EUE0.576<0.0010.574<0.0010.576<0.001
Abbreviations: FR = food responsiveness, EF = enjoyment of food, EOE = emotional overeating, DD = desire to drink, SR = satiety responsiveness, SE = slowness in eating, FUS = food fussiness, EUE = emotional undereating.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Jansen, E.; Thapaliya, G.; Beauchemin, J.; D’Sa, V.; Deoni, S.; Carnell, S., on behalf of RESONANCE Consortium. The Development of Appetite: Tracking and Age-Related Differences in Appetitive Traits in Childhood. Nutrients 2023, 15, 1377. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15061377

AMA Style

Jansen E, Thapaliya G, Beauchemin J, D’Sa V, Deoni S, Carnell S on behalf of RESONANCE Consortium. The Development of Appetite: Tracking and Age-Related Differences in Appetitive Traits in Childhood. Nutrients. 2023; 15(6):1377. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15061377

Chicago/Turabian Style

Jansen, Elena, Gita Thapaliya, Jennifer Beauchemin, Viren D’Sa, Sean Deoni, and Susan Carnell on behalf of RESONANCE Consortium. 2023. "The Development of Appetite: Tracking and Age-Related Differences in Appetitive Traits in Childhood" Nutrients 15, no. 6: 1377. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15061377

APA Style

Jansen, E., Thapaliya, G., Beauchemin, J., D’Sa, V., Deoni, S., & Carnell, S., on behalf of RESONANCE Consortium. (2023). The Development of Appetite: Tracking and Age-Related Differences in Appetitive Traits in Childhood. Nutrients, 15(6), 1377. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15061377

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop