Next Article in Journal
Diagnostic Performance of Biomarker-Based Scores as Predictors of Metabolic Dysfunction-Associated Fatty Liver Disease Risk in Healthy Children
Next Article in Special Issue
Biochemical and Anthropometric Outcomes in Paediatric Patients with Heterozygous Familial Hypercholesterolemia after COVID-19 Pandemic Lockdowns: An Exploratory Analysis
Previous Article in Journal
Toward Precision Weight-Loss Dietary Interventions: Findings from the POUNDS Lost Trial
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Association between Eating Patterns and Quality of Life in Patients with Familial Hypercholesterolemia

by
Alexandra Maștaleru
1,2,
Irina Mihaela Abdulan
1,2,*,
Andra Oancea
1,2,*,
Alexandru Dan Costache
1,2,
Raul-Alexandru Jigoranu
1,
Mădălina Ioana Zota
1,2,
Mihai Roca
1,2,†,
Ileana-Katerina Ioniuc
3,
Cristina Rusu
3,
Laura Mihaela Trandafir
3,
Elena Țarcă
4,
Maria Magdalena Leon
1,2,†,
Carmen Marinela Cumpăt
1,2,† and
Florin Mitu
1,2
1
Department of Medical Specialties I, “Grigore T. Popa” University of Medicine and Pharmacy, 700115 Iasi, Romania
2
Clinical Rehabilitation Hospital, 700661 Iasi, Romania
3
Department of Mother and Child, “Grigore T. Popa” University of Medicine and Pharmacy, 700115 Iasi, Romania
4
Department of Surgery II—Pediatric Surgery, “Grigore T. Popa” University of Medicine and Pharmacy, 700115 Iasi, Romania
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
Nutrients 2023, 15(16), 3666; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15163666
Submission received: 1 August 2023 / Revised: 16 August 2023 / Accepted: 18 August 2023 / Published: 21 August 2023

Abstract

:
(1) Background: Familial hypercholesterolemia (FH) is a genetic disease that has autosomal dominant inheritance, being characterized by increased levels of low-density lipoproteins (LDLs) due to a decreased clearance of the circulant LDLs. Alimentation is a key factor in patients with FH. Implementing a restrictive diet may have a significant impact on their quality of life, besides the social and environmental factors. (2) Methods: We realized a prospective study that was conducted in the Cardiovascular Rehabilitation Clinic from the Clinical Rehabilitation Hospital and that included 70 patients with FH and 20 controls (adults with no comorbidities). We evaluated their lipid profile, their quality of life through the Short Form—36 Questionnaire, and their eating habits. (3) Results: Lower scores in the quality-of-life questionnaire were obtained in the FH group both in the case of the physical (73.06 vs. 87.62) and the mental component (75.95 vs. 83.10). Women had better physical function (85 vs. 75) and physical role than men (100 vs. 75). The group aged over 65 has the score lowest for all 10 components. Overeating was driven by boredom and was more frequent on weekends in the FH group. None of the patients in the control group felt loneliness or depression associated with overeating. (4) Conclusions: Overeating in patients with FH is associated with a lower quality of life. The complexity of these patients needs a multidisciplinary approach. Thus, the quality-of-life questionnaire should be implemented in their periodic follow-ups in order to increase their general status, paying special attention to geriatric patients.

1. Introduction

Familial hypercholesterolemia (FH) is a genetic disease with autosomal dominant inheritance, characterized by increased levels of low-density lipoproteins (LDLs) due to a decreased clearance of the circulant LDLs. In time, they will deposit on the arterial walls, causing a significant increase in the risk of developing premature cardiovascular disease [1].
FH can have two genetic forms: homozygous and heterozygous. Meta-analyses using clinical definitions of FH with the inclusion of increasing numbers of patients have established a global prevalence for the heterozygous form of approximately 1:300, with considerable variability between regions, despite the fact that some populations are not included currently in studies. Regarding Europe, a meta-analysis that included more than 80% of cases from the general European population estimated the prevalence to be 1:311, ranging between 1:200 and 1:575 [2]. The homozygous form, however, is much rarer, with a prevalence of 1:170,000–1:300,000 [3].
The most frequent causes of FH are mutations in the LDLR gene (approximately 90%), followed by a mutation in the APOB (about 10%), while less than 5% can have a defect of the PCSK9 gene. Extremely rarely, less than 1% can have a defect in the LDLRAP1 gene [4].
Several factors contribute to blood cholesterol levels, such as diet, ethnicity, and family history. Nevertheless, an unhealthy diet is a risk factor for hypercholesterolemia that can be modified. Adopting a balanced diet low in saturated fats is a crucial measure individuals can take to decrease their chances of developing high blood cholesterol [5,6]. This aspect is also valid for patients with FH, where not only the genetic factor is involved.
But when eating habits turn into potential causes in the development or exacerbation of metabolic pathologies, an increased interest is placed on overeating, its causes, and its effects.
The lack of nutritional education programs, information on the harmful effects of a correct diet, as well as the interventions of psychologists on the impact that a genetic disease has on patients are direct causes of the inconsistent management of patients with FH.
The purpose of this research is to find out the effects of nutrition on the quality of life of this category of patients.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Type of Study

We realized a prospective study that was conducted in the Cardiovascular Rehabilitation Clinic from the Clinical Rehabilitation Hospital between 1 December 2020 and 31 March 2022. The study included 70 patients with familial hypercholesterolemia, a diagnosis that was based on a score of more than 8 points in the Dutch Lipid Clinic Network (DLCN). Because many of the patients had already had, in their daily medical treatment, lipid-lowering therapy, we corrected their LDL cholesterol value with a factor that was described by Haralambos et al. [7]. The value was corrected only for the patients that had administered a lipid-lowering treatment in the past six months, and for the others, the included LDL cholesterol value was the one obtained by our laboratory.

2.2. Patients’ Selection

During the aforementioned period, we identified, from a total of 2005 patients admitted to our clinic, 70 with FH who met the inclusion criteria to participate in the present study and 20 patients in the control group, patients with normal lipid profile values.
The inclusion criteria were a DLCN score of more than 8 points, an age over 18 years old, and a signing of the informed consent. The exclusion criteria were a DLCN score of less than 8 points and the diagnosis of at least one of the following conditions: hypothyroidism, chronic kidney disease with a low creatinine clearance, nephrotic syndrome, diabetes mellitus with uncontrolled glycemic values, severe liver disease, or a hypercaloric diet (total fat intake more than 40% of the daily caloric intake) [8], in order to diminish the possibility of secondary hypercholesterolemia (Figure 1).

2.3. Data Collection

A single investigator performed both history and physical examinations and collected general information (sex and age).
Eating habits were evaluated by applying the food frequency questionnaire. It includes information related to the number of daily meals, the number of snacks, favorite foods, the types of fats consumed or used in cooking, and the factors determining overeating and its effects.
Quality of life was studied using the Short Form Questionnaire 36 (SF-36). It is probably the most widely used tool for measuring the quality of life. It includes 36 items and was designed to provide a comprehensive assessment of the physical, mental, and social components of health status. More specifically, it uses 8 scales: physical function, social function, physical role limitation due to physical or emotional causes, mental health, energy level, somatic pain, and general health. The interpretation is complex: a scoring of the 36 answers, then 8 scales unify the items, finally obtaining a score consisting of the score of the two concepts—physical and mental. The mentioned scales are scored from 0 to 100. High scores indicate a better quality of life.

2.4. Ethics Committee

In order to be enrolled in the study, all included patients signed the informed consent. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee of both the “Grigore T. Popa” University of Medicine and Pharmacy Iași (15 June 2020) and the Iași Clinical Rehabilitation Hospital (25 November 2020).

2.5. Statistical Analysis

Statistical analysis was performed with SPSS 20.0 (Statistical Package for the Social Sciences, Chicago, IL, USA). Categorical variables were expressed as percentages and were compared by the Chi-Square test. The Shapiro–Wilk test was applied to assess the normality of distribution for continuous variables. The variables with a normal distribution were presented as mean values with standard deviations and were compared by the Student’s t-test. Non-normally distributed continuous variables were presented as medians with interquartile ranges and were compared with the Mann–Whitney U test. The threshold of statistical significance was a value of p ≤ 0.05 for all analyses.

3. Results

Table 1 shows the general characteristics of the study groups. There were no statistically significant differences between the two groups in terms of gender distribution and living environment, but the mean age of the FH patients was significantly higher than that of the control group.
The quality-of-life assessment reveals numerous statistically significant differences. We observe higher scores of patients in the control group, both in the physical and mental components (Table 2).
According to each dimension of the SF-36 questionnaire, the control group scores statistically significantly higher on almost all items (Table 3).
Even in the case of statistically insignificant results, the group of patients with FH presents lower scores, thus implying a lower quality of life.

3.1. General Characteristics

We found no statistically significant differences in terms of gender and quality of life in patients with FH, but we note that women had better physical function and physical role than men, and somatic pain, overall health, vitality, and social function had higher scores among males. Moreover, we note that physical function was better among rural people, but general and mental health, physical role, and social function had better scores in urban patients. Evaluating somatic pain, it was more intensely perceived by people from rural areas compared to those from urban areas (p = 0.002) (Table 4).
Significant gender differences were observed among patients included in the control group for the physical component and social function, with males having higher scores than females. Without significant differences, we mention that men presented a higher vitality, health, and mental component score compared to women. There were no differences when we evaluated the living environment of these patients, but we noted that people from the urban environment presented a higher score of general health, social function, and mental component compared to those from the rural environment (Table 5).
When comparing the two groups of patients, we observe that physical and social function, pain, general health, as well as the physical and mental components have much higher scores in the control group compared to those in the FH group.
With increasing age, we observe significant decreases in physical function, mental health, and the physical component, data that can be seen in Table 6. Although there is no statistical correlation, it is worth mentioning that the group aged over 65 has the lowest scores in all 10 components.
Using the Kruskal–Wallis test, we obtained the rejection of the null hypothesis between age and the physical function component (a p-value = 0.012 between those over 65 and under 45, and a p-value = 0.007 between those over 65 years and those in the 45–65 years group), general health (a p-value = 0.043 between those over 65 and those under 45, and a p-value = 0.003 between those over 65 and those in the 45–65 years), mental health (p = 0.043 between those over 65 and those under 45), as well as the physical component of the SF36 questionnaire (p = 0.032 between those over 65 and those in the group 45–65 years).
We note that in the control group, there were no differences in the evaluated age categories, but it is worth mentioning that the scores of patients over 65 years old were the lowest in the case of the physical component, social function, and emotional role (Table 7).

3.2. Eating Habits

We wanted to evaluate the emotional impact of eating, focusing on two aspects, which were component parts of the food frequency questionnaire: the causes of overeating and the effects determined by it.
We observed statistically significant differences in vitality and mental health in FH patients, with a higher score in patients who ate more due to loneliness. Among the patients who ate more out of boredom, no statistically significant differences were identified, but we note a lower somatic pain score and better physical and social function as well as better general and mental health compared to those who did not have this eating habit. Moreover, the physical and mental components were increased in patients who ate more out of loneliness and boredom than those who did not present this habit (Table 8).
No statistically significant differences were observed among patients who stated that they overeat when feeling upset. Still, we note that they present a lower physical and social function as well as a lower physical role compared to those who did not give this answer. Moreover, the scores given to the responses for general and mental health as well as for the somatic pain of these patients were higher among those who were overfed.
In the case of patients who motivated overeating through depression, the quality-of-life components had much lower scores. Values below 50 points were identified in the case of the physical role, somatic pain, general and mental health, as well as vitality. Statistically significant differences are highlighted in Table 9.
Regarding stress, no statistically significant differences were observed. Still, we observe that patients with FH who overeat due to a stressor present a better physical and social function as well as a better physical role, and a better mental component compared to those who did not state this.
Furthermore, the emotional role was statistically significantly influenced by the existence of social interactions and dining out. People who have lunch in society had a lower physical function score compared to those who did not declare this, but the physical role and social function were better. In addition, the physical and mental components of the quality-of-life score were higher in those who ate out, but without statistically significant differences (Table 10).
The patients included in the group of FH who eat more on weekends present a physical and social function as well as a physical and mental component with a better score compared to those who do not have this habit. Moreover, they present a more important somatic pain than those who did not declare this, but these results are without statistical significance (Table 11).
FH patients who overeat in front of the TV show lower scores in the following categories: physical role, somatic pain, social function, and physical component, but these differences are not statistically significant.
Assessing the control group, we noticed that there was not one person who ate more because of loneliness or depression. Moreover, in the control group, we observed higher quality-of-life component values in patients with FH (Table 12).
When we analyzed boredom as a determinant of overeating, we found significantly lower scores for somatic pain and general health compared to those who did not report this. We also observed lower scores for social function among the same patients, but the results were not statistically significant.
The number of those in the control group who overeat due to anger was low, but the scores obtained by them were clearly lower than the rest, a fact illustrated in Table 12.
Assessing people in the control group who overate due to stress, we observed that they had average values of quality-of-life components similar to those who had other reasons for overeating. Similar results were also observed in patients who overate in society, except for the mental component, where they had a slightly higher score than those who did not declare this (Table 13).
Comparing the group of patients with FH and the control group, we observe higher scores in almost all components of the quality-of-life questionnaire in the control group for overeating caused by stressful situations or eating out.
There were no statistically significant differences in patients who overate while sitting in front of the TV or during the weekend. However, we mention that vitality and mental health had better scores in patients from the control group who ate more in front of the TV compared to those who did not give this answer (Table 14).
Comparing the two groups, we observed data similar to those previously described, namely, those included in the control group had much higher values for all components of the quality-of-life score compared to those in the FH group.
Another aspect evaluated was the appearance of various reactions and feelings following overeating. We did not observe statistically significant differences in feelings of depression and guilt in the FH group (Table 15).
However, we note that patients who felt depressed after a large lunch had a better function and physical role but lower vitality, compared to those who did not experience these feelings. Similar results were observed for guilt, but they presented a better vitality score compared to those who did not give this answer.
Regarding the feeling of satisfaction produced by overeating, no statistically significant differences were observed, but those who experienced it had lower values of somatic pain, general health, and physical function. Patients with hypercholesterolemia who declared that they did not feel anything after a hearty lunch had statistically significantly lower values of somatic pain and vitality compared to those who did not give this answer (Table 16).
Moreover, those who gave up the next meal due to an exaggerated food intake had a significantly lower physical role compared to those who did not give this answer (Table 17). Also, patients who choose to exercise with the desire to balance overeating had the maximum physical role score.
No statistically significant differences were observed for patients who felt guilty after overeating. However, in their case, somatic pain had a greater impact on the quality of life, a fact also observed in the group of patients who felt a state of well-being (“satisfaction”) after a hearty meal. The same group of patients had a lower physical and mental component than those who did not give this answer, but the results are without significant correlations. In addition, the same group of patients had a clearly diminished social function, being statistically significant (Table 18).
Comparing the group of FH patients with the control group, we note that scores are higher for all components of the quality-of-life assessment questionnaire among the control subjects when assessing feelings of guilt and satisfaction after overeating.
We did not observe statistically significant differences in those who stated that overeating did not cause them any reaction, but we did note a low mean vitality score. Similar scores were calculated both for those who gave up the next meal and for the rest (Table 19).
Moreover, the subjects who declared that they exercised after an increased food intake had significantly higher scores of somatic pain, general health, and the physical component compared to those who did not exercise (Table 20).
Comparing the group of patients with FH with the control group, we noticed that people who declared that they exercise in the control group had much higher scores for all components of the quality-of-life score than those in the FH group. Similar results were observed for those who did not declare this.

4. Discussion

Familial hypercholesterolemia is a genetic condition with a strong impact both at the individual level (through the presence of arteriosclerosis, the increased risk of cardiovascular diseases, and death) and at the general (socio-economic) level. Numerous studies have considered therapeutic plans and the complications of FH but there are just a few dedicated to evaluating the quality of life in this category of patients.
Quality of life is a multidimensional concept defined from different individual perspectives, such as happiness, well-being, and satisfaction, from an intellectual and emotional standpoint. It is also considered to be good if patients can make their own decisions and have the ability to carry out various activities independently, maintaining their autonomy. Among the decisive factors that influence it is the state of health, but it can be defined as an individual perception of satisfaction in different areas considered important by each individual [9].
In the present study, both the overall scores and those obtained after the breakdown by quality-of-life components were lower in the FH group. Living with a high risk of developing a genetic disease can affect people’s lives in different ways. Awareness of personal risk can be one of the factors underlying a low quality of life. In addition, previous studies have shown that high cholesterol in adulthood is associated with a lower quality of life in the last few decades [10].
This explanation becomes even more plausible in the case of our study, since the average age of the patients in the study group is higher compared to the control group. Among all the components, somatic pain stands out, having the lowest score for those with FH. Older age and the presence of multiple comorbidities are two possible explanations.
Assessing the quality of life in patients with FH, we observed that women had better physical function and physical role scores. Still, the physical component was lower than that of the men included in the study group. Moreover, females showed lower social function and vitality compared to males but without statistically significant differences. For the control group, the results were similar to those of the FH patients, but we note that men had a significantly better social function and physical component than women.
Souto and his collaborators included, in a study, 658 patients who were suspected of having FH or were diagnosed with this disease and administered a quality-of-life questionnaire to them. The results described by the researchers demonstrated a decrease in the females’ physical and mental components, which are consistent with those observed in our study [11].
Similar results were also described by Mata and his collaborators, who performed a cohort study that included 1947 people, including 1321 patients with FH and 626 of their relatives. The authors evaluated the quality of life among the two groups, demonstrating a significant decrease in women and in the elderly [12].
Our study also quantified the area of origin of the patients, noting that those with FH from the rural environment presented a lower score for almost all components of the quality-of-life score compared to those from the urban environment, except the score obtained for physical function. A recently published study by Risal et al. aimed to assess the quality of life in a population of 439 subjects in Nepal. The authors demonstrated that the urban environment is a positive predictor of quality of life, and lower scores were obtained by patients from rural areas [13], these results being consistent with those of our study. The explanation as to why we can observe these effects in our population can be due to a deficient sanitary network in the rural environment or social deprivation (especially among the young or the elderly with multiple associated comorbidities), these being triggering factors for a decrease in the quality of life.
Furthermore, we observed statistically significant correlations between somatic pain in rural patients and a diminished quality of life. This was also described by Fujii et al., demonstrating in a cohort of 3100 patients that a more significant somatic pain caused a substantial decrease in quality of life, independent of the number of associated comorbidities [14]. However, the specialized literature published up to the present moment is poorly represented, and further studies including patients with FH are needed.
When it comes to age and quality of life, published studies show similar scores regardless of the age category, and some, on the contrary, note that young people have a lower quality of life. In a 2004 study, Torres et al. noted that the impact of cardiovascular disease on perceived health may not be as pronounced in older people as it is in younger people, a possible explanation being that health problems become more common with age, and these individuals may have adapted to their condition. Furthermore, older patients have lower expectations than younger individuals [15].
Hyttinen and his colleagues discuss, in their 2008 study, the “healthy survivor effect”, that is, those individuals who survive to old age even if they carry a genetic burden, such as FH, and who may have other positive protective factors, genetic or environmental. However, their research was conducted on a group of patients with a healthy, active lifestyle [16].
In the case of our study, the lowest scores occur in the FH patients over 65 years of age, with a score of 50 or even lower for half of the items. The control patients scored higher on all components, but even so, the geriatric patients scored lower. A possible explanation for this is that other factors negatively influence the quality of life, such as age, socio-economic status, or affective status.
Quantifying the eating habits of patients with FH, we observed that people who eat more in the context of loneliness or boredom present higher scores for most components of the quality-of-life score compared to those who do not have this habit. Santos and colleagues observed, in a recently published study, that uncontrolled overeating was associated with a significant decrease in vitality in patients with morbid obesity [17].
Hadar-Shoval and colleagues also assessed emotional eating and lifestyle changes during the COVID-19 pandemic. The authors divided the enrolled subjects into those who made positive or negative changes in their daily activities, as well as those who remained with the same habits before the appearance of the coronavirus. The researchers demonstrated that the patients who ate the most emotionally were those who had negative changes in the adopted lifestyle (fried foods and carbohydrates), followed by those who had positive changes (increased consumption of fruits and vegetables), and those who ate the least emotionally were the subjects who had the same habits as before the appearance of the coronavirus [18].
The differences observed in our study can be explained not only by differences in the associated chronic metabolic pathologies but also by cultural and geographical differences, underlining once again the multifactorial determinism of FH. Differences between our study and others may also be because our study enrolled patients both before and during the coronavirus pandemic.
In order to evaluate the correlations between stress and overeating, Costarelli and Patsai demonstrated, in a group of 60 female students from Greece, that under conditions of stress, 36.7% did not change their eating habits, 35% ate more, and 28.3% ate less [19]. Conversely, in a similar population of Chinese subjects, positive emotions were associated with an increased food intake compared to those experiencing negative emotions [20]. These results are consistent with our control group.
Thus, the results published in the specialized literature are contradictory and insufficient. Regarding the results of our study, we noticed that patients with FH who overeat for emotional reasons (loneliness, boredom, being upset, depression, or stress) present higher scores after completing the quality-of-life questionnaire compared to those who did not declare these behaviors. Possible explanations for these results can be considered the following:
-
Emotional overeating creates a generally good perception, amplifying satisfaction and implicitly the quality of life.
-
The general state of well-being is actually not due to emotional nutrition but to the correct prescription and its increased compliance with the appropriate treatment.
-
Some of the patients with FH are not aware of the fact that emotional overeating increases the cardiovascular risk through the appearance of obesity, diabetes, and metabolic syndrome, and they appreciate that their state of health is good.
-
In moments of psycho-emotional overload, it is possible for patients to resort to overeating, and then the patient returns to a healthy lifestyle, which overall will provide an increased quality of life score and, respectively, a better state of health.
-
The good quality of life in patients with FH can be an element that draws attention to the fact that many of the coexisting pathologies can be reversible by correcting modifiable risk factors (obesity and insulin resistance).
Moreover, our study assessed the feelings experienced after a large lunch. Jeong and Seo assessed satisfaction with food-related aspects of life in a group of patients. They demonstrated a positive correlation between food satisfaction and the overall quality of life [21].
In our study, we observed that FH patients who felt satisfaction after overeating presented lower scores of quality-of-life components compared to those who did not report this feeling. And in the control group, we observed the same tendency but with better scores compared to those with FH. A possible explanation can be given by the continuation of overeating due to the feeling of satisfaction, which will cause an increase in BMI, the appearance of metabolic syndrome, and all other complications associated with obesity, which will lead to a decrease in the quality of life.
FH patients who reported feeling depressed after overeating had a better physical component and a lower mental component than those who did not report this. Gonzales and his collaborators demonstrated, in a group of 180 patients, that subjects diagnosed with eating disorders presented a lower mental health, emotional role, and vitality compared to healthy individuals [22], results similar to those obtained in our study. We can thus claim that patients who experience depression as a result of food excesses have a better physical component compared to the mental one and a better vitality, these being low due to the depressive condition.
Regarding the patients with FH who gave up the next meal after an excessive food intake, we noticed that most scores in the quality-of-life score were lower in those with this habit than the rest of the patients. Ferrer-Cascales and his collaborators evaluated the correlations between eating or not eating breakfast and its quality with scores obtained when completing the quality-of-life questionnaire. The researchers demonstrated that the best scores were obtained by subjects who ate a high-quality breakfast, followed by those who did not eat the first meal of the day, and the lowest scores were obtained by individuals who ate a low-quality breakfast [23].
Furthermore, Tahara and his collaborators evaluated the correlations between eating meals irregularly and mental health in a group of Japanese subjects. The researchers showed that irregular lunches were associated with a decrease in the physical component, decreased work productivity, and an impaired sleep quality, possibly due to the lack of breakfast in the daily diet, an increased frequency of snacks and nutritional imbalances, as well as insufficient periods between the last meal and sleep [24]. In the context of overwork, stress, and excessive work, food and implicitly overeating bring with it, momentarily, a feeling of well-being. Awareness of the risk causes patients to skip the next meal, but over time, repeating this process can generate a feeling of frustration, the appearance of metabolic syndrome, and a decrease in quality of life.
Herrera-Espineira and colleagues assessed the differences in eating habits, physical activity, and quality of life in obese and overweight patients. These authors demonstrated that overweight patients had better indicators of a healthy diet, emotional eating, and physical activity when compared to obese patients [25]. In our study, patients with FH who chose physical activity after a food binge had better scores in almost all components of quality of life compared to those who were sedentary, results that are consistent with data from the literature.
To our knowledge, this is the first study in Romania that evaluates the quality of life of patients with FH, both in adults and in the geriatric population. The strength of this study is that we included the food frequency questionnaire besides the quality-of-live evaluation.

5. Conclusions

Overeating was driven by boredom and was more frequent on weekends in the FH group. None of the patients in the control group felt loneliness or depression associated with overeating, a possible explanation being the lower average age of this group. Half of the control group overeat during stressful times, on weekends, or in society. Emotional eating is a trigger for nutritional imbalances and obesity.
Regarding the patients with FH who skipped the next meal after an excessive food intake, we noticed that most scores were lower in those who had this habit compared to the rest of the patients, emphasizing the emotional component of this habit.
In the present study, both the overall scores and those obtained in each component were lower in the FH group. Lower scores were obtained by women, patients from rural living areas, and those over 65 years of age.
In conclusion, overeating in patients with FH is associated with a lower quality of life. The complexity of these patients needs a multidisciplinary approach; thus, the quality-of-life questionnaire should be implemented in their periodical follow-ups in order to increase their general status, with special attention paid to geriatric patients.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.M., M.M.L. and F.M.; methodology, M.I.Z., I.M.A. and C.M.C.; software, M.R.; validation, E.Ț., I.-K.I. and L.M.T.; investigation, A.M., C.R. and A.O.; resources, A.D.C. and R.-A.J.; writing—original draft preparation, I.M.A. and A.M.; writing—review and editing, F.M. and M.M.L.; visualization, A.M.; supervision, M.M.L., C.R. and F.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

This study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Institutional Review Board (or Ethics Committee) of both the “Grigore T. Popa” University of Medicine and Pharmacy Iasi (certificate approval from 15 June 2020) and Iasi Clinical Rehabilitation Hospital (certificate of approval from 25 November 2020).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the patient(s) in order to publish this paper.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Benito-Vicente, A.; Uribe, K.B.; Jebari, S.; Galicia-Garcia, U.; Ostolaza, H.; Martin, C. Familial Hypercholesterolemia: The Most Frequent Cholesterol Metabolism Disorder Caused Disease. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2018, 19, 3426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Hu, P.; Dharmayat, K.I.; Stevens, C.A.T.; Sharabiani, M.T.A.; Jones, R.S.; Watts, G.F.; Genest, J.; Ray, K.K.; Vallejo-Vaz, A.J. Prevalence of Familial Hypercholesterolemia among the General Population and Patients with Atherosclerotic Cardiovascular Disease: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Circulation 2020, 141, 1742–1759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Nohara, A.; Tada, H.; Ogura, M.; Okazaki, S.; Ono, K.; Shimano, H.; Daida, H.; Dobashi, K.; Hayashi, T.; Hori, M.; et al. Homozygous Familial Hypercholesterolemia. J. Atheroscler. Thromb. 2021, 28, 665–678. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Tokgozoglu, L.; Kayikcioglu, M. Familial Hypercholesterolemia: Global Burden and Approaches. Curr. Cardiol. Rep. 2021, 23, 151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Yang, T.; Yi, J.; He, Y.; Zhang, J.; Li, X.; Ke, S.; Xia, L.; Liu, L. Associations of Dietary Fats with All-Cause Mortality and Cardiovascular Disease Mortality among Patients with Cardiometabolic Disease. Nutrients 2022, 14, 3608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Teo, P.S.; van Dam, R.M.; Whitton, C.; Tan, L.W.L.; Forde, C.G. Association between Self-Reported Eating Rate, Energy Intake, and Cardiovascular Risk Factors in a Multi-Ethnic Asian Population. Nutrients 2020, 12, 1080. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Haralambos, K.; Whatley, S.D.; Edwards, R.; Gingell, R.; Townsend, D.; Ashfield-Watt, P.; Lansberg, P.; Datta, D.B.; McDowell, I.F. Clinical experience of scoring criteria for Familial Hypercholesterolaemia (FH) genetic testing in Wales. Atherosclerosis 2015, 240, 190–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Authors/Task Force Members; ESC Committee for Practice Guidelines (CPG); ESC National Cardiac Societies. 2019 ESC/EAS guidelines for the management of dyslipidaemias: Lipid modification to reduce cardiovascular risk. Atherosclerosis 2019, 290, 140–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Lopez-Ruiz, V.R.; Huete-Alcocer, N.; Alfaro-Navarro, J.L.; Nevado-Pena, D. The relationship between happiness and quality of life: A model for Spanish society. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0259528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Strandberg, T.E.; Strandberg, A.; Rantanen, K.; Salomaa, V.V.; Pitkala, K.; Miettinen, T.A. Low cholesterol, mortality, and quality of life in old age during a 39-year follow-up. J. Am. Coll. Cardiol. 2004, 44, 1002–1008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Souto, A.C.; Miname, M.H.; Fukushima, J.; Jannes, C.E.; Krieger, J.E.; Hagger, M.; Pereira, A.C.; Santos, R.D. Health related quality of life in individuals at high risk for familial hypercholesterolemia undergoing genetic cascade screening in Brazil. Atherosclerosis 2018, 277, 464–469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Mata, N.; Alonso, R.; Banegas, J.R.; Zambon, D.; Brea, A.; Mata, P. Quality of life in a cohort of familial hypercholesterolemia patients from the south of Europe. Eur. J. Public Health 2014, 24, 221–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Risal, A.; Manandhar, S.; Manandhar, K.; Manandhar, N.; Kunwar, D.; Holen, A. Quality of life and its predictors among aging people in urban and rural Nepal. Qual. Life Res. 2020, 29, 3201–3212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Fujii, T.; Oka, H.; Katsuhira, J.; Tonosu, J.; Kasahara, S.; Tanaka, S.; Matsudaira, K. Association between somatic symptom burden and health-related quality of life in people with chronic low back pain. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0193208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Soto Torres, M.; Marquez Calderon, S.; Ramos Diaz, I.; Barba Chacon, A.; Lopez Fernandez, F.; Failde Martinez, I. Health-related quality of life in coronary heart disease compared to norms in Spanish population. Qual. Life Res. 2004, 13, 1401–1407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Hyttinen, L.; Kekalainen, P.; Vuorio, A.F.; Sintonen, H.; Strandberg, T.E. Health-related quality of life in elderly patients with familial hypercholesterolemia. Int. J. Technol. Assess. Health Care 2008, 24, 228–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Santos, B.M.N.; dos Santos Vieira, D.A.; de Almeida, R.R.; Oliveira, V.B.; de Santana Pereira, J.; de Souza, M.F.C.; Rocha, R.M.S.; Almeida-Santos, M.A.; Aidar, F.J.; Sousa, A.C.S. Influence of eating behaviors on dietary intake and quality of life among low-income bariatric surgery candidates: A preliminary study. Obes. Med. 2022, 33, 100428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Hadar-Shoval, D.; Alon-Tirosh, M.; Asraf, K.; Tannous-Haddad, L.; Tzischinsky, O. Lifestyle Changes, Emotional Eating, Gender, and Stress during COVID-19 Lockdown. Nutrients 2022, 14, 3868. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Costarelli, V.; Patsai, A. Academic examination stress increases disordered eating symptomatology in female university students. Eat Weight Disord. 2012, 17, e164–e169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Zhu, H.; Cai, T.; Chen, G.; Zhang, B. Validation of the Emotional Eating Scale among Chinese Undergraduates. Soc. Behav. Personal. Int. J. 2013, 41, 123–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Jeong, J.; Seo, S. Importance of satisfaction with food for older adults’ quality of life. Br. Food J. 2014, 116, 1276–1290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Gonzalez, N.; Padierna, A.; Quintana, J.M.; Arostegui, I.; Horcajo, M.J. Quality of life in patients with eating disorders. Gac. Sanit. 2001, 15, 18–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Ferrer-Cascales, R.; Sanchez-SanSegundo, M.; Ruiz-Robledillo, N.; Albaladejo-Blazquez, N.; Laguna-Perez, A.; Zaragoza-Marti, A. Eat or Skip Breakfast? The Important Role of Breakfast Quality for Health-Related Quality of Life, Stress and Depression in Spanish Adolescents. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 1781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Tahara, Y.; Makino, S.; Suiko, T.; Nagamori, Y.; Iwai, T.; Aono, M.; Shibata, S. Association between Irregular Meal Timing and the Mental Health of Japanese Workers. Nutrients 2021, 13, 2775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Herrera-Espineira, C.; de Pascual, Y.M.A.M.; Lopez-Morales, M.; Diaz Jimenez, P.; Rodriguez Ruiz, A.; Exposito-Ruiz, M. Differences in Dietary Habits, Physical Exercise, and Quality of Life between Patients with Obesity and Overweight. Healthcare 2021, 9, 916. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Flow chart—study group selection.
Figure 1. Flow chart—study group selection.
Nutrients 15 03666 g001
Table 1. General characteristics of the study group.
Table 1. General characteristics of the study group.
General CharacteristicsFH
(n = 70)
Control
(n = 20)
p
Gender n (%)
  Male29 (41.4%)7 (35.0%)0.796
  Female41 (58.6%)13 (65.0%)
Living environment, n (%)
  Rural18 (25.7%)6 (30.0%)0.776
  Urban52 (74.3%)14 (70.0%)
Age, years (mean ± SD)54.65 ± 12.8141.70 ± 13.51<0.001
Table 2. Quality-of-life assessment.
Table 2. Quality-of-life assessment.
SF-36FHControlp
Physical component73.06 (54.67;83.42)87.62 (82.68;91)<0.001
Mental component75.95 (60.20;85.60)83.10 ± 13.070.004
Table 3. Dimensions of the quality-of-life assessment score.
Table 3. Dimensions of the quality-of-life assessment score.
SF-36FHControlp
Physical function75 (53.75;95)95 (77.5;100)0.003
Role functioning/physical75 (0;100)100 (100;100)0.001
Pain52 (41;76.5)84 (72.5;84)0.002
General health62 (45;77)81.35 ± 12.14<0.001
Vitality65 (50;80)72.5 ± 16.580.083
Social functioning75 (62.5;100)100 (87.5;100)0.014
Role functioning/emotional100 (66.6;100)100 (100;100)0.249
Emotional well-being76 (60;88)80.4 ± 10.610.147
Mean ± standard deviation or median (25th, 75th percentile), as appropriate.
Table 4. Quality-of-life components and demographics of the FH cohort.
Table 4. Quality-of-life components and demographics of the FH cohort.
SF-36GenderLiving Area
FemaleMalepRuralUrbanp
Physical function85
(42.50;95)
75
(55;90)
0.94785
(58.75;95)
72.5
(46.25;95)
0.512
Role functioning/physical100
(25;100)
75
(0;100)
0.51362.5
(0;100)
100
(25;100)
0.295
Pain51
(36.50;79)
61
(51;79)
0.33141
(22;54.5)
61.5
(51;84)
0.002
General health62
(44.5;73.5)
67
(45;77)
0.29956
(41.5;68.25)
67
(47;77)
0.161
Vitality65
(47.50;75)
70
(52.50;70)
0.25465
(53.75;75)
65
(50;80)
0.845
Social functioning75
(62.50;100)
87.5
(62.50;100)
0.37275
(62.5;87.5)
87.5
(62.5;100)
0.360
Role functioning/emotional100
(66.60;100)
100
(83.30;100)
0.792100
(33.3;100)
100
(74.95;100)
0.746
Emotional well-being76
(56;86)
76
(64;88)
0.49166
(59;81)
80
(60;88)
0.146
Physical component69.25
(51.44;83.87)
75.31
(58.23;83.59)
0.61764.79
(53.20;76.26)
75.59
(55.40;84.98)
0.093
Mental component73.60
(56.49;84.56)
77.32
(65.20;86.30)
0.41773.45
(55.92;79.85)
78.36
(61.15;87.95)
0.141
Table 5. Quality-of-life components and control group demographics.
Table 5. Quality-of-life components and control group demographics.
SF-36GenderLiving Area
FemaleMalepRuralUrbanp
Physical function95
(75;100)
95
(95;100)
0.38495
(85;100)
95
(75;100)
0.863
Role functioning/physical100
(100;100)
100
(100;100)
0.286100
(100;100)
100
(100;100)
0.342
Pain84
(67;84)
84
(84;100)
0.08084
(63;88)
84
(69.5;88)
0.696
General health81.53 ± 13.2581 ± 10.730.92885.16 ± 13.2479.71 ± 11.760.372
Vitality68.84 ± 16.7279.28 ± 15.110.18674.16 ±17.4471.78 ± 16.820.777
Social functioning87.5
(75;100)
100
(100;100)
0.01293.75
(75;100)
100
(84.37;100)
0.640
Role functioning/emotional100
(87.5;100)
100
(100;100)
0.180100
(75;100)
100
(100;100)
0.791
Emotional well-being77.84 ± 11.1485.14 ± 8.230.14781.33 ± 10.3280.00 ± 11.090.805
Physical component84.37
(78.5;90.18)
90.25
(86.37;91.87)
0.04789.53
(76.89;92.64)
85.68
(82.04;91.40)
0.650
Mental component79.87 ± 14.7689.08 ± 6.350.13781.88 ± 18.3783.62 ± 10.900.793
Table 6. Quality-of-life components by age groups in the FH cohort.
Table 6. Quality-of-life components by age groups in the FH cohort.
SF-36Under 4545–65Over 65p
Physical function92.50 (56.25;98.75)85 (55;95)52.5 (22.5;68.75)0.004
Role functioning/physical87.5 (0;100)100 (50;100)50 (0;93.75)0.114
Pain67.5 (43.5;100)52 (41;84)51 (19.25;63.5)0.194
General health67 (44.25;85.75)67 (53.75;77)45 (29;52)0.003
Vitality67.5 (51.25;78.75)67.5 (50;80)62.5 (42.5;65)0.242
Social functioning87.5 (62.5;100)87.5 (62.5;100)75 (40.62;87.5)0.262
Role functioning/emotional100 (74.95;100)100 (100;100)100 (33.3;100)0.403
Emotional well-being86 (69;91)76 (60;85)66 (40;80)0.050
Physical component75.68 (53.43;91.03)75.59 (63.05;84.35)59.1 (38.37;73.85)0.032
Mental component79.75 (56.8;88.85)77.36 (63.1;86.2)61.56 (44.59;80)0.060
Table 7. Quality-of-life components by age groups in the control group.
Table 7. Quality-of-life components by age groups in the control group.
SF-36Under 4545–65Over 65p
Physical function95 (95;100)95 (75;100)77.5 (55;100)0.893
Role functioning/physical100 (100;100)100 (100;100)100 (100;100)0.666
Pain84 (73;92)74 (73;84)50 (0;100)0.581
General health77 (72;87)87 (82;87)88.5 (77;100)0.553
Vitality75 (65;80)70 (70;80)75 (50;100)0.972
Social functioning100 (87.5;100)100 (87.5;100)68.75 (37.5;100)0.774
Role functioning/emotional100 (100;100)100 (100;100)50 (0;100)0.223
Emotional well-being80 (80;84)84 (80;84)84 (68;100)0.829
Physical component89.75 (86.25;90.25)84.37 (82.18;85.12)74.21 (48.43;100)0.637
Mental component86.4 (81.4;88.5)85.2 (82.7;87.4)73.25 (46.5;100)0.971
Table 8. Components of quality of life and factors determining overnutrition in the FH group.
Table 8. Components of quality of life and factors determining overnutrition in the FH group.
SF-36
FH Group
What Situations Make You Eat More?
LonelinesspBoredomp
YESNO YESNO
Physical function87.5
(57.5;91.25)
70
(51.25;95)
0.60685
(55;95)
70
(47.5;95)
0.480
Role functioning/physical75
(50;100)
87.5
(0;100)
0.62575
(25;100)
100
(0;100)
0.949
Pain61.5
(51;84)
51.5
(41;74)
0.30852
(41.5;84)
62
(41;74)
0.810
General health67
(56.5;77)
59.5
(42;77)
0.22567
(53.5;77)
57
(42;76)
0.131
Vitality77.5
(61.25;85)
65
(46.25;75)
0.04575
(52.5;82.5)
65
(40;75)
0.051
Social functioning87.5
(71.87;100)
75
(62.5;100)
0.28587.5
(62.5;100)
75
(50;100)
0.251
Role functioning/emotional100
(91.65;100)
100
(66.6;100)
0.497100
(100;100)
100
(49.95;100)
0.108
Emotional well-being84
(71;88)
76
(57;84)
0.05080
(62;88)
76
(52;84)
0.238
Physical component75.88
(64.79;84.28)
69.43
(51.56;83.18)
0.19175.81
(62.95;84.03)
67.87
(50.56;83.12)
0.289
Mental component81.35
(69.72;88.9)
74.55
(54.1;85.17)
0.14079.4
(67.05;87.4)
73.52
(51.58;84.4)
0.156
Table 9. Components of quality of life and factors determining overnutrition in the FH group.
Table 9. Components of quality of life and factors determining overnutrition in the FH group.
SF-36
FH Group
What Situations Make You Eat More?
SorrowpDepressionp
YESNO YESNO
Physical functioning65
(40;90)
80
(55;95)
0.24560
(13.75;90)
77.5
(55;95)
0.169
Role functioning/physical75
(0;100)
100
(25;100)
0.39450
(0;100)
87.5
(25;100)
0.362
Role functioning/emotional100
(66.6;100)
100
(66.6;100)
0.954100
(0;100)
100
(100;100)
0.125
Energy/fatigue65
(45;85)
65
(50;75)
0.40340
(13.75;65)
65
(55;80)
0.011
Emotional well-being84
(60;88)
72
(60;84)
0.10544
(19;78)
80
(64;88)
0.013
Social functioning75
(50;100)
87.5
(62.5;100)
0.50162.5
(12.5;87.5)
87.5
(62.5;100)
0.027
Pain62
(51;84)
51
(41;74)
0.23246
(0;67.5)
56.5
(41;81.5)
0.156
General health72
(42;77)
57
(45;75)
0.30141
(18.75;77)
64.5
(47.75;77)
0.088
Physical component75.31
(55.12;83.93)
69.62
(53.31;83.25)
0.83757.4
(10.09;79.34)
74.46
(57.32;83.76)
0.092
Mental component77.32
(61.3;88.8)
74
(58.96;83.9)
0.50060.15
(13.9;78.62)
77.36
(61.52;85.6)
0.038
Table 10. Quality-of-life components and factors determining overnutrition in the FH group.
Table 10. Quality-of-life components and factors determining overnutrition in the FH group.
SF-36
FH Group
What Situations Make You Eat More?
StresspIn Societyp
YESNO YESNO
Physical functioning85
(51.25;95)
70
(53.75;95)
0.63765
(45;100)
85
(55;95)
0.677
Role functioning/physical100
(75;100)
62.5
(0;100)
0.014100
(50;100)
75
(0;100)
0.549
Role functioning/emotional100
(100;100)
100
(66.6;100)
0.232100
(100;100)
100
(66.6;100)
0.047
Energy/fatigue65
(51.25;75)
65
(50;80)
0.74665
(60;80)
65
(50;80)
0.697
Emotional well-being82
(60;88)
76
(59;85)
0.43376
(64;88)
76
(56;88)
0.429
Social functioning87.5
(53.12;100)
75
(62.5;100)
0.76687.5
(62.5;100)
75
(62.5;100)
0.399
Pain57
(43.5;100)
51
(41;74)
0.34764
(51;100)
51
(41;74)
0.146
General health62
(51.25;77)
62
(42;77)
0.47667
(52;87)
62
(42;77)
0.162
Physical component75.56
(64.7;86.09)
67.81
(50.84;82.96)
0.27474.37
(62.25;86.81)
70.5
(51.12;83)
0.330
Mental component77.35
(64.3;87.6)
73.8
(56.62;85)
0.65680.2
(66.9;88.5)
74
(53.4;83.9)
0.167
Table 11. Components of quality of life and factors determining overnutrition in the FH group.
Table 11. Components of quality of life and factors determining overnutrition in the FH group.
SF-36
FH Group
What Situations Make You Eat More?
In WeekendpWatching TVp
YESNO YESNO
Physical functioning80
(57.5;97.5)
70
(42.5;92.5)
0.24070
(38.75;91.25)
77.5
(55;95)
0.575
Role functioning/physical75
(12.5;100)
100
(0;100)
0.82375
(18.75;100)
100
(0;100)
0.623
Pain62
(46;84)
51
(36.5;69)
0.15051
(29.5;84)
56.5
(41;74)
0.589
General health67
(43.5;79.5)
62
(47;77)
0.79364.5
(44.25;72.75)
62
(45.5;77)
0.973
Table 12. Components of quality of life and factors determining overeating in the control group.
Table 12. Components of quality of life and factors determining overeating in the control group.
SF-36
Control Group
What Situations Make You Eat More?
BoredompAngerp
DANU DANU
Physical functioning92.5
(77.5;100)
95
(80;100)
0.80577.5
(55;100)
95
(85;100)
0.693
Role functioning/physical100
(81.25;100)
100
(100;100)
0.276100
(100;100)
100
(100;100)
0.628
Role functioning/emotional100
(81.25;100)
100
(100;100)
0.59450
(0;100)
100
(100;100)
0.105
Energy/fatigue65 ± 22.7374.37 ± 15.040.32572.5
(50;95)
72.5
(65;80)
1.000
Emotional well-being74 ± 14.7882 ± 9.230.18580
(68;92)
80
(80;84)
0.897
Social functioning87.5
(46.87;100)
100
(87.5;100)
0.39168.75
(37.5;100)
100
(87.5;100)
0.475
Pain61.5
(46;78.5)
84
(76.5;96)
0.03730.5
(0;61)
84
(74;84)
0.024
General health67 ± 12.2484.93 ± 9.390.00579.5
(77;82)
82
(72;92)
0.751
Physical component80.87
(63.54;90.03)
89.31
(84.23;91.46)
0.29869.84
(48.43;91.25)
87.62
(84.18;90.25)
0.614
Mental component76.3
(59.72;90.7)
86.8
(83.17;90.52)
0.21970.15
(46.5;93.8)
85.9
(81.4;88.5)
0.801
Table 13. Components of quality of life and factors determining overeating in the control group.
Table 13. Components of quality of life and factors determining overeating in the control group.
SF-36
Control Group
What Situations Make You Eat More?
StresspIn Societyp
YESNO YESNO
Physical functioning95
(75;100)
95
(85;100)
0.75195
(95;100)
95
(75;100)
0.451
Role functioning/physical100
(100;100)
100
(100;100)
0.884100
(100;100)
100
(100;100)
0.884
Role functioning/emotional100
(100;100)
100
(100;100)
0.625100
(100;100)
100
(100;100)
0.625
Energy/fatigue72.72 ± 16.7872.22 ± 17.340.94873.33 ± 15.6171.81 ± 18.060.845
Emotional well-being81.45 ± 10.1679.11 ± 11.620.63680.88 ± 9.3380 ± 120.858
Social functioning100
(87.5;100)
100
(81.25;100)
0.73087.5
(87.5;100)
100
(75;100)
0.546
Pain84
(62;84)
84
(78;92)
0.44784
(79;100)
84
(61;84)
0.101
General health82.09 ± 13.0180.44 ± 11.70.77285.22 ± 9.6478.18 ± 13.460.205
Physical component85.12
(82.18;91.25)
88.87
(83.93;91.06)
0.62189.75
(83.18;90.21)
86.37
(75.37;91.87)
0.820
Mental component85.2
(77.9;92)
86.4
(78.8;89.85)
0.97085.66 ± 6.9281 ± 16.590.411
Table 14. Components of quality of life and factors determining overeating in control patients.
Table 14. Components of quality of life and factors determining overeating in control patients.
SF-36
Control Group
What Situations Make You Eat More?
In WeekendpWatching TVp
YESNO YESNO
Physical functioning95
(87.5;95)
97.5
(71.25;100)
0.60092.5
(66.25;100)
95
(80;100)
1.000
Role functioning/physical100
(81.25;100)
100
(100;100)
0.075100
(100;100)
100
(100;100)
0.468
Role functioning/emotional100
(81.25;100)
100
(100;100)
0.385100
(100;100)
100
(100;100)
0.361
Energy/fatigue77.5
(65;80)
70
(60;91.25)
1.00082.5 ± 17.5570 ± 15.910.184
Emotional well-being77 ± 9.9782.66 ± 10.830.25384 ± 15.6679.5 ± 9.450.463
Pain84
(65;84)
84
(72.5;96)
0.68573
(61.25;96)
84
(74;84)
0.654
General health79.87 ± 14.382.33 ± 11.030.67080.25 ± 20.381.62 ± 10.190.846
Physical component89.31
(77.07;91.45)
86.31
(84.23;91)
0.84788.18
(77.81;97.81)
87.62
(82.68;90.23)
0.670
Mental component85.5
(74.07;90.52)
86.3
(78.77;92.47)
0.67190.6
(75.25;98.45)
85.3
(78.77;88.5)
0.277
Table 15. Quality-of-life components and response to overeating in the FH group.
Table 15. Quality-of-life components and response to overeating in the FH group.
SF-36
FH Group
How Do You Feel after Overeating?
DepressionpGuiltp
YESNO YESNO
Physical functioning85
(25;90)
75
(55;95)
0.70185
(55;96.25)
72.25
(46.25;95)
0.380
Role functioning/physical100
(50;100)
75
(0;100)
0.260100
(43.75;100)
75
(0;100)
0.115
Pain62
(52;100)
51
(41;74)
0.22262
(46.25;84)
51
(41;74)
0.415
General health62
(42;72)
62
(45;77)
0.62464.5
(47;79.5)
59.5
(42;77)
0.265
Table 16. Quality-of-life components and response to overeating in the FH group.
Table 16. Quality-of-life components and response to overeating in the FH group.
SF-36
FH Group
How Do You Feel after Overeating?
SatisfactionpNothingp
YESNO YESNO
Physical functioning65
(37.5;90)
80
(55;90)
0.37390
(55;95)
70
(50;90)
0.430
Role functioning/physical75
(0;100)
75
(12.5;100)
0.71375
(0;100)
100
(25;100)
0.398
Role functioning/emotional100
(100;100)
100
(66.6;100)
0.277100
(66.6;100)
100
(66.6;100)
0.993
Energy/fatigue65
(50;72.5)
65
(50;80)
0.64665
(40;65)
70
(55;80)
0.048
Pain51
(26.5;73)
61
(41;79)
0.34551
(32;61)
62
(51;84)
0.020
General health57
(38.5;77)
62
(46;77)
0.45067
(50;77)
62
(42;77)
0.696
Social functioning62.5
(50;93.75)
87.5
(62.5;100)
0.20875
(62.5;100)
75
(62.5;100)
0.908
Emotional well-being72
(60;88)
76
(60;88)
0.90980
(56;88)
76
(60;88)
0.926
Physical component64.4
(47.45;79.71)
74.56
(54.76;83.59)
0.40069.62
(53.31;77.95)
74.56
(55.12;84.12)
0.484
Mental component67.1
(59.25;80.55)
77.32
(59.79;86.3)
0.50573.6
(61.1;82.5)
77.3
(58.96;87.8)
0.634
Table 17. Quality-of-life components and response to overeating in the FH group.
Table 17. Quality-of-life components and response to overeating in the FH group.
SF-36
FH Group
How Do You Feel after Overeating?
I Skip the Next MealI Exercise
YESNOpYESNOp
Physical function65
(50;85)
85
(55;95)
0.22275
(60;90)
75
(45;95)
0.685
Role functioning/physical50
(0;100)
100
(25;100)
0.043100
(50;100)
75
(0;100)
0.179
Pain62.5
(48.5;74)
51.5
(41;84)
0.59361
(51;84)
52
(32;74)
0.182
General health57
(44.25;73.25)
64.5
(47;77)
0.60455
(43.5;87)
62
(46;73.5)
0.658
Energy/fatigue65
(58.75;80)
65
(50;80)
0.66075
(50;80)
65
(50;75)
0.154
Social functioning75
(50;90.62)
87.5
(62.5;100)
0.33987.5
(62.5;100)
75
(62.5;100)
0.486
Role functioning/emotional100
(91.65;100)
100
(66.6;100)
0.726100
(100;100)
100
(66.6;100)
0.418
Emotional well-being76
(62;88)
76
(60;88)
0.95276
(66;88)
76
(58;88)
0.571
Physical component69.18
(47.58;78.60)
74.93
(57.29;84.59)
0.29579.2
(62.95;85.78)
69.62
(52;81.06)
0.209
Mental component73.7
(56.62;84.67)
76.8
(60.74;85.6)
0.70280.2
(60.03;88.95)
74
(58.69;83.05)
0.254
Table 18. Quality-of-life components and response to overeating in the control group.
Table 18. Quality-of-life components and response to overeating in the control group.
SF-36
Control Group
How Do You Feel after Overeating?
GuiltSatisfaction
YESNOpYESNOp
Physical function95
(87.5;98.75)
95
(75;100)
0.92185
(70;90)
95
(95;100)
0.109
Role functioning/physical100
(81.25;100)
100
(100;100)
0.276100
(100;100)
100
(100;100)
0.542
Pain68
(61.25;93.5)
84
(76.5;84)
0.32062
(31;73)
84
(74;84)
0.184
General health82
(59.5;85.75)
79.5
(73.25;94.25)
0.63474.66 ± 21.5982.52 ± 15.160.314
Energy/fatigue72.5
(65;91.25)
72.5
(60;80)
0.77565 ± 1573.82 ± 16.910.410
Social functioning93.75
(78.12;100)
100
(87.5;100)
0.74875
(56.2;81.25)
100
(87.5;100)
0.016
Role functioning/emotional100
(81.25;100)
100
(100;100)
0.594100
(50;100)
100
(100;100)
0.268
Emotional well-being80
(67;90)
80
(80;84)
0.84670.66 ± 8.3282.11 ± 10.20.085
Physical component85.96
(77.07;90.87)
87.62
(84.23;91.46)
0.57075.37
(61.90;82.78)
88.87
(84.37;91.25)
0.138
Mental component83.65
(74.07;91.5)
86.8
(78.77;90.52)
0.57071.2
(58.85;79.85)
86.4
(82.7;91.2)
0.168
Table 19. Quality-of-life components and response to overeating in the control group.
Table 19. Quality-of-life components and response to overeating in the control group.
SF-36
Control Group
How Do You Feel after Overeating?
NothingI Skip the Next Meal
YESNOpYESNOp
Physical function95
(75;100)
95
(80;100)
0.86895
(80;100)
95
(77.5;100)
0.778
Role functioning/physical100
(100;100)
100
(100;100)
0.648100
(100;100)
100
(100;100)
0.767
Pain84
(72;84)
84
(68;92)
1.00084
(76.5;96)
84
(61.25;84)
0.255
General health75.57 ± 7.4884.46 ± 13.250.12186.5 ± 9.377.91 ± 12.940.124
Energy/fatigue67.14 ± 15.2375.38 ± 17.130.30173.75 ± 15.7571.66 ± 17.750.791
Table 20. Quality-of-life components and response to overeating in the control group.
Table 20. Quality-of-life components and response to overeating in the control group.
SF-36
Control Group
How Do You Feel after Overeating?p
I Exercise
YESNO
Physical function100 (97.5;100)95 (75;100)0.167
Role functioning/physical100 (100;100)100 (100;100)0.542
Pain100 (100;100)84 (67;84)0.007
General health100 (91;100)77 (72;87)0.048
Energy/fatigue100 (82.5;100)70 (60;80)0.134
Social functioning100 (100;100)100 (87.5;100)0.149
Role functioning/emotional100 (100;100)100 (100;100)0.443
Emotional well-being100 (88;100)80 (80;84)0.174
Physical component100 (94.87;100)86.25 (82.18;90.18)0.039
Mental component100 (92.3;100)85.40 (77.90;88.5)0.101
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Maștaleru, A.; Abdulan, I.M.; Oancea, A.; Costache, A.D.; Jigoranu, R.-A.; Zota, M.I.; Roca, M.; Ioniuc, I.-K.; Rusu, C.; Trandafir, L.M.; et al. Association between Eating Patterns and Quality of Life in Patients with Familial Hypercholesterolemia. Nutrients 2023, 15, 3666. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15163666

AMA Style

Maștaleru A, Abdulan IM, Oancea A, Costache AD, Jigoranu R-A, Zota MI, Roca M, Ioniuc I-K, Rusu C, Trandafir LM, et al. Association between Eating Patterns and Quality of Life in Patients with Familial Hypercholesterolemia. Nutrients. 2023; 15(16):3666. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15163666

Chicago/Turabian Style

Maștaleru, Alexandra, Irina Mihaela Abdulan, Andra Oancea, Alexandru Dan Costache, Raul-Alexandru Jigoranu, Mădălina Ioana Zota, Mihai Roca, Ileana-Katerina Ioniuc, Cristina Rusu, Laura Mihaela Trandafir, and et al. 2023. "Association between Eating Patterns and Quality of Life in Patients with Familial Hypercholesterolemia" Nutrients 15, no. 16: 3666. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15163666

APA Style

Maștaleru, A., Abdulan, I. M., Oancea, A., Costache, A. D., Jigoranu, R. -A., Zota, M. I., Roca, M., Ioniuc, I. -K., Rusu, C., Trandafir, L. M., Țarcă, E., Leon, M. M., Cumpăt, C. M., & Mitu, F. (2023). Association between Eating Patterns and Quality of Life in Patients with Familial Hypercholesterolemia. Nutrients, 15(16), 3666. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15163666

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop