Next Article in Journal
Urinary Sodium Excretion and Adherence to the Mediterranean Diet in Older Adults
Next Article in Special Issue
Nutrition Interventions for Children with Cerebral Palsy in Low- and Middle-Income Countries: A Scoping Review
Previous Article in Journal
Mango Consumption Is Associated with Improved Nutrient Intakes, Diet Quality, and Weight-Related Health Outcomes
Previous Article in Special Issue
Associations of Nutritional Status with Full Immunization Coverage and Safe Hygiene Practices among Thai Children Aged 12–59 Months
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Methods for Assessing Willingness to Try and Vegetable Consumption among Children in Indigenous Early Childcare Settings: The FRESH Study

by
Marianna S. Wetherill
1,*,
Mary B. Williams
2,*,
Jessica Reese
3,
Tori Taniguchi
4,
Susan B. Sisson
5,
Adrien D. Malek-Lasater
6,
Charlotte V. Love
7 and
Valarie Blue Bird Jernigan
4
1
Department of Health Promotion Sciences, Hudson College of Public Health, University of Oklahoma-Tulsa Schusterman Center, Tulsa, OK 74135, USA
2
Department of Biostatistics and Epidemiology, Hudson College of Public Health, University of Oklahoma-Tulsa Schusterman Center, Tulsa, OK 74135, USA
3
Department of Biostatistics and Epidemiology, Hudson College of Public Health, University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center, Oklahoma City, OK 73117, USA
4
Center for Indigenous Health Research and Policy, Oklahoma State University Center for Health Sciences, Tulsa, OK 74106, USA
5
Department of Nutritional Sciences, College of Allied Health, University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center, Oklahoma City, OK 73117, USA
6
Department of Teaching, Learning, and Curriculum, College of Education and Human Services, University of North Florida, Jacksonville, FL 32246, USA
7
School of Health Care Administration, Oklahoma State University Center for Health Sciences, Tulsa, OK 74107, USA
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2022, 14(1), 58; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14010058
Submission received: 12 November 2021 / Revised: 12 December 2021 / Accepted: 22 December 2021 / Published: 24 December 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Nutritional Status among Vulnerable Populations)

Abstract

:
Food preferences begin in early childhood, and a child’s willingness to try (WTT) new vegetables is an important determinant of vegetable intake. Young children living in rural communities are at increased risk for food insecurity, which may limit exposure to and consumption opportunities for vegetables. This manuscript describes the validation of the Farfan-Ramirez WTT (FR-WTT) measure using baseline data from the FRESH study, a gardening intervention for Native American families with preschool-aged children in Osage Nation, Oklahoma. Individually weighed vegetable containers were prepared with six types of vegetables and ranch dip. Researchers presented children (n = 164; M = 4.3 years, SD = 0.8) with these vegetables preceding a snack- or lunch time and recorded the child’s FR-WTT for each vegetable using a 5-point scale, ranging from “did not remove food (0)” to “put food in mouth and swallowed (4)”. After the presentation period, contents were re-weighed to calculate vegetable consumption. Household parents/guardians completed the Child Food Neophobia Scale (CFNS) for their child. FR-WTT scores were positively correlated with consumption weights of all vegetables (r = 0.7613, p < 0.0001) and each vegetable individually (r = 0.2016–0.7664). The total FR-WTT score was inversely correlated with the CFNS score (r = 0.3268, p < 0.0001). Sensitivity analyses demonstrated similar relationships by BMI, food security, and age. In conclusion, the FR-WTT is a valid method for assessing young children’s vegetable eating behavior and intake.

1. Introduction

The preschool years are an influential time for the development of learned food preferences, which can ultimately shape lifelong eating habits [1,2,3]. Vegetables are a central component of healthy diets, and like many new or unfamiliar foods, many young children can often initially reject vegetables due to an increased sensitivity to their bitter taste [4]. The tendency to reject or be reluctant to try new foods, known as food neophobia [1], usually peaks between the ages of two and six years old [5], and is an important influence on a child’s food choices, preferences for and intake of vegetables [6], and overall quality of diet [7,8]. Interventions that repeatedly expose children to the same type of food may reduce food neophobia [9,10,11]. Thus, interventions that provide young children with multiple opportunities to taste a variety of vegetables may increase willingness to try new vegetables and overall intake of vegetables.
An estimated 59% of young children attend Early Care and Education (ECEs) programs [12], and these environments substantially contribute to a child’s dietary intake and exposure to new foods, such as vegetables [13,14,15]. Children from minority, rural, and lower-income households experience multiple nutritional disparities in healthy food access and consumption [16]. Thus, children who attend these ECEs may greatly benefit from healthy eating interventions [17]. However, researchers face unique methodological challenges for evaluating the impact of healthy eating interventions for young children in these settings. Most notably, children are unable to reliably self-report their own dietary intake [18,19], and errors in proxy reporting from parents or ECE caregivers can lead to biased estimates of intake [20]. When multiple caregivers participate in child feeding during the day, such as teachers, parents, and other child caretakers, these errors may be amplified. Weighed food records (plate waste) are the gold standard for assessing actual dietary intake of individuals [21]; however, this method is labor intensive and can be cost-prohibitive for large-scale intervention studies [22,23]. In addition, weighed plate waste may not be sensitive enough to detect subtle changes in a child’s willingness to try (WTT) a food, which includes willingness to taste, but not necessarily consume the food. Although multiple tools have been developed to measure school-aged children’s food neophobia [24,25], most are self-administered questionnaires [26], which cannot be administered to pre-school children and may be subject to proxy-reporting errors. Furthermore, these tools have also not been developed or validated to measure changes over time. Hence, a valid tool is needed to assess pre-school children’s WTT vegetables to better include the range of possible interactions with these foods.
Although researchers have developed various self-reported WTT tools for school-aged children [26,27], only one assesses young children’s WTT in ECE environments. Farfan-Ramirez and colleagues (2011) developed a five-point observational checklist to measure observed willingness to try (FR-WTT) specific fruits and vegetables among three- to five-year-olds, which could serve as an evaluation alternative to weighed plate waste, as well as to assess changes in neophobic food behaviors over time. This FR-WTT tool [28] served as an evaluation method for the “Nutrition Matters!” curriculum to assess preschoolers’ WTT for select fruits and vegetables (figs, raspberries, blanched snap peas, and roasted beets) [28]. Although the FR-WTT method has good face validity and many presumed benefits relative to more intensive dietary assessment methods, no validation studies have been conducted among preschool-aged children in ECE settings. The primary aim of this study was to validate the FR-WTT observational method for assessing two behavioral measures of acceptance for six different vegetables, willingness to taste and dietary consumption, among preschool-aged children in rural, Indigenous ECE settings.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. FRESH Study Overview

This research was part of the Food Resource Equity and Sustainability for Health “FRESH” Study. We conducted this five-year community-based participatory research study collaboratively with the Osage Nation (ON), the Hudson College of Public Health at the University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center (OUHSC), and the Center for Indigenous Health Research and Policy at the Oklahoma State University Center for Health Sciences. The purpose was to address nutrition-related obesity disparities among Native American families living in rural Oklahoma with young children attending one of nine ON tribally operated ECEs, including four ON Head Start programs, four ON Wah-Zha-Zhi Early Learning Academies (WELAs), and one ON Language Immersion School (Daposka Ahnkodapi). The University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center Institutional Review Board (IRB) served as the IRB of record for the study, as per the request of the Osage Nation leadership, and approved all data collection procedures involved in this study. These analyses used the baseline data from the FRESH study.

2.2. Recruitment and Study Population

We implemented multiple strategies to recruit families with children enrolled at one of the nine ON ECE programs from August 2017 through to January 2018. Research staff recruited families at parent orientations, back-to-school nights, and at school drop-offs and pick-ups. Working with ECE leadership, we contacted remaining eligible adults via telephone to inform them about the study and invite them to participate. We distributed promotional study materials, such as a letter signed by the Osage Nation Chief, in children’s backpacks, through the mail and via email, and we posted the materials on school bulletin boards.
Households with one or more family members identifying as AI and having at least one child, aged three to six years, attending one of the nine participating ON ECE sites were eligible to enroll in the study. Following parental consent, we enrolled 176 children and at least one household caregiver (parent or guardian). We collected child dietary assessment measures, including weighed food consumption and FR-WTT observations, between December 2017 and February 2018 on 164 of the 176 (93%) enrolled children. We collected body composition measures between November 2017 and February 2018 on 159 children (90%). Household caregivers received compensation for their time for completing questionnaires regarding themselves and their children.

2.3. Demographics, Food Security and Biometric Measures

Upon enrollment, household caregivers completed questionnaires about their children and themselves. Questionnaire data used for analyses reported in this manuscript included demographics, such as the child’s age, sex, race/ethnicity, the highest education of the caregiver, caregiver’s employment, annual household income, as well as the United States Department of Agriculture (USDA) 18-item Household Food Security Survey module [29]. In addition, we followed a standard protocol to measure the child’s height and weight without shoes using the Hopkins Road Rod Portable Stadiometer (#680214) and Health o meter® 349KLX medical weight scale, respectively.

2.4. Selection of Test Vegetables

The FRESH study selected six target vegetables (carrots, spinach, tomatoes, lima beans, bell peppers, and butternut squash) as the focus vegetables of a future ECE gardening curriculum intervention. Vegetables were selected based on varying degree of novelty, regional availability, cultural preferences, and cost using participatory methods with tribal partners described elsewhere [30]. Menus for the nine ECE programs were prepared by two central planning groups, one for the Head Start programs and one for the WELA and Language Immersion schools. The relative novelty of each vegetable was determined by examining a six-week cycle menu for both central planning groups and one-week, on-site observations of foods served at lunch for each of the nine ECE schools prior to the intervention. Butternut squash, raw tomatoes, and lima beans were not documented on menus or observations at any site. It is worth noting that lima beans can be included in mixed vegetable blends, but no vegetable blends observed on-site included lima beans. Raw spinach was not documented on any menu but observed as part of a lettuce–spinach salad blend at one site during observation. Bell pepper was documented on only the WELA menu, but not observed at any site. Raw carrots were documented on menus and observations at all sites.

2.5. Weighed Plate Waste

We measured target vegetable consumption by weighed plate waste as the gold standard for objectively measuring food intake. Research staff were trained by the study’s research dietitian using a standardized written protocol. Trained university research staff numbered, weighed, and prepared vegetable containers with standardized pieces of the six target vegetables. Vegetables were portioned so that each vegetable variety visually occupied equal space in the container. Containers included three raw grape tomatoes (M = 23.2 g), three raw baby carrots (M = 29.9 g), five fresh spinach leaves (M = 5.1 g), five cubes of roasted butternut squash (M = 29.1 g), five cooked lima beans (M = 15.0 g), and three slices of raw green bell pepper (M = 22.3 g) along with a 2 oz. pre-packaged container of ranch dip. Trained research staff weighed each vegetable container prior to and after the addition of each vegetable and the ranch dip container. We used these serial weights to calculate an objectively measured pre-presentation weight for each vegetable before they were served to the child. We used digital scales (Tanita model KW-002) to obtain all the weights.
We recorded container numbers for each child as they were served to children in their classrooms. Containers were provided immediately preceding a routine morning snack, lunch, or an afternoon snack period, and all children had previously received at least one meal or snack by the school earlier in the school day. The meals provided by the program were in compliance with the United States Department of Agriculture Child and Adult Care Food Program and were all similar in nature. Breakfast consists of three food components and included milk, a grain or meat/meat alternate (i.e., eggs), and a fruit or vegetable. Lunch consists of five components and includes milk, grain, meat/meat alternate, fruit and vegetable (or double vegetable). After the vegetable presentation, trained researchers replaced any vegetable particles not consumed back into the vegetable container, such as vegetables that had dropped on the floor or that were on the table or in the child’s chair. Next, trained research staff weighed each vegetable container with all remaining contents, then re-weighed after removing each vegetable and unconsumed dip, starting with the unconsumed dip. We used these post-presentation serial weights to measure the post-presentation weights for each vegetable. We estimated the weight of each child’s vegetable consumption as the weight difference between pre-presentation and post-presentation weights for each vegetable type (e.g., Consumption weighttomatoes = pre-presentation weighttomatoes−post-presentation weighttomatoes) [31]. We estimated the total consumption weight for each child by summing the consumption weight of all six target vegetables.

2.6. Direct Observation of Willingness-to-Try Measures

All research staff conducting WTT observations had previously completed laboratory training and passed field observation testing for the Ball plate waste observation method [31], which required simultaneous observation of four preschool children by each researcher to visually estimate consumption of individual items from a mixed plate of food. For this study, these same research staff were oriented to the FR-WTT method by the research dietitian and trained using a written standardized protocol.
At the time that the containers were presented, the trained observers rated each child’s interaction with each vegetable in the vegetable container using the FR-WTT checklist [28]. The FR-WTT rating options are: (0) Did not remove vegetable from box, (1) removed food, but did not bring to nose/mouth, (2) removed food and brought to nose/mouth, but did not put food in mouth, (3) put food in mouth, but did not swallow food (including taking a bite and spitting it out or licking an item), (4) put food in mouth and swallowed [28]. The maximum ratio of observer to children was 1:4, which is consistent with ratios used in other direct observation methods of dietary intake in young children [31]. We calculated the total FR-WTT by adding the observer ratings (0–4) for each of the six vegetables, for a total possible score of 0–24 for all six vegetables.

2.7. Child Food Neophobia

Household caregivers completed the six-item Child Food Neophobia Scale (CFNS) for their children, which was adapted by Cooke and colleagues [32] from the original CFNS developed by Pliner [33]. A higher CNFS score has been associated with lower consumption of fruits and vegetables [6]. The CFNS asks caregivers to rate six of their child’s behaviors when presented with novel foods on a 4-point Likert scale. For example, the questionnaire asks household caregivers: “My child does not trust new foods”. The response options are: (1) Strongly disagree, (2) somewhat disagree, (3) somewhat agree, or (4) strongly agree with the statement. We calculated the total CFNS score by summing the individual responses (1–4) for all six questions included in the survey. The total scores ranged from 6 to 24, with higher scores indicating higher food neophobia.

2.8. Validation of FR-WTT Observational Method

We assessed the criterion validity of the FR-WTT observational scale using a combination of approaches. The first approach assessed whether children observed to eat the vegetable(s) had a consumption weight higher than those who were not observed to eat the vegetable(s). Specifically, children observed as not touching any of the vegetables (FR-WTT = 0), touching but not putting any vegetables in their mouth (FR-WTT = 1 or 2), or putting a vegetable in their mouth and spitting it out (FR-WTT = 3) would have a negligible measured weighed vegetable consumption. In contrast, children observed putting a vegetable in their mouth and swallowing (FR-WTT = 4) would have weighed vegetable consumption above zero. Therefore, to determine if the mean consumption weights differed significantly among children, we observed consuming each vegetable compared to children who did not; we re-categorized FR-WTT into two categories—consuming the vegetables (WTT = 4) and not consuming the vegetables (WTT = 0, 1, 2, or 3). We hypothesized consumption weights would be significantly higher for children with a FR-WTT of 4 compared to those with a FR-WTT of 0–3.
Secondly, since the CFNS is designed to identify children reluctant to try new foods and most young children infrequently eat a variety of vegetables in the US [34], we assessed convergent validity of the FR-WTT with the CFNS using correlations between the FR-WTT and the CFNS. Based on our literature review [1,6], we hypothesized children with higher food neophobia would be less likely to interact with and try the vegetables. Therefore, we expected to observe an inverse relationship between these two constructs.

2.9. Statistical Analyses

We used Statistical Analysis Software package (version 9.4, SAS Institute, Inc., Cary, NC, USA, 2016) to conduct statistical analyses. We estimated Body Mass Index (BMI) percentiles based on growth charts from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [35]. We defined child obesity as above the 95th percentile and overweight was defined as above the 85th and below the 95th percentile [35]. To describe continuous variables, we calculated means and standard deviations, and calculated percentages for categorical variables.
We used single-family analysis of variance (ANOVA) to determine if the mean vegetable weight consumption differed between children who did (FR-WTT = 4) and did not consume the vegetables (FR-WTT = 0–3). We assessed the criterion validity of the FR-WTT scale by assessing the correlation between the FR-WTT scores and the CFNS scores. Prior to assessing this convergent validity, we assessed the correlation between CFNS and weighed plate waste to confirm an inverse relationship of child neophobia and food consumption, as reported in previous literature [7,8]. Additionally, we examined the internal consistency of the FR-WTT scale using a split sample. We split vegetables into two similar groups with one red/orange vegetable (tomatoes or carrots), one green vegetable (peppers or spinach), and one cooked vegetable (squash or beans); then, we summed the WTT scores for each group. Next, we examined the correlation between the group WTT scores as a measure of internal consistency. Since the FR-WTT scores did not meet the assumptions for the Pearson correlation, we used Spearman correlations for all correlation analyses.
Finally, to examine the robustness of these analyses, we performed four sets of sensitivity analyses. First, to adjust for measurement error of consumption weights, we adjusted all negative vegetable consumption weights to zero and re-assessed the criterion validity of the FR-WTT with these adjusted consumption weights. Next, we performed three sets of analyses stratified by BMI, age, and food security groups to assess whether relationships were similar in these subgroups. We used single-family ANOVA to compare FR-WTT with consumption weights and CFNS in these stratified analyses.

3. Results

3.1. Participant Demographics, Food Security and Biometric Measures

A total of 164 children and their household caregivers enrolled in the study and completed the questionnaire. The mean age of the children included in our study was 4.7 years (SD = 0.8), 56% were female, 71% were Native American, 18% were overweight and 18% were obese. Among the adults caring for the children in our study, 44% had a high school education, 72% were employed, and 29% had a household income of USD 20,000 or less. In addition, 39% of children were living in households with low or very low food security and 17% were living with low or very low child food security (Table 1).

3.2. Description of Weighted Plate Waste, FR-WTT and CFNS

For this study population, the total mean vegetable consumption was 22.2 g (SD = 21.8 g). On average, children consumed over 40% of the presented carrots (M = 12.6 g, SD = 12.8 g) over 20% of the presented spinach (M = 1.3 g, SD = 2.9 g) and tomatoes (M = 4.9 g, SD = 7.9 g), less than 10% of the presented beans (M = 1.9 g, SD = 4.5 g) and peppers (M = 1.4 g, SD = 1.9 g), with minimal consumption of the presented squash (Figure 1).
The mean total FR-WTT score was 9.3 (SD = 7.3, range = 0–24). We observed only 5% of children placing all six vegetables into their mouths and swallowing and a little over one-quarter of children (30%) did not touch any of the vegetables. Figure 2 displays the distribution of children’s willingness-to-try behaviors according to the FR-WTT for each vegetable. More children were willing to try carrots (70%) and spinach (42%) by at least putting these vegetables in their mouths (FR-WTT scores of 3 or 4). Children were least willing to try squash and beans with 64% and 62% not removing these vegetables from the vegetable snack box, respectively.
The mean CFNS score reported by household caregivers was 14.0 (SD = 5.0, median = 14.0, range: 6–24) among the 146 children with CFNS scores. Seventeen children (12%) were rated at the lowest possible score on the CFNS scale (CFNS = 6) by their household caregiver (i.e., parents indicated that their children were willing to try new foods), 43% were rated with low neophobia (CFNS = 7–14) and 45% were rated with high neophobia (CFNS = 15–24) by household caregivers (i.e., parents indicated that their children were not willing to try new foods).
We did not find any differences in measures of interest (average vegetable weighed plate waste, total FR-WTT score, and total CFNS score) according to household caregiver race/ethnicity, child race/ethnicity, obesity, or household demographics (Table S1). Additionally, there were no differences in total CFNS score by the child’s age. However, older children consumed more vegetables by weight (t(df) = 2.97(161), p < 0.01) and had higher total FR-WTT scores (t(df) = 2.33(161), p = 0.02).

3.3. Criterion and Convergent Validity of Willingness-to-Try Method

Table 2 presents how the mean consumption weights differed among children who were observed consuming each vegetable versus those observed not consuming vegetables. Mean consumption weights among children observed swallowing the vegetables ranged between 3 and 19 g, while mean weights for children not removing vegetables were negligible.
Furthermore, correlation results to assess convergent validity of the FR-WTT with the CFNS score supported moderate to weak inverse correlations (Table 3) for all vegetables except beans. Proxy-reported CFNS scores were moderately and inversely correlated with total FR-WTT scores and with FR-WTT scores for peppers and carrots. CFNS scores were weakly and inversely correlated with FR-WTT scores for tomatoes, squash, and spinach. Additionally, the results of the total CFNS score were inversely and moderately correlated with total vegetable consumption.
In addition, the internal consistency was high for the FR-WTT scale based on a split sample analysis. The FR-WTT scores for two groups of vegetables, which each included a red or orange vegetable, a green vegetable, and a cooked vegetable, were highly correlated with a correlation coefficient of 0.73 (p < 0.0001).

3.4. Sensitivity Analysis by BMI and Food Security

We repeated these analyses to assess the validity of the FR-WTT among subgroups of children, including children with BMI classified as overweight/obese and normal, children in households experiencing food insecurity and no food insecurity, and older and younger children. The results of these sensitivity analysis found comparable correlations between FR-WTT and measured vegetable consumption weights between groups (Tables S1–S3). Total correlations for FR-WTT and vegetable consumption and for FR-WTT and CFNS scales were slightly stronger for obese/overweight children, for children living in food-insecure households, and for older children.

4. Discussion

Early childhood is a critical time for the development of healthy eating preferences, which has led public health researchers and interventionists to focus on this developmental period for the narrowing of nutrition-related intergenerational health disparities among marginalized populations. However, evaluating these interventions presents unique challenges, since most validated dietary assessment tools require participants to accurately recall and quantify food consumption. The validation of early childhood dietary assessment measures is essential for the advancement of evidence-based strategies that promote the willingness to try and, ultimately, the consumption of vegetables, a learned food preference in young children.
The present study establishes the FR-WTT is a valid method to assess willingness-to-try behaviors and consumption of a variety of vegetables. We established criterion validity of the FR-WTT with significant and moderate to strong positive correlations with the gold standard of weighed consumption for five of the six vegetables presented, which supports the FR-WTT’s use in the evaluation of WTT behaviors targeted at nutrition interventions for children aged three to six years old. The FR-WTT also offers several advantages for weighed plate waste, including potential to capture discrete changes in willingness to try over time and lower research burden.
Further, our analyses support the convergent construct validity of the total FR-WTT score as an observational method for identifying young children with food neophobia. FR-WTT was negatively correlated with CFNS scores reported by the household caregiver. Although these correlations were not strong, there may be multiple reasons for these findings. First, CFNS measures the construct of general willingness to try new foods, while the FR-WTT measures the child’s willingness to try specific vegetables at one point in time. The weak yet significant inverse correlations between CFNS and vegetable intake observed in our study are consistent with correlations in a previous study [6].
Sensitivity analyses additionally demonstrated that FR-WTT similarly correlates with vegetable intake among children with different BMIs, different household food security statuses, and older and younger preschool children. Given the vegetable intake disparities documented in both childhood obesity [36,37,38] and child food insecurity [39,40,41,42], researchers should consider the FR-WTT as a valid measure for these special populations.
Our research experience implementing the FR-WTT also supports its benefit as a lower-cost alternative to weighed plate waste. Although this FR-WTT method requires trained observers to evaluate children’s eating behaviors, it does not require pre- and post-weighing of each food item. Weighed plate waste methods are resource intensive, requiring a substantial amount of staff time and expensive scales to ensure accuracy. In addition, the FR-WTT scale has the ability to identify children who would only touch or taste (but not eat) vegetables, which cannot be determined through weighed plate waste.
This study has some limitations. Although we did not measure hunger for each child, all the children were provided breakfast or a morning snack at all ON ECE schools, so the children were presumed to have a similar level of hunger when the target vegetables were presented just prior to their usual snack or lunch. We were unable to measure the inter-rater reliability or test–retest reliability of the FR-WTT scale in the field due to the additional burden on the ECE sites and staffing requirements. However, we found relatively high internal consistency using a split half design with two groups of similar vegetables. Another limitation was that consumption weights for some target vegetables for a limited number of children had negative values, which indicates a lack of precision in the weighing procedures, possibly due to field data collection, and can introduce additional sources of error. However, only 15% of the weighed plate waste records had any negative consumption weights with the majority (76%) under one gram. Additionally, we performed sensitivity analysis using adjusted weights by setting negative weights to zero and found comparable correlations between the FR-WTT and vegetable consumption weights. Since we collected these study data in a community-based setting, vegetable containers were transported to and from the feeding site in coolers. However, some temperature changes may have occurred to the vegetables in transit that could affect weights (e.g., water evaporation from or absorption by vegetables). Potential moisture absorption may explain the negative consumption weights observed for the cooked butternut squash and the observed weaker correlations between the FR-WTT and consumption weights for this vegetable. Finally, the school menu review confirmed that all but one or two target vegetables (carrots for all sites, bell pepper only for WELA sites) were novel within the children’s school food environment. We did not capture information regarding whether any of the target vegetables were familiar to the children in their home environments, which could have been informative when assessing the correlations between the FR-WTT and the CFNS, since neophobia is the avoidance or reluctance to eat unfamiliar foods. If some children with high CFNS were more familiar with one or more of the target vegetables, their FR-WTT score for that vegetable may not correlate as highly with their CFNS. Despite our inability to identify familiarity with target vegetables at home, CFNS scores were moderately or weakly inversely correlated with the FR-WTT scores, which is similar to correlations of willingness to try novel foods and child food neophobia scales reported in a review article [43].
Despite these limitations, this study establishes that the FR-WTT is a valid method to assess willingness-to-try behaviors and consumption of a variety of vegetables in pre-school children. The study also contributes to the significant lack of high-quality, validated data available for low-income racial/ethnic communities, specifically Native Americans, and is the first of its kind to be implemented in a reservation setting. Further research should determine the predictive validity of FR-WTT score on total daily vegetable intake and overall dietary quality, since this study only evaluated the FR-WTT in estimating vegetable intake during a single eating period. Inclusion of children’s prior exposure to selected novel vegetables and parental feeding practices, specifically regarding encouragement to try new foods and vegetables, would be prudent to include in future studies examining WTT and dietary intake. The current literature shows that repeated exposure to vegetables as snacks is effective in increasing total vegetable intake in children [44,45,46].

5. Conclusions

The FR-WTT observational scale is a valid method for assessing vegetable intake among pre-school children during a single eating period. This study supports its use in the evaluation of community-based interventions designed to assess young children’s willingness to try and, ultimately, consume more vegetables.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/nu14010058/s1, Table S1a: Sensitivity analysis of mean consumption of vegetables during observations by Farfan-Ramirez willingness-to-try (FR-WTT) categories by child’s Body Mass Index (BMI), Table S1b: Sensitivity analysis of criterion and convergent validity stratified by child’s Body Mass Index (BMI). Correlations between Farfan-Ramirez willingness-to-try (FR-WTT) scale and the Child Food Neophobia Scale (CFNS), Table S2a: Sensitivity analysis of mean consumption of vegetables during observations by Farfan-Ramirez willingness-to-try (FR-WTT) categories by household food security, Table S2b: Sensitivity analysis of criterion and convergent validity stratified by household food security. Correlations between Farfan-Ramirez willingness-to-try (FR-WTT) scale and the Child Food Neophobia Scale (CFNS), Table S3a: Sensitivity analysis of mean consumption of vegetables during observations by Farfan-Ramirez willingness-to-try (FR-WTT) categories by child’s age, Table S3b: Sensitivity analysis of criterion and convergent validity stratified by child’s age. Correlations between Farfan-Ramirez willingness-to-try (FR-WTT) scale and the Child Food Neophobia Scale (CFNS).

Author Contributions

M.S.W. and M.B.W. contributed equally as first authors on this paper. Both M.S.W. and M.B.W. conceived and designed the analysis. M.S.W. contributed to the introduction, methods, and discussion sections. M.B.W. contributed to the methods, results, and discussion sections. J.R. performed data analysis, prepared data tables, and contributed to the methods and results narrative. T.T. contributed to the methods narrative and assisted with the literature review. S.B.S. contributed to study design and methodology, interpretation, and revising the manuscript in its entirety. A.D.M.-L. assisted with the literature review and contributed to the introduction. C.V.L. supervised all data collection and contributed to the introduction, methods, and results sections of the manuscript. V.B.B.J. designed and directed the FRESH study, oversaw all aspects of its implementation, and assisted in the review and editing of the manuscript. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was funded by the National Institute on Minority Health and Health Disparities (R01MD011266). The funding agency did not participate in the study design, data collection, analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Oklahoma Health Sciences Center (protocol #6531; initial approval date of 5 June 2016), as per the request of the Osage Nation leadership.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all parents of subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The de-identified data presented in this study are available on reasonable request from the corresponding authors and can only be released following permission from the senior author and Osage Nation. The data are not publicly available to protect privacy of tribal citizen human subject participants.

Acknowledgments

We thank the leaders and citizens of the Osage Nation for their participation in and continued support of the Food Resource Equity and Sustainability for Health (FRESH) Study. We also thank the Osage Nation Executive Committee who served as the guiding body throughout the duration of the FRESH study and their commitment to address diet-related chronic disease in the young families in their communities through participation in the committee and numerous other endeavors. We also thank the teachers, school site managers, cooks, and other staff for their support of this project and their continued commitment to improving the health of the children in their care. We thank all research study staff who contributed to the data collection efforts throughout the project both at the University of Oklahoma and the Oklahoma State University. This manuscript has been reviewed by the Osage Nation and approved to publish; we thank them for their input and approval.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Birch, L.L. Development of food preferences. Annu. Rev. Nutr. 1999, 19, 41–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Birch, L.L. Development of food acceptance patterns in the first years of life. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 1998, 57, 617–624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Birch, L.L.; Fisher, J.O. Development of eating behaviors among children and adolescents. Pediatrics 1998, 101, 539–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Mennella, J.A.; Bobowski, N.K. The sweetness and bitterness of childhood: Insights from basic research on taste preferences. Physiol. Behav. 2015, 152, 502–507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Addessi, E.; Galloway, A.T.; Visalberghi, E.; Birch, L.L. Specific social influences on the acceptance of novel foods in 2–5-year-old children. Appetite 2005, 45, 264–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Cooke, L.; Wardle, J.; Gibson, E.L. Relationship between parental report of food neophobia and everyday food consumption in 2–6-year-old children. Appetite 2003, 41, 205–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Lafraire, J.; Rioux, C.; Giboreau, A.; Picard, D. Food rejections in children: Cognitive and social/environmental factors involved in food neophobia and picky/fussy eating behavior. Appetite 2016, 96, 347–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Falciglia, G.A.; Couch, S.C.; Gribble, L.S.; Pabst, S.M.; Frank, R. Food neophobia in childhood affects dietary variety. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2000, 100, 1474–1481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Birch, L.L.; McPhee, L.; Shoba, B.C.; Pirok, E.; Steinberg, L. What kind of exposure reduces children’s food neophobia? Looking vs. tasting. Appetite 1987, 9, 171–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Wardle, J.; Cooke, L.J.; Gibson, E.L.; Sapochnik, M.; Sheiham, A.; Lawson, M. Increasing children’s acceptance of vegetables; a randomized trial of parent-led exposure. Appetite 2003, 40, 155–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Wardle, J.; Carnell, S.; Cooke, L. Parental control over feeding and children’s fruit and vegetable intake: How are they related? J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2005, 105, 227–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Corcoran, L.; Steinley, K. Early Childhood Program Participation, From the National Household Education Surveys Program of 2016 (NCES 2017-101.REV); National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education: Washington, DC, USA, 2019. Available online: http://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch (accessed on 11 November 2021).
  13. Benjamin Neelon, S.E.; Briley, M.E. Position of the American Dietetic Association: Benchmarks for nutrition in child care. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2011, 111, 607–615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Robson, S.M.; Khoury, J.C.; Kalkwarf, H.J.; Copeland, K. Dietary intake of children attending full-time child care: What are they eating away from the child-care center? J. Acad. Nutr. Diet. 2015, 115, 1472–1478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Sisson, S.B.; Kiger, A.C.; Anundson, K.C.; Rasbold, A.H.; Krampe, M.; Campbell, J.; DeGrace, B.; Hoffman, L. Differences in preschool-age children’s dietary intake between meals consumed at childcare and at home. Prev. Med. Rep. 2017, 6, 33–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Larson, N.; Story, M. Barriers to Equity in Nutritional Health for U.S. Children and Adolescents: A Review of the Literature. Curr. Nutr. Rep. 2015, 4, 102–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Ward, D.S.; Welker, E.; Choate, A.; Henderson, K.E.; Lott, M.; Tovar, A.; Wilson, A.; Sallis, J.F. Strength of obesity prevention interventions in early care and education settings: A systematic review. Prev. Med. 2017, 95, S37–S52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Livingstone, M.B.; Robson, P.J.; Wallace, J.M. Issues in dietary intake assessment of children and adolescents. Br. J. Nutr. 2004, 92 (Suppl 2), S213–S222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Magarey, A.; Watson, J.; Golley, R.K.; Burrows, T.; Sutherland, R.; McNaughton, S.A.; Denney-Wilson, E.; Campbell, K.; Collins, C. Assessing dietary intake in children and adolescents: Considerations and recommendations for obesity research. Int. J. Pediatr. Obes. 2011, 6, 2–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Baranowski, T. 24-Hour Recall and Diet Record Methods. In Nutritional Epidemiology; Willett, W.C., Ed.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 50–69. [Google Scholar]
  21. National Cancer Institute. Dietary Assessment Primer, Principles Underlying Recommendations. Available online: https://dietassessmentprimer.cancer.gov/approach/principles.html (accessed on 16 December 2019).
  22. Thompson, F.E.; Byers, T. Dietary assessment resource manual. J. Nutr. 1994, 124, 2245s–2317s. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Institute of Medicine Committee on Dietary Risk Assessment in the WIC Program. Dietary Risk Assessment in the WIC Program. In Food-Based Assessment of Dietary Intake; National Academies Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  24. Hollar, D.; Paxton-Aiken, A.; Fleming, P. Exploratory validation of the Fruit and Vegetable Neophobia Instrument among third-to fifth-grade students. Appetite 2013, 60, 226–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Loewen, R.; Pliner, P. The Food Situations Questionnaire: A measure of children’s willingness to try novel foods in stimulating and non-stimulating situations. Appetite 2000, 35, 239–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Thomson, J.L.; McCabe-Sellers, B.J.; Strickland, E.; Lovera, D.; Nuss, H.J.; Yadrick, K.; Duke, S.; Bogle, M.L. Development and evaluation of WillTry. An instrument for measuring children’s willingness to try fruits and vegetables. Appetite 2010, 54, 465–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Kaiser, L.L.; Schneider, C.; Mendoza, C.; George, G.; Neelon, M.; Roche, B.; Ginsburg, D. Development and use of an evaluation tool for taste-testing activities by school-aged children. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet. 2012, 112, 2028–2034. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Farfan-Ramirez, L.; Diemoz, L.; Gong, E.J.; Lagura, M.A. Curriculum intervention in preschool children: Nutrition Matters! J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2011, 43, S162–S165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. USDA Economic Research Service. U.S. Adult Food Security Survey Module: Three-Stage Design, with Screeners; U.S. Department of Agriculture: Honolulu, HI, USA, 2012.
  30. Wetherill, M.S.; Bourque, E.E.; Taniguchi, T.; Love, C.V.; Sisk, M.; Jernigan, V.B.B. Development of a Tribally-led Gardening Curriculum for Indigenous Preschool Children: The FRESH Study. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2021, 53, 991–995. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Bollella, M.C.; Boccia, L.A.; Nicklas, T.A.; Lefkowitz, K.B.; Pittman, B.P.; Zang, E.A.; Williams, C.L. Assessing dietary intake in preschool children: The healthy start project-New York. Nutr. Res. 1999, 19, 37–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Cooke, L.; Carnell, S.; Wardle, J. Food neophobia and mealtime food consumption in 4–5 year old children. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2006, 3, 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Pliner, P.; Hobden, K. Development of a scale to measure the trait of food neophobia in humans. Appetite 1992, 19, 105–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Johnson, S.L. Developmental and Environmental Influences on Young Children’s Vegetable Preferences and Consumption. Adv. Nutr. 2016, 7, 220S–231S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Kuczmarski, R.J.; Ogden, C.L.; Guo, S.S.; Grummer-Strawn, L.M.; Flegal, K.M.; Mei, Z.; Wei, R.; Curtin, L.R.; Roche, A.F.; Johnson, C.L. 2000 CDC Growth Charts for the United States: Methods and development. Vital Health Stat. 2002, 11, 1–190. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  36. Saavedra, J.M.; Deming, D.; Dattilo, A.; Reidy, K. Lessons from the feeding infants and toddlers study in North America: What children eat, and implications for obesity prevention. Ann. Nutr. Metab. 2013, 62 (Suppl 3), 27–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Field, A.E.; Gillman, M.W.; Rosner, B.; Rockett, H.R.; Colditz, G.A. Association between fruit and vegetable intake and change in body mass index among a large sample of children and adolescents in the United States. Int. J. Obes. Relat. Metab. Disord. 2003, 27, 821–826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Miller, P.; Moore, R.H.; Kral, T.V. Children’s daily fruit and vegetable intake: Associations with maternal intake and child weight status. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2011, 43, 396–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Dave, J.M.; Evans, A.E.; Saunders, R.P.; Watkins, K.W.; Pfeiffer, K.A. Associations among food insecurity, acculturation, demographic factors, and fruit and vegetable intake at home in Hispanic children. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2009, 109, 697–701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Fram, M.S.; Ritchie, L.D.; Rosen, N.; Frongillo, E.A. Child experience of food insecurity is associated with child diet and physical activity. J. Nutr. 2015, 145, 499–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Eicher-Miller, H.A.; Zhao, Y. Evidence for the age-specific relationship of food insecurity and key dietary outcomes among US children and adolescents. Nutr. Res. Rev. 2018, 31, 98–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Canter, K.S.; Roberts, M.C.; Davis, A.M. The role of health behaviors and food insecurity in predicting fruit and vegetable intake in low-income children. Child. Health Care 2017, 46, 131–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Dovey, T.M.; Staples, P.A.; Gibson, E.L.; Halford, J.C. Food neophobia and ’picky/fussy’ eating in children: A review. Appetite 2008, 50, 181–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Ahern, S.M.; Caton, S.J.; Blundell-Birtill, P.; Hetherington, M.M. The effects of repeated exposure and variety on vegetable intake in pre-school children. Appetite 2019, 132, 37–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Caton, S.J.; Ahern, S.M.; Remy, E.; Nicklaus, S.; Blundell, P.; Hetherington, M.M. Repetition counts: Repeated exposure increases intake of a novel vegetable in UK pre-school children compared to flavour-flavour and flavour-nutrient learning. Br. J. Nutr. 2013, 109, 2089–2097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Olsho, L.E.; Klerman, J.A.; Ritchie, L.; Wakimoto, P.; Webb, K.L.; Bartlett, S. Increasing Child Fruit and Vegetable Intake: Findings from the US Department of Agriculture Fresh Fruit and Vegetable Program. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet. 2015, 115, 1283–1290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Mean pre-consumption weights and mean consumption weights for each vegetable.
Figure 1. Mean pre-consumption weights and mean consumption weights for each vegetable.
Nutrients 14 00058 g001
Figure 2. Percent of preschool children observed willing to try six vegetables according to the Farfan-Ramirez willingness to try (FR-WTT) scale.
Figure 2. Percent of preschool children observed willing to try six vegetables according to the Farfan-Ramirez willingness to try (FR-WTT) scale.
Nutrients 14 00058 g002
Table 1. Descriptive summary of child, adult, and household characteristics (n = 164).
Table 1. Descriptive summary of child, adult, and household characteristics (n = 164).
CharacteristicNumber (%) a
Child characteristics
Age (M, SD)4.7 (0.8)
Sex
Male75 (46)
Female87 (56)
Race/ethnicity
Native American115 (71)
White/Caucasian only37 (23)
Other10 (6)
Body Mass Index Percentile
Above 95% (Obese)28 (18)
85–95% (Overweight)28 (18)
Below 85% (Normal or underweight)103 (65)
Child food neophobia score (M, SD)14.0 (5.0)
Adult and household characteristics
Caregiver highest education
Some high school/high school degree/GED70 (44)
Technical/Vocational/Associate’s degree50 (32)
Bachelor’s/Post-graduate degree38 (24)
Caregiver employment
Employed b114 (72)
Not employed14 (9)
Retired/student/homemaker30 (19)
Annual household income
USD 20,000 and under45 (29)
USD 20,001–35,00036 (23)
USD 35,001–50,00032 (21)
Over USD 50,00043 (28)
Household food security
Very low food security12 (8)
Low food security47 (31)
Marginal food security27 (18)
High food security65 (43)
Child food security
Low or very low child food security25 (17)
High or marginal child food security126 (83)
a Percentages adjusted for missing values. b Includes employed full time, employed part time, seasonally/occasionally employed, self-employed.
Table 2. Mean consumption of vegetables during observations by Farfan-Ramirez willingness-to-try (FR-WTT) categories.
Table 2. Mean consumption of vegetables during observations by Farfan-Ramirez willingness-to-try (FR-WTT) categories.
Vegetable FR-WTTnMean Consumption
in Grams 1 (SD)
p Value
Tomatoes FR-WTT = 4337.6 (7.6)<0.001
Tomatoes FR-WTT = 0, 1, 2, 31310.4 (0.9)
Carrots FR-WTT = 410818.9 (11.4)<0.001
Carrots FR-WTT = 0, 1, 2, 3560.4 (1.0)
Spinach FR-WTT = 4632.7 (2.2)<0.001
Spinach FR-WTT = 0, 1, 2, 31010.6 (0.9)
Beans FR-WTT = 4314.9 (5.0)<0.001
Beans FR-WTT = 0, 1, 2, 31330.5 (1.1)
Squash FR-WTT = 4254.1 (7.7)<0.001
Squash FR-WTT = 0, 1, 2, 3139−0.6 (1.1)
Peppers FR-WTT = 4337.6 (7.6)<0.001
Peppers FR-WTT = 0, 1, 2, 31310.5 (0.9)
1 Consumption values are rounded to one gram. Bold denotes significant p value < 0.05.
Table 3. Evaluation of correlations a between Farfan-Ramirez willingness-to-try (FR-WTT) vegetables and the Child Food Neophobia Scale (CFNS).
Table 3. Evaluation of correlations a between Farfan-Ramirez willingness-to-try (FR-WTT) vegetables and the Child Food Neophobia Scale (CFNS).
VegetableCorrelation a between FR-WTT and CFNS Scales
Coefficientp Value
Tomatoes−0.2786<0.01
Carrots−0.3004<0.01
Spinach−0.17620.03
Beans−0.09860.24
Squash−0.2454<0.01
Peppers−0.3259<0.001
Total−0.3268<0.001
a Spearman correlation. Bold denotes significant p value < 0.05.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Wetherill, M.S.; Williams, M.B.; Reese, J.; Taniguchi, T.; Sisson, S.B.; Malek-Lasater, A.D.; Love, C.V.; Jernigan, V.B.B. Methods for Assessing Willingness to Try and Vegetable Consumption among Children in Indigenous Early Childcare Settings: The FRESH Study. Nutrients 2022, 14, 58. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14010058

AMA Style

Wetherill MS, Williams MB, Reese J, Taniguchi T, Sisson SB, Malek-Lasater AD, Love CV, Jernigan VBB. Methods for Assessing Willingness to Try and Vegetable Consumption among Children in Indigenous Early Childcare Settings: The FRESH Study. Nutrients. 2022; 14(1):58. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14010058

Chicago/Turabian Style

Wetherill, Marianna S., Mary B. Williams, Jessica Reese, Tori Taniguchi, Susan B. Sisson, Adrien D. Malek-Lasater, Charlotte V. Love, and Valarie Blue Bird Jernigan. 2022. "Methods for Assessing Willingness to Try and Vegetable Consumption among Children in Indigenous Early Childcare Settings: The FRESH Study" Nutrients 14, no. 1: 58. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14010058

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop