Next Article in Journal
The Hepcidin-25/Ferritin Ratio Is Increased in University Rugby Players with Lower Fat Mass
Previous Article in Journal
Atractylodes chinensis Water Extract Ameliorates Obesity via Promotion of the SIRT1/AMPK Expression in High-Fat Diet-Induced Obese Mice
Previous Article in Special Issue
Fatty Acids Quality in Middle Eastern Traditional Dishes, Arabic Sweets and Market Foods Frequently Consumed in Lebanon
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Influence of Prenatal DHA Supplementation on Individual Domains of Behavioral Functioning in School-Aged Children: Follow-Up of a Randomized Controlled Trial

by
Jacqueline F. Gould
1,2,*,
Peter J. Anderson
3,4,
Lisa N. Yelland
1,5,
Robert A. Gibson
1,6 and
Maria Makrides
1,7
1
Women and Kids, South Australian Health and Medical Research Institute, 72 King William Road, Adelaide 5006, Australia
2
School of Psychology & Discipline of Paediatrics, Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences, The University of Adelaide, North Terrace, Adelaide 5000, Australia
3
Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, School of Psychological Sciences, Monash University, Melbourne 3800, Australia
4
Clinical Sciences, Murdoch Children’s Research Institute, Melbourne 3052, Australia
5
School of Public Health, Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences, The University of Adelaide, North Terrace, Adelaide 5000, Australia
6
School of Agriculture, Food and Wine, Waite Campus, University of Adelaide, Adelaide 5000, Australia
7
Discipline of Paediatrics, Faculty of Health and Medical Sciences, The University of Adelaide, North Terrace, Adelaide 5000, Australia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2021, 13(9), 2996; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13092996
Submission received: 29 July 2021 / Revised: 23 August 2021 / Accepted: 25 August 2021 / Published: 27 August 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Dietary Fat and Human Health Outcomes)

Abstract

:
Docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) accumulates in the fetal brain during pregnancy and is thought to have a role in supporting neurodevelopment. We conducted a multicenter, double-blind, randomized controlled trial in women with a singleton pregnancy who were <21 weeks’ gestation at trial entry. Women were provided with 800 mg DHA/day or a placebo supplement from trial entry until birth. When children reached seven years of age, we invited parents to complete the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ), the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF), and the Conners 3rd Edition Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) Index to assess child behavior and behavioral manifestations of executive dysfunction. There were 543 parent–child pairs (85% of those eligible) that participated in the follow-up. Scores were worse in the DHA group than the placebo group for the BRIEF Global Executive, Behavioral Regulation and Metacognition Indexes, and the Shift, Inhibit, Monitor, Working Memory, and Organization of Materials scales, as well as for the Conners 3 ADHD index, and the SDQ Total Difficulties score, Hyperactivity/Inattention score, and Peer Relationship Problems score. In this healthy, largely term-born sample of children, prenatal DHA supplementation conferred no advantage to childhood behavior, and instead appeared to have an adverse effect on behavioral functioning, as assessed by standardized parental report scales.

1. Introduction

The omega-3 long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid docosahexaenoic acid (DHA, 22:6n − 3) is known to accumulate in the fetal brain in the last trimester of pregnancy [1,2]. DHA is thought to be critical for appropriate neurodevelopment [3,4,5], which occurs at a rapid rate over the last trimester of pregnancy [6]. Both animal models [7,8,9] and observational studies in humans [10,11,12,13,14] have suggested neurodevelopmental benefits for the offspring after ensuring adequate dietary DHA during pregnancy. Numerous randomized controlled trials (RCTs) have attempted to evaluate the effect of providing supplemental DHA to pregnant women on child brain development through performance-based measures such as intelligence quotient tests. Multiple reviews of these RCTs have concluded very little evidence of an effect of a DHA intervention on cognition [15,16,17,18], executive functioning (higher-order cognitive abilities) [15,18,19], motor [15,16,17,18], or language abilities [15,16,17,18,20]; however, a recent review suggested some behavioral effects [21].
Behavioral functioning can be broadly dichotomized into internalizing or externalizing behaviors [22]. Internalizing behaviors reflect the internal psychological environment in terms of emotions and thoughts where problems may result in a child being overly shy or anxious, withdrawing socially, having poor self-esteem, or being less interested in social, academic, or recreational activities [23,24]. Internalizing behavior disorders include depression, anxiety, dissociative disorders, and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Externalizing problems are behaviors or actions that are outwardly expressed towards the environment or other individuals, such as hyperactivity and impulsivity, verbal and physical aggression, opposition to authority, inattention, poor inhibition, destruction or theft of others property, and poor temper control [23,24]. Externalizing behavior problems are considered disruptive disorders and include ADHD, and issues with conduct such as oppositional defiant disorder.
Whilst behavioral functioning has a complex etiology, DHA has been highlighted as potentially influential [25,26,27,28,29,30,31]. Case–control studies have revealed lower DHA status among children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder [32] and ADHD [33] when compared with controls. Associations between low cord blood DHA status (reflecting DHA exposure in late pregnancy) and increased likelihood of ADHD symptoms (externalizing behavior) were reported in one observational study [34], and were reported contrastingly with internalizing behaviors but not externalizing behaviors in another study [35]. High seafood intake, the predominant source of dietary DHA, during pregnancy was associated with reduced hyperactivity (externalizing behavior) at 9 years of age [10], while low seafood intake was associated with poorer prosocial behavior in children at 8 years of age [11].
Despite positive associations detected in observational studies, RCTs of DHA supplementation have not clearly demonstrated benefits of DHA interventions for behavioral functioning [16,21]. One of the largest RCTs of prenatal DHA supplementation included assessments of child behavior at 1.5 [36], 4 [37], and 7 years of age [38] through standardized parent rating scales. At 1.5 years of age, parents completed the Social-Emotional and Adaptive-Behavior scales of the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, Third Edition [36]. There was no effect of the DHA intervention on either of the overall scale scores, although there was an unexpected sex by treatment interaction where females in the DHA group had poorer adaptive behavior scores compared with females in the placebo group [36]. When parents rated behavior again at 4 years of age on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) and the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF), Preschool Edition, children in the DHA group were reported to exhibit more hyperactivity and inattention (symptoms of externalizing behaviors) as well as worse emergent metacognition and ability to plan and organize compared with children in the placebo group [37]. Scores reflecting internalizing behaviors did not differ between the randomization groups [37]. When the children reached 7 years of age, parents again completed the SDQ and BRIEF as well as a measure of symptoms of ADHD (indicating poor externalizing behavior).
Given that DHA is thought to be beneficial for brain development in general [10,11,12,13,14], alongside suggestions that DHA supplementation may reduce externalizing behavioral problems such as ADHD [29,39] or oppositional behaviors [40], it is important to further explore the unexpected effects on behavior and whether there are effects on global behavior or specific behavioral domains. The assessments of behavior in this large prenatal DHA RCT offer an ideal opportunity to explore the effect of the intervention on numerous domains of behavioral development, as well as the severity of any effect. By 7 years of age, behavioral functioning is reasonably stable [22,41] and, if problematic, may start to adversely affect schooling [42,43,44,45,46,47], and health-related quality of life [48], as well as being predictive of employment and income in adulthood [49]. Only overall summary scores were reported from this study, all of which revealed more behavioral symptoms in the DHA group compared with the placebo group [38]. Summary scores reflect overall functioning (combining internalizing and externalizing behaviors), and do not indicate whether effects are limited to a specific domain [50]. To better understand the pattern of behavioral problems related to DHA supplementation, analysis of the individual clinical scales is necessary. Furthermore, as only mean scores were compared between randomization groups, it is impossible to determine whether there was an increased risk of children being categorized as possibly having dysfunctional behavior requiring follow-up. The objective of the current study was to determine the effect of prenatal DHA supplementation on the individual scales of the behavioral assessments completed at 7 years of age, particularly measures of externalizing behavior, in addition to whether any differences resulted in behavioral symptoms that could be considered clinically dysfunctional. Based on the behavioral assessments in this cohort at 1.5 [36] and 4 [37] years of age, this study explored the hypothesis that effects of DHA would be restricted to externalizing behaviors.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Methods and results for the DHA to Optimize Maternal Infant Outcome (DOMInO) trial and follow-up studies are published [36,38,51,52]. To summarize, women were eligible to participate in this multicenter, parallel, double-blind, randomized controlled trial if they had a singleton pregnancy less than 21 weeks’ gestation. Exclusion criteria were current use of a DHA supplement or anticoagulant therapy, bleeding disorders (contraindicating fish oil consumption), history of drug or alcohol abuse, current participation in another fatty acid trial, inability to provide written informed consent, known major fetal abnormality, and any language but English being the main language spoken at home. A computer-driven telephone randomization service employing an independently generated randomization schedule was used to allocate women to receive three capsules of either DHA-rich fish oil (800 mg DHA/day and 100 mg/d eicosapentaenoic acid; Incromega 500 TG, Croda Chemicals, East Yorkshire, England) or vegetable oil daily from trial entry until delivery [36]. The randomization schedule used balanced variable-sized blocks with stratification by enrolment (medical) center and parity (first birth vs. subsequent birth) to ensure approximately equal numbers in each randomization group. All investigators and study staff were blinded to group allocation throughout the trial and follow-ups. Women could request knowledge of their group allocation from an independent statistician once the primary outcome analyses from the 1.5-year assessments were complete, although the majority (92%) remained blinded throughout the follow-up studies to 7 years of age. Women who requested to know their group allocation were instructed to keep this knowledge confidential, and not to discuss this with study staff.
A total of 2399 women from five hospitals were recruited to the trial. There were 726 mother–child pairs (DHA group n = 351, placebo n = 375) from two hospitals selected a priori for long-term neurodevelopmental follow-up, including all children born preterm (n = 96) and a random selection of children born full-term (n = 630) [36]. By the 7-year follow-up, there were 638 mother–child pairs (DHA group n = 310, placebo n = 328) remaining in the study (who had not previously withdrawn from the DOMInO trial or died). Families were invited to the follow-up prior to the child’s seventh birthday. Consenting families attended an appointment at a hospital clinic room (at the Women’s and Children’s Hospital or the Flinders Medical Centre in Adelaide, South Australia) or a local community center (such as a library or school) and were assessed with a battery of developmental assessments and parents completed questionnaires [38,51]. If this was not possible, children were assessed at home.
Parents completed a hard (paper) copy of each behavioral questionnaire whilst their child was undergoing the assessments in a different room. If preferred, parents could complete the questionnaires through interview with study staff at the appointment, or over the phone. The results of the developmental assessments, as well as the overall summary scores from the parent-completed questionnaires have been previously published [38].
All procedures were approved by an overseeing Human Research Ethics Committee. The original trial was reviewed and approved by the ethics committee for the Children, Youth, and Women’s Health Service (approval code REC1657/12/2007, approved 6 September 2005) and the 7-year follow-up was reviewed and approved by the committee of the Women’s and Children’s Health Network (approval code REC2526/12/15;HREC/12/WCHN/112, approved 4 February 2013). Mothers provided written informed consent prior to enrolment in the DOMInO Trial, and a legal guardian provided written informed consent on behalf of the child for the 7-year assessments. Pregnant women were originally enrolled into the DOMInO trial between October 2005 and January 2008, and the 7-year assessments were completed between March 2013 and August 2015.

2.2. Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)

The SDQ is a 25-item report of symptoms of behavioral problems as well as strengths, with parents rating each item on a -point scale [53]. There are five scales, four of which assess problematic behaviors. The Conduct Problems scale assesses behaviors that indicate a child does not conform to societal behavioral expectations or respect the rights of others, such as disobedience, fighting with other children, lying, and stealing. The Hyperactivity/Inattention scale reflects symptoms of ADHD, for example distractibility, excessive fidgeting, poor concentration and attention span, impulsivity, and restlessness. The Emotional Symptoms scale captures mood and emotional reactions disproportionate to the stimulus, such as fearfulness, anxiousness, low confidence, and unhappiness. The Peer Relationship Problems scale assesses the ability to form relationships with other children their age, such as having friends their own age, being liked by other children, and preferring to play with other children rather than solitary play. The fifth scale, the Prosocial Behavior scale, measures strengths in terms of voluntary consideration of others, for example helpfulness, kindness, and thoughtful actions towards others.
The Conduct Problems and Hyperactivity/Inattention scales reflect externalizing behaviors, whilst the Emotional Symptoms and Peer Relationship Problems scales indicate internalizing behavioral problems [54]. The four behavior problem scales are summarized in an overall Total Difficulties Score [53]. An Impact score also captures whether behavioral difficulties are perceived to have an adverse impact on the child or their family.
SDQ scores are not age standardized and higher scores on any scale indicate more perceived behavioral problems, with the exception of the Prosocial Behavior scale, where higher scores indicate more strengths in this area [53]. A Total Difficulties Score >16 is considered abnormal and warrants further clinical investigation for a possible behavioral problem. Among the scales, scores are categorized as abnormal when they are >4 for Emotional Symptoms, >3 for Conduct Problems, >6 for Hyperactivity/Inattention, >3 for Peer Problems, and <5 for Prosocial Behavior. A score > 1 on the Impact scale is also considered to be elevated.

2.3. Conners 3rd Edition ADHD (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Version IV) Index (Conners 3TM AI-Parent)

The Conners 3TM AI-parent is a questionnaire capturing symptoms of ADHD, as defined by the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders version IV [55]. The Conners 3TM AI-parent is an abbreviated, 10-item version of a larger questionnaire and as such does not contain any subscales that could be explored here. The Conners 3TM AI-parent generates one overall score that is age standardized to a mean of 50 and standard deviation of 10. Higher scores are suggest more symptoms and a score ≥70 (in the very elevated range) is indicative of possible ADHD.

2.4. Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF)

The BRIEF is an 86-item report of behavioral manifestations of executive dysfunction [56]. Parents rate each item on a three-point scale and higher scores indicate more perceived problems. Scores are normed for age to a mean of 50 and standard deviation of 10. Higher scores reflect poorer perceived behavior and scores ≥ 65 are considered indicative of a dysfunction.
The BRIEF generates an overall Global Executive Composite score, as well as the Behavioral Regulation Index (BRI) and Metacognition Index (MI). These two indices are composed of several individual scale scores capturing individual aspects of functioning [56].
The BRI reflects the ability to self-monitor and regulate behavior and emotion appropriately, and consists of the Inhibit, Shift, and Emotional Control scales. The Inhibit scale assesses being able to inhibit impulses, reactions, or inappropriate behavior and resist distractions. The Shift scale captures mental flexibility, or the ability to switch between thinking about two different concepts, situations, or tasks. The Emotional Control scale refers to the ability to regulate one’s own emotions and emotional responses.
The MI captures the ability to control and regulate conscious cognitive processes, such as sustaining ideas and tasks in working memory as well as planning and organizing problem-solving approaches, and consists of the Initiate, Working Memory, Plan/Organize, Monitor, and Organization of Materials scales. The Initiate scale reflects an individual’s ability to commence a task or instigate a response or problem-solving strategy independently. The Working Memory scale measures representational memory, or the capacity to hold key pieces of information in mind, possibly involving some encoding of the information, in order to achieve a specific short-term goal. The Plan/Organize scale captures the ability to anticipate, plan, and manage current and future-oriented tasks such as organizing key information and ideas and developing the appropriate sequential steps needed to accomplish a task. The Monitor scale assesses the ability to monitor and check one’s own work or performance whilst undertaking a task or shortly afterwards, such as to improve accuracy. The Organization of Materials scale is the ability to organize materials in an orderly fashion.

2.5. Statistical Methods

A sample size of n = 726 was originally selected for neurodevelopmental follow-up based on detecting differences between treatment groups in the mean Cognitive Scale of the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, Third Edition, at 1.5 years [36]. At 7 years, this sample size provides >80% power to detect a 0.25 SD difference (a small effect size) between the treatment groups in the mean SDQ subscale scores, assuming 75% follow-up.
All analyses were performed on an intention-to-treat basis according to the mother’s allocation to the treatment or placebo group. Analyses were performed according to the pre-specified statistical analysis plan using SAS Version 9.3 and Stata Release 13. Data collected on participants up to the point of withdrawal were included in the analyses. Multiple imputation was performed separately by treatment group using chained equations to create 100 complete datasets for analysis, under the assumption that data were missing at random [57]. The primary analysis was based on imputed data and included all participants who consented to the follow-up study. Secondary analyses were performed on the available data and on imputed data for the 726 children in the original sub-sample, excluding four deaths. All analyses produced similar results and only the results of the primary analysis are presented.
Continuous behavior scores were analyzed using linear regression models, with treatment effects expressed as adjusted mean differences (AMDs; DHA-placebo). Binary outcomes (defined as scores in the normal range versus scores indicative of a possible problem) were analyzed using log binomial regression models, with treatment effects expressed as adjusted relative risks (ARRs; DHA/placebo).
Analyses took into account both the sampling design and probability weights, calculated as the inverse of the probability of selection. A priori secondary analyses were performed to estimate treatment effects separately by infant sex and test for evidence of effect modification by sex, due to the previously reported sex differences in treatment effects on and language at 18 months in this cohort, and results from other studies suggesting that boys and girls may respond differently to supplementation [36,58].
Both unadjusted and adjusted analyses were performed, with adjustment for the stratification variables (center and parity), as well as child sex and mother’s secondary education, further education, and smoking status at baseline. Unadjusted and adjusted analyses produced similar results, and both are presented in the results tables, but only the adjusted analyses are reported or interpreted in the text. Statistical significance was assessed at the two-sided p < 0.05 level. All outcomes presented are secondary and no adjustment was made for multiple comparisons; hence, the results of these analyses should be interpreted with caution.
Post-randomization child demographics and clinical characteristics were compared between treatment groups based on the available data using t-tests for continuous variables, and chi-squared tests for categorical variables, accounting for the sampling design and weights.

3. Results

Of the 726 infants selected a priori for follow-up, there were 638 eligible and approached for the 7-year follow-up. There were 95 families that did not consent to the follow-up (DHA group n = 51, placebo n = 44), 52 of which were not able to be located and contacted, and 43 declined the follow-up with the predominant reason being that they were too busy to participate. A total of 543 families consented to the assessment at 7 years (75% of the 726 originally selected for neurodevelopment follow-up and 85% of the 638 invited). Participant characteristics in the subset consenting to the follow-up were comparable between randomized groups at baseline and at 7 years (Table 1), with the exception of maternal smoking, which was accounted for in analyses. There were similar proportions of females and males in each randomization group (DHA group females n = 133, 51.4%; placebo group females n = 140, 49.3%).

3.1. Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)

As previously reported, the Total Difficulties score of the SDQ was 1.09 points higher on average in the DHA group compared with the placebo group (p = 0.02, Table 2) [38]. This appeared to be driven by effects within the Hyperactivity/Inattention (AMD 0.49, 95% CI 0.13 to 0.85; p = 0.01) and Peer Relationship Problems (AMD 0.27, 95% CI 0.01 to 0.53; p = 0.04) subscales. Children in the DHA group were more likely to be categorized as being in the at-risk range on the SDQ Hyperactivity subscale (Table 3; ARR 1.56, 95% CI 1.12 to 2.17; p = 0.01) and to have an Impact score indicative of a possible behavior problem that was substantially adversely impacting their everyday functioning (ARR 1.44, 95% CI 1.07 to 1.94; p = 0.02) compared with the placebo group. There was evidence of a sex by treatment interaction on the Hyperactivity/Inattention mean score (AMD 0.97, 95% CI 0.43 to 1.51; p < 0.01, interaction p = 0.01) and the proportion of scores in the abnormal range (ARR 2.27, 95% CI 1.50 to 3.44; p = 0.01; interaction p = 0.01), where males in the DHA group scored more poorly than males in the placebo group. Males in the DHA group also appeared to have worse scores on the Total Difficulties score, although the interaction tests did not reach statistical significance.

3.2. Conners 3rd Edition ADHD Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Version IV Index (Conners 3TM AI-Parent)

As stated in the primary results paper [38], children in the DHA group had scores that were 2.84 points higher on average (reflecting more symptoms) on Conners 3TM AI-parent ADHD index than children from the placebo group (p = 0.02). The mean score for children in the DHA group (60.94) fell just within the elevated range (scores 60–69), indicating more concerns about symptoms of ADHD than is typical. However, there was limited evidence of a difference between the randomization groups in the proportion of children who scored in the clinical range (p = 0.06). There was evidence of a sex by treatment interaction (p = 0.02), where males in the DHA group had higher scores on average than placebo group males (AMD 5.79, 95% CI 2.13 to 9.44; p < 0.01) and were more likely to fall within the abnormal range, although the interaction test was not statistically significant (p = 0.36).

3.3. BRIEF

Parent-reported assessments indicated more perceived problems in the DHA group than the placebo group (Table 2). As previously reported [38], overall scores on the BRIEF and the Global Executive Composite were 2.38 points higher on average in the DHA group (p = 0.01). More children in the DHA group also had Global Executive Composite scores in the at-risk range than did children in the placebo group (Table 3; ARR 1.48, 95% CI 1.07 to 2.05; p = 0.02). These effects appeared to have been driven by a negative effect of DHA supplementation predominantly within males; however, the sex interaction effects did not reach statistical significance (p = 0.08).
The BRI was 2.09 points higher on average in the DHA group (95% CI 0.40 to 3.79; p = 0.02). The Inhibit (AMD 2.40, 95% CI 0.79 to 4.00; p < 0.01) and Shift (AMD 2.05, 95% CI 0.33 to 3.78; p = 0.02) scales were the only scores to differ between groups and were elevated in the DHA group, and children in the DHA group were more likely to score in the at-risk range for the Inhibit scale (Table 3; ARR 1.49, 95% CI 1.04 to 2.13; p = 0.03). Exploratory analyses by sex suggested that there were no effects within females, whereas the DHA group males had elevated (ie. poorer) Shift scale (53.89 vs. 49.81, AMD 3.89, 95% CI 1.37 to 6.41; p < 0.01; interaction p = 0.04), BRI, and Inhibit scale scores than placebo group males, although the interaction test did not reach statistical significance for the BRI and Inhibit scale.
The MI was 2.25 points higher on average in the DHA group compared with the placebo group (95% CI 0.57 to 3.92; p = 0.01). The Working Memory (AMD 1.88, 95% CI 0.20 to 3.57; p = 0.03), Plan/Organize (AMD 2.20, 95% CI 0.49 to 3.92; p = 0.01), Monitor (AMD 2.99, 95% CI 1.25 to 4.72; p < 0.01), and Organization of Materials (AMD 1.80, 95% CI 0.29 to 3.31; p = 0.02) scales were all elevated in the DHA group, as was the proportion of children in the at-risk range for the Monitor scale (ARR 1.72, 95% CI 1.17 to 2.51; p = 0.01). Although the mean Initiate scale scores did not differ between groups overall, there was evidence of a sex by treatment interaction effect (p = 0.03), where males in the DHA group scored 2.75 points higher on average than males in the placebo group (95% CI 0.69 to 4.80; p = 0.01). Males in the DHA group also scored more poorly on the Working Memory scale (AMD 3.73, 95% CI 1.19 to 6.28; p < 0.01; interaction p = 0.03) and were more likely to score in the at-risk range compared with placebo group males (ARR 1.59, 95% CI 1.08 to 2.35; p = 0.02; interaction p = 0.01). The MI, Plan/Organize scale, and Monitor scale also appeared to be worse in the DHA group males, with a higher proportion having a problematic score than the placebo group males, although the interaction effects did not reach statistical significance.

4. Discussion

It has previously been reported that in this large RCT, DHA supplementation in pregnancy resulted in more parent-rated behavioral symptoms and ADHD symptoms, as well as behavioral manifestations of executive dysfunction at 7 years of age [38]. The current in-depth exploration of the behavioral ratings supported the hypothesis that effects of prenatal DHA (800 mg per day) were most prominent in indices reflecting externalizing behaviors. Additionally, DHA group children appeared to be more likely to score in the at-risk range for externalizing behavioral problems more frequently than placebo group children. The poorer scores in the DHA group were largely driven by an effect within males. These results are somewhat consistent with the findings of potentially adverse effects on externalizing behavior at 1.5 [36] and 4 [37] years of age; although, at 1.5 years, adverse effects were detected within females only [36], and at 4 [37] years of age, there was no sex by treatment interaction. Performance-based assessments of executive functioning (administered by study assessors) were largely null at 4 and 7 years of age, with the exception that males in the DHA group had slightly poorer inhibition and mental flexibility compared with males in the placebo group [38], which is consistent with the differences found on the parent-rated Shift and Inhibit scales. Importantly, the effect sizes of the potentially adverse effects of the DHA intervention were relatively small, even though slightly more DHA group children were classified as being in the at-risk range, suggesting the magnitude of an effect is likely negligible. The differences in parent-rated behavior were not reflected in the clinical diagnoses of behavioral problems [38]. It may be that the DOMInO trial has highlighted a true, small underlying adverse effect of DHA on behavior, or these may be chance findings due to the large number of comparisons made.
The most comparable trial to DOMInO with long-term developmental follow-up is a large RCT of DHA in Australian infants born <33 weeks’ gestation [59]. Supplementation with a high-dose of DHA or a standard-dose of DHA occurred from within a week of birth until infants reached 40 weeks’ gestation (equivalent to full term) [59], which corresponds to the same supplementation period in the DOMInO trial, although supplementation took place ex utero. Parents completed the BRIEF, SDQ, and Conners 3TM AI-parent questionnaires when the children reached 7 years of age [59]. Comparable to DOMInO, behavioral outcomes, particularly externalizing behaviors and executive dysfunction, were slightly worse in infants who received the high-dose DHA; however, the effects appeared to be driven by differences in females [59]. Whilst similar aspects of behavioral functioning were affected by DHA in these large trials, the sex by treatment interaction is unexpectedly inconsistent. It is important to note that in both trials, behavioral functioning was a secondary outcome and may be subject to random error.
Nine other prenatal DHA RCTs with behavior assessments have reported no positive or negative effects on behavioral functioning [60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68,69,70,71]. However, follow-up was typically prior to school age [60,61,62,64,69], samples were small in all but two trials [67,68], none used the measures administered in DOMInO, only two explored a treatment by sex interaction [67,69], and the dose of DHA was generally low and may have been insufficient to elicit an effect [61,62,63,65,66,67,68,69,70,71]. A recent review identified 25 RCTs of DHA supplementation in the first 1000 days that administered a measure of behavior, of which 19 detected no effect of DHA, 5 reported a negative effect, and 1 reported both positive and negative effects of DHA intervention on an aspect of behavior [21]. In a trial of breastfeeding mothers, males in the DHA group had poorer ratings of prosocial behavior compared with males in the placebo group [72]. A separate trial found temperamental indices of externalizing behavior worse in formula-fed infants randomized to DHA compared with infants in the placebo group [73]. When the behavior was measured in a trial conducted in preterm infants, effortful control in early childhood was worse in the DHA group among infants from higher income households when compared with children in the placebo group who were also from higher income households, although children in the DHA group were conversely likely to have fewer symptoms of autism spectrum disorder [74,75]. A subsequent follow-up of these children resulted in no differences in parent-rated behavior [76]. The same investigators conducted a DHA intervention in older children displaying symptoms of autism spectrum disorder and found fewer symptoms after the intervention [77]. A RCT conducted in Australian formula-fed infants reported that the DHA group children had poorer scores for externalizing behavior at 6 years of age than did placebo group children, particularly for oppositional defiant behavior and effects were most prominent within males [78]. As with the DOMInO trial, negative effects were largely limited to externalizing behaviors, but again, these were secondary outcomes in studies conducted in developed countries (Australia, the United States of America, and Denmark) with populations likely to be nutritionally replete.
Although there is evidence for a possible adverse effect of early DHA interventions on externalizing behavior, in all trials, behavior was a secondary outcome, and attrition was often high [21]. No differences have been detected in diagnoses of behavioral problems or the use of medications for behavioral problems [21], suggesting that if there are true adverse effects, they are not having a clinically significant impact on functioning. Importantly, there are well over 100 published DHA intervention trials in the first 1000 days, and less than a quarter have assessed behavior [21].
DHA is considered necessary for optimal brain development; however, as with many nutrients, it is possible that there is an upper limit for safe levels of DHA. Children in both randomization groups of DOMInO were doing well overall, with average intelligence quotient close to the expected mean of 100 [38] and very few diagnoses of neurological disorders (such as ADHD or a language disorder) or chronic health conditions, likely because the mothers were from a well-nourished population and the majority of children were born full term [38]. It may be that fetal brain development is protected against insufficient DHA [79,80], but may not be protected against excess DHA. Another recent large prenatal RCT in Australia found that high-dose DHA supplementation protected against early preterm birth in women with low DHA status, but conversely increased the risk of early preterm birth in women whose baseline status was high [81]. A RCT in formula-fed infants tested the effects of three formulas with differing doses of DHA and a placebo formula on a range of cognitive, language, and executive functioning tasks across childhood [82]. Authors consistently found that the two middle-DHA doses performed best, whilst the group receiving the highest dose of DHA and the placebo group performed slightly more poorly by comparison [82]. A recent, large observational study identified an inverted U-shaped relation between omega-3 long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid concentration mid-pregnancy and total grey and white matter volume in children at 10 years of age [83]. Lower maternal omega-3 concentrations were associated with lower child brain volume, but a ceiling effect may have been present at higher concentrations [83]. When considered together, there is evidence emerging that may suggest a dose effect of DHA, in which an excess may cause harm to some outcomes. However, further research is needed to explore this.
Whilst the DOMInO trial is the largest and most comprehensive study and has the advantages of a robust trial design and low attrition, with a low risk of bias, there are limitations to consider. Primarily, the results presented here are secondary outcomes and there are a substantial number of group comparisons, meaning we cannot exclude the possibility of random error. The behavioral questionnaires administered are designed as screening tools to identify potential problems and are not diagnostic. Including teacher ratings of child behavior would have been a beneficial addition to the study, although two smaller trials that included both parent- and teacher-rated behavior found null effects on both [78,84]. Women were able to request knowledge of group allocation after the primary outcome analysis was complete. At the 7-year follow-up, 92% remained blinded, and knowledge of the intervention did not appear to influence any of the post-randomization characteristics, such as DHA intake or home environment, so it is unlikely that knowledge influenced parents’ perceptions of their child’s behavior. The DOMInO sample was comparable to women who gave birth in South Australia at the time of study recruitment, with the exception that the proportion of pregnant women who smoked was higher in the DOMInO sample [85,86], which was adjusted for in all analyses. Given the large sample size of DOMInO, and a relatively low rate of attrition even sever years after enrolment, our results are likely generalizable to other populations in developed Western countries.
Further research to clarify any effects of DHA on behavior and whether an upper limit of DHA exposure is needed. Whilst it would not be ethical to endorse future additional trials of DHA supplementation for the purpose of detecting harm, current DHA intervention trials should conduct measures of behavior, particularly externalizing behaviors. A new, larger, follow-up of a DHA RCT in almost 1000 children born <29 weeks’ gestation is currently underway, with behavioral assessments administered at 5 years of age [87,88]. The results of this follow-up study will be important for confirming any adverse effects of DHA supplementation on behavioral functioning, and for further investigating whether there is a sex by treatment interaction. Likewise, behavioral assessments of children in the recent and largest prenatal DHA trial (involving over 5000 women) [89] would provide invaluable information to address the ongoing uncertainty.

5. Conclusions

An intervention with about 800 mg DHA per day in a large general sample of well-nourished pregnant women resulted in possible adverse effects on externalizing behavior in the children. This finding needs to be confirmed in future, robust research, preferably through the follow-up of existing, large DHA RCTs.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.F.G., P.J.A., L.N.Y., R.A.G. and M.M.; methodology, J.F.G., P.J.A., L.N.Y., R.A.G. and M.M.; formal analysis, L.N.Y.; resources, J.F.G. and M.M.; data curation, J.F.G. and L.N.Y.; writing—original draft preparation, JFG; writing—review and editing, J.F.G., M.M., L.N.Y., P.J.A. and R.A.G.; supervision, M.M. and R.A.G.; project administration, J.F.G., M.M. and P.J.A.; funding acquisition, M.M., L.N.Y., P.J.A. and R.A.G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC), grant number for the original trial: 349301, and for the 7-year follow-up: 1048493. MM and LNY were supported by Australian National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) fellowships: MM (Principal Research Fellow APP1061704), LNY (Early Career Fellowship APP1052388), and PJA (Leadership Fellowship APP1176077). Treatment and placebo capsules were donated by Incromega 500 TG, Croda Chemicals, East Yorkshire, England.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Human Research Ethics Committees of the Children, Youth, and Women’s Health Service (approval code REC1657/12/2007, approved 6 September 2005) and Women’s and Children’s Health Network (approval code REC2526/12/15;HREC/12/WCHN/112, approved 4 February 2013).

Informed Consent Statement

Written informed consent was obtained from the pregnant women and parents of all children participating in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented this manuscript may be made available upon request to the authors, and with approval from the governing ethics committee.

Acknowledgments

We thank Efamol UK for donating the treatment and placebo capsules used in the DOMInO trial, the families and their children who generously participated, and the DOMInO Trial Steering Committee for management of the DOMInO Trial (Chair: Maria Makrides, Deputy chair: Robert A. Gibson, members: Andrew J. McPhee, Lisa Yelland, Julie Quinlivan, Phillip Ryan).

Conflicts of Interest

Honoraria have been paid to Dr. Gould’s institution to support conference travel by the Nestle Nutrition Institute. Dr. Makrides and Dr. Gibson report serving on the board for Trajan Nutrition until 2020. No other authors reported any financial disclosures or conflicts of interest to declare. The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Clandinin, M.T.; Chappell, J.E.; Leong, S.; Heim, T.; Swyer, P.R.; Chance, G.W. Intrauterine fatty acid accretion rates in human brain: Implications for fatty acid requirements. Early Hum. Dev. 1980, 4, 121–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Martinez, M. Tissue levels of polyunsaturated fatty acids during early human development. J. Pediatrics 1992, 120, S129–S138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Crawford, M.A.; Golfetto, I.; Ghebremeskel, K.; Min, Y.; Moodley, T.; Poston, L.; Phylactos, A.; Cunnane, S.; Schmidt, W. The potential role for arachidonic and docosahexaenoic acids in protection against some central nervous system injuries in preterm infants. Lipids 2003, 38, 303–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Innis, S.M. Perinatal biochemistry and physiology of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids. J. Pediatrics 2003, 143, S1–S8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Huettner, J.E. Kainate receptors and synaptic transmission. Prog. Neurobiol. 2003, 70, 387–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Volpe, J.J. Neuronal proliferation, migration, organization, and myelination. In Neurology of the Newborn, 2nd ed.; Saunders, W.B., Staff, W.B., Eds.; Saunders Company: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 1987; pp. 33–68. [Google Scholar]
  7. Moriguchi, T.; Greiner, R.S.; Salem, N., Jr. Behavioral deficits associated with dietary induction of decreased brain docosahexaenoic acid concentration. J. Neurochem. 2000, 75, 2563–2573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Catalan, J.; Moriguchi, T.; Slotnick, B.; Murthy, M.; Greiner, R.S.; Salem, N.J. Cognitive deficits in docosahexaenoic acid-deficient rats. Behav. Neurosci. 2002, 116, 1022–1031. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Ahmad, A.; Murthy, M.; Greiner, R.S.; Moriguchi, T.; Salem, N., Jr. A decrease in cell size accompanies a loss of docosahexaenoate in the rat hippocampus. Nutr. Neurosci. 2002, 5, 103–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Gale, C.R.; Robinson, S.M.; Godfrey, K.M.; Law, C.M.; Schlotz, W.; O’Callaghan, F.J. Oily fish intake during pregnancy–association with lower hyperactivity but not with higher full-scale IQ in offspring. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2008, 49, 1061–1068. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Hibbeln, J.R.; Davis, J.M.; Steer, C.; Emmett, P.M.; Rogers, I.; Williams, C.; Golding, J. Maternal seafood consumption in pregnancy and neurodevelopment outcomes in childhood (ALSPAC study): An observational cohort study. Lancet 2007, 369, 578–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Daniels, J.L.; Longnecker, M.P.; Rowland, A.S.; Golding, J. Fish intake during pregnancy and early cognitive development of offspring. Epidemiology 2004, 15, 394–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Oken, E.; Radesky, J.S.; Wright, R.O.; Bellinger, D.C.; Amarasiriwardena, C.J.; Kleinman, K.P.; Hu, H.; Gillman, M.W. Maternal fish intake during pregnancy, blood mercury levels, and child cognition at age 3 years in a US cohort. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2008, 167, 1171–1181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Mendez, M.A.; Torrent, M.; Julvez, J.; Ribas-Fito, N.; Kogevinas, M.; Sunyer, J. Maternal fish and other seafood intakes during pregnancy and child neurodevelopment at age 4 years. Public Health Nutr. 2009, 12, 1702–1710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Gould, J.F.; Smithers, L.G.; Makrides, M. The effect of maternal omega-3 (n-3) LCPUFA supplementation during pregnancy on early childhood cognitive and visual development: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2013, 97, 531–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Middleton, P.; Gomersall, J.C.; Gould, J.F.; Shepherd, E.; Olsen, S.F.; Makrides, M. Omega-3 fatty acid addition during pregnancy. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2018, 11, Cd003402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Shulkin, M.; Pimpin, L.; Bellinger, D.; Kranz, S.; Fawzi, W.; Duggan, C.; Mozaffarian, D. n-3 fatty acid supplementation in mothers, preterm infants, and term infants and childhood psychomotor and visual development: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Nutr. 2018, 148, 409–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Lehner, A.; Staub, K.; Aldakak, L.; Eppenberger, P.; Rühli, F.; Martin, R.D.; Bender, N. Impact of omega-3 fatty acid DHA and EPA supplementation in pregnant or breast-feeding women on cognitive performance of children: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutr. Rev. 2021, 79, 585–598. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Gould, J.F.; Smithers, L.G. Chapter 6-prenatal n-3 long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids and children’s executive functions. In Omega Fatty Acids in Brain and Neurological Health, 2nd ed.; Watson, R.R., Preedy, V.R., Eds.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2019; pp. 83–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Gawlik, N.R.; Anderson, A.J.; Makrides, M.; Kettler, L.; Gould, J.F. The influence of DHA on language development: A review of randomized controlled trials of DHA supplementation in pregnancy, the neonatal period, and infancy. Nutrients 2020, 12, 3106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Gould, J.F.; Roberts, R.M.; Makrides, M. The influence of omega-3 long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid, docosahexaenoic acid, on child behavioral functioning: A review of randomized controlled trials of DHA supplementation in pregnancy, the neonatal period and infancy. Nutrients 2021, 13, 415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Bayer, J.K.; Ukoumunne, O.C.; Mathers, M.; Wake, M.; Abdi, N.; Hiscock, H. Development of children’s internalising and externalising problems from infancy to five years of age. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry 2012, 46, 659–668. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Flouri, E.; Papachristou, E.; Midouhas, E.; Ploubidis, G.B.; Lewis, G.; Joshi, H. Developmental cascades of internalising symptoms, externalising problems and cognitive ability from early childhood to middle adolescence. Eur. Psychiatry 2019, 57, 61–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Papachristou, E.; Flouri, E. Distinct developmental trajectories of internalising and externalising symptoms in childhood: Links with mental health and risky behaviours in early adolescence. J. Affect. Disord. 2020, 276, 1052–1060. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Martins, B.P.; Bandarra, N.M.; Figueiredo-Braga, M. The role of marine omega-3 in human neurodevelopment, including Autism Spectrum Disorders and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder—A review. Crit. Rev. Food Sci. Nutr. 2019, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. James, S.; Montgomery, P.; Williams, K. Omega-3 fatty acids supplementation for autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2011, CD007992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Appleton, K.M.; Rogers, P.J.; Ness, A.R. Is there a role for n-3 long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids in the regulation of mood and behaviour? A review of the evidence to date from epidemiological studies, clinical studies and intervention trials. Nutr. Res. Rev. 2008, 21, 13–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Tan, M.L.; Ho, J.J.; Teh, K.H. Polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFAs) for children with specific learning disorders. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2012, 12, CD009398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Sonuga-Barke, E.J.; Brandeis, D.; Cortese, S.; Daley, D.; Ferrin, M.; Holtmann, M.; Stevenson, J.; Danckaerts, M.; van der Oord, S.; Dopfner, M.; et al. Nonpharmacological interventions for ADHD: Systematic review and meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials of dietary and psychological treatments. Am. J. Psychiatry 2013, 170, 275–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ryan, A.S.; Astwood, J.D.; Gautier, S.; Kuratko, C.N.; Nelson, E.B.; Salem, N., Jr. Effects of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation on neurodevelopment in childhood: A review of human studies. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fat. Acids 2010, 82, 305–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Cardoso, C.; Afonso, C.; Bandarra, N.M. Dietary DHA, bioaccessibility, and neurobehavioral development in children. Crit. Rev. Food Sci. Nutr. 2018, 58, 2617–2631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Jory, J. Abnormal fatty acids in Canadian children with autism. Nutrition 2016, 32, 474–477. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Crippa, A.; Agostoni, C.; Mauri, M.; Molteni, M.; Nobile, M. Polyunsaturated fatty acids are associated with behavior but not with cognition in children with and without ADHD: An Italian study. J. Atten. Disord. 2018, 22, 971–983. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Lopez-Vicente, M.; Ribas Fito, N.; Vilor-Tejedor, N.; Garcia-Esteban, R.; Fernandez-Barres, S.; Dadvand, P.; Murcia, M.; Rebagliato, M.; Ibarluzea, J.; Lertxundi, A.; et al. Prenatal omega-6:omega-3 ratio and attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder symptoms. J. Pediatr. 2019, 209, 204–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Krabbendam, L.; Bakker, E.; Hornstra, G.; van Os, J. Relationship between DHA status at birth and child problem behaviour at 7 years of age. Prostaglandins Leukot. Essent. Fat. Acids 2007, 76, 29–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Makrides, M.; Gibson, R.A.; McPhee, A.J.; Yelland, L.; Quinlivan, J.; Ryan, P. Effect of DHA supplementation during pregnancy on maternal depression and neurodevelopment of young children: A randomized controlled trial. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 2010, 304, 1675–1683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Makrides, M.; Gould, J.F.; Gawlik, N.R.; Yelland, L.N.; Smithers, L.G.; Anderson, P.J.; Gibson, R.A. Four-year follow-up of children born to women in a randomized trial of prenatal DHA supplementation. JAMA 2014, 311, 1802–1804. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Gould, J.F.; Treyvaud, K.; Yelland, L.N.; Anderson, P.J.; Smithers, L.G.; McPhee, A.J.; Makrides, M. Seven-year follow-up of children born to women in a randomized trial of prenatal DHA supplementation. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 2017, 317, 1173–1175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Chang, J.P.; Su, K.P.; Mondelli, V.; Pariante, C.M. Omega-3 polyunsaturated fatty acids in youths with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis of clinical trials and biological studies. Neuropsychopharmacology 2018, 43, 534–545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Milte, C.M.; Parletta, N.; Buckley, J.D.; Coates, A.M.; Young, R.M.; Howe, P.R. Increased erythrocyte eicosapentaenoic acid and docosahexaenoic acid are associated with improved attention and behavior in children with ADHD in a randomized controlled three-way crossover trial. J. Atten. Disord. 2015, 19, 954–964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Beesdo, K.; Knappe, S.; Pine, D.S. Anxiety and anxiety disorders in children and adolescents: Developmental issues and implications for DSM-V. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 2009, 32, 483–524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Willcutt, E.G.; Pennington, B.F. Psychiatric comorbidity in children and adolescents with reading disability. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2000, 41, 1039–1048. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Metsapelto, R.-L.; Silinskas, G.; Kiuru, N.; Poikkeus, A.-M.; Pakarinen, E.; Vasalampi, K.; Lerkkanen, M.-K.; Nurmi, J.-E. Externalizing behavior problems and interest in reading as predictors of later reading skills and educational aspirations. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 2017, 49, 324–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Baker, J.; Grant, S.; Morlock, L. The teacher-student relationship as a developmental context for children with internalizing or externalizing behavior problems. Sch. Psychol. Q. 2008, 23, 3–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Nelson, J.; Benner, G.; Lane, K.; Smith, B. Academic achievement of K-12 students with emotional and behavioral disorders. Except. Child. 2004, 71, 59–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Masten, A.S.; Roisman, G.I.; Long, J.D.; Burt, K.B.; Obradović, J.; Riley, J.R.; Boelcke-Stennes, K.; Tellegen, A. Developmental cascades: Linking academic achievement and externalizing and internalizing symptoms over 20 years. Dev. Psychol. 2005, 41, 733–746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Reinke, W.M.; Herman, K.C.; Petras, H.; Ialongo, N.S. Empirically derived subtypes of child academic and behavior problems: Co-occurrence and distal outcomes. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2008, 36, 759–770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Klassen, A.F.; Miller, A.; Fine, S. Health-related quality of life in children and adolescents who have a diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics 2004, 114, e541–e547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  49. Vergunst, F.; Tremblay, R.E.; Nagin, D.; Algan, Y.; Beasley, E.; Park, J.; Galera, C.; Vitaro, F.; Cote, S.M. Association of behavior in boys from low socioeconomic neighborhoods with employment earnings in adulthood. JAMA Pediatrics 2019, 173, 334–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Wainwright, P.E.; Colombo, J. Nutrition and the development of cognitive functions: Interpretation of behavioral studies in animals and human infants. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2006, 84, 961–970. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Gould, J.F.; Treyvaud, K.; Yelland, L.N.; Anderson, P.J.; Smithers, L.G.; Gibson, R.A.; McPhee, A.J.; Makrides, M. Does n-3 LCPUFA supplementation during pregnancy increase the IQ of children at school age? Follow-up of a randomised controlled trial. BMJ Open 2016, 6, e011465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Gawlik, N.R.; Makrides, M.; Kettler, L.; Yelland, L.N.; Leemaqz, S.; Gould, J.F. The influence of DHA supplementation during pregnancy on language development across childhood: Follow-up of a randomised controlled trial. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fat. Acids 2020, 163, 102207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Goodman, R. The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1997, 38, 581–586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Goodman, A.; Lamping, D.L.; Ploubidis, G.B. When to use broader internalising and externalising subscales instead of the hypothesised five subscales on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ): Data from British parents, teachers and children. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 2010, 38, 1179–1191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Conners, C.K. Conners 3TM ADHD Index-Parent; Multi-Health Systems Inc.: North Tonawanda, NY, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  56. Gioia, G.A.; Isquith, P.K.; Guy, S.C.; Kenworthy, L. Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function; Psychological Assessment Resources: Lutz, FL, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  57. Sullivan, T.R.; White, I.R.; Salter, A.B.; Ryan, P.; Lee, K.J. Should multiple imputation be the method of choice for handling missing data in randomized trials? Stat. Methods Med. Res. 2018, 27, 2610–2626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Makrides, M.; Gibson, R.A.; McPhee, A.J.; Collins, C.T.; Davis, P.G.; Doyle, L.W.; Simmer, K.; Colditz, P.B.; Morris, S.; Smithers, L.G.; et al. Neurodevelopmental outcomes of preterm infants fed high-dose docosahexaenoic acid: A randomized controlled trial. J. Am. Med. Assoc. 2009, 301, 175–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Collins, C.T.; Gibson, R.A.; Anderson, P.J.; McPhee, A.J.; Sullivan, T.R.; Gould, J.F.; Ryan, P.; Doyle, L.W.; Davis, P.G.; McMichael, J.E.; et al. Neurodevelopmental outcomes at 7 years’ corrected age in preterm infants who were fed high-dose docosahexaenoic acid to term equivalent: A follow-up of a randomised controlled trial. Br. Med. J. Open 2015, 5, e007314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Tofail, F.; Kabir, I.; Hamadani, J.D.; Chowdhury, F.; Yesmin, S.; Mehreen, F.; Huda, S.N. Supplementation of fish-oil and soy-oil during pregnancy and psychomotor development of infants. J. Health Popul. Nutr. 2006, 24, 48–56. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  61. Judge, M.P. Impact of Maternal Docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) Supplementation in the Form of a Functional Food during Pregnancy on Infant Neurodevelopment: A Comparison of Vision, Memory, Temperament and Problem-Solving Abilities. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  62. Carlson, S.E.; Colombo, J.; Gajewski, B.J.; Gustafson, K.M.; Mundy, D.; Yeast, J.; Georgieff, M.K.; Markley, L.A.; Kerling, E.H.; Shaddy, D.J. DHA supplementation and pregnancy outcomes. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2013, 97, 808–815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Colombo, J.; Shaddy, D.J.; Gustafson, K.; Gajewski, B.J.; Thodosoff, J.M.; Kerling, E.; Carlson, S.E. The Kansas University DHA Outcomes Study (KUDOS) clinical trial: Long-term behavioral follow-up of the effects of prenatal DHA supplementation. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2019, 109, 1380–1392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Dunstan, J.A.; Simmer, K.; Dixon, G.; Prescott, S.L. Cognitive assessment of children at 2 1/2 years following fish oil supplementation in pregnancy: A randomized controlled trial. Arch. Dis. Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2008, 93, F45–F50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Meldrum, S.; Dunstan, J.A.; Foster, J.K.; Simmer, K.; Prescott, S.L. Maternal fish oil supplementation in pregnancy: A 12 year follow-up of a randomised controlled trial. Nutrients 2015, 7, 2061–2067. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Ramakrishnan, U.; Martorell, R.; Stein, A.D.; Wang, M.; DiGirolamo, A.; Schnaas, L.; Flores, R.; Neufeld, L.; Rivera, J. Effect of Prenatal Supplementation with Docosahexanoic acid on Child Size and Development at 18 Mo: Randomized Placebo-Controlled Trial in Mexico; ISSFAL: Maastricht, The Netherlands, 2010; p. 112. [Google Scholar]
  67. Ramakrishnan, U.; Gonzalez-Casanova, I.; Schnaas, L.; DiGirolamo, A.; Quezada, A.D.; Pallo, B.C.; Hao, W.; Neufeld, L.M.; Rivera, J.A.; Stein, A.D.; et al. Prenatal supplementation with DHA improves attention at 5 y of age: A randomized controlled trial. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2016, 104, 1075–1082. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Strom, M.; Maslova, E.; Hansen, S.; Mortensen, E.L.; Olsen, S. Fish oil supplementation during pregnancy and offspring risk of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and depression: 14–20 year follow up of two randomized controlled trials. Acta Obstet. Gynecol. Scand. 2013, 92, 31. [Google Scholar]
  69. Miller, S.M.; Harris, M.A.; Baker, S.S.; Davalos, D.B.; Clark, A.M.; McGirr, K.A. Intake of total omega-3 docosahexaenoic acid associated with increased gestational length and improved cognitive performance at 1 year of age. J. Nutr. Health Food Eng. 2016, 5, 642–651. [Google Scholar]
  70. Ostadrahimi, A.; Salehi-Pourmehr, H.; Mohammad-Alizadeh-Charandabi, S.; Heidarabady, S.; Farshbaf-Khalili, A. The effect of perinatal fish oil supplementation on neurodevelopment and growth of infants: A randomized controlled trial. Eur. J. Nutr. 2018, 57, 2387–2397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Brei, C.; Stecher, L.; Brunner, S.; Ensenauer, R.; Heinen, F.; Wagner, P.D.; Hermsdorfer, J.; Hauner, H. Impact of the n-6:n-3 long-chain PUFA ratio during pregnancy and lactation on offspring neurodevelopment: 5-year follow-up of a randomized controlled trial. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2017, 71, 1114–1120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Cheatham, C.L.; Nerhammer, A.S.; Asserhoj, M.; Michaelsen, K.F.; Lauritzen, L. Fish oil supplementation during lactation: Effects on cognition and behavior at 7 years of age. Lipids 2011, 46, 637–645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Auestad, N.; Halter, R.; Hall, R.T.; Blatter, M.; Bogle, M.L.; Burks, W.; Erickson, J.R.; Fitzgerald, K.M.; Dobson, V.; Innis, S.M.; et al. Growth and development in term infants fed long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids: A double-masked, randomized, parallel, prospective, multivariate study. Pediatrics 2001, 108, 372–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  74. Keim, S.A.; Boone, K.M.; Klebanoff, M.A.; Turner, A.N.; Rausch, J.; Nelin, M.A.; Rogers, L.K.; Yeates, K.O.; Nelin, L.; Sheppard, K.W. Effect of docosahexaenoic acid supplementation vs. placebo on developmental outcomes of toddlers born preterm: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatrics 2018, 172, 1126–1134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Boone, K.M.; Parrott, A.; Rausch, J.; Yeates, K.O.; Klebanoff, M.A.; Norris Turner, A.; Keim, S.A. Fatty acid supplementation and socioemotional outcomes: Secondary analysis of a randomized trial. Pediatrics 2020, 146, e20200284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Boone, K.M.; Pattison, K.; Pelak, G.; Sheppard, K.W.; Rausch, J.; Yeates, K.O.; Nelin, M.A.; Klebanoff, M.A.; Turner, A.N.; Rogers, L.K.; et al. Docosahexaenoic and arachidonic acid supplementation at 1 year has mixed effects on development and behaviour at age 2 for preterm children. Acta Paediatr. 2021, 110, 2082–2083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Keim, S.A.; Gracious, B.; Boone, K.M.; Klebanoff, M.A.; Rogers, L.K.; Rausch, J.; Coury, D.L.; Sheppard, K.W.; Husk, J.; Rhoda, D.A. Omega-3 and omega-6 fatty acid supplementation may reduce autism symptoms based on parent report in preterm toddlers. J. Nutr. 2018, 148, 227–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Meldrum, S.J.; Heaton, A.E.; Foster, J.K.; Prescott, S.L.; Simmer Ao, K. Do infants of breast-feeding mothers benefit from additional LCPUFA from fish oil? A six-year follow up. Br. J. Nutr. 2020, 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  79. Burdge, G.C.; Calder, P.C. Conversion of alpha-linolenic acid to longer-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids in human adults. Reprod. Nutr. Dev. 2005, 45, 581–597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Hanebutt, F.L.; Demmelmair, H.; Schiessl, B.; Larque, E.; Koletzko, B. Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid (LC-PUFA) transfer across the placenta. Clin. Nutr. 2008, 27, 685–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Simmonds, L.A.; Sullivan, T.R.; Skubisz, M.; Middleton, P.F.; Best, K.P.; Yelland, L.N.; Quinlivan, J.; Zhou, S.J.; Liu, G.; McPhee, A.J.; et al. Omega-3 fatty acid supplementation in pregnancy-baseline omega-3 status and early preterm birth: Exploratory analysis of a randomised controlled trial. BJOG 2020, 127, 975–981. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Colombo, J.; Carlson, S.E.; Cheatham, C.L.; Shaddy, D.J.; Kerling, E.H.; Thodosoff, J.M.; Gustafson, K.M.; Brez, C. Long-term effects of LCPUFA supplementation on childhood cognitive outcomes. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2013, 98, 403–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  83. Zou, R.; El Marroun, H.; Voortman, T.; Hillegers, M.; White, T.; Tiemeier, H. Maternal polyunsaturated fatty acids during pregnancy and offspring brain development in childhood. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2021, 114, 124–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. de Jong, C.; Kikkert, H.K.; Fidler, V.; Hadders-Algra, M. Effects of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation of infant formula on cognition and behaviour at 9 years of age. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2012, 54, 1102–1108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  85. Chan, A.; Scott, J.; Nguyen, A.-M.; Green, P. Pregnancy Outcome in South Australia 2006; Pregnancy Outcome Unit, Epidemiology Branch, Department of Health: Adelaide, Australia, 2007.
  86. Chan, A.; Scott, J.; Nguyen, A.-M.; Green, P. Pregnancy Outcome in South Australia 2007; Pregnancy Outcome Unit, Epidemiology Branch, Department of Health: Adelaide, Australia, 2008.
  87. Gould, J.F.; Makrides, M.; Sullivan, T.R.; Anderson, P.J.; Gibson, R.A.; Best, K.P.; McPhee, A.J.; Doyle, L.W.; Opie, G.; Travadi, J.; et al. A protocol for assessing whether cognition of preterm infants <29 weeks’ gestation can be improved by an intervention with the omega-3 long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid docosahexaenoic acid (DHA): A follow-up of a randomised controlled trial. BMJ Open 2021, 11, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Gould, J.F.; Roberts, R.; Anderson, P.J.; Makrides, M.; Sullivan, T.R.; Gibson, R.A.; McPhee, A.J.; Doyle, L.W.; Opie, G.; Travadi, J.; et al. Protocol for assessing if behavioural functioning of infants born <29 weeks’ gestation is improved by omega-3 long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids: Follow-up of a randomised controlled trial. BMJ Open 2021, 11, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Makrides, M.; Best, K.; Yelland, L.; McPhee, A.; Zhou, S.; Quinlivan, J.; Dodd, J.; Atkinson, E.; Safa, H.; van Dam, J.; et al. A randomized trial of prenatal n-3 fatty acid supplementation and preterm delivery. N. Engl. J. Med. 2019, 381, 1035–1045. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
Table 1. Treatment group comparison of baseline and post-randomization demographic, social, and clinical characteristics 1.
Table 1. Treatment group comparison of baseline and post-randomization demographic, social, and clinical characteristics 1.
DHA Placebop Value
Characteristics at Study Entryn = 259n = 284
Mother primiparous, n (%)142 (54.8)158 (55.6)
Mother completed secondary education, n (%) 171 (66.0)192 (67.6)
Mother completed further education, n (%) 2175 (67.6)203 (71.5)
Non-smoker before and during early pregnancy, n (%)190 (73.4)187 (65.8)
Characteristics at 7 Yearsn = 259n = 284
Age at assessment, mean (SD) days2648 (110)2652 (134)0.73
Living with both natural parents, n/N (%)188/256 (73.4)191/283 (67.5)0.36
Primary language English at child’s home, n/N (%)246/256 (96.1)271/283 (95.8)0.85
1 Data are presented as mean (SD) with p-value based on t-tests, or n (%) with p-values from a chi-square test. Analyses are based on the raw data and account for the sampling design and weights. No statistical tests were performed for baseline characteristics. 2 Degree, diploma, certificate, trade.
Table 2. Parent-reported outcomes of behavior at 7 years of age 1.
Table 2. Parent-reported outcomes of behavior at 7 years of age 1.
OutcomeDHA Weighted Mean (95% CI)
n = 259
Placebo Weighted Mean (95% CI)
n = 284
Un-Adjusted
Estimate (95% CI)
Un-Adjusted pUn-Adjusted Interaction p 3Adjusted Estimate (95% CI) 2Adjusted pAdjusted Interaction p 3
BRIEF
Global Executive Composite 4 54.89 (53.71, 56.07)52.54 (51.32, 53.76)2.35 (0.66, 4.04)0.010.102.38 (0.67, 4.08)0.010.08
Females 54.33 (52.87, 55.80)53.43 (51.66, 55.21)0.90 (−1.39, 3.20)0.44 0.86 (−1.44, 3.16)0.46
Males 55.49 (53.62, 57.36)51.69 (49.99, 53.38)3.80 (1.28, 6.33)<0.01 3.93 (1.43, 6.44)<0.01
Behavioral Regulation Index 453.66 (52.49, 54.83)51.54 (50.31, 52.76)2.12 (0.43, 3.81)0.010.102.09 (0.40, 3.79)0.020.10
Females 53.44 (51.95, 54.93)52.76 (51.09, 54.43)0.68 (−1.55, 2.91)0.55 0.68 (−1.59, 2.96)0.56
Males 53.89 (52.06, 55.73)50.36 (48.58, 52.15)3.53 (0.97, 6.08)0.01 3.55 (1.02, 6.07)0.01
Inhibit scale53.23 (52.08, 54.39)50.86 (49.73, 51.99)2.37 (0.76, 3.98)<0.010.392.40 (0.79, 4.00)<0.010.31
Females 53.02 (51.56, 54.49)51.37 (49.85, 52.88)1.66 (−0.45, 3.76)0.12 1.57 (−0.54, 3.68)0.15
Males 53.46 (51.65, 55.28)50.38 (48.71, 52.05)3.08 (0.63, 5.54)0.01 3.24 (0.80, 5.69)<0.01
Shift scale53.14 (51.89, 54.39)51.07 (49.88, 52.25)2.07 (0.36, 3.78)0.020.022.05 (0.33, 3.78)0.020.04
Females 52.44 (50.77, 54.11)52.39 (50.72, 54.05)0.05 (−2.28, 2.39)0.96 0.27 (−2.13, 2.66)0.83
Males 53.89 (52.01, 55.77)49.81 (48.12, 51.50)4.08 (1.56, 6.60)<0.01 3.89 (1.37, 6.41)<0.01
Emotional Control scale53.48 (52.32, 54.65)52.48 (51.22, 53.74)1.01 (−0.71, 2.72)0.250.200.95 (−0.78, 2.67)0.280.18
Females 53.40 (51.85, 54.95)53.55 (51.79, 55.31)−0.15 (−2.49, 2.19)0.90 −0.21 (−2.59, 2.16)0.86
Males 53.57 (51.80, 55.34)51.45 (49.64, 53.26)2.12 (−0.41, 4.66)0.10 2.14 (−0.36, 4.64)0.09
Metacognition Index 4 54.68 (53.51, 55.84)52.49 (51.29, 53.69)2.19 (0.52, 3.86)0.010.152.25 (0.57, 3.92)0.010.12
Females 54.01 (52.51, 55.50)53.03 (51.25, 54.81)0.98 (−1.34, 3.30)0.41 0.94 (−1.37, 3.24)0.43
Males 55.40 (53.59, 57.21)51.97 (50.35, 53.59)3.43 (1.01, 5.86)0.01 3.60 (1.19, 6.02)<0.01
Initiate scale53.12 (52.08, 54.17)52.09 (50.96, 53.22)1.03 (−0.51, 2.57)0.190.051.05 (−0.51, 2.62)0.190.03
Females 52.03 (50.55, 53.50)52.53 (50.75, 54.31)−0.50 (−2.82, 1.81)0.67 −0.59 (−2.90, 1.71)0.62
Males 54.31 (52.83, 55.78)51.67 (50.26, 53.08)2.63 (0.59, 4.68)0.01 2.75 (0.69, 4.80)0.01
Working Memory scale54.46 (53.28, 55.65)52.79 (51.60, 53.98)1.67 (−0.02, 3.35)0.050.071.88 (0.20, 3.57)0.030.03
Females 52.67 (51.24, 54.10)52.50 (50.83, 54.17)0.17 (−2.03, 2.37)0.88 0.09 (−2.13, 2.31)0.94
Males 56.40 (54.50, 58.31)53.08 (51.38, 54.78)3.32 (0.76, 5.89)0.01 3.73 (1.19, 6.28)<0.01
Plan/Organize scale55.52 (54.28, 56.76)53.31 (52.14, 54.48)2.21 (0.51, 3.91)0.010.052.20 (0.49, 3.92)0.010.06
Females 55.01 (53.52, 56.50)54.52 (52.80, 56.24)0.49 (−1.78, 2.75)0.67 0.56 (−1.71, 2.84)0.63
Males 56.07 (54.04, 58.09)52.15 (50.56, 53.74)3.91 (1.34, 6.49)<0.01 3.89 (1.30, 6.48)<0.01
Organization of Materials scale54.71 (53.60, 55.81)52.85 (51.85, 53.86)1.85 (0.35, 3.35)0.020.731.80 (0.29, 3.31)0.020.84
Females 55.37 (53.81, 56.93)53.28 (51.80, 54.76)2.09 (−0.07, 4.24)0.06 1.95 (−0.19, 4.10)0.07
Males 54.00 (52.43, 55.57)52.45 (51.07, 53.82)1.55 (−0.53, 3.63)0.14 1.64 (−0.47, 3.75)0.13
Monitor scale52.51 (51.28, 53.73)49.48 (48.24, 50.72)3.03 (1.29, 4.77)<0.010.372.99 (1.25, 4.72)<0.010.35
Females 52.42 (50.90, 53.94)50.20 (48.39, 52.02)2.21 (−0.14, 4.57)0.07 2.17 (−0.17, 4.51)0.07
Males 52.61 (50.65, 54.56)48.79 (47.08, 50.50)3.82 (1.23, 6.41)<0.01 3.83 (1.26, 6.40)<0.01
Conners 3TM AI-parent
ADHD t score 4 60.94 (59.10, 62.78)58.37 (56.74, 60.00)2.56 (0.13, 5.00)0.040.042.84 (0.38, 5.30)0.020.02
Females 59.43 (57.14, 61.73)59.36 (56.96, 61.76)0.07 (−3.23, 3.38)0.97 −0.03 (−3.35, 3.29)0.99
Males 62.56 (59.64, 65.48)57.43 (55.21, 59.64)5.13 (1.50, 8.77)0.01 5.79 (2.13, 9.44)<0.01
SDQ
Total Difficulties Score 49.71 (9.07, 10.35)8.63 (7.99, 9.28)1.08 (0.17, 1.98)0.020.161.09 (0.18, 2.00)0.020.14
Females 8.89 (8.11, 9.67)8.43 (7.56, 9.29)0.47 (−0.69, 1.62)0.43 0.42 (−0.75, 1.58)0.48
Males 10.60 (9.58, 11.62)8.83 (7.86, 9.80)1.76 (0.36, 3.16)0.01 1.79 (0.40, 3.17)0.01
Emotional Symptoms score2.21 (2.01, 2.41)1.97 (1.76, 2.18)0.24 (−0.05, 0.53)0.100.980.23 (−0.07, 0.52)0.130.84
Females 2.38 (2.07, 2.69)2.14 (1.85, 2.44)0.24 (−0.19, 0.66)0.28 0.25 (−0.18, 0.69)0.25
Males 2.03 (1.77, 2.28)1.80 (1.50, 2.10)0.23 (−0.16, 0.62)0.25 0.20 (−0.19, 0.58)0.32
Conduct Problems score1.74 (1.56, 1.92)1.63 (1.45, 1.81)0.11 (−0.14, 0.36)0.390.210.10 (−0.15, 0.36)0.430.17
Females 1.55 (1.33, 1.77)1.59 (1.35, 1.84)−0.04 (−0.37, 0.28)0.79 −0.08 (−0.41, 0.26)0.65
Males 1.94 (1.65, 2.24)1.66 (1.40, 1.92)0.28 (−0.11, 0.67)0.16 0.29 (−0.10, 0.68)0.15
Hyperactivity score4.13 (3.87, 4.39)3.67 (3.42, 3.92)0.45 (0.09, 0.82)0.010.020.49 (0.13, 0.85)0.010.01
Females 3.58 (3.27, 3.89)3.52 (3.16, 3.88)0.06 (−0.42, 0.54)0.80 0.03 (−0.45, 0.51)0.91
Males 4.71 (4.29, 5.13)3.82 (3.47, 4.17)0.90 (0.35, 1.44)<0.01 0.97 (0.43, 1.51)<0.01
Peer Problems score1.61 (1.41, 1.80)1.34 (1.16, 1.52)0.27 (0.01, 0.53)0.050.650.27 (0.01, 0.53)0.040.75
Females 1.38 (1.15, 1.61)1.16 (0.94, 1.38)0.22 (−0.10, 0.54)0.18 0.23 (−0.09, 0.54)0.17
Males 1.85 (1.54, 2.17)1.51 (1.23, 1.79)0.34 (−0.07, 0.76)0.11 0.31 (−0.10, 0.72)0.14
Prosocial Behavior score8.12 (7.92, 8.32)8.18 (7.99, 8.38)−0.06 (−0.34, 0.22)0.690.65−0.05 (−0.32, 0.23)0.740.45
Females 8.44 (8.19, 8.69)8.45 (8.19, 8.71)−0.01 (−0.37, 0.35)0.95 0.06 (−0.30, 0.42)0.75
Males 7.78 (7.47, 8.09)7.92 (7.63, 8.21)−0.14 (−0.56, 0.28)0.52 −0.16 (−0.58, 0.27)0.47
Impact score0.90 (0.73, 1.08)0.69 (0.53, 0.85)0.21 (−0.02, 0.45)0.08 0.650.23 (−0.01, 0.46)0.060.57
Females 0.59 (0.41, 0.78)0.42 (0.27, 0.57)0.18 (−0.07, 0.42)0.15 0.16 (−0.09, 0.41)0.22
Males 1.24 (0.94, 1.54)0.95 (0.68, 1.22)0.29 (−0.12, 0.69)0.17 0.30 (−0.10, 0.69)0.15
1 Data are presented as weighted mean (95% CI) with the effect estimate (95% CI) being the mean difference (DHA-Placebo). Analyses are based on 100 imputed datasets and were performed using linear regression models accounting for the sampling design and weights. 2 Adjusted for center, parity, infant sex, mother’s secondary education, mother’s further education, and mother’s smoking status. 3 p-value for infant sex by treatment group interaction term. 4 Data reported previously [38]. BRIEF: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. Conners 3TM AI-parent: Conners 3rd Edition Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders version IV Index. SDQ: Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire.
Table 3. Proportion of children with scores within the clinically significant range for behavior and behavioral manifestation of executive functions at 7 years of age 1.
Table 3. Proportion of children with scores within the clinically significant range for behavior and behavioral manifestation of executive functions at 7 years of age 1.
OutcomeDHA Weighted % (95% CI) n = 259Placebo Weighted % (95% CI) n = 284Unadjusted Estimate (95% CI)Un-Adjusted pUn-Adjusted Interaction p 3Adjusted Estimate (95% CI) 2Adjusted pAdjusted Interaction p 3
BRIEF
Global Executive Composite ≥6520.62 (16.42, 24.81)14.24 (10.80, 17.67)1.45 (1.06, 1.98)0.020.241.48 (1.07, 2.05)0.020.27
Females 19.51 (13.76, 25.27)16.17 (10.98, 21.36)1.21 (0.78, 1.86)0.40 1.25 (0.79, 1.95)0.34
Males 21.80 (15.67, 27.94)12.38 (7.87, 16.89)1.76 (1.11, 2.79)0.02 1.80 (1.13, 2.85)0.01
Behavioral Regulation Index ≥6518.12 (14.10, 22.14)16.56 (12.83, 20.28)1.09 (0.80, 1.50)0.570.991.11 (0.80, 1.53)0.540.93
Females 16.88 (11.47, 22.28)15.33 (10.12, 20.54)1.10 (0.69, 1.75)0.68 1.12 (0.69, 1.84)0.64
Males 19.47 (13.48, 25.45)17.73 (12.42, 23.04)1.10 (0.71, 1.69)0.67 1.09 (0.72, 1.67)0.68
Inhibit scale ≥6517.79 (13.77, 21.81)11.92 (8.70, 15.15)1.49 (1.05, 2.12)0.030.781.49 (1.04, 2.13)0.030.68
Females 14.97 (9.80, 20.15)10.54 (6.06, 15.03)1.42 (0.82, 2.45)0.21 1.36 (0.76, 2.44)0.30
Males 20.83 (14.63, 27.03)13.24 (8.60, 17.88)1.57 (0.99, 2.49)0.05 1.59 (1.00, 2.52)0.05
Shift scale ≥6513.53 (10.01, 17.05)14.41 (10.93, 17.88)0.94 (0.66, 1.34)0.730.190.96 (0.67, 1.39)0.830.39
Females 10.99 (6.63, 15.34)15.01 (10.07, 19.95)0.73 (0.44, 1.23)0.24 0.80 (0.46, 1.40)0.44
Males 16.27 (10.68, 21.87)13.83 (8.93, 18.74)1.18 (0.72, 1.93)0.52 1.12 (0.68, 1.84)0.66
Emotional Control scale ≥6517.62 (13.69, 21.55)18.93 (15.06, 22.81)0.93 (0.69, 1.26)0.640.360.94 (0.68, 1.28)0.690.35
Females 15.25 (10.11, 20.40)18.99 (13.43, 24.56)0.80 (0.51, 1.26)0.34 0.80 (0.49, 1.28)0.35
Males 20.18 (14.18, 26.18)18.88 (13.46, 24.29)1.07 (0.71, 1.62)0.75 1.08 (0.71, 1.63)0.72
Metacognition Index ≥6519.36 (15.23, 23.49)15.01 (11.44, 18.57)1.29 (0.94, 1.77)0.120.121.28 (0.93, 1.76)0.140.14
Females17.18 (11.68, 22.67)17.12 (11.69, 22.55)1.00 (0.64, 1.57)0.99 1.01 (0.64, 1.58)0.98
Males21.72 (15.51, 27.93)12.99 (8.37, 17.61)1.67 (1.06, 2.64)0.03 1.63 (1.03, 2.58)0.04
Initiate scale ≥6516.29 (12.49, 20.09)14.18 (10.62, 17.73)1.15 (0.82, 1.62)0.430.391.14 (0.80, 1.63)0.470.38
Females13.72 (8.82, 18.61)13.99 (8.92, 19.07)0.98 (0.59, 1.63)0.94 0.96 (0.57, 1.62)0.88
Males19.06 (13.21, 24.91)14.35 (9.39, 19.31)1.33 (0.84, 2.11)0.23 1.31 (0.82, 2.11)0.26
Working Memory scale ≥6518.93 (14.89, 22.98)17.23 (13.52, 20.95)1.10 (0.81, 1.49)0.540.011.16 (0.85, 1.58)0.360.01
Female 11.31 (6.74, 15.89)16.69 (11.42, 21.97)0.68 (0.41, 1.13)0.14 0.67 (0.40, 1.12)0.13
Males27.15 (20.44, 33.86)17.75 (12.51, 22.99)1.53 (1.04, 2.25)0.03 1.59 (1.08, 2.35)0.02
Plan/Organize scale ≥6521.13 (16.87, 25.38)17.42 (13.71, 21.13)1.21 (0.91, 1.62)0.190.031.20 (0.90, 1.62)0.220.05
Females18.81 (13.17, 24.45)21.31 (15.54, 27.07)0.88 (0.59, 1.32)0.55 0.91 (0.61, 1.36)0.66
Males23.63 (17.22, 30.04)13.69 (9.04, 18.35)1.73 (1.12, 2.66)0.01 1.67 (1.07, 2.59)0.02
Organization of Materials scale ≥6523.47 (19.08, 27.86)17.09 (13.41, 20.78)1.37 (1.03, 1.83)0.030.691.32 (0.98, 1.78)0.070.60
Females26.86 (20.55, 33.16)20.74 (15.06, 26.42)1.29 (0.90, 1.86)0.16 1.24 (0.86, 1.80)0.25
Males19.83 (13.77, 25.88)13.60 (8.89, 18.30)1.46 (0.92, 2.31)0.11 1.45 (0.92, 2.30)0.11
Monitor scale ≥6517.17 (13.18, 21.16)10.16 (7.17, 13.14)1.69 (1.16, 2.46)0.010.501.72 (1.17, 2.51)0.010.52
Females15.94 (10.57, 21.31)10.73 (6.31, 15.14)1.49 (0.87, 2.53)0.14 1.51 (0.88, 2.60)0.13
Males18.50 (12.56, 24.44)9.61 (5.56, 13.66)1.93 (1.14, 3.26)0.02 1.94 (1.14, 3.30)0.01
Conners 3TM AI-parent
ADHD t score ≥7031.05 (26.25, 35.85)25.55 (21.27, 29.83)1.22 (0.97, 1.52)0.090.431.25 (0.99, 1.58)0.060.36
Females25.69 (19.41, 31.97)23.27 (17.36, 29.18)1.10 (0.78, 1.57)0.58 1.10 (0.76, 1.58)0.62
Males36.84 (29.56, 44.11)27.74 (21.56, 33.92)1.33 (0.99, 1.78)0.06 1.37 (1.01, 1.84)0.04
SDQ
Total Difficulties score >1614.33 (10.78, 17.88)12.02 (8.76, 15.27)1.19 (0.83, 1.72)0.350.381.20 (0.82, 1.74)0.350.42
Females10.72 (6.51, 14.93)10.80 (6.34, 15.26)0.99 (0.56, 1.75)0.98 0.99 (0.56, 1.77)0.98
Males18.22 (12.45, 24.00)13.18 (8.45, 17.91)1.38 (0.86, 2.23)0.19 1.36 (0.83, 2.20)0.22
Emotional Symptoms score >413.30 (9.85, 16.75)13.33 (9.89, 16.76)1.00 (0.69, 1.44)0.990.560.97 (0.66, 1.43)0.890.64
Females16.65 (11.46, 21.84)15.49 (10.30, 20.67)1.08 (0.68, 1.70)0.76 1.04 (0.65, 1.68)0.87
Males9.68 (5.22, 14.14)11.25 (6.72, 15.78)0.86 (0.47, 1.58)0.62 0.87 (0.46, 1.62)0.65
Conduct Problems score >3 415.98 (12.19, 19.78)12.02 (8.84, 15.21)1.33 (0.93, 1.90)0.120.891.34 (0.94, 1.91)0.110.84
Females11.19 (6.86, 15.51)8.58 (4.51, 12.64)1.31 (0.71, 2.40)0.39 1.27 (0.69, 2.35)0.44
Males21.15 (14.85, 27.46)15.32 (10.45, 20.20)1.38 (0.89, 2.13)0.15 1.37 (0.89, 2.13)0.15
Hyperactivity score >619.80 (15.71, 23.90)13.28 (9.93, 16.62)1.49 (1.08, 2.07)0.020.011.56 (1.12, 2.17)0.01<0.01
Females10.56 (6.13, 14.98)13.08 (8.35, 17.82)0.81 (0.46, 1.41)0.45 0.77 (0.44, 1.37)0.38
Males29.78 (22.90, 36.66)13.46 (8.75, 18.18)2.21 (1.46, 3.36)<0.01 2.27 (1.50, 3.44)<0.01
Peer Problems score >313.74 (10.16, 17.33)10.76 (7.59, 13.93)1.28 (0.86, 1.89)0.220.551.28 (0.86, 1.89)0.220.69
Females10.25 (5.86, 14.64)9.18 (5.07, 13.29)1.12 (0.60, 2.07)0.72 1.16 (0.63, 2.13)0.64
Males 17.51 (11.78, 23.24)12.27 (7.49, 17.06)1.43 (0.86, 2.37)0.17 1.36 (0.82, 2.27)0.24
Prosocial Behavior score <53.98 (1.87, 6.09)4.40 (2.37, 6.43)0.90 (0.45, 1.82)0.780.580.97 (0.48, 1.95)0.930.50
Females2.86 (0.50, 5.22)3.97 (1.13, 6.81)0.72 (0.24, 2.15)0.56 0.73 (0.25, 2.18)0.58
Males5.19 (1.63, 8.76)4.81 (1.91, 7.71)1.08 (0.43, 2.67)0.87 1.18 (0.50, 2.79)0.71
Impact score >122.64 (18.27, 27.00)15.57 (12.02, 19.12)1.45 (1.08, 1.96)0.020.341.44 (1.07, 1.94)0.020.31
Females16.71 (11.29, 22.14)9.12 (5.02, 13.23)1.83 (1.05, 3.20)0.03 1.84 (1.04, 3.25)0.04
Males29.03 (22.17, 35.90)21.74 (16.07, 27.41)1.34 (0.94, 1.90)0.11 1.30 (0.91, 1.85)0.15
1 Data are presented as weighted percentage (95% CI) with the effect estimate (95% CI) being the relative risk (DHA/placebo). Analyses are based on 100 imputed datasets and were performed using log binomial regression accounting for the sampling design and weights. 2 Adjusted for center, parity, infant sex, mother’s secondary education, mother’s further education, and mother’s smoking status. 3 p-value for infant sex by treatment group interaction terms. 4 Analyzed using log Poisson regression as convergence issues were encountered with log binomial regression for some imputed datasets. BRIEF: Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. Conners 3TM AI-parent: Conners 3rd Edition Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders version IV Index. SDQ: Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Gould, J.F.; Anderson, P.J.; Yelland, L.N.; Gibson, R.A.; Makrides, M. The Influence of Prenatal DHA Supplementation on Individual Domains of Behavioral Functioning in School-Aged Children: Follow-Up of a Randomized Controlled Trial. Nutrients 2021, 13, 2996. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13092996

AMA Style

Gould JF, Anderson PJ, Yelland LN, Gibson RA, Makrides M. The Influence of Prenatal DHA Supplementation on Individual Domains of Behavioral Functioning in School-Aged Children: Follow-Up of a Randomized Controlled Trial. Nutrients. 2021; 13(9):2996. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13092996

Chicago/Turabian Style

Gould, Jacqueline F., Peter J. Anderson, Lisa N. Yelland, Robert A. Gibson, and Maria Makrides. 2021. "The Influence of Prenatal DHA Supplementation on Individual Domains of Behavioral Functioning in School-Aged Children: Follow-Up of a Randomized Controlled Trial" Nutrients 13, no. 9: 2996. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13092996

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop