Next Article in Journal
Exploring the Influences of Innovation Climate and Resource Endowments through Two Types of University–Industry Collaborative Activities on Regional Sustainable Development
Next Article in Special Issue
Data Mining for Attitudinal and Belief Profiles Determination towards Hypnosis
Previous Article in Journal
The Relationship between Organizational Learning at the Individual Level and Perceived Employability: A Model-Based Approach
Previous Article in Special Issue
Validation of a Football Competence Observation System (FOCOS), Linked to Procedural Tactical Knowledge
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Observational Analysis of Corner Kicks in High-Level Football: A Mixed Methods Study

1
Department of Science of Physical Activity and Sport, Pontifical University of Salamanca, 37002 Salamanca, Spain
2
Department of Social Psychology and Quantitative Psychology, University of Barcelona, 08035 Barcelona, Spain
3
Department of Psychobiology and Methodology of Health Sciences, Autonomus University of Barcelona, 18193 Barcelona, Spain
4
Department of Physical and Sport Education, University of A Coruña, 15179 A Coruña, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Sustainability 2021, 13(14), 7562; https://doi.org/10.3390/su13147562
Submission received: 8 April 2021 / Revised: 28 June 2021 / Accepted: 1 July 2021 / Published: 6 July 2021

Abstract

:
Corner kicks are one of the most important set pieces in high-level football. The present study aimed to analyze the evolution of the tactical approach to corner kicks in high-performance football. For this, a total of 1704 corner kicks executed in the 192 matches corresponding to the 2010, 2014 and 2018 FIFA World Cups were analyzed. To achieve the proposed objectives, the observational methodology was used. The results show an evolution in the mode of execution of these actions, but instead the success rate remains low. The log-linear test allowed to find significant relationships between some of the most important categorical variables in these actions: match status, number of intervening attackers and time. The decision tree models show that the number of players involved in these actions is the criterion that presents the greatest information gain. These results corroborate previous multivariate studies, although more research is still needed. Finally, the results of the present study can be used by coaches to create different training situations where success in this type of action can be enhanced.

1. Introduction

Set pieces actions are those that have great importance in football, both men’s and women’s. These are static actions within a dynamic context such as soccer, where teams can achieve a significant number of goals [1,2]. Various studies consider these actions as performance indicators in football [3,4] due to their high relevance in the applied field. Many high-level games are decided on the scoreboard by a goal from set pieces actions [5]. Traditionally, it is found that there are 110 set pieces actions per game (44.9 throw-ins per game; 0.49 penalties; 10.5 corners; 35 free kicks and 20.5 goal kicks), which represent 96.9% of the total of all interruptions, and which account for 41.7% (37.7 min) of the total match [5,6].
Set pieces actions have been related in retrospective studies as actions associated with a high probability of a goal [1,2,7], specifically between 35% and 48% of goals. More recent studies have corroborated that soccer has evolved, and that this percentage has drastically decreased, both in men’s soccer [7,8,9,10,11] and in women’s soccer [6,12,13]. Corner kicks are one of the set pieces actions that most regularly occur during matches. Specifically, there are an average of 10 corner kicks per game, regardless of the competition analyzed, and the type of men’s or women’s football [14,15]. Furthermore, in view of the available data, these are actions of very low efficacy, although with different efficacy indices depending on the championship analyzed. Thus, higher percentages of shot on goal are found in world cups (FIFA World Cup) [16,17], than in European championships between nations (UEFA Euro) [7,10]. Regarding whether the shot goes to the goal (between the three posts), there are no notable differences between different men’s [5] and women’s [13] championships. Concerning the goal, and despite the fact that the differences are still small, it is possible to verify that more goals are achieved in women’s football [13,18] than in men’s football [11]. On the other hand, there are better percentages in the Champions League (percentages between 8% and 10%) [19] than in domestic league competitions [11] (3.1%), or world competitions between nations [6,16] (2.3%).
On the other hand, on the final significance of these actions, there is unanimity among the authors consulted that these are actions that produce transcendental goals that help to achieve victory [20,21]. Specifically, between 69% and 76% of the goals resulting from corners have contributed to awarding victory or points to the teams [11,22].
In terms of delivery areas, there are also differences in terms of competitions. Thus, in world cups and European cups between nations there are more deliveries to the far post [5]. Although in league championships, more are sent to the first post [23]. It is important to highlight that these differences may be due to the different zoning proposed by the different authors. Regarding how match status affects the effectiveness of the corners, there are differences depending on the championship. Thus, the teams that are losing on the scoreboard take more corners than the teams that are winning [14], although the highest number of corners is executed with the score in victory for the executor [16].
In relation to the form of execution, the results are even more mixed. Schmicker [24] and Pulling [25] analyze the Major League Soccer and the Premier League, respectively, and they find higher rates of goals with a direct delivery (center and shot), than indirect plays. On the other hand, in national and world championships, corners that end in goals are characterized by indirect deliveries [5].
As regards the number of players who intervene on the ball during the execution of the corner kick, we find again notable differences depending on the competition, the season and the type of football. Specifically, in league championships more goals are achieved with the intervention of only two players, the server and the auctioneer [25]. On the other hand, in world championships, it was found that despite the fact that in most of the corners only the server intervenes [11], different studies have shown that the highest efficiency rates occur when 3–4 attackers intervene with a minimum of elaboration [16] in both men’s and women’s soccer [6]. As for the type of defense used in the corners, the data seem to indicate that the most effective type of defense is the defense that performs a mixed marking, that is to say, combining zone defense with individual defense. These results are consistent regardless of the competition analyzed, such as world cups, UEFAS Euro and Champions League [5,17]. Differences are also found between the best and worst teams, specifically the best teams use mixed defenses, while the worst teams use zonal defenses [11]. Finally, the organization of the attacking players is also a variable that modulates the success of these actions. The dynamic organization in attacking, that is to say, the mobility of the players in the opponent’s goal area, such as maneuvers to create and occupy spaces, clearings, dragging and numerical superiorities are behaviors that increase the potential for success of corners both in league championships [11], and of world cup [16] championships such as the UEFA Euro [10]. In women’s soccer, this variable still needs more research.
In view of these results, a study on the evolution of corner kicks in top-level football is justified. The objective of this research was to analyze the relationship between different variables that may have modulated or conditioned the corner kicks throughout the 2010, 2014 and 2018 FIFA World Cups. For this, three types of complementary analysis were carried out: the statistic x2 was calculated to know if there were differences between the categories of each variable; next, a log linear analysis was implemented to know the relationship between the selected categorical variables, in terms of odds; finally, the decision tree technique was implemented to predict, classify and select the predictor variables that present the greatest gain in information in this type of action in top-level football.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Design

Among the possible designs that observational methodology can present, a nomothetic, intersessional follow-up and multidimensional design was applied [26,27]. The systematic observation carried out was non-participant and active, using an observational sampling “all occurrence” [28]. The study of physical and sports activity opens up immense possibilities for research planning, considering observation as mixed methods in itself [29,30,31].
The observational methodology has a good fit in the analysis of behaviors in soccer, mainly due to the following characteristics [27,29,30,31]: the degree of intervention and manipulation is zero; the study of behavior is based on terms of spontaneity and genuineness; the behavior occurs in its natural context, guaranteeing the absence of intrusively caused alterations. This methodology is integrated within the mixed methods perspective [29,30,31,32,33,34,35] since it allows the integration of qualitative and quantitative data.

2.2. Sample

In the present study, the units of analysis were the corner kicks taken in the high performing football, in this case the FIFA World Cup 2010 (n = 577), FIFA World Cup 2014 (n = 585) and FIFA World Cup 2018 (n = 542). In total, 192 matches and 1704 corner kicks were analyzed. The observation sample was a convenience sample [26,36].

2.3. Instruments

The observation instrument proposed by Casal et al. [5] was used. An observation instrument consisting of a combination of field formats and category systems was used, as advised [29].

2.4. Procedure

The matches were obtained through public television images and have been recorded and analyzed post-event. The quality control of the data collected was carried out by means of an inter-observer concordance analysis, using Cohen’s Kappa coefficient [37] for each criterion, the global value being 0.90 according to the scale of Fleiss, Levin & Paik [38] (Table 1).
The reliability of the dataset was analyzed by comparing the data recorded by the four observers who analyzed and annotated the video recordings. The four observers are expert participants (two Graduate and two Doctors in Physical Activity and Sport Sciences and certified national soccer coaches, and they participated in a purpose-designed 16 h training program [28,29]). These 16 h of training took place across eight face-to-face training sessions.
First, four training sessions were conducted with the observers, each lasting two hours. The study problem statement was presented to the observers, the behaviors to be observed were defined, the observation instrument was presented and explained, and the observers were trained in the use of the observation instrument. Subsequently, two sessions were held that consisted of the observation and recording of 50 actions, where the observers became familiar with the conduct record and the discrepancies found were discussed. Finally, in the last two sessions, doubts about the observation instrument and the corner kicks were discussed again, terminating the training when all the observers reached a Cohen’s Kappa [37] concordance higher than 0.9.
For the present study, the R program was used to find the Kappa coefficient, in order to contrast the observations. This coefficient is used to quantify the degree of agreement between observers, correcting the random factor, mathematically exposed as follows: κ = 1 k p o p e 1 p e × 100 , where “k” is the number of categories, “po” is the observed probability and “pe” is the expected probability.
The software used for the subsequent analyzes was also the R program (version R 1.0-37). The data were collected and coded using LINCE PLUS software [39].
The University Ethics Committee confirmed that the study respects the data protection of European law (General Data Protection Regulation), as well as the Declaration of Helsinki (2013).

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive and Bivariate Results

To begin this comparative analysis of the corner kicks, a description of the three competitions studied was carried out. Given the categorical format of the variables, the frequencies accompanied by their total percentages were recorded. The chi-square statistic was calculated to know if there were differences between the categories of the variable (Table 2).
Table 2 shows all the criteria and categories considered for each of the competitions, as well as the frequencies and percentages for each category, in order to know if there are differences between the different FIFA World Cups considered. The tests indicate that the criteria time, number of attackers, number of defenders, delivery mode, interaction context, trajectory of the ball, type of marking, defenders at the posts, number of attackers involved, delivery zone, match status and criterion2.ShotOnGoal show significant differences between the different FIFA World Cups.

3.2. Results of the Log Linear Analysis for Three Categorical Variables

A model of independence of the three variables MatchStatus, Time and InterveningAttackers was proposed using a function of the Poisson family. The lack of fit of the model made it necessary to propose a more complex model; the homogeneous association model was selected, which allowed the variables to be related to each other. Observing the residual deviation in relation to the degrees of freedom, the fit was verified, and the association between the variables and the effects of one on the other was described, in addition to calculating the coefficients of the interactions and their odds ratios.
In the tested model, interactions were added for each combination of variables in pairs, which would be a homogeneous association.
log ( μ i j k ) = λ + λ i X + λ j Y + λ k Z + λ i j X Y + λ i k X K + λ j k Y K
Log-linear models for three-dimensional tables
μ i j k : expected frequency of the cell (i, j, k): the average of the λ la media de los logaritmos de las frecuencias
λ : the mean of the logarithms of the frequencies
λ i X : effect of category i of factor X;
λ j Y : effect of category j of factor J;
λ k Z : effect of category k of factor Z;
λ i j X Y : interaction effect of category i of factor X and category j of factor Y;
λ i k X K : interaction effect of category i of factor X and category K of factor;
λ j k Y K : interaction effect of category j of factor Y and category k of factor K.
In Table 3, the results for the 2010 FIFA World Cup competition are presented.
As a general rule, the value of the residual deviance should be close to the value of the degrees of freedom. In this case, the value of the residual deviance is 1.7381 and the degrees of freedom are 4. The deviation statistic has an approximate chi-square distribution, so the value of the |2 test was calculated, which was 0.783, which indicated the probability of obtaining a deviation as large or larger than the observed one. It is concluded that this model shows a good fit.
In Table 4, the results for the 2014 FIFA World Cup competition are presented.
In this case the value of the residual deviance is 5.8082 and the degrees of freedom are 4. The value of the |2 statistic was 0.213, which indicated a good fit.
Table 5 presents the results for the 2018 FIFA World Cup competition.
The value of the residual deviance is 8.86 and the degrees of freedom are 4. The value of the |2 statistic was 0.064, which indicated a minimum adjustment.
Next, Table 6 shows the Odds ratio values by competition.
In the 2010 FIFA World Cup, in a first interaction (InterveningAttackers*MatchStatus), the situations of InterveningAttackers with 3–4 players were estimated to have a probability of missing 1512 times higher than play situations InterveningAttackers 1–2. In the case of InterveningAttackers with 3–4 players, the probability of winning was 2022 times higher than with InterveningAttackers 1–2.
For the interaction (InterveningAttackers*Time), the situation InterveningAttackers 3–4 had an estimate that the probability of using Time31–60 was 1111 times higher than InterveningAttackers 1–2. However, in the case of InterveningAttackers 3–4, the probability of using the Time31–60 increased 1911 compared to InterveningAttackers 1–2.
Finally, the interaction (MatchStatus*Time) in the 2010 competition, the situation of losing in the scoreboard made the estimated probability of using Time31–60 was 1422 times higher than with a winning scoreboard. In the winning situation, the estimated probability of using Time31–60 was 2464 times higher than with a losing scoreboard. In the case of losing the estimated probability of using Time61–90 increases 2891 times over compared to a winning scoreboard. In the situation of winning, the estimated probability of using Time61–90 increases 4195 times compared to losing (Table 6).
In FIFA World Cup 2014, first interaction (InterveningAttackers*MatchStatus), in the case of InterveningAttackers being 3–4 the probability of losing scoreboard was 1644 higher than the situations of InterveningAttackers 1–2 players, a slightly higher value compared to the 2010 competition. For the case of InterveningAttackers with 3–4 players, the estimated probability of winning was 2850 higher than the situation of InterveningAttackers 1–2.
The interaction (InterveningAttackers*Time), in the situation InterveningAttackers 3–4, the estimate of the probability of using Time31–60 was 0.950 times greater than in the case of using InterveningAttackers 1–2. For the case of InterveningAttackers 3–4 the probability to use Time61–90 was 2557 times higher compared to using InterveningAttackers 1–2.
The interaction (MatchStatus*Time), the situation of a losing scoreboard gave an estimated probability of using Time31–60 that was 1378 times higher than winning. In the situation of winning, the estimated probability of using Time31–60 was 2460 times higher than losing. In the case of losing the estimated probability of using Time61–90, it increases 3035 times compared to winning. In the situation of winning, the estimated probability of using Time61–90 increases by 4676 times compared to losing (Table 6).
Finally, FIFA World Cup 2018, the first interaction (InterveningAttackers*MatchStatus) in the category InterveningAttackers 3–4 the estimate of the probability of a losing scoreboard was 0.997 higher than in the situations of InterveningAttackers 1–2, a lower value than in the other two competitions. In the InterveningAttackers 3–4 situation it was estimated that the probability of winning was 2912 times higher than in the InterveningAttackers 1–2 situation.
In the second interaction (InterveningAttackers*Time), in the situation InterveningAttackers 3–4 it was estimated that the probability of using the Time31–60 was 0.163 higher than using the category InterveningAttackers 1–2. For InterveningAttackers 3–4 using Time61–90, the probability increases by 0.837 compared to InterveningAttackers 1–2.
The interaction (MatchStatus*Time), in the situation to a losing scoreboard, the estimated probability of using a Time31–60 was higher by 2.66 times than winning, while in the case of winning the estimated probability using a Time31–60 increase 2.99 compared to losing. Regarding the category Time61–90, the situation of losing has an estimated probability of 5.76 times higher than winning. In the situation of winning, the estimated probability of using the category Time61–90 increases 4.80 compared to losing (Table 6).

3.3. Decision Tree Analysis Results

The decision tree technique was applied for decision making in the three competitions, 2010, 2014 and 2018, for the explained variable “criterion1.Shot”. This criterion refers to whether the attacking team makes a shot on goal as a result of the execution of the corner kick. Following previous studies [5,13], the achievement of a shot in the corner kicks is considered a performance indicator. It has been tried to find the predictor variable that presents the greatest gain in information compared to the explained variable. In this way, it is possible to efficiently identify the most important variables in the tactical construction of corner kicks to achieve a shot on goal.
In the development of the decision trees, two samples were built, a training sample to generate a predictive model that consists of 70% of the total records, and a test sample of 30%, to verify the effectiveness of the model.

3.3.1. FIFA World Cup 2010

The starting variable uses 404 observations with a loss of 107 and a probability of 78.61% that “no” will occur, while there is a probability of 32.38% that “yes” will occur. Next, the algorithm includes the categories InterveningAttackers = 1–2, terminal node, with 360 observations and a loss of 77 observations where the probability of not achieving “criterion1.Shot”, is 78.61% and a probability of achieving it of 21.38%, the InterveningAttackers = 3–4 category with 44 observations and 14 losses, shows a probability of 68.18% of not being able to achieve “criterion1.Shot” and a probability of 31.81% of achieving it. The next variable included by the algorithm was “Time” with the intervention of two categories: Time = 0’–30’,31’–60’, terminal node, with 19 observations with 8 losses with a probability of not achieving “criterion1.Shot” of 57.89% and 42.10% of achieving it, and another category Time = 61’–90’, collects 25 observations with a loss of 3, obtained a favorable probability of 88% and a probability of not obtaining it of 12%. It is a terminal node (Figure 1).
Training data (Table 7)
Learning data (Figure 2)
The area under the curve is 1.
This metric represents the overall accuracy and measures the overall agreement between what the model predicted and what was observed in the test data set.
Below is a table with the results of prediction accuracy, error rate, precision, sensitivity, specificity and F1 score (Table 8).
The usual process we followed consisted of dividing the sample into two sets, a training set and a test set. The training data are used to fit a prediction model and the test data are used to check the behavior of the estimated model.
The supervised learning model is group into sets with similar characteristics, and the response variable is the group to which each item belongs. These are the results (Table 9):
To study whether we need to keep the whole tree or only a part of it, we resort to Cross-Validation as a measure of “efficiency” of the number of branches.
The fitting process optimizes the model parameters so that the model fits the training data as well as it can. Most commonly, 10-fold cross-validation is used.
This model can be evaluated in terms of prediction, in such a way that it is verified that it has predicted that there is no “criterion1.Shot” a total of 67 times when it really has not been given, and 4 times when “criterion1.Shot” really has been given. The model estimates that the “criterion1.Shot” has not occurred 42 times when it has actually not occurred. The model says that “criterion1.Shot” does not occur 8 times while actually “criterion1.Shot” has been given.
Finally, the effectiveness of the model is evaluated, adding all the successes and dividing by the number of predictions, obtaining an effectiveness of 16.13% of successes.
An alternative method for evaluating classifiers to the above metric is the Receiver Operating Characteristic (ROC) curve. The ROC curve is a graphical representation of classifier performance showing the distribution of the fractions of true positives and false positives. The fraction of true positives is known as sensitivity, which is the probability of correctly classifying an individual whose true status is defined as positive. Specificity is the probability of correctly classifying an individual whose true status is classified as negative. This is equal to subtracting one from the fraction of false positives.

3.3.2. FIFA World Cup 2014

The terminal branches of this tree are six. The root node is criterion1.Shot with 410 observations and 120 losses, with a 70.73% probability of not obtaining criterion1.Shot, and a 29.26% of obtaining it. Combined with InterveningAttackers = 1–2 and its 221 observations and a loss of 2, it obtained a 99.09% chance of not getting criterion1.Shot and 0.90% of getting it.
Another terminal branch is formed by InterveningAttackers = 3–4 → OffensiveOrganization = static → DeliveryOfBall = indirect, with a total of 34 observations and a loss of 9 with a probability of not achieving criterion1.Shot of 73.52% and a probability of achieving it of 26.47%. The third branch of the decision tree would be formed by InterveningAttackers = 3–4 → DeliveryOfBall = direct → NoOfAttackers = 4–5 with 36 observations and 16 losses. The probability of not reaching criterion1.Shot was 55.55% and 44.44% of achieving it. Branch four would be formed by InterveningAttackers = 3–4 → DeliveryOfBall = direct → NoOfAttackers = 2–3, between 6 and 12 observations with 3 losses and with a probability of not reaching criterion1.Shot of 25% and therefore a probability to achieve criterion1.Shot of 75%. The fifth terminal node presents the following Development: InterveningAttackers = 3–4 → OffensiveOrganization = static → ZoneToWhichPassIsMade = far post with 36 observations and a loss of 9, where the probability of not achieving criterion1.Shot was 25% and of achieving it 75%. The last terminal node was configured InterveningAttackers = 3–4 → OffensiveOrganization=dynamic with 71 observations with 14 losses where the probability of not getting criterion1.Shot was 19.71% and of getting criterion1.Shot was 80.28% (Figure 3).
This model can be evaluated in terms of prediction (Table 10), in such a way that it is verified that it has predicted that there is no “criterion1.Shot” a total of 69 times when it really has not been given, and 11 times when “criterion1.Shot” really has been given. The model estimates that the “criterion1.Shot” has not occurred 13 times when it has actually not occurred. The model says that “criterion1.Shot” does not occur 35 times when “criterion1.Shot” has actually been given.
Finally, the effectiveness of the model is evaluated, adding all the successes and dividing by the number of predictions, obtaining an effectiveness of 81.25% of successes.
Training data
Learning data (Figure 4)
The area under the curve is 0.649 with a deviation of 0.022. In Table 11 below, the learning model is presented.
Below is a table with the results of prediction accuracy, error rate, precision, sensitivity, specificity and F1 score (Table 12).

3.3.3. FIFA World Cup 2018

The root node was formed by 379 observations with 109 losses and with a probability of not achieving criterion1.Shot of 71.24% and a probability of achieving it of 28.75%. The first terminal node is formed by: Transcendence = no → shot EndZone = near post with 167 observations and 33 losses; it has a probability of not getting criterion1.Shot of 80.23% and 19.76% of getting it. The second terminal node: Transcendence = no shot → EndZone = far post → ZoneToWhichPassIsMade = far post, with 131 observations and 29 losses with a probability of not obtaining criterion1.Shot of 77.86% and 22.13% obtaining it. The third terminal node was Transcendence = no shot → EndZone = far post → ZoneToWhichPassIsMade = near post → Time = 0’–30’ with 19 observations and 5 missed. The probability of not achieving criterion1.Shot was 73.68% and of achieving it was 26.31%. The fourth terminal node: Transcendence = no shot → EndZone = far post → ZoneToWhichPassIsMade = near post → Time = 31’–60’, 61’–90’ → MatchStatus = winning, losing, based on 26 observations with 11 losses and the probability of not obtaining criterion1.Shot was 57.69%, and to obtain it, 42.30%. The fifth terminal node: Transcendence = no shot → EndZone = far post → ZoneToWhichPassIsMade = near post → Time = 31’–60’, 61’–90’ → MatchStatus = drawing had 17 observations with a loss of 5, generating a probability of not achieving criterion1.Shot of 29.41% and 70.58% of achieving it. Finally, the sixth terminal node: Transcendence = victory, draw, Open the marker, sentence/consolidate, inconsequential, help to victory with 19 observations and no losses, had a probability of achieving criterion1.Shot of 100% (Figure 5).
It is verified that the model has predicted that there is no “criterion1.Shot” a total of 63 times when it really has not been given, and 4 times when “criterion1.Shot” really has been given. The model estimates that the “criterion1.Shot” has not occurred 29 times when it has actually not occurred. The model says that “criterion1.Shot” does not occur 9 times when “criterion1.Shot” has actually been given 9 times.
Finally, the effectiveness of the model is evaluated, adding all the successes and dividing by the number of predictions, obtaining an effectiveness of 69.57% of successes.
Training data (Table 13)
Learning data (Figure 6)
The area under the curve is 0.560 with a deviation of 0.016. Below, in Table 14, the learning model is presented.
Below is a table with the results of prediction accuracy, error rate, precision, sensitivity, specificity and F1 score (Table 15).
Analyzing the results, similar results are found in the FWC2010 and FWC2014 competitions, although with notable differences. Specifically, for both championships the algorithm selects the criterion “intervening attackers” as the one that presents the most information gain. When 3–4 players intervene and the corner kick is executed 61–90‘, the probability of finishing on goal is 88%. On the other hand, in FWC2014, with the intervention of 3–4 attackers, and dynamic offensive organization, the probability of getting a shot is 80.28%. Finally, in the FWC2018, all the goals have contributed to add points to the teams, but instead the “no shot” predicts and classifies better.

4. Discussion

The main objective of this study was to find out the possible differences in the execution and tactical approach of one of the most important set pieces actions in high-performance soccer, such as corner kicks. Studies such as the one by Carling et al. [1] have shown that these are actions of high regularity, with different forms of execution [25], which produce very important goals in decisive moments for the teams [6] although of reduced effectiveness [8,11].
Knowing the evolution of the tactical behavior associated with corner kicks in high–level football stands as an important aspect in order to continue deepening the criteria of effectiveness of these actions, which according to previous literature is between 2% and 4% (1:50) [5,16]. Specifically, it takes 50 corner kicks to reach a goal, 10 to reach a shot between the three goalposts, and 4 to reach a shot.
A first interpretation that emerges as a result of the results presented in Table 2 confirms that the offensive and defensive tactical approach to corner kicks has changed over the last three world championships. This shows that high-performance football, and more specifically corner kicks, are not immobile and fixed tactical situations, but rather evolve by adapting according to gender (men’s or women’s football) [5,13], to the type of defense used and the goalkeepers (Kubayi & Larkin, 2019), the circumstances of the competition [24], the different players [25], as well as depending on the match status [23].
At the offensive level, it is possible to verify that the teams attack more in the first third of the game (0–30’), since they execute more deliveries at this moment of the game, than in the previous FIFA World Cups (p = 0.003). This may be due to the different styles of play implemented by the teams, where from the first minutes they look for greater options to get ahead on the scoreboard.
Regarding the number of attacking players ready to shoot on goal, it is possible to affirm that the number of players within the area has increased significantly (p = < 0.001) in the last three world championships. This shows that offensive soccer has changed, and that teams are currently more offensive, with a greater number of players attacking in search of finishing the move [40]. This increase in the will to attack in professional football may be due to the low success rate of the attacks, since it only reaches 1% in dynamic play [41], and 2.3% in set pieces [16,42].
Regarding the delivery of the ball to the finishing area (penalty area), most of the deliveries are made by direct shots and by air routes. Once again, it is possible to verify that despite the fact that the variable that improves the prediction of the shot on goal is the one that involves 3–4 players [6], the teams opt for a quick finish on goal, with only the intervention of the server and auctioneer.
Regarding the area for delivering and finishing the corner kick, once again the teams opt for delivering and finishing at the first post of the goal, a fact that corroborates previous studies [1].
Regarding the match status at the time of executing the corner kick, it is possible to verify that more and more actions are executed with the team losing on the scoreboard. This a priori may be logical, since losing invites teams to seek more attack in order to equalize the score.
Finally, with regard to the results of the log linear analysis (Table 6), an independence model of three specific variables was proposed, such as: “Match Status”, “Time” and “Intervening Attackers”. Based on the results in Table 6, it is possible to affirm that in the three FIFA World Cups, the teams have a higher probability of winning on the scoreboard with the intervention of 3–4 players. This result is especially outstanding, since on the one hand it corroborates previous studies that highlighted the need to provide more tactical construction to corner kicks [5,6]; and on the other hand it highlights that the usual practices of the teams in these actions (based on more than 90% of direct deliveries, where potentially only 1–2 players intervene) are clearly ineffective (recent studies confirm that through this conduct, only a goal is scored in 1 out of every 50 corner kick).
On the other hand, and due to the different styles of play implemented and the technical and tactical characteristics of the different players who have participated in the three FIFA World Cups analyzed, it is possible to affirm that the probability of winning during the different championships is higher in the last third of the game (61–90’) (OR = 4.19 for FWC2010; OR = 4.67 for FWC2014; OR = 4.80 for FWC2018). This confirms that the best teams impose their individual and collective quality in the final moments of the match.
Finally, decision tree-based analysis made it possible to predict, classify, and segment the variables considered in the present study, in addition to creating a classification model based on flow diagrams. The criterion1: shot has been selected, since it is of interest to know which is the most robust predictor variable associated with the shot on goal in this type of action. This would provide the different teams with recommendations at the offensive level to achieve a shot on goal in the highest-level football, such as the FIFA World Cup.
Specifically, for the FIFA World Cup 2010, with the intervention of 1–2 attackers, only 20% of the corner kicks were finished, on the other hand, with the intervention of 3–4 attackers in the last third of the match, almost 90% of the corner kicks are finished off. The reasons for these high percentages can be found in the rival team’s fatigue, the surprise generated by a corner kick executed under a minimum of elaboration and with the association of several players (it is important to remember that, in view of the results of the Table 1, 81% of the actions are executed with direct delivery, and practically 88% only involve 1 or 2 players). These results confirm previous studies [5], where mention is made of the last minutes of the match, as well as giving the corner kick a tactical elaboration involving more than two players. This can create uncertainty and surprise for the defending team, as their defensive positions are prepared for direct deliveries.
Regarding the decision tree for the 2014 FIFA World Cup championship, once again the variable that presents the greatest gain in information is the variable “intervening attackers”. Specifically, when 3–4 attackers intervene in the corner kick, through a dynamic offensive organization (with movements of the players), practically 80% of the actions are finished off. The same percentage occurs when the delivery is made to the far post, forcing defenders and rival goalkeeper to move, and change their starting positions. This can generate misalignments and doubts in the opponent’s defensive tactical approach, since they must maintain visual contact with the attackers and with the ball simultaneously.
Finally, the data collected in Figure 5 refer to the 2018 FIFA World Cup. The variable with the greatest gain in information is the variable “transcendence”. This variable refers to the significance of the goal that is scored as a result of a corner kick. The specifications of this variable can be consulted in the study of [16]. Specifically, the goals produced with the intervention of 3–4 attackers, which were finished off in the far post, have awarded points for the teams, or have been transcendental. These results once again show that, although a majority of corner kicks are sent to the near post of the goal [2], the highest efficiency rates are found in the far post.
Making a comparison of the results for the 2010, 2014 and 2018 FIFA World Cup championships, it is possible to affirm that there are similarities and differences between the three analyzed championships. In the first place, and in relation to the 1: shot criterion, the number of attackers involved is the most important variable in the 2010 and 2014 championships. This corroborates previous studies [5,6], where similar results were found. On the other hand, the model does not find significant results for the 2018 championship. A possible explanation may be due to the evolution of football and its players in this type of competition, as recorded by Castellano, Perea and Hernández-Mendo [43]. Differences are also found in the next node of the three arranged models. Specifically, the “time” criterion that appears in Figure 1 highlights the importance of executing the corner kick in the final minutes to get a shot [6], but on the other hand, in the 2014 and 2018 championships, it does not seem be an important criterion. One of the possible causes may be the intensity of the match, or the type of defense used [44]. In previous works it is observed that teams evolve and adapt to the changing circumstances of the environment in these actions [5].

5. Limitations

The limitations of the present study can be summarized in two points. First, in this study only one world championship was analyzed. In order to generalize the results, it is also necessary to analyze other types of tournaments. Second, the paucity of scientific studies on the tactical aspects of women’s football has not made it possible to compare our results in depth with previous studies.

6. Conclusions

The objective of this study was to analyze the evolution of one of the actions that is repeated most regularly during high-level soccer. To do this, the corner kicks executed in the last three world championships were analyzed. The available results allow us to affirm that the technical and tactical behaviors associated with these actions have evolved from one championship to another, probably due to the tactical training work of the teams. In addition, the results of the multivariate decision tree and linear log analysis made it possible to know which are the variables that decisively intervene in the success of this type of action. Finally, the results of the present study can be used by coaches to create different training situations where success in these types of actions can be enhanced.

7. Practical Applications

Coaches can use these findings to manipulate the training tasks related to the management and success of these types of actions, thus increasing the options for shooting at goal. On the other hand, knowing the variables that are conditioning a shot or a goal in corners will also allow to propose new defensive alternatives to the teams, providing information that allows opting for novel defensive mechanisms.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, R.M. and A.A.; methodology, M.P.; software, J.L.L.; formal analysis, J.L.L.; investigation, R.M., A.A., M.P. and J.L.L.; writing—original draft preparation, R.M.; writing—review and editing, A.A., M.P. and J.L.L.; supervision, J.L.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Acknowledgments

The authors gratefully acknowledge the support of a Spanish government subproject Mixed method approach on performance analysis (in training and competition) in elite and academy sport (PGC2018–098742–B–C33) (2019–2021) (del Ministerio de Ciencia, Innovación y Universidades (MCIU), la Agencia Estatal de Investigación (AEI) y el Fondo Europeo de Desarrollo Regional (FEDER)), that is part of the coordinated project New approach of research in physical activity and sport from mixed methods perspective (NARPAS_MM) (SPGC201800X098742CV0).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Carling, C.; Williams, A.M.; Reilly, T. Handbook of Soccer Match Analysis: A Systematic Approach to Improving Performance; Routledge: London, UK, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  2. Taylor, J.B.; James, N.; Mellalieu, S.D. National analysis of corner kicks inenglish premier league soccer. In Science and Football V, Proceedings of the Fifth World Congress on Science and Football, Lisbon, Portugal, April 2003; Reilly, T., Cabri, J., Araujo, D., Eds.; Routlegde: London, UK, 2005; pp. 225–230. [Google Scholar]
  3. Liu, H.; Yi, Q.; Giménez, J.; Gómez, M.A.; Lago-Peñas, C. Performance profiles of football teams in the UEFA Champions League considering situational efficiency. Int. J. Perform. Anal. Sport 2015, 15, 371–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Maneiro, R.; Ardá, A.; Losada, J.L.; Casal, C.A.; Rial, A. El saque de esquina como indicador de rendimiento en fútbol. Una revisión empírica [The corner kick as an indicator of football performance. An empirical review]. Rev. Cienc. Deporte 2017, 13, 273–286. [Google Scholar]
  5. Casal, C.A.; Maneiro, R.; Ardá, T.; Losada, J.L.; Rial, A. Analysis of corner kick success in elite football. Int. J. Perform. Anal. Sport 2015, 15, 430–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Maneiro, R.; Casal, C.; Ardá, A.; Losada, J.L. Application of multivariant decision tree technique in high performance football: The female and male corner kick. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0212549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Yiannakos, A.; Armatas, V. Evaluation of the goal scoring patterns in European Championship in Portugal 2004. Int. J. Perform. Anal. Sport 2006, 6, 178–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Casal, A.C.; Maneiro, R.; Ardá, T.; Losada, J.L.; Rial, A. Effectiveness of indirect free kicks in elite soccer. Int. J. Perform. Anal. Sport 2014, 14, 744–760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. López-García, S.; Maneiro, R.; Ardá-Suárez, A.; Rial-Boubeta, A.; Losada-López, J.L.; Casal-Sanjurjo, C.A. Tiros libres indirectos en fútbol de alto nivel: Identificación de variables explicativas [Indirect free kicks in high-level football: Identification of explanatory variables]. Int. J. Med. Sci. Phys. Act. Sports 2018, 18, 247–268. [Google Scholar]
  10. Maneiro, R.; Ardá, T.; Rial, A.; Losada, J.L.; Casal, C.A.; López-García, S. Análisis descriptivo y comparativo de los saques de esquina. UEFA Euro 2012 [Descriptive and comparative analysis of corner kicks. UEFA Euro 2012]. Rev. Andal. Med. Deporte 2017, 10, 95–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Strafford, B.; Smith, A.; North, J.; Stone, J.A. Comparative analysis of the top six and bottom six teams’ corner kick strategies in the 2015/2016 English Premier League. Int. J. Perform. Anal. Sport 2019, 19, 904–918. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Alcock, A. Analysis of direct free kicks in the women’s football World Cup 2007. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 2010, 10, 279–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Beare, H.; Stone, J.A. Analysis of attacking corner kick strategies in the FA women’s super league 2017/2018. Int. J. Perform. Anal. Sport 2019, 19, 893–903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Sainz de Baranda, P.; López-Riquelme, D. Analysis of corner kicks in relation to match status in the 2006 World Cup. Eur. J. Sports Sci. 2012, 12, 121–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Siegle, M.; Lames, M. Games interruptions in elite soccer. J. Sport Sci. 2012, 30, 619–624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Ardá, A.; Maneiro, R.; Rial, A.; Losada, J.L.; Casal, C. Análisis de la eficacia de los saques de esquina en la copa del mundo de fútbol 2010. Un intento de identificación de variables explicativas [Analysis of the effectiveness of corner kicks in the 2010 soccer world cup. An attempt to identify explanatory variables]. Rev. Psicol. Deporte. 2014, 23, 165–172. [Google Scholar]
  17. Kubayi, A.; Larkin, P. Analysis of teams’ corner kicks defensive strategies at the FIFA World Cup 2018. Int. J. Perform. Anal. Sport 2019, 19, 809–819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Mara, J.; Wheeler, K.; Lyons, K. Attacking Strategies That Lead to Goal Scoring Opportunities in High Level Women’s Football. Int. J. Sports Sci. Coach. 2012, 7, 565–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Roxburgh, A.; Turner, G. UEFA Champions League Technical Report 2010/2011; UEFA: Nyon, Switzerland, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  20. Castelo, J. Tratado General de Fútbol. Guía Práctica de Ejercicios de Entrenamiento [General Football Treaty. Practical Guide to Training Exercises]; Paidotribo: Badalona, Spain, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  21. Mombaerts, E. Fútbol. Del análisis del Juego a la Formación del Jugador [Football. From Game Analysis to Player Training]; Inde: Barcelona, Spain, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  22. Maneiro, R. Análisis de Las Acciones a Balón Parado en el Fútbol de Alto Nivel: Saques de Esquina y Tiro Libre Indirecto. Un Intento de Identificación de Variables Explicativas [Analysis of Set Plays in High Performance Soccer: Corner Kick and Indirect Free Kick. An Attempt to Identify Explanatory Variables]. 2014. Available online: http://ruc.udc.es/dspace/handle/2183/12426 (accessed on 24 May 2021).
  23. Casal, C.; Losada, J.; Maneiro, R.; Ardá, T. Influencia táctica del resultado parcial en los saques de esquina en fútbol [Tactical influence of the partial result in corner kicks in football]. Int. J. Med. Sci. Phys. Act. Sport 2017, 17, 715–728. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Schmicker, R.H. An application of satscan to evaluate the spatial distribution of corner kick goals in major league soccer. Int. J. Comput. Sci. Sport 2013, 12, 70–79. [Google Scholar]
  25. Pulling, C. Long corner kicks in the English Premier League: Deliveries into the goal área and critical area. Kineziologija 2015, 47, 193–201. [Google Scholar]
  26. Anguera, M.T.; Hernández-Mendo, A. Avances en estudios observacionales de Ciencias del Deporte desde los mixed methods [Advances in observational studies of Sports Sciences from mixed methods]. Cuad Psicol. 2016, 16, 17–30. [Google Scholar]
  27. Anguera, M.T.; Camerino, O.; Castañer, M.; Sánchez-Algarra, P.; Onwuegbuzie, A.J. The Specificity of Observational Studies in Physical Activity and Sports Sciences: Moving Forward in Mixed Methods Research and Proposals for Achieving Quantitative and Qualitative Symmetry. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 2196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  28. Losada, J.L.; Manolov, R. The process of basic training, applied training, maintaining the performance of an observer. Qual. Quant. 2014, 49, 339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Anguera, M.T.; Portell, M.; Chacón-Moscoso, S.; Sanduvete-Chaves, S. Indirect Observation in Everyday Contexts: Concepts y Methodological Guidelines within a Mixed Methods Framework. Front. Psych. 2018, 9, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Anguera, M.T. Is It Possible to Perform “Liquefying” Actions in Conversational Analysis? The Detection of Structures in Indirect Observation. In The Temporal Structure of Multimodal Communication; Hunyadi, L., Szekrényes, I., Eds.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2020; Volume 164, pp. 45–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Anguera, M.T.; Blanco-Villaseñor, A.; Losada, J.L.; Sánchez-Algarra, P.; Onwuegbuzie, A.J. Revisiting the Difference Between Mixed Methods y Multimethods: Is It All in the Name? Qual. Quant. 2018, 2757–2770. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Creswell, J.W.; Plano Clark, V.L. Designing y Conducting Mixed Methods Research; Sage: Thousy Oaks, CA, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  33. Johnson, R.B.; Onwuegbuzie, A.J.; Turner, L.A. Toward a definition of mixed methods research. J. Mix. Methods Res. 2007, 1, 112–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Bazeley, P. Integrative analysis strategies for mixed data sources. Am. Behav. Sci. 2012, 56, 814–828. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Bazeley, P.; Kemp, L. Mosaics, triangles, y DNA: Metaphors for integrated analysis in mixed methods research. J. Mix. Methods Res. 2012, 6, 55–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Anguera, M.T.; Arnau, J.; Ato, M.; Martínez, R.; Pascual, J.; Vallejo, G. Métodos de Investigación en Psicología; Síntesis: Madrid, Spain, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  37. Cohen, J. A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 1960, 20, 37–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Fleiss, J.L.; Levin, B.; Paik, M.C. Statistical Methods for Rates and Proportions, 3rd ed.; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  39. Soto, A.; Camerino, O.; Iglesias, X.; Anguera, M.T.; Castañer, M. LINCE PLUS: Research software for behavior video analysis. Apunts. Educ. Física Esports 2019, 3, 149–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Sgrò, F.; Aiello, F.; Casella, A.; Lipoma, M. Offensive strategies in the European Football Championship 2012. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2016, 123, 792–809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Tenga, A.; Ronglan, L.T.; Bahr, R. Measuring the effectiveness of offensive match-play in professional soccer. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 2010, 10, 269–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Maneiro, R.; Losada, J.L.; Casal, C.; Ardá, A. Multivariate analysis of indirect free kick in the FIFA World Cup 2014. An. Psicol. 2017, 33, 461–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Castellano, J.; Perea, A.; Hernández-Mendo, A. Análisis de la evolución del fútbol a lo largo de los mundiales [Analysis of the evolution of football throughout the World Cups]. Psicothema 2008, 20, 928–932. [Google Scholar]
  44. Frias, T.; Duarte, R. Man-to-man or zone defense? Measuring team dispersion behaviors in small-sided soccer games. Trends Sport Sci. 2014, 21, 135. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Pruned classification tree of “criterion1.Shot” data with class distribution in the leaves. Championship: FIFA World Cup 2010.
Figure 1. Pruned classification tree of “criterion1.Shot” data with class distribution in the leaves. Championship: FIFA World Cup 2010.
Sustainability 13 07562 g001
Figure 2. ROC curve for 2010 FIFA World Cup competition.
Figure 2. ROC curve for 2010 FIFA World Cup competition.
Sustainability 13 07562 g002
Figure 3. Pruned classification tree of “criterion1.Shot” data with class distribution in the leaves. Championship: FIFA World Cup 2014.
Figure 3. Pruned classification tree of “criterion1.Shot” data with class distribution in the leaves. Championship: FIFA World Cup 2014.
Sustainability 13 07562 g003
Figure 4. ROC curve for 2014 FIFA World Cup Competition.
Figure 4. ROC curve for 2014 FIFA World Cup Competition.
Sustainability 13 07562 g004
Figure 5. Classification tree pruned from criterion1.Shot data in the 2018 competition, with class distribution on the leaves. Championship: FIFA World Cup 2018.
Figure 5. Classification tree pruned from criterion1.Shot data in the 2018 competition, with class distribution on the leaves. Championship: FIFA World Cup 2018.
Sustainability 13 07562 g005
Figure 6. ROC curve for 2018 FIFA World Cup Competition.
Figure 6. ROC curve for 2018 FIFA World Cup Competition.
Sustainability 13 07562 g006
Table 1. Data quality control. Cohen’s Kappa Values.
Table 1. Data quality control. Cohen’s Kappa Values.
CriterionO1O2O1O3O1O4O2O3O2O4O3O4Kappa
Time0.9760.9580.9430.9820.9760.9490.987
Position of corner0.9390.9230.9060.8530.8600.8470.986
Laterality of corner0.9950.9770.9860.9720.9810.9630.979
Number of attackers0.9150.9810.9710.8960.8860.9710.936
Number of defenders0.8390.9530.9160.8640.8380.9430.890
Interaction context0.9110.9470.9260.8800.8480.8740.897
Delivery of ball0.8820.8270.8760.9260.9880.9260.904
Path of ball0.8070.9070.8390.6570.6130.7630.846
Type of marking0.8950.7770.7890.9700.9700.8180.853
Defenders on the posts0.8280.8230.8130.9510.7790.7230.817
Intervening attackers0.8720.8870.9110.7930.8010.8160.846
Zone to which pass is made0.8290.7900.8390.6180.6700.6320.729
EndZone0.9190.9280.9300.9880.9910.9880.957
Offensive Organization0.8460.8550.8620.9720.9630.9930.916
Match status0.9580.9530.9580.9740.9800.9950.969
Criterion1.shot0.9490.9480.9490.9730.9880.9830.966
Criterion2.ShotOnGoal0.8910.9300.9610.8220.8680.8920.894
Criterion3.Goal0.8230.8090.7770.8700.8300.9060.819
Kappa0.9100.8910.8890.8610.8490.9520.903
Table 2. Descriptive analysis and |2 to know differences between championships.
Table 2. Descriptive analysis and |2 to know differences between championships.
World Cup 2010
N = 577
World Cup 2014
N = 585
World Cup 2018
N = 542
p. Overall
Time 0.003
0–30169 (29.3%)142 (24.3%)189 (34.9%)
31–60191 (33.1%)206 (35.2%)177 (32.7%)
61–90217 (37.6%)237 (40.5%)176 (32.5%)
PositionOfCorner 0.582
right316 (54.8%)312 (53.3%)280 (51.7%)
left261 (52.3%)273 (46.7%)262 (48.3%)
LateralityOfCorner 0.278
Natural275 (47.7%)289 (49.4%)284 (52.4%)
Switched302 (52.3%)296 (50.6%)258 (47.6%)
NoOfAttackers <0.001
2–311 (1.91%)14 (2.39%)10 (1.85%)
4–5435 (75.4%)441 (75.4%)353 (65.1%)
6 or more131 (22.7%)130 (22.2%)179 (33.0%)
NoOfDefenders <0.001
4–534 (5.89%)8 (1.37%)15 (2.77%)
6 or more543 (94.1%)577 (98.6%)527 (97.2%)
Interaction context 0.028
Inferiority559 (96.9%)551 (94.2%)525 (96.9%)
Equility18 (3.12%)34 (5.81%)17 (3.14%)
Delivery OfBall 0.001
Direct469 (81.3%)510 (87.2%)481 (88.7%)
Indirect108 (18.7%)75 (12.8%)61 (11.3%)
PathOfBall <0.001
Ground58 (10.1%)30 (5.13%)22 (4.06%)
Air519 (89.9%)555 (94.9%)520 (95.9%)
TypeOfMarking <0.001
Individual26 (4.51%)128 (21.9%)16 (2.95%)
Zone179 (31.0%)46 (7.86%)70 (12.9%)
Combined372 (64.5%)411 (70.3%)456 (84.1%)
DefenderOnThePosts <0.001
1308 (53.4%)297 (50.8%)326 (60.1%)
2119 (20.6%)26 (4.44%)175 (32.3%)
Zero150 (26.0%)262 (44.8%)41 (7.56%)
InterveningAttackers <0.001
1–2507 (87.9%)321 (54.9%)519 (95.8%)
3–470 (12.1%)264 (45.1%)23 (4.24%)
ZoneToWhichPassIsMade 0.023
Near post360 (62.4%)403 (68.9%)336 (62.0%)
Far post217 (37.6%)182 (31.1%)206 (38.0%)
EndZone <0.001
Near post321 (55.6%)339 (57.9%)246 (45.4%)
Far post256 (44.4%)246 (42.1%)296 (54.6%)
OffensiveOrganization 0.578
Static391 (67.8%)409 (69.9%)382 (70.5%)
dynamic186 (32.2%)176 (30.1%)160 (29.5%)
MatchStatus 0.001
Winning119 (20.6%)93 (15.9%)68 (12.5%)
Drawing326 (56.5%)318 (54.4%)327 (60.3%)
Losing132 (22.9%)174 (29.7%)147 (27.1%)
Criterion1.shot 0.702
No416 (72.1%)412 (70.4%)379 (69.9%)
Yes161 (27.9%)173 (29.6%)163 (30.1%)
Criterion2.ShotOnGoal 0.006
No516 (89.4%)521 (89.1%)545 (83.8%)
Yes61 (10.6%)64 (10.9%)88 (16.2%)
Criterion3.Goal 0.115
No564 (97.7%)562 (96.1%)518 (95.6%)
Yes13 (2.25%)23 (3.93%)24 (4.43%)
Table 3. Coefficients for the 2010 FIFA World Cup.
Table 3. Coefficients for the 2010 FIFA World Cup.
Estimate Std.ErrorZ ValuePr (>|z|)
(Intercept)4.730800.0929450.903<2e-16 ***
InterveningAttackers 3–4−2.577440.29732−8.669<2e-16 ***
MatchStatusl−1.472280.20900−7.0441.86e-12 ***
MatchStatusw−1.983360.25581−7.7538.96e-15 ***
Time31–60−0.093340.13376−0.6980.4853
Time61–90−0.344630.14222−2.4230.154 *
InterveningAttackers3–4: MatchStatusl0.413850.321941.2850.1986
InterveningAttackers3–4: MatchStatusw0.704340.314632.2390.0252 *
InterveningAttackers3–4: Time31–600.105410.373340.2820.7777
InterveningAttackers3–4: Time61–900.648120.342811.8910.0587
MatchStatusl: Time31–600.352240.279441.2610.2075
MatchStatusw: Time31–600.901840.312492.8860.0039 **
MatchStatusl: Time61–901.061890.263664.0275.64e-05 ***
MatchStatusw: Time61–901.433980.303864.7192.37e-06 ***
Signif. codes: 0 ‘***’ 0.001 ‘**’ 0.01 ‘*’ 0.05 ‘.’ 0.1 ‘ ’ 1.
Table 4. Coefficients for the 2014 FIFA World Cup.
Table 4. Coefficients for the 2014 FIFA World Cup.
Estimate Std.ErrorZ ValuePr (>|z|)
(Intercept)4.147410.1207634.343<25e-16 ***
InterveningAttackers 3–4–0.281840.17363–1.6230.104547
MatchStatusl–1.779430.26299–6.7661.32e-11 ***
MatchStatusw–2.346610.32618–7.1946.28e-13 ***
Time31–600.035170.164290.2140.830470
Time61–90–0.211760.17168–1.2330.217417
InterveningAttackers3–4: MatchStatusl0.033040.198540.1660.867835
InterveningAttackers3–4: MatchStatusw0.263690.241991.0900.275869
InterveningAttackers3–4: Time31–60–0.200300.22669–0.8840.376925
InterveningAttackers3–4: Time61–900.254300.222921.1410.253965
MatchStatusl: Time31–601.092240.299493.6470.000265 ***
MatchStatusw: Time31–601.402800.352053.9856.76e-05 ***
MatchStatusl: Time61–901.763000.285646.1726.74e-10 ***
MatchStatusw: Time61–901.148140.362213.1700.001525 **
Signif. codes: 0 ‘***’ 0.001 ‘**’ 0.05 ‘.’ 0.1 ‘ ’ 1.
Table 5. Table of coefficients for the 2018 FIFA World Cup.
Table 5. Table of coefficients for the 2018 FIFA World Cup.
Estimate Std.ErrorZ ValuePr (>|z|)
(Intercept)4.9569240.08353659.339<2e-16 ***
InterveningAttackers 3–4–2.8971510.337551–8.583<2e-16 ***
MatchStatusl–1.7523840.214541–8.1683.13e16 ***
MatchStatusw–2.5410080.298018–8.526<2e-16 ***
Time31–60–0.3215240.128877–2.4950.012603 *
Time61–90–0.7116470.144539–4.9248.50e-07 ***
InterveningAttackers3–4: MatchStatusl–0.0029710.574836–0.0050.995876
InterveningAttackers3–4: MatchStatusw1.0690110.5512921.9390.052489
InterveningAttackers3–4: Time31–60–1.8098800.786873–2.3000.021443 *
InterveningAttackers3–4: Time61–90–0.1772110.488453–0.3630.716754
MatchStatusl: Time31–600.9792200.2760343.5470.000389 ***
MatchStatusw: Time31–601.0985200.3704932.9650.003027 **
MatchStatusl: Time61–901.7525150.2693456.5077.69e-11 ***
MatchStatusw: Time61–901.5706370.3638954.3161.59e-05 ***
Signif. codes: 0 ‘***’ 0.001 ‘**’ 0.01 ‘*’ 0.05 ‘.’ 0.1 ‘ ’ 1.
Table 6. Odds ratio by competition.
Table 6. Odds ratio by competition.
FIFA World Cup 2010FIFA World Cup 2014FIFA World Cup 2018
(Intercept)113.38626696114.00000000142.15581459
InterveningAttackers 3–40.075968050.070175440.05518019
MatchStatusl0.229402000.228070180.17336023
MatchStatusw0.137606580.131578950.07878693
Time31–600.910883520.921052630.72504333
Time61–900.708481320.684210530.49083507
InterveningAttackers3–4: MatchStatusl1.512627691.644230770.99703313
InterveningAttackers3–4: MatchStatusw2.022517762.850000002.91249695
InterveningAttackers3–4: Time31–601.111164820.950000000.16367383
InterveningAttackers3–4: Time61–901.911941902.557692310.83760302
MatchStatusl: Time31–601.422247291.378021982.66237947
MatchStatusw: Time31–602.464142242.460952382.99972224
MatchStatusl: Time61–902.891830963.035502965.76909181
MatchStatusw: Time61–904.195350514.676923084.80971226
Table 7. Training model for the 2010 FIFA World Cup.
Table 7. Training model for the 2010 FIFA World Cup.
Prediction\ObservationNo   Criterion1.ShotYes   Criterion1.Shot
No   criterion1.Shot5110
Yes   criterion1.Shot049
Table 8. Classification metrics FIFA World Cup 2010.
Table 8. Classification metrics FIFA World Cup 2010.
Prediction AccuracyError RatePrecisionSensitivitySpecificityF1 Score
0.770.230.470.650.800.54
Stratified dummy classifier. Accuracy is 0.734841.
Table 9. Learning model for the 2010 FIFA World Cup.
Table 9. Learning model for the 2010 FIFA World Cup.
Prediction\ObservationNo   Criterion1.ShotYes   Criterion1.Shot
No   criterion1.Shot1110
Yes   criterion1.Shot04
Table 10. Training model for the 2014 FIFA World Cup.
Table 10. Training model for the 2014 FIFA World Cup.
Prediction\ObservationNo   Criterion1.ShotYes   Criterion1.Shot
No   criterion1.Shot5110
Yes   criterion1.Shot09
Table 11. Learning model for the 2014 FIFA World Cup.
Table 11. Learning model for the 2014 FIFA World Cup.
Prediction\ObservationNo   Criterion1.ShotYes   Criterion1.Shot
No   criterion1.Shot366199
Yes   criterion1.Shot218241
Table 12. Classification metrics FIFA World Cup 2014.
Table 12. Classification metrics FIFA World Cup 2014.
Prediction AccuracyError RatePrecisionSensitivitySpecificityF1 Score
0.830.170.980.640.990.78
Stratified dummy classifier. Accuracy is 0.699127.
Table 13. Training model for the 2018 FIFA World Cup.
Table 13. Training model for the 2018 FIFA World Cup.
Prediction\ObservationNo   Criterion1.ShotYes   Criterion1.Shot
No   criterion1.Shot37567
Yes   criterion1.Shot293103
Table 14. Learning model for the 2018 FIFA World Cup.
Table 14. Learning model for the 2018 FIFA World Cup.
Prediction\ObservationNo   Criterion1.ShotYes   Criterion1.Shot
No     criterion1.Shot634
Yes      criterion1.Shot299
Table 15. Classification metrics FIFA World Cup 2018.
Table 15. Classification metrics FIFA World Cup 2018.
Prediction AccuracyError RatePrecisionSensitiitySpecificityF1 Score
0.830.170.980.640.990.78
Stratified dummy classifier. Accuracy is 0.611298.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Maneiro, R.; Losada, J.L.; Portell, M.; Ardá, A. Observational Analysis of Corner Kicks in High-Level Football: A Mixed Methods Study. Sustainability 2021, 13, 7562. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13147562

AMA Style

Maneiro R, Losada JL, Portell M, Ardá A. Observational Analysis of Corner Kicks in High-Level Football: A Mixed Methods Study. Sustainability. 2021; 13(14):7562. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13147562

Chicago/Turabian Style

Maneiro, Rubén, José Luís Losada, Mariona Portell, and Antonio Ardá. 2021. "Observational Analysis of Corner Kicks in High-Level Football: A Mixed Methods Study" Sustainability 13, no. 14: 7562. https://doi.org/10.3390/su13147562

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop