Introduction
Our experience of the visual world as a rich, highly detailed and continuous space is an illusion constructed from a series of momentary snapshots encoded while the eyes are still (fixations) and stitched together across periods of blindness as the eyes move (saccadic eye movements) (see
Henderson, 2003, for summary). In order for us to maintain a representation of the scene across eye movements we must make predictions about the existence, location, and visual properties of objects not currently at the centre of our attention. The assumed continuity of these features make it possible for large and dramatic changes to go undetected (Change Blindness, see Simons and Levin, 1997) and unexpected and unattended features to go unnoticed (Inattentional Blindness, see Simons, 2000).
Change Blindness occurs when a visual change is masked by an artificial occlusion (
Simons, 1996;
Levin & Simons, 1997;
Rensink et al., 1997;
Simons & Levin, 1998;
O’Regan et al., 1999;
Rensink et al., 2000) or natural period of perceptual insensitivity, such as a saccadic eye movement or eye blink (
Carlson-Radvansky & Irwin, 1995;
Grimes, 1996;
Henderson & Hollingworth, 1999;
O’Regan et al., 2000), and participants fail to compare the changed scene to their memory of the scene (
Brockmole & Henderson, 2005;
Hollingworth & Henderson, 2002; Mitroff, Simons, & Levin, 2004). Transsaccadic and long term memory studies have shown that relatively detailed information is retained across saccades and over time (
Henderson & Hollingworth, 1999;
Hollingworth et al., 2001;
Melcher, 2001;
Hollingworth & Henderson, 2002; Tatler, Gilchrist, & Land, 2005;
Hollingworth, 2006; also see Võ, Schneider, & Matthias, and Humphrey & Underwood in this volume) but changes to this information do not reach the level of awareness unless the change violates critical expectations about the continuity of the scene and objects within it (
Levin & Simons, 2000). The likelihood of violating these expectations and as a result detecting the change increases as the significance and size of a change within a scene increases (
Rensink et al., 1997;
Hollingworth & Henderson, 2004;
Levin & Varakin, 2004; Henderson, Brockmole, & Gajewski, 2008). Small local changes to object features (
Henderson & Hollingworth, 1999;
Hollingworth et al., 2001;
Hollingworth & Henderson, 2002) and even slight global changes to the visual form of an entire scene (
Henderson & Hollingworth, 2003a;
Hollingworth & Henderson, 2004; Henderson, Brockmole, & Gajewski, 2008) have been shown to sometimes go undetected, but it is assumed that a total change from one visual scene to a completely different visual scene that is not obscured in any way, e.g. by a mask, would be guaranteed to be detected.
Most of us experience total scene changes thousands of times every day in film and television edits, yet we seem to be unaware of most of them (
Messaris, 1994). A typical ninety minute Hollywood film contains between one thousand and two thousand edits, a change in viewpoint (‘shot’) every 2.7 to 5.4 seconds (
Bordwell & Thompson, 2001), yet film editors assume that the majority of these edits are “
invisible” as the “
spectator’s illusion of seeing a continuous piece of action is not interrupted” (
Reisz & Millar, 1953; pg 216). This assumption is prevalent throughout the filmmaking community but has never been empirically tested. If film viewers are unaware of some film edits - a phenomenon we call
edit blindness - this would be evidence of extreme global change blindness. The main goal of this study was to see if there is any evidence that edit blindness occurs during the viewing of authentic feature films. If evidence of edit blindness can be found, the next step would then be to identify the conditions under which it occurs, how these relate to conventional editing practices, and what impact edit blindness has on our understanding of how we perceive a continuous visual world.
Although film has existed for over a century it has received very little psychological investigation. What little empirical research has been performed has typically investigated the relationship between the main structural conventions of Hollywood filmmaking, known as the Continuity Editing Rules, and our ability to comprehend narrative and spatial relationships (
Frith & Robson, 1975;
Carroll & Bever, 1976;
Hochberg & Brooks, 1978;
Kraft, 1987; d’Ydewalle & Vanderbeeken, 1990; Geiger & Reeves, 1993; d’Ydewalle, Desmet, & Van Rensbergen, 1998;
Germeys & d’Ydewalle, 2007). The Continuity Editing Rules are a suite of staging, filming, and editing conventions that a film maker can follow to ensure that the resulting film is not confusing, disorienting, or uncomfortable to watch (
Reisz & Millar, 1953;
Bordwell & Thompson, 2001; see Smith, 2006 for discussion). For example, one of the most prominent Continuity Editing Rules is the 180° Rule (
Bordwell & Thompson, 2001, pages 263-265). This rule states that when filming a scene a main
axis of action, down which most action occurs should be identified e.g. the line joining two characters engaged in a conversation. All static cameras used to film this scene should be positioned on the same side of this line. Keeping all cameras within the same 180° arc ensures that when shots from these cameras are edited together the relative position of objects on the screen and direction of any action is consistent. It also ensures that the eyelines of multiple characters match even when the characters do not appear on screen at the same time. Cutting to a camera located on the opposite side of the
axis of action (known as
Crossing the Line) is believed to lead to confusion and disorientation (For empirical investigations of the impact of
crossing the line see
Frith & Robson (
1975),
Kraft (
1987), d'Ydewalle, Desmet, & Van Rensbergen (1998), and Germeys & d’Ydewalle (2007).).
Film editors assume that one of the main benefits of adhering to the Continuity Editing Rules is
edit blindness. This hypothesis has only been tested in two indirect empirical studies. Both studies asked participants to detect transitions from one viewpoint to another while watching films that either adhered to or violated the Continuity Editing Rules (
d'Ydewalle & Vanderbeeken, 1990;
Schröder, 1990). Both studies found that cuts that violated the Continuity Editing Rules were detected faster than those that adhered to the rules, indicating more awareness of the discontinuity cuts (
d'Ydewalle & Vanderbeeken, 1990;
Schröder, 1990). However, these studies do no speak to the question of whether participants were aware of continuity cuts. In order to investigate this issue we first need to understand the conditions under which film editors believe edit blindness may occur.
Only a few film theorists have questioned how the Continuity Editing Rules create edit blindness. These theorists have provided a range of intriguing hypotheses that, on examination, may relate Continuity Editing directly to Inattentional Blindness and Change Blindness. Editors believe awareness of film editing can be minimised by coinciding cuts with moments when attention is occupied by another task such as comprehending the narrative (
Reisz & Millar, 1953;
Bordwell & Thompson, 2001), or hiding the cut transients during sudden onsets of motion (
Reisz & Millar, 1953;
Dmytryk, 1986;
Katz, 1991;
Anderson, 1996;
Bordwell & Thompson, 2001;
Pepperman, 2004), saccadic eye movements (
Dmytryk, 1986), or eye blinks (
Dmytryk, 1986;
Murch, 2001). These techniques parallel the methods used in Change Blindness studies to obscure the visual transients associated with a change (Carlson-Radvansky & Irwin, 1995;
Grimes, 1996;
Henderson & Hollingworth, 1999;
O’Regan et al., 1999;
Rensink et al., 2000;
O’Regan et al., 2000).
Both eye blinks and saccadic eye movements provide a period of perceptual insensitivity due to neural suppression (
Bristow et al., 2005; Diamond, Ross, & Morrone, 2000). Eye blinks occur 10-15 times a minute (once every 4-6 seconds) and last 100-150ms with the period of perceptual insensitivity extending for a few extra milliseconds before and after the blink itself (
Burr, 2005). Saccadic eye movements occur more frequently, 2-5 times per second (once every 200-500ms;
Yarbus, 1967), last less time (20-50ms), but have a period of perceptual insensitivity similar in duration to blinks, around 75ms before and 50ms after the eye movement (Diamond, Ross, & Morrone, 2000). The relative infrequency of blinks and the short duration of saccadic eye movements would mean that deliberately using these periods of perceptual insensitivity to hide cuts would require very precise timing. Such timing may be provided by the Continuity Editing Rules.
The most important Continuity Editing technique, to which most other rules are applied, is known as the analytical breakdown of scenes or Within Scene editing (
Reisz & Millar, 1953;
Bordwell & Thompson, 2001). Presenting a scene as a series of shots, beginning with the camera further away from the action (Long Shots) and gradually moving closer (Medium Shots and Close-Ups; see
Figure 1, Within Scenes) adds interest to the presentation of the scene and provides the viewer with the optimum viewpoint on the action (
Hochberg & Brooks, 1978). Given that a film viewer’s primary interest is following the film’s narrative, engaging them in this task by constantly changing viewpoint within a scene is believed to occupy attention and limit awareness of the editing (
Reisz & Millar, 1953;
Bordwell & Thompson, 2001). The technique of focusing attention on one task, e.g. instructing viewers to count the number of passes during a basketball game, in order to limit awareness of unrelated visual features, e.g. a man in a Gorilla suit, is commonly referred to as Inattentional Blindness (
Mack & Rock, 1998;
Simons & Chabris, 1999;
Simons, 2000). Within Scene editing may utilise the Inattentional Blindness created by focussing attention on the narratively significant action in order to limit the resources available for noticing the editing. However, Inattentional Blindess is typically only reported for local features within a scene (
Mack & Rock, 1998;
Simons & Chabris, 1999). Edit Blindness following Within Scene edits would be the first example of Inattentional Blindness for total global scene changes.
Inducing inattentional blindness via Within Scene editing may not be sufficient to minimize awareness of the visual transients associated with the cut. As in Change Blindness, the transients may need to be obscured, occluded, or suppressed. According to film editors, two sub-types of Within Scene edits may provide such periods of insensitivity: Match Action and Gaze Match edits. The Match Action editing rule (see
Figure 1, third row) states that a smooth cut between two viewpoints of the same action will be achieved by coinciding the cut with a sudden onset of motion (
Reisz & Millar, 1953;
Dmytryk, 1986;
Katz, 1991;
Anderson, 1996;
Bordwell & Thompson, 2001;
Pepperman, 2004). This technique may function by either obscuring the cut with motion blur (Pepperman, 2001), creating a saccadic eye movement towards the motion (
Dmytryk, 1986) or, with extreme changes in action, causing an eye blink (
Dmytryk, 1986;
Murch, 2001).
Saccadic suppression of the cut transients is also thought to be used by Gaze Match editing to limit awareness of cuts. The Gaze Match editing rule (see
Figure 1, fourth row) suggests that cutting from a shot of an actor looking off-screen to a shot of the target of the actor’s gaze will result in a seamless transition between the shots (
Reisz & Millar, 1953). This technique is believed to result in a saccadic eye movement along the line of the actor’s gaze in anticipation of the cut (
Dmytryk, 1986). Such attentional cuing by gaze shifts has been demonstrated using abstract and isolated human faces (
Friesen & Kingstone, 1998; Langton, Watt & Bruce, 2000), realistic faces in static photographs (Castelhano, Wieth, & Henderson, 2007; Birmingham, Biscof, & Kingstone, 2007) and has also been observed in dynamic scenes (
Kuhn & Tatler, 2005;
Tatler & Kuhn, 2007). However, in order for gaze cues to be used reliably by editors to create
edit blindness the majority of viewers would have to respond to the cue at the same time and the Continuity Editing rules would have to identify the precise timing of this response. Whether both of these requirements exist will be investigated in this study.
The present study investigated the existence of edit blindness by instructing participants to detect edits while watching excerpts from seven feature films. Eye movements were recorded during the task in order to identify whether undetected cuts coincided with periods of perceptual insensitivity such as saccades or blinks as hypothesized by film editors. All edits were categorised according to their adherence to the Continuity Editing Rules: Between Scenes (no continuity), Within Scenes (scene continuity), Match Action (scene and action continuity), and Gaze Match (scene and gaze continuity). The four edit types are illustrated in
Figure 1. If the application of the Continuity Editing techniques results in edit blindness, A) participants should be less likely to detect cuts that utilise these techniques compared with cuts that do not, i.e. Between Scene, B) detection of these cuts should be slower, and C) there should be evidence that the cuts that are not detected utilise one or more of the techniques for obscuring the transients of the cut outlined above: coincidence with motion blur, saccadic eye movement, or eye blink. Support for these hypotheses would provide the first empirical evidence of edit blindness during film viewing.
Method
Participants.
Seven members of the Edinburgh University community (3 female; mean age = 28.7 years) participated for payment (£5). All participants had normal or corrected to normal vision. Participation was voluntary. The experiment was conducted according to the British Psychological Society’s ethics guidelines.
Apparatus.
Eye movements were monitored by an SR Research Eyelink II eyetracker sampling eye position at 500Hz. Viewing was binocular, but only the right eye was tracked. The images were presented on a 21 inch cathode ray tube (CRT) monitor at a viewing distance of around 60 cm (participants were free to move their head (Participants mostly kept their heads stationary during each trial and any minor head movements were automatically compensated for by the eye tracker without impairing tracker accuracy.)) with a refresh rate of 100 Hz, resolution of 800 x 600 pixels x 32 bit. The experiment was controlled with SR Research Experiment Builder software. Responses were made via a Microsoft Sidewinder joypad. Raw gaze data and responses were time-locked to the video frames and recorded to the hard disk after the experiment. The analysis utilised both the raw and filtered data using SR Research Data Viewer to identify saccades, fixations and eye blinks. Saccades were detected using a 9-sample saccade detection model with a velocity threshold of 30°/sec, an acceleration threshold of 8000°/sec2, and a minimum amplitude of 0.5°. Eye blinks were identified whenever the pupil was occluded, e.g. by the eyelid, lost or severely distorted. Analysis of the eye movement data was performed via Matlab and SPSS.
Stimuli.
Participants were presented seven five-minute continuous excerpts extracted from feature films: Blade Runner (Ridley Scott, 1982), Citizen Kane (Orson Welles, 1941), Dogville (Lars Von Trier, 2003), October (Sergei Eisenstein, 1928), Requiem for a Dream (Darren Aronofsky, 2000), Dancer in the Dark (Lars Von Trier, 2000), and Koyaanisqatsi (Godfrey Reggio, 1982). These films were chosen as they covered a cross-section of film making styles and practices and vary in the degree to which they adhere to the continuity editing rules. For example, Blade Runner (Ridley Scott, 1982) utilises very strict and classic continuity while Dancer in the Dark (Lars Von Trier, 2000) consciously violates the continuity editing rules.
Excerpts from these films were chosen according to the following constraints. Each excerpt must begin at the start of a scene, not contain titles or subtitles (An exception was made for October (Sergei Eisenstein, 1928) a silent film using textual descriptions (intertitles) inserted between shots to communicate the narrative. All cuts to and from intertitles were excluded in the subsequent analysis.), last for exactly five minutes, and not contain offensive or violent imagery. Excerpts were extracted from digital versions of each film and rendered in the XviD MPEG-4 digital video format at a resolution of 720x576 and 25fps (DVD quality). Stereo audio was also extracted and synchronised to the video as a Wave file. These formats were chosen as they ensure synchronisation of the gaze recording and the video playback through Experiment Builder. The order in which the videos were presented was randomised across participants.
Procedure.
Participants were told that they would be shown seven clips from feature films. Each clip would last for five minutes. Their task was to watch the videos and press a button every time they saw an edit. An edit was defined as “Any transition from one viewpoint to another that could not have been produced by a single continuously filming camera.” A broad definition of ‘edit’ was used to ensure that participants would also identify optical transitions such as fades and wipes and digital effects such as the transformation of one image into another as edits. Participants were instructed that their responses would be timed so they should respond as quickly as possible. Participant understanding of the edit detection task was tested using a 2 minute practice film.
The Eyelink II head-mounted eye tracker was securely but comfortably fastened on the participants head. A latex swimming cap was worn over their hair to minimise slippage. The head position and distance relative to the monitor was checked and the right eyetracker camera was positioned. The main experiment began with a nine-point calibration of the eyetracker. Calibration was deemed acceptable if the gaze position was within 0.5° of a visual angle of the target. Each video was presented without interruption while the participant detected edits by pressing a button on the joypad. After each video a further drift correction was performed to compensate for drift and slippage.
After all seven videos had been presented participants completed a short questionnaire about the films. The questionnaire checked whether they had previously seen the films and how familiar the films were. All participants stated that they had either never seen the films before or could not recall them in detail. The whole experiment lasted less than 45 minutes.
Coding and Analysis.
In order to identify the impact of the continuity editing rules on participant awareness of film edits, all edits in the sample films were hand coded for their adherence to the rules. The films were coded according to five categories:
- 1)
Edit Type = Cut, Other. Was the transition between the two shots a straight cut?
- 2)
Shot Size = eXtreme Close-Up (XCU), Close-Up (CU), Close Medium Shot (CMS), Medium Shot (MS), Medium Long Shot (MLS), Long Shot (LS), eXtreme Long Shot (XLS). All shot sizes are specified relative to how much of a human figure would be visible in the frame if the top of the head was touching the top of the screen (Katz, 2001). A CU shows an entire head, MS shows the upper torso and LS a full body. The shot size following the cut is coded.
- 3)
Scene Continuity = Within Scene (Continuity), Between Scene (Discontinuity). Two shots are coded as belonging to the same scene if a focal object e.g. a person, belonging to the current scene is immediately present in the new shot.
- 4)
Match on Action = Yes, No. Two shots are coded as being joined by a Match Action edit if the subject of both shots is the same e.g. a person or object, and the subject begins an action in the first shot and continues it in the second shot.
- 5)
Gaze Match = Yes, No. Two shots are coded as being joined by a Gaze Match edit if the shots depict different subjects and both are looking offscreen in the direction of the other person, i.e. if the two images were overlaid their eyelines would meet. Cuts involving eyelines that do not meet or in which the screen location of the two people are identical across the cut were excluded.
Table 1.
General edit statistics for the seven feature film excerpts used in this study: Number of edits in the 5 minute excerpt (Num. Edits), Average Shot Length (ASL; seconds), Percentage of edits that are cuts (Cut; %), Percentage of cuts that were of shot size (Size; %) Close-Up (CU), Close Medium Shot ( CMS), orMedium Shot ( MS), Percentage of cuts of size CU, CMS, or MS that were classified as Between Scene, Within Scene, Match Action, or Gaze Match (%). Averages across all seven films are displayed in the bottom row.
Table 1.
General edit statistics for the seven feature film excerpts used in this study: Number of edits in the 5 minute excerpt (Num. Edits), Average Shot Length (ASL; seconds), Percentage of edits that are cuts (Cut; %), Percentage of cuts that were of shot size (Size; %) Close-Up (CU), Close Medium Shot ( CMS), orMedium Shot ( MS), Percentage of cuts of size CU, CMS, or MS that were classified as Between Scene, Within Scene, Match Action, or Gaze Match (%). Averages across all seven films are displayed in the bottom row.
Coding was performed by two independent coders both following the same guidelines. At the beginning of every shot the coder identified the size of the shot (Shot Size), whether the edit preceding the shot was a cut (Edit Type), and whether the two shots either side of the edit had continuity of scene (Within Scene), action (Match Action), or gaze (Gaze Match). Intercoder reliability across the five categories was on average 85%.
Discussion
The main hypothesis tested in this investigation was that the application of the Continuity Editing Rules would result in greater edit blindness than edits not composed according to the rules. Overall, the results support this hypothesis. Edits without any continuity of scene, action, or gaze (Between scene) were missed significantly less often (9.4%) than edits with continuity of scene (Within Scene=25.1%) or scene and action (Match Action=32.4%). The only continuity edits that did not exhibit significantly greater edit blindness were Gaze Match cuts (10.9%). The percentage of missed Within Scene and Match Action cuts may not seem remarkably high considering that other change blindness experiments report higher rates of change blindness, e.g. 66% failure to detect change to a centrally attended actor across a cut (
Levin & Simons, 1997). However, participants in these experiments are not aware of the change detection task until after the stimulus has been presented and when they are told to look for the change, a task comparable to our “detect the edit” task, their failure rate drops to <5% (
Levin & Simons, 1997). In static scenes, even slight changes to the global luminance or contrast of a scene occurring without any occlusion (e.g. saccade) or distraction (e.g. flicker) are missed 0% of the time (Henderson, Brockmole, & Gajewski, 2008). Therefore, compared to existing studies, the 9-32% failure to detect total, global scene changes (i.e. edits) in this study is remarkable considering that the participants only task was cut detection. This indicates that edit blindness occurs to some degree across all edit types and can be increased by adhering to the continuity editing rules.
In terms of the other measure of edit blindness - cut detection times - these were longest for Match Action cuts and shortest for Gaze Match cuts. The cut detection times for all continuity edits were expected to be longer than the no continuity condition (Between Scene) but this is clearly not the case (except a trend in the right direction for Match Action cuts). These quick detection times seem to suggest that for Within Scene and Gaze Match cuts participants either detect or miss a cut, there is never any ambiguity that might lead to longer detection times. By comparison, Match Action cuts may exhibit longer detection times due to the ambiguous timing of the cut: the motion blur caused by the sudden onset of motion prior to the cut may obscure the cut itself, increasing the probability of edit blindness and delaying recognition that a cut has occurred until the image has stabilised.
For those cuts that were missed, only Within Scene cuts showed any signs that the visual transients associated with the cuts may have been masked by eye blinks. Most cut types (except Match Action) showed a greater probability of saccadic eye movements during missed compared with detected cuts but these differences were not significant. Only Gaze Match cuts showed any sign of anticipatory saccadic eye movements that may have masked cuts. However, given the low percentage of missed Gaze Match cuts it is unclear whether these anticipatory eye movements resulted in edit blindness. In general, the most reliable method for creating edit blindness appears to be maintaining scene continuity across a cut and coinciding the cut with a sudden onset of visual motion, i.e. creating a Match Action cut.
Why did maintaining scene continuity (Within Scene cuts) result in a significantly greater degree of edit blindness compared with cuts with no continuity (Between Scene cuts)? All cuts used in the main analysis of this study were visually very similar: the shot following the cut had to contain at least one human-like figure framed by the shot from the waist up or closer (CU, CMS, or MS). The only difference between Within Scene and Between Scene cuts was whether the new shot was immediately recognisable as belonging to the same scene as the previous shot. This slight difference appears to have had a significant impact on how participants attended to the new shot and the degree to which they were aware of the cut.
Although the only task in the present study was cut detection, it appears that participants divided their attention between this task and following the film narrative. Allocating attention to this secondary task may have resulted in insufficient attention being made available to the cut detection task. Such an absence of awareness due to failure to attend to a visual feature of a scene is referred to as inattentional blindness (
Mack & Rock, 1998). Studies investigating how we perceive realistic human activities, such as washing the dishes or ironing a shirt, have revealed that we parse observed activities into perceptual units (
Newtson, 1973;
Zacks et al., 2001). During the boundaries between these units, cognitive resources may be occupied with encoding the perceptual unit in memory, leaving insufficient resources to detect disruptions to the image (
Levin & Varakin, 2004). Within Scene cuts typically coincide with shifts in action that may be perceived as perceptual boundaries (Schwan, Garsoffky, & Hesse, 2000). Evidence for the modulation of viewer attention during the perception of human events is still speculative (
see Smith, 2006, chapter 3 for further discussion) but if it were to be corroborated it may explain why editors choose to cut during changes in action. By choosing to cut during a perceptual boundary, a film editor may be limiting the attentional resources available for detecting a cut while ensuring that the cut itself is not processed as an artificial perceptual boundary. By comparison, a Between Scene cut typically occurs at the end of a scene when all action has ended and the viewer has no expectation of what will happen next. This lack of expectation and the disorientation caused by a Between Scene cut can be seen in the time taken to orient to the new shot. Within Scene cuts exhibit an early peak in saccadic activity beginning 0-100ms following a cut (
Figure 2). By comparison, participants took longer to orient to breaks in scene continuity: the peak in saccadic activity for Between Scene cuts begins 100ms after Within Scene cuts (A similar peak in saccadic activity 200-400ms following film cuts has been reported in previous studies and is thought to indicate saccades initiated in response to the new shot (
Hochberg & Brooks, 1978; d’Ydewalle and Venderbeeken, 1990; d’Ydewalle, Desmet, & Van Rensbergen, 1998; May, Dean, & Barnard, 2003;
Carmi & Itti, 2006).). The break in attention experienced after Between Scene cuts may be responsible for the lower incidence of missed cuts: viewers were unable to immediately orient to the new shot allowing them instead to allocate attention to the cut detection task.
Alternatively, Between Scene cuts may actually increase expectation of a cut due to the termination of all predictable action (This interpretation was suggested by an anonymous reviewer.). This would enable viewers to quickly and reliably detect the cut when it occurs but slow their orienting to the new shot due to its unexpected content and lack of relationship to the previous shot. In this situation viewers may initially orient to visually salient features of the new shot rather than semantically or narratively significant objects (Carmi & Itti, 2007; also see Nyström & Holmqvist in this volume for discussion of saliency vs. semantics). Such fresh appraisal of the scene may take longer than orienting to expected features and therefore account for the delayed rise in saccade frequency observed after Between Scene compared with Within Scene cuts. However, a direct comparison of the influence of semantic relatedness and visual saliency on overt attention following cuts would be required to further understand the differences in edit blindness and saccadic activity between Between Scene and Within Scene cuts.
The relationship between the timing of Within Scene cuts and cognitive event boundaries may also explain the higher coincidence of blinks with missed Within Scene cuts. The film editor Walter
Murch (
2001) hypothesised that blinks function as punctuation to cognitive events, and by identifying these cognitive event boundaries, blinks could be used to hide cuts. Some evidence of a relationship between blink frequency and cognitive processing does exist (
Fogarty & Stern, 1989;
Ichikawa & Ohira, 2004;
Fukuda et al., 2005) although attempts to find a direct relationship between blinks and event boundaries have failed (Smith, Whitwell, and Lee, 2006). Further examination of the relationship between blinks and Within Scene cuts is required to establish if the relationship found in the present study is replicable.
Why did the addition of gaze continuity to scene continuity (Gaze Match cuts) result in less edit blindness? Match Action and Gaze Match cuts are visually and conceptually very similar to Within Scene cuts. They all depict on-going scenes containing recognisable human-like figures framed in a similar way. The only difference is that Match Action cuts are preceded by a sudden onset of motion and Gaze Match cuts by a look off-screen. How can these slight differences result in such varying degrees of edit blindness?
Match Action cuts appear to improve upon Within Scene cuts by adding motion blur to scene continuity. This motion blur seems to function in a similar way to the ‘mudsplats’ used in change blindness studies (
O’Regan et al., 1999): filling the scene with motion transients so that the transients associated with the cut do not capture attention (a similar effect has been reported in attention capture paradigms: Pinto, Olivers, & Theeuwes, 2008). By comparison, Gaze Match cuts direct viewer attention across the cut using a gaze cue. The higher incidence in saccadic activity before and after Gaze Match cuts indicates that the gaze cue is effective in directing attention (see
Figure 2) but this does not result in an increase in edit blindness. Instead what appears to happen is that some of these attentional shifts may coincide with the cut, suppressing sensitivity to the cut transients, but the majority of shifts will probably terminate before or begin after the cut allowing the viewer to perceive the cut transients. The majority of Within Scene cuts seem to overcome the problem of visible cut transients by occupying the viewer with the task of orienting to the new shot. For Gaze Match cuts, no such orienting is required as the gaze cue prior to the cut has already indicated where the centre of interest will be in the new shot. This may mean that greater attentional resources are available for the cut to be processed to the level of awareness if the viewing task requires it. However, under normal viewing conditions the viewer’s primary interest should be on following the depicted action, not detecting edits. This may mean that Gaze Match cuts result in quick and direct orienting of attention across the cuts without the cut itself reaching the level of conscious awareness. More subtle methods for gauging viewer awareness of cuts, such as testing implicit memory for the cut (similar to implicit change detection: Levin, Simons, Angelone, & Chabris, 2002;
Hollingworth & Henderson, 2002;
Henderson & Hollingworth, 2003b; Angelone, Levin, & Simons, 2003) may be required to examine edit blindness during normal film viewing. The precise timecourse of gaze cuing in dynamic scenes, the reliability of such cues, and detailed analysis of where the viewers eyes go in response to the cue will be investigated in subsequent studies.
What is the implication of edit blindness on our understanding of how we represent dynamic visual scenes? One interpretation of edit blindness could be that we retain little or no information about scene content during film viewing and are, therefore unable to detect changes to this information. The results presented here clearly do not support this view. The majority of cuts are detected (70-90%; the inverse of
Table 2, bottom row, 1
st block) even when the transients associated with some of those cuts are suppressed during saccadic eye movements (~10%,
Table 3, bottom row, 2
nd block). Viewers must be retaining some form of visual representation in order for them to compare the new shot to the old and detect the change. However, when edit blindness occurs either A) this comparison is not performed, or B) the representation is too sparse for the comparison to reveal the difference. Studies investigating the representations constructed during static scene viewing have revealed that these representations are richly detailed both at the object and scene level (
Henderson & Hollingworth, 1999;
Hollingworth et al., 2001;
Melcher, 2001;
Hollingworth & Henderson, 2002; Tatler, Gilchrist, & Land, 2005;
Hollingworth, 2006). However, it is currently unknown if similarly rich representations are retained during dynamic scene viewing. Recognition memory for dynamic scenes has been shown to be better than static scenes (Matthews, Benjamin, & Osborne, 2007) but the content of this memory does not seem to be used to detect changes (
Levin & Simons, 1997; 2000; Angelone, Levin, & Simons, 2003). Evidence from developmental studies (
Xu & Carey, 1996), multiple object tracking (Kahneman, Treisman, & Gibbs, 1992;
Scholl, 2001;
Mitroff & Alvarez, 2007), and change blindness (
Levin & Simons, 1997) suggest that changes to dynamic scenes will only be detected if they violate the spatiotemporal continuity of focal objects within the scene. Our results support this view by indicating that edit blindness is more prevalent for cuts that present a continuing action (Within Scene and Match Action). However, assumptions about the spatiotemporal continuity of objects presented in film cannot be the same as the spatiotemporal continuity of objects in the real-world as 3D space is transformed and contorted to accommodate the constraints of the 2D screen. What form do these spatiotemporal expectations take during film viewing? How detailed are our representations of object and scene details? How are these representations updated across edits in the absence of explicit awareness of the editing? How do the representations and expectations of continuity during film viewing differ from those constructed during real-world dynamic scene viewing?
All of these questions are currently unanswered and require dedicated empirical investigation if we are to know whether our current theories about how static visual scenes are perceived and represented in memory scale up to film and real-world dynamic scene viewing. For instance, it is hard to believe that we would fail to notice the equivalent of an edit occurring during natural viewing of a real-world scene. If we were instantaneously transported to a different location in the real-world we would expect to notice even if we were attending to the same object before and after the change (equivalent to a Match Action cut) and the change coincided with a saccade or a sudden onset of motion. It could be argued that Edit Blindness is unique to film viewing due to the artificial nature of film and the specialised viewing behaviour we have developed to accommodate the differences between film and reality. However, other change blindness phenomenon initially reported in film (
Levin & Simons, 1997) have been shown to replicate in the realworld (
Simons & Levin, 1998). If the same real-world replication could be demonstrated for edit blindness it would have serious implications for theories about how we represent peripheral information, maintain object representations during dynamic scenes, orient our viewpoint within space, and distribute attention during dynamic scenes.