Next Article in Journal
An Eye-Tracking-Based Investigation on the Principle of Closure in Logo Design
Previous Article in Journal
The Interference Effect of Low-Relevant Animated Elements on Digital Picture-Book Comprehension in Preschoolers: An Eye-Movement Study
 
 
Journal of Eye Movement Research is published by MDPI from Volume 18 Issue 1 (2025). Previous articles were published by another publisher in Open Access under a CC-BY (or CC-BY-NC-ND) licence, and they are hosted by MDPI on mdpi.com as a courtesy and upon agreement with Bern Open Publishing (BOP).
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Relationship Between Ocular Motility and Motor Skills

by
Carmen López-de-la-Fuente
1,2,†,
Eider Eider Bereau Iridoy
1,†,
Paula Pardo Sofín
1,†,
José Luis Cebrián Lafuente
3,†,
Víctor Berdejo
1,†,
Cristina Ruiz-Garros
4,† and
María José López-de-la-Fuente
2,4,*
1
Department of Applied Physics, University of Zaragoza, Zaragoza, Spain
2
iHealthy Research Group, IIS Aragon, University of Zaragoza, Zaragoza, Spain
3
Department of Ophthalmology, Hospital University La Paz, Madrid, Spain
4
Department of Physiatry and Nursing University of Zaragoza, Zaragoza, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Eye Mov. Res. 2024, 17(4), 1-14; https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.17.4.2
Submission received: 29 June 2024 / Published: 10 October 2024

Abstract

:
The primary aim of this descriptive cross-sectional study was to examine the relationship between ocular motility and motor skills in school-age children. Participants included 142 schoolchildren (mean age: 7.08 ± 0.61 years) who completed a computerised version of the Developmental Eye Movement (DEM) test while their eye movements were recorded, and Northeastern State University College of Optometry's Oculomotor test (NSUCO). Children were classified into three groups based on their level of motor performance, which was measured by the Movement Assessment Battery for Children-2 (MABC-2). The group with typical motor performance had higher percentiles for both vertical and horizontal time, fewer errors, number of saccades, fixations, and regressions, and faster test performance. Visual test results correlate with the motor assessment outcomes; correlations are weak or moderate. Our findings emphasise the interconnectedness of motor and ocular motility. Hence, including evaluation of visual and motor proficiencies at school age would help to detect struggles in these crucial areas of development.

Introduction

Visual and motor deficits can present in several developmental disorders such as developmental coordination disorder (DCD), attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), learning disabilities, dyslexia, autism spectrum disorder (ASD) or sensory processing disorder, among others (Allison et al., 2007; Cho et al., 2014; Kim et al., 2016; Rafique & Northway, 2015, 2021; Sumner et al., 2018) and interfere with the performance of daily activities. Therefore, increasing knowledge about the interrelationship of both development areas could facilitate early detection and the implementation of appropriate interventions.
Motor skills are fundamental for a child's overall development and can influence other seemingly unrelated areas. For instance, there is evidence of a profound interconnection between motor and cognitive skills (Davis et al., 2011) at the level of underlying brain structures, where one can influence the other (Pangelinan et al., 2011). Additionally, several studies have highlighted the impact of motor proficiency on academic achievements (Abdelkarim et al., 2017; Kaiser et al., 2015), and the association between fine motor skills and social competence (Dehghan et al., 2017). Moreover, contrary to what is believed, these motor difficulties may persist as children grow (Jelovčan & Zurc, 2016; Cho et al., 2014). Therefore, they may be more likely to experience challenges in other development domains, which justifies early assessment.
The prevalence of DCD, according to the Diagnostics and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5), is around 5-6% in children between 5 and 11 years old (American Psychiatric Association, 2014). However, recent studies carried out in Spain estimate that between 12.2% and 17.4% of school children can present suspected DCD or are at risk and suggest that DCD is an underdiagnosed disorder in Spain (Amador-Ruiz et al., 2018; Delgado-Lobete et al., 2019). Besides, the DCD can co-occur with ADHD, specific language impairment (especially reading and written expression) and ASD (American Psychiatric Association, 2014).
Moreover, the visual system's development is intricately intertwined with motor development during childhood. Both systems improve as children grow older, and integrating these two developmental areas is essential for activities that require hand-eye coordination, spatial awareness, and overall cognitive development. Several authors have examined the relationship between binocular vision and motor performance, indicating that conditions like strabismus and amblyopia are associated with impaired motor development (Birch & Kelly, 2023; Kelly et al., 2020). Additionally, recent research points out that the visual system has a complex influence on postural control, as different eye movements elicit varying postural responses (Lafleur & Lajoie, 2023).
In recent years, various fields of knowledge have used eye tracking technology to record and analyse eye movements, positions, and gaze points. This method enables valuable data collection about performance in natural or laboratory settings (Drai-Zerbib & Baccino, 2014; Feis et al., 2021; Gidlöf et al., 2013; McDonald et al., 2022; Pereira et al., 2024; Reitstätter et al., 2020; Robinski & Stein, 2013; Smyrnakis et al., 2021; Taragin et al., 2019). Furthermore, the use of eye-tracking devices in ophthalmology and optometry has grown exponentially in recent times and has been employed in various areas such as the evaluation, treatment, and analysis of diverse ocular disorders, including strabismus, amblyopia, nystagmus and the sequelae of concussion (González-Vides et al., 2023; Mihara et al., 2023).
In addition, the relationship between eye movements and reading is well documented in the literature. Recent studies using the eye tracker show that poor readers present abnormal patterns in eye movements compared to children with average or above reading ability (Hindmarsh et al., 2021; Strandberg et al., 2022; Smyrnakis et al., 2021). Furthermore, individuals with poor reading skills, diagnosed with Dyslexia or not, experience more significant motor coordination challenges than proficient readers (Iversen et al., 2005). These data highlight the necessity of interdisciplinary assessments for a comprehensive approach to development.
Finally, recent studies show that visual skills such as accommodation and binocular vision impact motor performance (Kelly et al., 2020; 2021; Rafique & Northway, 2021). Therefore, research could benefit from exploring the relationships between motor skills and a broader range of visual abilities, including ocular motility (saccades, fixations, and pursuits).
The present study aims to improve understanding of the relationship between eye movements and motor skills. We hypothesised that children with better oculomotor abilities would present higher motor performance and vice versa. We examine how eye movement variables are linked to motor test scores in first and second-grade children.

Methods

Participants

This descriptive cross-sectional study was conducted in two schools located in Zaragoza, Spain. Prior to their participation, the parents or legal guardians of the students had to provide informed consent. The research received ethical approval from the Ethics Committee of Aragon (CEICA) under reference (PI22/459) and it adheres to the principles outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki.
The inclusion criteria were as follows: first and second-grade students whose guardians provided informed consent. Additionally, participants were required to understand and perform the specified tests in the study. The following individuals were excluded: those who showed cooperation difficulties and those who could not complete all the tests for other reasons, such as severe visual impairment.
This study's data come from a sample of 174 children, but only 142 (62 girls and 80 boys) met the inclusion criteria and completed all visual and motor tests. This avoided missing data for all variables. Their mean age was 7.08 ± 0.61 years.

Materials

The Developmental Eye Movement (DEM) and Northeastern State University College of Optometry's Oculomotor test (NSUCO) tests are commonly applied in optometric evaluation of ocular motility (Hindmarsh et al., 2021; Maples & Ficklin, 1988; Orduna-Hospital et al., 2023). In this study, eye tracking was employed to obtain objective measurements of various eye movement parameters (saccades, regressions, fixations, as well duration of fixations) during the DEM test.
DEM is a visual-verbal assessment test designed to indirectly evaluate saccadic eye movements. It begins with a preliminary assessment phase to determine the child's familiarity with numbers and its ability to perform the test. The test comprises two cards (Test A and B) with 40 numbers presented in two columns, which the subject must read vertically. Additionally, there is a card containing 80 numbers arranged horizontally in 16 rows with variable spacing (Test C). The subject is asked to read the numbers as quickly as possible while minimising errors. The time taken for each card and the mistakes made (addition, omission, substitution, transposition) are recorded. The horizontal time should be adjusted based on the number and type of errors, since the difference between reading 80 numbers arranged vertically and the same set of digits arranged horizontally is evaluated. The ratio is computed by dividing the adjusted horizontal time by the vertical time. Subsequently, values for horizontal and vertical times, ratio, and the number of errors are converted into percentiles (Facchin, 2021).
Eye tracking was executed using the Tobii Eye X eye tracker (Tobii, Stockholm, Sweden) during the DEM test, and data analysis was performed through Clinical Eye Tracker software (Thomson Software Solutions, Welham Green, UK). The eye tracker is a device that objectively records eye position and movement during different visual tasks by detecting the pupil and corneal reflex using infrared light. The eye tracking device was positioned approximately 60 cm from the subjects, adjacent to a 23.8-inch auxiliary screen. The eye tracker boasts a sampling rate of 55 Hz (Gibaldi et al., 2017). The eye tracker was calibrated for each participant before administering the DEM test.
The NSUCO test is a standardised evaluation that subjectively assesses tracking and saccadic eye movements. The assessment is conducted by observing how the patient, who stands facing the examiner, performs eye movements during the test. In the pursuits test, a spherical fixation point, approximately 0.5 cm in size, is placed at a distance of about 40 cm. While the subject maintains fixation on this object, two circles with a diameter of about 20 cm are traced clockwise, followed by two counterclockwise rotations. For the saccadic eye movement test, two fixation points spaced approximately 20 cm apart are used. The subject, standing about 40 cm away, is required to alternate their gaze between these points, completing ten cycles. Scores ranging from 1 to 5 points are assigned to evaluate the subject's ability, precision, and head and body movements during the test, depending on their performance. This test, therefore, evaluates how well the eyes can move independently from the head and body movements. (Maples & Ficklin, 1988).
The Spanish version of the Movement Assessment Battery for Children-2 (MABC-2) was used to determine the children’s degree of motor performance (Henderson et al., 2012). The test is administered according to three age bands (age band 1: 4–6 years, age band 2: 7–10 years, age band 3: 11–16 years), each with eight age-appropriate test items grouped into three motor domains: manual dexterity, aiming and catching, and balance. The raw score for each item is converted into a scaled score with a mean of 10 and a standard deviation of 3. The score for each domain is obtained by adding the corresponding items and is converted again into a scaled score (M=10; SD=3) and percentiles. The sum of the three domains provides the test's total score, which is again transformed into a scaled score and percentile. The 5th and lower percentiles indicate that the child has severe movement difficulties, and scores between the 6th and 15th percentiles represent children with borderline motor impairment. The often-standard cut-off value of 15% is unavailable in the MABC-2 (Smits-Engelsman et al., 2015). However, the authors provide the correspondence between the 5th and 15th percentiles and the total test score, 59 and 68 points, respectively. Only items designed for age bands 1 and 2 were used in this study.

Procedure

After consulting with principals, the investigators explained the study's objectives and provided information in a meeting with teachers and families. Adequate space for evaluations was provided by each school.
Visual function assessments were conducted by three optometrists, encompassing the following tests: visual acuity for both far and near vision, objective refraction utilising retinoscopy, cover tests for far and near vision, near point of convergence, Worth's test, stereopsis evaluation using the Random Dot test, NSUCO test, horizontal vergences measured at steps for both far and near vision, vergence facility employing ±2.00 D lenses, accommodative amplitude determined via the Donders method, and monocular as well as binocular accommodative facility. A digitised version of the DEM test was also created to gather the participants' performance through an eye tracker.
Concurrently, five occupational therapists conducted motor performance assessments using the MABC-2 test. It was administered individually and took between 20 and 30 minutes.

Data Analysis

The measurements were recorded in a database created with Microsoft Office Excel 2016. Statistical analysis for each variable was conducted using a tool based on the R programming language, specifically designed for statistical applications.
For statistical analysis, subjects were categorised into three groups (Group 1: Significant movement difficulty, Group 2: “At risk” of developing motor difficulties, Group 3: Typical Motor Development) based on their total test score (<60, 60-68, >68) obtained from the MABC-2.
The Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was used to compare the distribution of variables to assess normality. Since most of the variables did not follow a normal distribution the non-parametric Wilcoxon-Mann-Whitney test was employed to compare the distribution of various variables among the three independent groups, with a significance level set at 0.05. The degree of association between quantitative variables was determined through Spearman's correlation with Holm-Bonferroni correction, characterised by the correlation coefficient provided by the aforementioned test. All these statistical tests were performed at a significance level of 95%. The minimum power for the different tests used with our sample size was established at 0.8.
Figure 1. Examples of tasks to assess eye movements and motor performance.
Figure 1. Examples of tasks to assess eye movements and motor performance.
Jemr 17 00020 g001

Results

Percentiles for vertical time, adjusted horizontal time, and the number of errors were obtained using the software included in the DEM test. Furthermore, Table 1 presents the data concerning parameters obtained through the eye tracker during the horizontal Test C. Additionally, the findings from the Wilcoxon test are displayed. Statistically significant differences have been emphasised in bold typeface.
The relationship between total scores on the MABC-2 and performance on the DEM test is evident. A higher total score on the MABC-2 corresponds to better performance on the DEM test. In the group with typical motor development, we observed higher percentiles for both vertical and horizontal time, fewer errors, and faster test performance. Additionally, this group's average number of saccades, fixations and regressions decreased. Concerning fixation duration, it was generally lower in the group with the poorest motor performance. Statistically significant differences were found between most parameters of Group 1 and Group 3, while no significant differences were observed between Group 2 and the other groups.
Table 2 presents the values of the NSUCO test. The results of the Wilcoxon test are displayed, with statistically significant differences highlighted in bold typeface.
For most parameters, the group with typical motor development scored closer to 5, which represents the highest score in the NSUCO test compared to the other groups. Statistically significant differences were observed between most parameters of Group 1 and Group 3, as well as some differences between Group 2 (“at risk”) and Group 3.
Table 3 highlights values with p <0.05 in bold, indicating correlations between various ocular motility parameters obtained from the DEM test and data from the MABC-2 test. The table also displays the correlation coefficients between different variables.
While correlations exist between several parameters, they tend to be weak or moderate. Positive correlations were found between most motor tests and vertical and horizontal time percentiles, indicating that better motor performance corresponds to better DEM test performance. Negative correlations were observed between motor tests and the number of saccades, regressions, and fixations, suggesting that a higher number of these eye movements may indicate less efficient ocular motility. However, no correlations were found between motor test values and the number of errors or fixation duration.
Table 4 illustrates correlations between MABC-2 and certain NSUCO test parameters, although the correlation coefficients are generally low. All correlations identified are positive, suggesting that better motor performance is associated with greater precision in both saccades and pursuits. Furthermore, a greater dissociation between eye movements and head or body movements corresponds to better motor performance.

Discussion

The primary aim of this study was to examine the relationship between ocular motility and motor skills in school-age children. We assessed eye movement with the NSUCO test and recorded eye movements during the DEM test through an eye tracker. Additionally, these first and second-grade children were classified according to their motor performance in the MABC-2. Findings increase knowledge of the complex interplay between visual and motor domains and may be helpful for professionals from different fields.
Hand-eye coordination and ocular motility are pivotal for catching, grasping, object manipulation, and aiming and targeting tasks. Pursuits allow tracking of moving objects and saccades shift focus from one fixation point to another. Both eye movements facilitate the precise alignment of the fovea with the object of interest. Eye movements precede motor actions because vision provides essential information about our surroundings, including the location, size, and characteristics of objects, enabling us to plan movements, such as reaching, grasping, walking, and going upstairs (Land & Hauhoe, 2001).
Our findings suggest that ocular motility plays a crucial role in hand-eye coordination. Various parameters from the DEM test, which indirectly assesses small-amplitude saccades and the NSUCO test, which evaluates pursuits and large-amplitude saccades, demonstrate correlations with the outcomes of the MABC-2. While these correlations exhibit weak or moderate correlation coefficients, they indicate that individuals with superior ocular motility tend to perform better in specific motor skills.
Tasks involving aiming and catching demand spatial mastery. When a child is asked to aim at a target, movement planning is required to locate the target in space and accurately throw the object, whether a ball or a beanbag. During catching tasks, the visual and motor systems interact before the motor action. Therefore, visual object tracking is essential to anticipate the movement and appropriately position both the body and upper limbs to intercept the moving object (Jelovčan & Zurc, 2016).
Groups 1 and 2 presented significant differences compared to group 3 (typical motor development) regarding head movements in the NSUCO test. Eyes and head movements must be coordinated to meet the demands of different tasks and environments. Therefore, they must be flexible and can be coupled or uncoupled. Research indicates that even young children stabilise their heads before reaching (Shen et al., 2010) and that poor readers tend to make a more significant number of movements and a greater head span than good readers (Maples & Ficklin, 1988); so during reading head movements are suppressed to allow stable fixation (Proudlock et al., 2003). Therefore, children in the “risk group” may struggle to perform adequately in school.
Our study observed that a higher number of saccades, regressions, and fixations, along with poorer scores on ocular motility tests, were associated with lower motor performance. Few studies have explored the relationship between ocular motility, specifically saccades and pursuits, and motor performance in children. Some previous investigations have focused on patients with strabismus and amblyopia. Kelly et al. (2021) conducted a study examining saccades and temporal hand-eye coordination during a simple reaching task on a touch screen in 7- to 12-year-old children with esotropia, who had previously been treated for strabismus. They discovered that children with strabismus exhibited altered saccadic kinematic and slower reaching during the final stages of oculomotor coordination. Consequently, these children might struggle to adopt an efficient strategy during visually guided reaching tasks. Saccade latency was longer in strabismic children compared to controls, and the accuracy of primary saccades was approximately 25% lower, with final saccades exhibiting a 45% lower accuracy. Furthermore, the absence of stereopsis was associated with reduced precision in primary saccades and a higher number of saccades related to reaching. This study suggests that binocular dysfunction leads to slower reaching movements. In addition, children with strabismus show a less accurate and longer deceleration phase. Stereoacuity also plays a role in precisely locating objects in space, influencing the deceleration phase of hand movement when attempting to touch a target on the screen.
It is essential to note that while some authors have supported the DEM test for assessing ocular motility, especially saccadic movements (Garzia et al., 1990; Tassinari & DeLand, 2005), others have raised concerns due to the involvement of verbal and visual processing skills (Ayton et al., 2009; Orlansky et al., 2011). Since the standard parameters provided by the test (vertical time, adjusted horizontal time, ratio, and number of errors) do not appear to be directly linked to ocular motility (Ayton et al., 2009; Powers et al., 2008). However, several studies have identified a correlation between reading ability and DEM test results.
In our study, the vertical and horizontal DEM percentiles and test C duration correlate with the total motor score and the three dimensions from the MABC-2. Hopkins et al. (2019), who evaluated 222 Grade 2 children, showed that Visual-motor integration (VMI) and the vertical and horizontal DEM times were associated with reading and mathematics performance. Besides, recent Eye-tracking studies have shown that shorter fixations during reading correlate with higher reading speed (Strandberg et al., 2022; Hindmarsh et al., 2022). Furthermore, in the study by Hindmarsh et al. (2022), second-grade children completed a standardised reading comprehension test and a computerised version of the DEM test while their eye movements were recorded. They observed that the characteristic which most differentiated the eye movement behaviour between children exhibiting below-average and average or above-average reading ability was the proportion of inter-line eye movements (vertical movements that shifted fixation away from the current line). These movements were more frequent in the group with lower reading abilities. On the other hand, Iversen's (2005) study showed that children aged 10 to 12 with dyslexia and poor reading abilities performed worse than good readers on manual dexterity and balance tasks on the MABC. Hence, a multidisciplinary and multifaceted evaluation from a broad, dynamic perspective is indispensable. This approach is essential due to the intricate relationship between different areas of development.
Other visual skills are also related to motor performance. Binocularity is crucial for tasks like judging distances and perceiving three-dimensional shapes, significantly impacting hand-eye coordination's accuracy, speed, and efficiency. Poor binocular vision, as seen in conditions like amblyopia and strabismus, correlates with impaired motor performance due to compromised stereopsis, essential for spatial perception. (Birch & Kelly, 2023; Kelly et al., 2020). Rafique and Northway (2015) assessed motor skills and accommodative control in children with DCD, and their findings revealed a correlation between accommodative dysfunctions and the performance of visuomotor tasks involving upper extremities and fine dexterity.
Finally, our results emphasise the association between saccadic and pursuit eye movements and motor performance. Coetzee and Pienaar (2013) assessed the impact of an 18-week visual training program on a group of children with DCD. This program significantly improved visual skills related to visual search, fixation, and convergence, ultimately leading to enhanced scores in the MABC-2. Similarly, other researchers have observed that improving eye movements can enhance performance in activities such as object throwing and catching and overall motor coordination (Słowiński et al., 2019; Wood et al., 2017). Furthermore, eye movements contribute in a complex way to postural control since performing saccadic movements during balance tasks causes a decrease in sway, unlike visual pursuit (Lafleur & Lajoie, 2023). Thus, understanding how different systems influence each other can facilitate the implementation of appropriate interdisciplinary interventions.
This study is not without limitations. The size of the groups with poorer and borderline motor performance is small. The results of this study should be corroborated with a larger sample in the groups with worse motor performance. Participants attended two schools in the same neighbourhood, and we used non-probability convenience sampling, which may have implied a selection bias and limited the results' generalizability. However, a state-funded school and another state-subsidized private school from different socioeconomic levels were selected to cover a more diverse population. Finally, this study did not consider sociodemographic and contextual variables. Future studies should explore the associations between those confounding variables and motor-visual development.
Our results support previous research findings by emphasising the connection between motor and visual skills. Therefore, it is essential to assess these skills thoroughly during school years. Early identification of motor and visual struggles can help prevent learning difficulties and support the child's overall development.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank all the children and families for participating in this study and the teachers and principals for their help and collaboration.

Ethics and Conflict of Interest

The author(s) declare(s) that the contents of the article are in agreement with the ethics described in http://biblio.unibe.ch/portale/elibrary/BOP/jemr/ethics.html and that there is no conflict of interest regarding the publication of this paper.

References

  1. Abdelkarim, O., A. Ammar, H. Chtourou, M. Wagner, E. Knisel, A. Hökelmann, and K Bös. 2017. Relationship between motor and cognitive learning abilities among primary schoolaged children. Alexandria Journal of Medicine 53: 325–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Allison, C. L., H. Gabriel, D. Schlange, and S. Fredrickson. 2007. An optometric approach to patients with sensory integration dysfunction. Optometry 78, 12: 644–651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Amador-Ruiz, S., D. Gutierrez, V. Martínez-Vizcaíno, R. Gulías-González, M. J. Pardo-Guijarro, and M. Sánchez-López. 2018. Motor competence levels and prevalence of developmental coordination disorder in Spanish children: The MOVI-KIDS study. Journal of School Health 88, 7: 538–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. American Psychiatric Association. 2014. DSM-5: Manual diagnóstico y estadístico de los trastornos mentales, 5th ed. Editorial Médica Panamericana. [Google Scholar]
  5. Ayton, L. N., L. A. Abel, T. R. Fricke, and N. A. McBrien. 2009. Developmental eye movement test: what is it really measuring? Optometry and vision science: official publication of the American Academy of Optometry 86, 6: 722–730. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Birch, E. E., and K. R. Kelly. 2023. Amblyopia and the whole child. Progress in Retinal and Eye Research 93: 101168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Caputo, R., F. Tinelli, A. Bancale, L. Campa, R. Frosini, A. Guzzetta, E. Mercuri, and G. Cioni. 2007. Motor coordination in children with congenital strabismus: effects of late surgery. European Journal of Paediatric Neurology 11: 285–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Cho, H., S. Ji, S. Chung, M. Kim, and Y. S. Joung. 2014. Motor function in school-aged children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in Korea. Psychiatry Investigation 11, 3: 223–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Coetzee, D., and A. E. Pienaar. 2013. The effect of visual therapy on the ocular motor control of seven-to eight-year-old children with Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD). Research in Developmental Disabilities 34, 11: 4073–4084. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Davis, E. E., N. J. Pitchford, and E. Limback. 2011. The interrelation between cognitive and motor development in typically developing children aged 4-11 years is underpinned by visual processing and fine manual control. British Journal of Psychology 102, 3: 569–584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Dehghan, L., N. Mirzakhani, M. Rezaee, and M. Tabatabaee. 2017. The relationship between fine motor skills and social development and maturation. Iranian Rehabilitation Journal 15: 407–414. http://irj.uswr.ac.ir/article-1-734-en.html.
  12. Delgado-Lobete, L., S. Santos-del-Riego, S. Pértega-Díaz, and R. Montes-Montes. 2019. Prevalence of suspected developmental coordination disorder and associated factors in Spanish classrooms. Research in Developmental Disabilities 86: 31–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Drai-Zerbib, V., and T. Baccino. 2014. The effect of expertise in music reading: cross-modal competence. Journal of Eye Movement Research 6, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Facchin, A. 2021. Spotlight on the developmental eye movement (DEM) test. Clinical Optometry 13: 73–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Feis, A., A. Lallensack, E. Pallante, M. Nielsen, N. Demarco, and B. Vasudevan. 2021. Reading eye movements performance on iPad vs print using a visagraph. Journal of Eye Movement Research 14, 2: 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Garzia, R. P., J. E. Richman, S. B. Nicholson, and C. S. Gaines. 1990. A new visual-verbal saccade test: the development eye movement test (DEM). Journal of the American Optometric Association 61, 2: 124–135. [Google Scholar]
  17. Gibaldi, A., M. Vanegas, P. J. Bex, and G. Maiello. 2017. Evaluation of the Tobii EyeX eye tracking controller and Matlab toolkit for research. Behavior Research Methods 49, 3: 923–946. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Gidlöf, K., A. Wallin, R. Dewhurst, and K. Holmqvist. 2013. Using Eye Tracking to Trace a Cognitive Process: Gaze Behaviour During Decision Making in a Natural Environment. Journal of Eye Movement Research 6, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. González-Vides, L, J.L. Hernández-Verdejo, and P. Cañadas-Suárez. 2023. Eye tracking in optometry: A systematic review (JEMR). Journal of Eye Movement Research 16, 3: 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Henderson, S., and D. Sudgen. 2012. Batería de Evaluación del Movimiento para niños-2 (MABC-2). Pearson Clinical & Talent Assessment. [Google Scholar]
  21. Hindmarsh, G. P., A. A. Black, S. L. J. White, S. Hopkins, and J. M. Wood. 2021. Eye movement patterns and reading ability in children. Ophthalmic & Physiological Optics 41, 5: 1134–1143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Hopkins, S., A. A. Black, S. L. J. White, and J. M. Wood. 2019. Visual information processing skills are associated with academic performance in Grade 2 school children. Acta Ophthalmologica 97, 8: e1141–e1148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Iversen, S., K. Berg, B. Ellertsen, and F. E. Tønnessen. 2005. Motor coordination difficulties in a municipality group and in a clinical sample of poor readers. Dyslexia (Chichester, England) 11, 3: 217–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Jelovčan, G., and J. Zurc. 2016. Preschool children’s results in movement ABC tests: Differences between girls and boys in movement deficit. Annals of Kinesiology 7, 1: 3–19. [Google Scholar]
  25. Kaiser, M. L., M. M. Schoemaker, J. M. Albaret, and R. H. Geuze. 2015. What is the evidence of impaired motor skills and motor control among children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)? A systematic review of the literature. Research in Developmental Disabilities 36C: 338–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Kelly, K. R., S. E. Morale, C. L. Beauchamp, L. M. Dao, B. A. Luu, and E. E. Birch. 2020. Factors associated with impaired motor skills in strabismic and anisometropic children. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science 61, 10: 43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Kelly, K. R., J. Hunter, Jr., D. M. Norouzi, R. M. Jost, A. J. White, C. S. Cheng-Patel, C. L. Beauchamp, L. M. Dao, B. A. Luu, D. Stager, Jr., J. Y. Tung, and E. Niechwiej-Szwedo. 2021. Reach kinematics during binocular viewing in 7-to 12-year-old children with strabismus. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science 62, 15: 21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Kim, H., A. G. Carlson, T. W. Curby, and A. Winsler. 2016. Relations among motor, social, and cognitive skills in pre-kindergarten children with developmental disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities 53–54: 43–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Land, M. F., and M. Hayhoe. 2001. In what ways do eye movements contribute to everyday activities? Vision Research 41, 25-26: 3559–3565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Lafleur, D., and Y. Lajoie. 2023. The impact of eye movement on postural control depends on the type of oculomotor behavior and the visual task. Gait & Posture 100: 65–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. McDonald, M. A., S. J. Holdsworth, and H. V. Danesh-Meyer. 2022. Eye movements in mild traumatic brain injury: Clinical challenges. Journal of Eye Movement Research 15, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Maples, W. C., and T. W. Ficklin. 1988. Interrater and test-retest reliability of pursuits and saccades. Journal of the American Optometric Association 59, 7: 549–552. [Google Scholar]
  33. Mihara, M., A. Hayashi, K. Kakeue, and R. Tamura. 2023. Changes in saccadic eye movement and smooth pursuit gain in patients with acquired comitant esotropia after strabismus surgery. Journal of Eye Movement Re-search 16, 4: 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Orduna-Hospital, E., A. Navarro-Marqués, C. López-de-la-Fuente, and A. Sanchez-Cano. 2023. Eye-tracker study of the Developmental Eye Movement Test in young people without binocular dysfunctions. Life 13, 3: 773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Orlansky, G., K. B. Hopkins, G. L. Mitchell, K. Huang, M. Frazier, C. Heyman, and M. Scheiman. 2011. Reliability of the developmental eye movement test. Optometry and vision science: official publication of the American Academy of Optometry 88, 12: 1507–1519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Pangelinan, M. M., G. Zhang, J. W. VanMeter, J. E. Clark, B. D. Hatfield, and A. J. Haufler. 2011. Beyond age and gender: Relationships between cortical and subcortical brain volume and cognitive-motor abilities in school-age children. NeuroImage 54, 4: 3093–3100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Pereira, N., M. A. Costa, and M. Guerreiro. 2024. Integrating cognitive factors and eye movement data in reading predictive models for children with dyslexia and ADHD-I. Journal of Eye Movement Research 16, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Powers, M., D. Grisham, and P. Riles. 2008. Saccadic tracking skills of poor readers in high school. Optometry (St. Louis, Mo.) 79, 5: 228–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Proudlock, F. A., H. Shekhar, and I. Gottlob. 2003. Coordination of eye and head movements during reading. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science 44, 7: 2991–2998. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Rafique, S. A., and N. Northway. 2015. Relationship of ocular accommodation and motor skills performance in developmental coordination disorder. Human Movement Science 42: 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Rafique, S. A., and N. Northway. 2021. Reliance on visual feedback from ocular accommodation on motor skills in children with developmental coordination disorder and typically developing controls. Human Movement Science 76: 102767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Reitstätter, L., H. Brinkmann, T. Santini, E. Specker, Z. Dare, F. Bakondi, A. Miscená, E. Kasneci, H. Leder, and R. Rosenberg. 2020. The display makes a difference: A mobile eye tracking study on the perception of art before and after a museum’s rearrangement. Journal of Eye Movement Research 13, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Robinski, M., and M. Stein. 2013. Tracking Visual Scanning Techniques in Training Simulation for Helicopter Landing. Journal of Eye Movement Research 6, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Shen, H., T. J. Baker, T. R. Candy, C. Yu, and L. B. Smith. 2010. Using the head to stabilize action: Reaching by young children. 2010 IEEE 9th International Conference on Development and Learning, Development and Learning (ICDL), 2010 IEEE 9th International Conference On; pp. 108–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Słowiński, P., H. Baldemir, G. Wood, O. Alizadehkhaiyat, G. Coyles, S. Vine, G. Williams, K. Tsaneva-Atanasova, and M. Wilson. 2019. Gaze training supports self-organization of movement coordination in children with developmental coordination disorder. Scientific reports 9, 1: 1712. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Smits-Engelsman, B., M. Schoemaker, T. Delabastita, J. Hoskens, and R. Geuze. 2015. Diagnostic criteria for DCD: Past and future. Human Movement Science 42: 293–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Smyrnakis, I., V. Andreadakis, A. Rina, N. Bοufachrentin, and I. M. Aslanides. 2021. Silent versus reading out loud modes: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Eye Movement Research 14, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Strandberg, A., M. Nilsson, P. Östberg, and G. Ö. Seimyr. 2022. Eye movements during reading and their relationship to reading assessment outcomes in Swedish elementary school children. Journal of Eye Movement Research 15, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Sumner, E., S. B. Hutton, G. Kuhn, and E. L. Hill. 2018. Oculomotor atypicalities in developmental coordination disorder. Developmental Science 21, 1: e12501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Taragin, D., D. Tzuriel, and E. Vakil. 2019. Mental rotation: The effects of processing strategy, gender and task characteristics on children’s accuracy, reaction time and eye movements’ pattern. Journal of Eye Movement Research 12, 8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Tassinari, J. T., and P. DeLand. 2005. Developmental Eye Movement Test: reliability and symptomatology. Optometry (St. Louis, Mo.) 76, 7: 387–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Wood, G., C. A. L. Miles, G. Coyles, O. Alizadehkhaiyat, S. J. Vine, J. N. Vickers, and M. R. Wilson. 2017. A randomized controlled trial of a group-based gaze training intervention for children with Developmental Coordination Disorder. PLoS One 12, 2: e0171782. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Results of the DEM test of the total sample and the groups selected according to motor performance.
Table 1. Results of the DEM test of the total sample and the groups selected according to motor performance.
TotalG1G2G3
M±IQRM±IQRM±IQRM±IQR p value
DEMN=142n=28n=21n=93G1vsG2G2vsG3G1vsG3
Vertical Time (pctl)28±51.55±26.517±26.54936±45.50.0610.098<0.001
Horizontal Time (pctl)37.5±51.511.5±2926±4345±440.1450.165<0.001
Errors (pctl)28±5019.5±36.7528±36.7536.5±48.750.6500.4980.142
Duration test C (s)86.4±38.62599.85±43.22594.3±43.981.7±35.7750.3320.1160.005
Number of saccades (n)107±46.75111.5±50.5106±57106±46.250.5240.7340.224
Regressions (n)56.5±35.570±44.2553±4253±30.50.1600.3480.003
Fixations (n)164±80.75176.5±68.25148±95159±690.2660.5550.034
Duration fixations (ms)443±138445±136437±191443±1380.7090.5850.701
Note. G1: Group 1 (Significant movement difficulty), G2: Group 2 (“At risk” of developing motor difficulties, G3: Group 3 (Typical motor development). M: Median, IQR: Interquartile Range, pctl: percentile, s: seconds, ms: milliseconds.
Table 2. Results of the NSUCO test of the total sample and the groups selected according to motor performance.
Table 2. Results of the NSUCO test of the total sample and the groups selected according to motor performance.
TotalG1G2G3
M±IQRM±IQRM±IQRM±IQRp value
NSUCON=142n=28n=21n=93G1vsG2G2vsG3G1vsG3
PURSUITSAbility5±05±15±05±00.7610.3990.098
Accuracy4±23±2.254±24±20.33490.1770.003
Head Movement24±24±2.254±34±10.9170.0460.033
Body Movement5±15±15±15±04±10.1010.036
SACCADESAbility5±15±15±15±00.5490.1100.249
Accuracy4±33±2.253±24±20.4830.017<0.001
Head Movement4±23±32±34±30.8840.0130.017
Body Movement5±14±15±15±10.3690.5410.075
Note. G1: Group 1 (Significant movement difficulty), G2: Group 2 (“At risk” of developing motor difficulties, G3: Group 3 (Typical motor development). M: Median, IQR: Interquartile Range,
Table 3. Correlations between MABC-2 test and DEM test.
Table 3. Correlations between MABC-2 test and DEM test.
Vertical Time
(pctl)
Adjusted Horizontal
Time (pctl)
Errors (pctl)Duration Test CNumber saccadesNumber RegressionNumber fixationsDuration fixations
Total ScoreCC0.3380.3140.076-0.263-0.170-0.282-0.237-0.006
p0.0000.0000.3660.0020.0430.0010.0050.943
Manual
Dexterity
CC0.2260.1790.029-0.171-0.166-0.231-0.2160.082
p0.0070.0340.7320.0420.0490,.060.0100.330
Aiming &
Catching
CC0.1840.2030.046-0.188-0.031-0.117-0.073-0.103
p0.0290.0150.5900.0250.7120.1650.3880.223
BalanceCC0.3200.2680.078-0.205-0.142-0.251-0.2010.020
p0.0000.0010.3590.0140.0920.0030.0160.814
Note. CC: correlation coefficient. Pctl= percentile.
Table 4. Correlations between MABC-2 test and NSUCO test.
Table 4. Correlations between MABC-2 test and NSUCO test.
PursuitsSaccades
AbilityAccuracyHead MovementBody MovementAbilitAccuracyHead MovementBody Movement
Total
Score
CC0.1330.2430.2030.2240.1580.3720.2690.158
p0.1140.0040.0150.0070.0610.0000.0010.060
Manual Dex-
terity
CC0.1120.1100.1790.1960.0930.2980.2440.132
p0.1840.1920.0330.0190.2700.0000.0030.117
Aiming &
Catching
CC0.0390.1980.0660.1170.0790.1860.0970.083
p0.6450.0180.4330.1650.3470.0270.2530.326
BalanceCC0.1540.2060.1590.1830.1440.2800.2500.161
p0.0670.0140.0590.0290.0880.0010.0030.055
Note. CC: correlation coefficient.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

López-de-la-Fuente, C.; Iridoy, E.E.B.; Sofín, P.P.; Lafuente, J.L.C.; Berdejo, V.; Ruiz-Garros, C.; López-de-la-Fuente, M.J. Relationship Between Ocular Motility and Motor Skills. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2024, 17, 1-14. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.17.4.2

AMA Style

López-de-la-Fuente C, Iridoy EEB, Sofín PP, Lafuente JLC, Berdejo V, Ruiz-Garros C, López-de-la-Fuente MJ. Relationship Between Ocular Motility and Motor Skills. Journal of Eye Movement Research. 2024; 17(4):1-14. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.17.4.2

Chicago/Turabian Style

López-de-la-Fuente, Carmen, Eider Eider Bereau Iridoy, Paula Pardo Sofín, José Luis Cebrián Lafuente, Víctor Berdejo, Cristina Ruiz-Garros, and María José López-de-la-Fuente. 2024. "Relationship Between Ocular Motility and Motor Skills" Journal of Eye Movement Research 17, no. 4: 1-14. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.17.4.2

APA Style

López-de-la-Fuente, C., Iridoy, E. E. B., Sofín, P. P., Lafuente, J. L. C., Berdejo, V., Ruiz-Garros, C., & López-de-la-Fuente, M. J. (2024). Relationship Between Ocular Motility and Motor Skills. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 17(4), 1-14. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.17.4.2

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop