1. Introduction
The autonomous sensory meridian response (ASMR) is a sensory phenomenon typically characterized by tingling across the scalp, following the line of the spine downward, extending to the arms and further depending on the intensity of the response [
1]. Assuming a complex idiosyncratic reaction in the sample, with this study, we examined the ASMR experience by observing the pupillary diameter and the arousal level. Our findings showed differences in pupillary diameter, regardless of the perception of tingles, and in the arousal level, in comparison with other video conditions. Furthermore, we utilized self-report measures to explore the ASMR condition, finding significant differences in the Telepresence Scale and Interpersonal Reactivity Index Scale. ASMR is a complex phenomenon mediated by the viewing of videos that are proven to induce tingling sensations across the scalp, following the line of the spine downwards, extending to the arms and further depending on the intensity of the response [
1]. Tingles are elicited by a number of audio and visual triggers—such as whispers, crispy sounds, personal care, and slow or repetitive movements. The experience of ASMR “tingles” is often accompanied by feelings of calm and relaxation [
2,
3], which the subjects compare to falling asleep. ASMR was recently brought to the attention of the public and a few examples of research have been published about the phenomenon since 2015. Some of these have analyzed the relation that ASMR has with depression and anxiety [
1], intimacy [
4], emotional regulation strategies [
5], personality traits and empathy [
6], telepresence [
7], and other psychological aspects. The underlying mechanisms in ASMR are still understudied. In a study examining the default mode network (DMN) in relation to the ASMR, Smith et al. [
8,
9] using fMRI found that ASMR experiencers reported reduced resting-state functional connectivity between frontal, sensory, and attentional regions of the DMN. Lochte et al. [
10] using fMRI found that ASMR experiencers showed significant activation in the medial Prefrontal Cortex (mPFC) associated with self-awareness and social cognition, in the Nucleo Accumbens (NAcc) associated with reward, and in the Anterior Cingulate Cortex (dACC) and Insula associated with emotional arousal. Furthermore, Wang et al. [
11] investigated the influence of ASMR on the performance of executive functions in both ASMR and control individuals, finding that some executive functions of ASMR individuals slowed down after the ASMR video (specifically set shifting and inhibitory control). Finally, recent studies compared brain activity studied through the use of EEG: Fredborg et al. [
12] examined brain waves related to ASMR through the use of electroencephalography (EEG). They found that ASMR auditory stimuli increased alpha wave activity (alpha power—in addition to theta—has been associated with meditative states [
13]) in participants with self-reported ASMR. This did not seem to occur in control participants. Seifzadeh et al. [
14] examined the variations in EEG frequency bands in pre-ASMR versus post-ASMR [
14]. Finally, Koo et al. [
15] found that arousal was a key feature of ASMR [
15]. In 2018, Poerio et al. [
2] compared ASMR tingles with the well-known aesthetic chills recognizing similarities between the two phenomena; considering both as phenomenologically complex and idiosyncratic experiences, they asserted that ASMR is typically considered to be relaxing, while aesthetic chills are associated with excitement and physiological arousal. The findings of the study showed that ASMR is a relaxing experience also accompanied by arousing effects, such as increased skin conductance levels. In line with recent literature that adopted eye-tracking and brain waves to study various fields of application, including cognition [
16], arousal [
17], marketing [
18], and, more recently, advanced learning technologies [
19], we conducted experimental research in which we combined such technologies to investigate arousal level under ASMR conditions.
Regarding eye tracking, the first study about pupillometry and ASMR was conducted by Valtakari et al. [
20]; they found out that ASMR videos cause a statistically significant increase in pupil diameter (4.07 mm), observing a significant difference only during reported episodes of tingles.
Several arousal studies used pupillary diameter measurement [
21,
22], finding that, in general, the pupil tends to dilate in subjects submitted to attentional stimuli, triggering an elevation in the arousal (the arousal in these studies was further measured by detecting the heartbeat or skin conductance). In this way, pupil size proved to be a reliable indicator of emotional arousal, and it is also related to other physiological arousal measures, such as skin conductance [
21]. Furthermore, as discussed by Steinhauer et al. [
23], changes in pupil diameter are controlled by two muscles—the dilator and the sphincter—which are differentially influenced by activity in the sympathetic and parasympathetic systems. Increased sympathetic activity amplifies the activity of the dilator muscle, causing dilation, whereas activity of the parasympathetic system causes the constriction of the sphincter muscle, which results in a decrease in pupil diameter. Taking these argumentations as a point of departure, we assumed that ASMR experience could be characterized by activating as much as deactivating responses, on a physiological and psychological level. In order to verify this assumption, the aim of the study was the measurement of the ASMR phenomenon, through the use of two tools. We compared an ASMR video with other contents—resting state, high arousal, low arousal, control videos—and, at the same time, we submitted a battery of self-reported tests, regarding psychological variables and different aspects of the ASMR experience. About the tools, we decided to use two new instruments for the detection of physiological measures: NeuroSky MindWave Mobile 2, connectable to a laptop via Bluetooth, allows one to collect data around brain frequencies, states of attention and relaxation, and also blinks; PupilCore, a monocular or binocular eye tracking device developed by Pupil Labs, includes an open-source software suite and a wearable headset. We expected that, during the viewing of an ASMR video, the participants would show a greater average pupil diameter increase compared to other video contents. Furthermore, we expected, under the same condition, an increased brain activity, also confirmed by results in the self-report measures.
Hypothesis 1 (H1). People during the viewing of an ASMR video will have a larger pupil diameter compared to the viewing of other videos.
Hypothesis 2 (H2). People during the viewing of an ASMR video will have a general reduction in arousal, measured by Neurosky brain waves, compared to the viewing of other videos.
Hypothesis 3 (H3). ASMR experiencers will have different scores in psychological dimensions compared to Non-ASMR experiencers.
Hypothesis 4 (H4). People with high values in ASMR-15 will have a higher brain activation.
3. Results
First, we cover the sociodemographic and descriptive outcomes. Secondly, we show the inferential statistics starting with the comparison between pupil diameter and brain waves frequencies among different video contents. Finally, we discuss the outcomes.
3.1. Descriptive Statistics
A total of 76 participants (69.7% females; 30.3% males) were recruited for the study. The sociodemographic information included age (M = 24.96, SD = 4.867) and gender (male, female). No statistically significant difference can be found with regard to sex and age.
3.2. Psychological Characteristics
We investigated the following psychological measures: empathy and telepresence. Specifically, the data were configured as follows (see
Table 1):
Empathy: for the experiment, we used the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) [
33].
The test has four subscales: Perspective-Taking Scale (M = 27, SD = 4.7), Fantasy Scale (M = 26.9, SD = 4.6), Empathic-Concern Scale (M = 27.1, SD = 4.5), Personal Distress Scale (M = 19.1, SD = 5.4).
Telepresence: for the experiment, we used the Telepresence Scale [
34] after the ASMR experience. The test has two dimensions: Being present in the mediated environment, and Not being present in the mediated environment. For the second administration, the results were: Being present in the mediated environment (
M = 18.5,
SD = 5.0), Not being present in the mediated environment (
M = 10.1,
SD = 3.1).
3.3. Neurophysiological Characteristics
We investigated pupil diameter and brain waves for each video that was submitted to the sample. For pupil diameter, we report in
Table 2 the mean diameter,
SD, skewness, and kurtosis. ASMR determined the higher pupil diameter (
M = 4.32,
SD = 1.31) compared to the other videos (Rest, High Arousal, Low Arousal, Control). The data obtained from the differences between the conditions of Low Arousal, High Arousal, and Control videos were not significant.
For frequency bands results, we report in
Table 3 the values (expressed in ASIC-EEG-POWER) of mean,
SD, skewness, and kurtosis of the ASMR video. As the EEG variables did not distribute normally, they were log-transformed before correlation calculation. mean, skewness, and kurtosis refer to the 30 s frequency band measurement time window of each video. Even if it is not a standard way of presenting data, it was sufficient for the level of detail we were interested in.
3.4. Descriptive ASMR Experience
For the measurement of ASMR experience, we used ASMR-Experience [
2] and ASMR 15 [
33]. ASMR POST [
29] administration did not produce significant results.
ASMR-Experience is composed of the item “How frequently did you experience tingling sensations during the video?”, rated on a Likert scale from 1 (none of the time) to 7 (all of the time). The results showed a mean of 1.97 (SD = 1.29).
ASMR 15 has four subscales: Altered Consciousness (
M = 8.1,
SD = 4), Sensation (
M = 11.6,
SD = 5.6), Relaxation (
M = 7.6,
SD = 3.5), Affect (
M = 7.4,
SD = 3.4). Results are reported in
Table 4.
3.5. Inferential Statistics
H1: Pupil dilation during ASMR video compared to other conditions.Table 5 illustrates the comparison between average pupil diameter for ASMR videos [
29] and for other videos, showing that pupil diameter is always greater during ASMR videos. As a reminder, the other videos had different contents. In particular, the REST video showed a white screen with a fixed black dot in the center, to induce a “resting state,” as suggested by Owens and Leibowitz [
31]; High-Arousal and Low-Arousal videos were selected from the database of Li et al. [
30], to induce different levels of arousal; the Control video was selected from Cash et al.’s [
29] research. The difference registered in pupil diameter with the ASMR and REST video was 0.92 (mm); therefore, this was the largest. On the contrary, the smallest difference in pupil diameter was registered in the ASMR and CONTROL video (only 0.36 mm). Of the conditions ASMR-HIGH AROUSAL and ASMR-LOW AROUSAL, the pupil diameter differed, respectively, by 0.53 mm and 0.43 mm.
H2: Brain activation between ASMR and other contents.Table 6 presents the results of a Student’s
t-test for the EEG delta between the ASMR video and other videos. Delta values during the ASMR video were lower than during the High-Arousal (
t =
−2.12,
p < 0.05), Low-Arousal (
t =
−2.61,
p < 0.01), and Control videos (
t =
−2.94,
p < 0.001). Theta values during the ASMR video were lower than during the Low-Arousal (
t =
−2.15,
p < 0.05) and Control videos (
t =
−2.66,
p < 0.01). Low Alpha differed only during the Low-Arousal video experience, being higher than during the ASMR video (
t =
−1.92,
p < 0.05). High Alpha Delta appeared lower during the ASMR video than during the Low-Arousal (
t =
−3.49,
p < 0.001) and Control videos (
t =
−2.56,
p < 0.05). Low Beta values were lower during the ASMR video than during the High-Arousal, Low-Arousal, and Control videos, respectively, with
t =
−2.64,
p < 0.01;
t =
−2.38,
p < 0.05; and
t = 2.02,
p < 0.05. High Beta values did not present significant differences between the conditions, while Low Gamma (
t =
−2.89,
p < 0.001) and Mid Gamma values (
t =
−2.19,
p < 0.05) during ASMR were lower than during the Control video. Interestingly, from the brain activity point of view, the ASMR video did not show any differences with the Rest video.
H3: Psychological measures in ASMR experiencers and Non-ASMR experiencers. We found differences only in the correlation between brain wave dimensions; to investigate the predictors of ASMR experience (e.g., tingles perception), we took into account as a dependent variable the answers provided to the question “How frequently did you experience tingling sensations during the video?” Due to the non-Gaussian distribution of the answers, we first eliminated those participants who answered “never” (n = 39), in order to investigate only those participants who reported an ASMR experience to any degree. Further, in order to respect the inferential analysis preconditions, we discretized the variable within two levels, defining two balanced subsamples, respectively, labeled as “Few” and “Many” tingles perceived. The “Few” subsample was composed by those participants that reported tingles only a “Few times” (n = 17), while the “Many” subsample was composed by those who reported tingles “Sometimes” (n = 10), “Half the time” (n = 4), “Quite frequently” (n = 5), and “Frequently” (n = 1).
The differences between the subsample reporting more tingles experiences (“Many”) and the subsample that reported few tingles (“Few”) are summarized in
Table 7; in particular, we present the results of a Student’s
t test. Regarding IRI, higher scores in the subscale Empathic Concern appeared to determine higher tingles perception (
t = 2.34,
p < 0.05), but they reported lower scores in the subscale Personal Distress (
t =
−2.27,
p < 0.05). The subscales “Perspective-Taking” and “Fantasy” were not statistically significant.
Of the Telepresence Scale, lower scores in the subscale “Being Present in the Mediated Environment” appeared to be associated with higher tingles receivers (t = −2.03, p < 0.05).
H4: Higher brain activation with tingles experiences is shown by the ASMR-15 results.Table 8 illustrates the correlations between frequency bands and ASMR-15 subscales scores. This analysis aimed to highlight any linear relationships between ASMR-15 subscales (as indicators of subjective psychological experience) and NeuroSky as an indicator of physiological arousal. The scores provided by the ASMR-15 subscale “Sensations” appeared to be related to almost all the frequency bands considered by our study, with the only exceptions of Low Alpha and High Alpha. The degree of sensation related to ASMR appeared to increase whenever the brain arousal increased. While the “Relaxation” scale scores did not show any significant correlation with brain arousal, the “Altered Consciousness” and “Affect” scores reported small correlations with Gamma (Altered consciousness and Affect) and Beta (Affect) waves.
4. Conclusions
Autonomous Sensory Meridian Response (ASMR) is a phenomenon still to be explored, but a large amount of research has analyzed its characteristics. For our study, to perform a multi-dimensional analysis, we chose to investigate ASMR through an eye-tracking device (PupilCore) and a wireless electroencephalogram (NeuroSky), concurrently with the submission of some self-report measures (IRI, Telepresence Scales, ASMR-Experience, and ASMR 15). The sensitivity to the phenomenon could be linked to different psychological characteristics, including greater sensory suggestibility [
38].
For our study, we chose to investigate the influence of empathy levels through the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) [
33], previously used also by McErlean and Banissy [
6]. For the IRI-Empathic Concern subscale, we found that the subsample that felt more tingles (“Many”) reported a higher score that the other subsample (“Few”), according to McErlean and Banissy [
6]; further, for the IRI-Personal Distress subscale, the subsample “Few” reported a higher score that the other subsample “Many.” In our opinion, according to Bjelić [
39], ASMR could concern a sort of digital care, a kind of care transmitted through digital technology. According to the author, “The popularity of ASMR videos can be viewed as symptomatic of an existing social need to be cared for, loved, and connected with. Moreover, this kind of care is being sought through digital technology, through digital intimacy” [
39]. In the same way, individuals with a higher IRI-Empathic Concern score—which evaluates “other-oriented” feelings of sympathy and concern for the unfortunate—could be more receptive toward the tingling phenomenon. Even Lochte et al. [
10] claimed that ASMR could activate brain regions and release substances normally associated with affiliated behaviors (crinkling, tapping, brushing, or massaging). We also used the Telepresence Scale [
34] to analyze the sense of presence within virtual environments. Comparing the two subsamples of lower vs. higher tingles receivers (“Few” and “Many”), we found that the subsample “Few” reported a higher score in the subscale Being Present in the Mediated Environment. Furthermore, we analyzed the ASMR sensations through two specific self-report measures: ASMR-Experience [
2] and ASMR 15 [
35]. By the comparison between the results of ASMR 15 and NeuroSky dimensions, our results showed different correlations between brain waves and the subscale Sensations (the subscale that explores the localization and the intensity of tingles). For this reason, we assumed that changes in brain activity were due to the perception of tingles.
Neurophysiological parameters are also involved in the ASMR phenomenon: for example, Poerio et al. [
2] claimed that ASMR videos produced sensations of both calmness and excitement, measured by different neurophysiological tools; Lochte et al. [
10] claimed that ASMR increased activity in brain regions typically related to reward activation, emotional arousal, and social engagement, suggesting that ASMR may be similar to social grooming; Fredborg et al. [
12] reported an increased alpha, gamma, and sensorimotor rhythm activity, which may indicate changes in attentional control; in addition, Smith et al. [
8,
9] sustained that ASMR participants showed significantly reduced functional connectivity in the DMN brain regions. An important contribution to our study is given by Valtakari et al. [
20]: they examined the tingles’ experiences and found them to be accompanied by a growing pupil diameter (4.03 mm), considering this as a physiological characteristic trait of ASMR-experiencers. Unexpectedly, our results reported that the increase in pupil diameter (4.32 mm) was present in the whole sample, regardless of the tingles experience. As much as these results need further insight, it seems that the whole experience of ASMR might involve different features, and it manifests itself in each subject, regardless of their sensitivity to consciously perceive the phenomenon. It may be that the effect of ASMR reaches everyone but remains unconscious for most of the population. Furthermore, from the comparison between pupil diameter during ASMR videos and Arousal (Low and High) videos, our results showed a significant difference (Low Arousal = 3.89 mm; High Arousal = 3.79 mm), demonstrating that ASMR pupil dilation is the largest.
Regarding the measurement of arousal, today, thanks to new technologies, arousal can be investigated by using different tools: first, the detection of skin conductance (biofeedback). This index is very sensitive to minimal fluctuations in arousal, measured without any time difference. Another measurement method often used is the detection of the heart rate [
40]. By these detection methods, the authors claimed that an elevated arousal manifests itself with an accelerated heart rate and high skin conductance. Likewise, pupillary dilation also reflects elevated arousal [
21]. Regarding the studies related to arousal, measured through the use of the electroencephalogram (EEG), increases in the frontal-right Theta frequencies and in the parietal Beta frequencies can be noticed (in moments of high perceived arousal), together with specific changes in the valence in the temporal Beta [
41].
Another point of view we considered to examine the ASMR phenomenon was the brain activity. A lot of research has been reported; for example, Fredborg et al. [
12] and Lee et al. [
13] found an increased Alpha wave activity, associated with a certain form of meditative state, and Smith et al. [
8,
9] observed a greater brain connectivity in the areas involved with the Default Mode Network (DMN). The DMN [
42] is a brain network that comprises the hippocampus, medial prefrontal cortex, temporal lateral and temporal-parietal regions, and posterior medial cortex [
43,
44]: these brain areas are activated by states of resting, spontaneous mental activity, spontaneous thinking, self-referenced thoughts, daydreaming imagination, plans, and expectations. Andreasen et al. [
45] noted that the resting state “consists of a mixture of freely wandering past recollection, future plans, and other personal thoughts and experiences.” These regions are central components of the core brain system that is consistently activated in humans during undirected mental states. From our NeuroSky brain waves track, comparing ASMR and other videos, it is clear that ASMR does not differ from the Rest video. We believe that the lack of differences in brain activity between ASMR and Rest is caused by this brain activity. In brief, according to Smith et al. [
8,
9] and Fredborg et al. [
12], it could be that ASMR could trigger the brain’s DMN status and elicit a sort of meditative state. The usage of NeuroSky technology is, of course, a “double-edged sword”: on the one hand, it is very robust to motion artifacts, so it allows ecological data mining; on the other hand, its sensitivity and precision are very low. Such limitations, of course, make it not necessarily meaningful, nor correct, to directly compare the NeuroSky output values with other EEG systems. It is also conceivable that NeuroSky could not be sensitive enough to recognize the EEG frequency differences between Rest and ASMR conditions, because they could be too subtle.
As a lot of research has supported, ASMR could be a complex emotional experience that comprises feelings of both relaxation and excitement; paradoxical physiological responses to ASMR showed decreased heart rate and increased skin conductance levels [
2]. It was also demonstrated by Koo et al. [
15] that ASMR is strictly related to arousal. From our results, we detected for the first time in the literature a discrepancy between brain arousal and pupil diameter, apparently specific to ASMR videos (similar to the dyscrasia found by Poerio et al. [
2]). At the same time, we noted some features usually associated with an increase in arousal, demonstrated by the increase in pupil diameter (
Table 5), and at the same time, some features usually coupled with a decrease in arousal were demonstrated with data collected by NeuroSky (
Table 6). In line with Poerio et al. [
2], we hypothesized a possible causal effect of such dyscrasia on the ASMR-related perceptual complex phenomenology. The data presented so far showed that ASMR appears as a complex phenomenon characterized by paradoxical manifestations. Until today, several studies have assumed the existence of this idiosyncrasy, but there is no clear explanation for this yet. In our opinion, in all the individuals, in front of an ASMR video, the DMN is activated and the pupillary diameter is increased, but some subjects, who claimed to perceive “Many” tingles, tended to resist these idiosyncratic manifestations (probably because of the activation of the ASMR). A possible explanation could rely on a struggle of cognitive systems in restoring balance, homeostasis, so that tingles could be the manifestation of this idiosyncrasy/mechanism. People who felt more tingles probably tended to resist flooding, and probably did not want to let loose; this could be justified both by the results of IRI-Empathic Concern (higher) and by those of the Telepresence Scale (lower).
Similarly, also with the results derived from ASMR 15 [
35], it is noted that the Relaxation subscale is not related to the NeuroSky brain waves; this could mean that the relaxation resulting from the ASMR is not related to changes in brain frequencies. This does not mean that relaxation does not occur during ASMR, but it is probably linked to other aspects, still not explored. In our view, this mental state could represent a sort of paradoxical desynchronization. Obviously, all that has been said above represents a starting point that needs more research to deepen the idea of paradoxical desynchronization and to understand its relationship with the perceptive phenomenology of ASMR. It should be clarified that, although the average amount of tingles was very low, the difference between “Few” and “Many” tingles was significant, and the amount of tingles detected is more or less in line with the literature. Furthermore, the low amount of tingles could certainly be due to the laboratory conditions, which probably reduced immersion and relaxation. Nevertheless, this could be a limitation, and these data are extremely interesting and we believe that further research is necessary.
In conclusion, it is clear that people use ASMR videos to relax, induce sleep, and reduce anxiety, but despite the relaxing state in which people affirm to be, physiological data also suggest an activation component. Our data can be a starting point to understanding the general framework regarding the mechanisms underlying the ASMR; because of the complex nature of the phenomenon, it is necessary to understand how this activation is consciously perceived by individuals, and also, how, and when, this idiosyncrasy occurs.