Next Article in Journal
The Association of Emotional Eating with Overweight/Obesity, Depression, Anxiety/Stress, and Dietary Patterns: A Review of the Current Clinical Evidence
Previous Article in Journal
Urban–Rural Disparities in Knowledge, Use and Perceived Benefits of Nutrition Labels in China: Evidence from 10 Provinces
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Effects of Culinary Spices on Liking and Consumption of Protein Rich Foods in Community-Dwelling Older Adults

1
Anschutz Health and Wellness Center, 12348 E. Montview Blvd., MailStop C263, Aurora, CO 80045, USA
2
Division of Endocrinology, Diabetes and Metabolism, University of Colorado Denver, Anschutz Medical Campus, 12801 E. 17th Ave., RC1 South Rm 7103, Aurora, CO 80045, USA
3
Department of Pediatrics, University of Colorado Denver, Anschutz Medical Campus, 13123 E. 16th Ave., B065, Aurora, CO 80045, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2023, 15(5), 1172; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15051172
Submission received: 2 February 2023 / Revised: 23 February 2023 / Accepted: 24 February 2023 / Published: 26 February 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Exercise and Nutrition for Older People)

Abstract

:
Insufficient protein intake is a common challenge among older adults, leading to loss of muscle mass, decreased function and reduced quality of life. A protein intake of 0.4 g/kg body weight/meal is recommended to help prevent muscle loss. The purpose of this study was to assess whether the protein intake of 0.4 g/kg body weight/meal could be achieved with typical foods and whether culinary spices could enhance protein intake. A lunch meal test was conducted in 100 community-dwelling volunteers; 50 were served a meat entrée and 50 were served a vegetarian entrée with or without added culinary spices. Food consumption, liking and perceived flavor intensity were assessed using a randomized, two-period, within subjects crossover design. Within the meat or vegetarian treatments, there were no differences in entrée or meal intakes between spiced and non-spiced meals. Participants fed meat consumed 0.41 g protein/kg body weight/meal, while the vegetarian intake was 0.25 g protein/kg body weight/meal. The addition of spice to the vegetarian entrée significantly increased liking and flavor intensity of both the entrée and the entire meal, while spice addition only increased flavor for the meat offering. Culinary spices may be a useful tool to improve the liking and flavor of high-quality protein sources among older adults, especially when used with plant-based foods, although improving liking and flavor alone are insufficient to increase protein intake.

1. Introduction

Loss of muscle mass is an inevitable consequence of aging, with a 3–8% decline per decade after age 30 [1] and even greater losses after the age of 60 [2,3]. This can lead to frailty, osteoporosis and loss of physical function, diminishing the quality of life of older individuals [4,5]. Adequate nutrition, including sufficient energy and protein intakes as well as exercise, can help slow the rate of muscle decline [6,7]. Protein intakes that are insufficient to slow age-related muscle loss are a prevalent problem among older adults (≥60 years) [8]. Recent evidence also suggests that inadequate protein intake may contribute to loss of cognition with age [9].
Over the past decade, a number of experts and scientific advisory groups have evaluated the adequacy of protein intake recommendations for adults over the age of 60–65 [10,11,12,13,14]. In the US, for adults over the age of 60, the Recommended Dietary Allowance for protein intake established by the Food and Nutrition Board of the National Academy of Medicine is 0.8 g protein/kg body weight/d, which is the same as for younger adults [15]. The European Society for Clinical Nutrition and Metabolism (ESPEN) recommends 1–1.2 g protein/kg body weight/d for adults over the age of 65 to sustain muscle strength and function, and even greater amounts for individuals with severe illness or injury [13]. In 2017, the nutrition societies for Germany, Austria and Switzerland (D-A-CH) revised the reference intakes for protein for adults over the age of 65 to 1.0 g protein/kg body weight/d [12]. The PROT-AGE Study Group, a consortium of the European Union Geriatric Medicine Society (EUGMS), in cooperation with other scientific organizations, recommends an intake of 1–1.2 g protein/g body weight/d for adults over age 65 [10]. Additional insights developed in recent years have prompted independent experts to recommend a protein intake of at least 1.2 g protein/kg body weight for adults age >65 [11,16,17,18]. While there is a range of protein intake recommendations by authoritative sources from 0.8 to 1.2 g protein intake/kg body weight/d, the higher daily intake target is associated with more favorable conditions for maximal protein synthesis in older adults (e.g., [18]). The 1.2 g protein/kg body weight/d target equates to 0.4 g protein/kg body weight/meal, assuming a three-meal per day dietary pattern.
Inadequate protein intake among older adults may be caused by reduced appetite, a loss of sensory acuity with age, difficulty chewing tough and fibrous foods, the cost of high-quality protein sources, perceived intolerance to certain foods and other factors [19,20,21]. It may be possible to address the reduced appetite and sensory perception that occurs with age by enhancing the flavor intensity and liking of high-quality protein sources through the use of culinary herbs and spices [22,23]. In addition, preparing protein-rich foods that are easy to chew and swallow may facilitate greater consumption. Both improving food texture and increasing the flavor intensity and food liking of dietary protein sources while still adhering to dietary guidelines may contribute to increasing protein intake among older adults, helping improve physiological function and overall quality of life.
Few studies have specifically examined the role of culinary spices as a tool to improve protein intake among community-dwelling older adults. Published information from focus groups and quantitative surveys among community-dwelling older adults have suggested that increasing flavor would be advantageous to increasing intake [22,23]. This could be accomplished through addition of seasonings or adding sauces and gravies to protein entrees [24,25]. Other approaches could include adding flavor enhancers [26]. No studies to our knowledge have compared protein consumption from lunch meals with meat-based protein to those with plant-based protein sources. Likewise, we are not aware of studies in community-dwelling older adults that have assessed the potential effect of self-perceived declines in sensory acuity on protein intake and the effects of culinary spices on flavor perception, food liking and intake.
The purpose of the current study was to test whether a protein intake of 0.4 g/kg body weight could be achieved in a standardized meal and whether the addition of culinary spices could enhance protein intake compared to a meal without added spices. In addition, the study assessed whether the addition of culinary spices to a meal prepared with only salt and pepper improves liking and flavor intensity, which are often correlated with intake. The study looks at both a meat-based protein meal (M) and a plant-based protein (V) meal. This is important, as current dietary recommendations advocate consuming more plant-based diets [15], and this study can provide some perspective on the effectiveness of vegetable-derived protein sources for meeting the higher protein requirements of older individuals. Both of the test meal entrée recipes (M and V) were chosen to reduce the likelihood that food texture (e.g., fibrous, difficult to chew and swallow) would limit consumption. Information about liking, hunger and satiety also provide insight into how these factors may correlate with protein intake. Finally, we assessed flavor intensity as well as self-reported losses in smell and taste acuity to see if enhancing the flavor of high-protein dishes is associated with greater consumption in both individuals with and without reported sensory loss.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Design

A randomized, two-period, within subjects crossover design was used to examine and compare the consumption, liking, flavor intensity and subjective appetite ratings of two different lunch meal treatments in two separate participant groups fed different protein rich meals: 1. A meat-derived protein meal (M) and 2. A plant-derived (vegetarian) protein meal (V). The two test conditions for each protein meal type were (1) salt and pepper only preparation (No Spice, “NS”) as one meal and (2) herb and spice preparation with salt and pepper as the other meal (Spice, “S”). Within each protein meal type (M or V), the two test meals were matched as closely as possible for macronutrients and calories. A one-week washout period separated the two test meal treatments.
Sample size: There were limited data from intervention studies in the literature on which to base a power calculation. Previous studies of older adult populations have used treatment sample sizes of 18–50 individuals [22,25,27]. We used data from one large observational study that reported daily and meal-based protein intake to estimate sample size for this study [28]. Sample size was chosen to detect the efficacy of the S meal on protein consumption (g protein/kg body weight/meal), which was the primary outcome of interest. We estimated the standard deviation of protein intake (g/kg body weight/meal) to be 0.30 [28] and conservatively estimated the standard deviation of the individual difference between the two meal conditions as 1.414 × 0.30 = 0.424. We considered an increase by 0.2 g kg body weight−1 (i.e., 10 g more protein intake for a subject of 50 kg) for lunch protein intake to be of clinical importance. In this case, a sample size of 50 participants per protein source (100 total participants) ensures a 90% power at 5% significance to detect the intervention effect of meals enhanced with culinary spices. A sample size of 50 also provides a 90% power at 5% significance to detect a correlation of 0.44.

2.2. Participants

One hundred and nine community-dwelling male and female participants aged ≥60 years were enrolled in the study (52 for M treatment; 57 for V meal treatment), recruited from the Anschutz Medical Campus (drawing from over 17,000 employees on the campus) and Denver Metro area. Fifty participants in each treatment group completed the testing procedure (see CONSORT diagram in Figure 1). Participants were screened by telephone. Age 60 was selected as the lower age limit because this age is included in published literature on older adults when considering protein intake challenges (e.g., [8]). Participants of all body weights were accepted. Eligible participants had to be regular lunch consumers (≥5 days/week). Exclusion criteria were as follows: 1. diagnosed taste or sensory disorders that would prevent them from evaluating the food, 2. known eating disorders, 3. allergies to the test food/ingredients (including herbs and spices), 4. medications or medical conditions that may adversely affect taste (e.g., dysgeusia), 5. inability to complete the protocol, 6. personal dietary restrictions towards test meal items, and 7. dislike of the particular food items or herb/spices in the test meals. They were also asked their preference for being offered either the M or V meal entrée for testing (once the M group was filled, eligible participants were asked if they were okay with eating the V meal). Qualified individuals were assigned a schedule for their visits to the testing facility for participating in the study.
Participants in each treatment group (M or V) were randomized into a meal type sequence group (NS to S or S to NS), with a washout week in between each treatment.
All subjects provided informed consent for participation in the study. Participants received compensation having a value of USD 75 for completing both lunch visits.

2.3. Meal Testing

Testing consisted of serving complete meals at lunch and evaluating the consumption, liking and perceived flavor intensity of each item and the liking of the entire meal. In addition, we evaluated appetite before and after the test meals using a visual analog scale (VAS) instrument. Lunch was chosen as the eating occasion to study because it has been identified as an underutilized meal for leveraging protein intake [27].
For each test, the meals consisted of a protein entrée (M or V) and the same three accompanying side items (two butter lettuce leaves, two whole-wheat crackers and one half cup of seedless grapes). One version of the meal (NS) was prepared using only salt and/or pepper as added flavor elements (salt levels targeted to be consistent with current dietary recommendations) while the second version of the meal (S) was prepared with a more diverse blend of culinary spices to boost flavor. Test meals were served one week apart, aimed at determining the absolute liking of the meal/items vs. assessing relative liking by providing a side-by-side comparison at the same seating. This scheme is more relevant to a free-living condition in which people likely choose food items and recipes based on their absolute liking.

2.4. Test Meal Composition

The NS version of the M entrée consisted of chicken salad using commercially available canned, shredded skinless chicken breast, 2% fat yogurt, mayonnaise, celery, lemon juice and pepper. The S version of M entrée had the same composition as the NS version, but with added garlic powder, onion powder, ground mustard seed and dill. See recipes for both versions in Appendix A. Canned, skinless chicken breast was chosen as the protein source because it is a widely available, high-quality lean protein source that is familiar and well-liked by most people.
The NS version of the V entrée consisted of garbanzo beans (chickpeas) and brown rice, unflavored soy protein powder, 2% fat yogurt, mayonnaise, celery, lemon juice, water, salt and pepper. The S version had the same composition as the NS version plus added pepper, garlic powder, onion powder, ground mustard seed, parsley, oregano, dill, rosemary and cumin. See recipes for both V preparations in Appendix A. Addition of rice as a “complimentary protein” to the beans was necessary to ensure that the composite protein content of the entrée would have a complete indespensible amino acid profile. In addition, powdered soy protein was added (at a level that did not affect product acceptance and mouthfeel) to increase the protein density of the entrée.
We served entrée portions to each participant based on their body weight to facilitate consumption of at least 0.36 g/kg body weight/meal protein (representing 30% of a daily intake target of 1.2 g/kg body weight). We aimed to deliver between 18 and 40 g of protein in the meal entrée depending on body weight. We served an additional 20% of the entrée to ensure that intake was not constrained to the initial serving size. This would allow for consumption sufficient to meet the 0.4 g/kg body weight/meal level suggested for supporting optimal protein synthesis in older adults. For example, a 50 kg woman would be served an entrée containing 23.8 g protein, representing 55 kg × 0.36 g protein/kg body weight/meal × 1.2 (20% more) = 23.8 g protein/meal. If individuals consumed the entire entrée, they were free to ask for more. If requested, we provided additional portions (half the size of the first serving), and any remaining food was weighed back to determine energy and protein consumption.

2.5. Study Procedures

Participants were asked to eat their usual breakfast, but instructed to not eat any food or caloric beverages within 4 h of coming to the testing facility (The Anschutz Health and Wellness Center on the University of Colorado Denver, Anschutz Medical Campus in Aurora, Colorado). At their first visit, participants were weighed and their height was measured. Body weight information was used to calculate the entrée serving size as described earlier. Participants were assigned to individual tables, in a café environment with tables spaced six feet apart. Test meals were served at two times each test day: 11:30 a.m. and 1 p.m. Up to 20 participants were served on each test day, 10 at each seating. Participants were tested on the same day of the week and same time for the two test meals. Participants were blinded to the test conditions (meals with or without culinary spices), but because of the different visual appearance of the two entrees (the spiced version having green flecks), it was not possible to blind the study staff. Separate staff prepared and served the meals.
Once seated, participants were asked if there had been any change in their health status or other factors that may affect their ability to objectively complete the test meal. They were instructed on how to complete the meal liking and flavor intensity evaluations, VAS appetite measures [29] and VARSEEK [30] questionnaires. Participants were served the test meal for that visit according to the randomization treatment sequence schedule and were provided with 12 oz. of water to drink with the meal. Participants were allowed 45 min to complete the pre-meal assessments (VAS appetite assessment at both visits; sensory acuity and VARSEEK questionnaire at the first visit only), consume the meal and complete the post-meal assessments (VAS, liking and flavor intensity). All food items were weighed prior to serving and again after meal completion to assess protein and energy intakes.
Energy and protein intakes were calculated using the Nutrition Data System for Research database [31] based on the meal item composition (from the recipes) and the amounts consumed.
Assessments: On the first test day, prior to meal consumption, participants filled out a three-question assessment of self-perceived sensory acuity [32] (Appendix B 1). Participants also filled out a 9-point hedonic scale-rating instrument anchored by “dislike extremely” to “like extremely” that assessed their liking of the test entrée after the first few bites, liking after finishing the entrée and liking of the overall composite meal (Appendix B 2). Prior to meal presentation and after consumption of the meal, participants also completed a four-item, 100 mm visual analog scale (VAS) asking about their degree of hunger, satiety, fullness and prospective consumption feelings (Appendix B 3). The perceived flavor intensity of the entrée after the first few bites and after finishing it, and the rating for the entire meal was rated using a 5-point Likert Scale anchored by “no intensity at all” at one extreme and “extremely intense” at the other extreme (Appendix B 4). Finally, the VARSEEK questionnaire [30] was administered at the first seating prior to meal consumption as an exploratory probe to see if participants’ food-variety-seeking tendencies might correlate with consumption of the foods offered, given that the test foods may be perceived as not a part of their usual diet (Appendix B 5).

2.6. Data Analysis

Study data were double data entered into REDCap (Electronic Data Capture), a secure internal computerized database system [33]. The REDCap data comparison tool was used to compare and resolve discrepancies between the two copies of each subject’s records.
Participants’ demographic characteristics, self-reported sensory change since 35 years old and responses to the VARSEEK questionnaire were tabulated using summary statistics. All participants attended both sittings for each meal type so there were no missing data for the study.
Differences in protein and energy intakes, food liking, flavor intensity and satiety between test meal conditions (i.e., S or NS) were analyzed using a linear mixed effects model (LMM) with a random subject effect (Model 1). The LMM model consisted of the treatment sequence group (i.e., S->NS or NS->S), test meal period (i.e., visit 1 or visit 2), and test meal condition (i.e., S or NS) as fixed effects. Differences in the effect of spice addition between protein sources (M or V) was tested using a second model. In Model 2, we analyzed data from both entrée types together by introducing protein source (i.e., M or V protein) and its interaction term with test meal condition (i.e., S or NS) into Model 1 while allowing protein source to have different random subject effects. Under Model 2, the protein source effect was assessed by testing the interaction terms of protein source and meal condition. Separate exploratory analyses of any moderation effects of change in self-reported sensory acuity or food-variety-seeking tendency were conducted for the two protein sources. To investigate whether the spices’ effect was stronger in participants with vs. without decreased sensory acuity, we analyzed the data in the same fashion as Model 2 with sensory loss status substituting for protein source.
Other analyses: Individual scores for the difference between meal conditions in protein consumption were correlated with changes in liking scores, flavor intensity ratings, and hunger and satiety scores, respectively. In addition, the amount food consumed was correlated with the VARSEEK questionnaire composite score to assess any effect of food-variety-seeking behavior on intake. Finally, whether or not the participant had experienced COVID-19 (more than 6 months prior to testing) was correlated with protein intake. The appropriateness of normality assumptions was assessed by examining residual plots from the mixed model.

3. Results

3.1. Participant Characteristics

Characteristics of the 100 participants (50 for each meal test, all unique and not overlapping between treatments) enrolled in the study are shown in Table 1. There were no statistically significant differences between participants in the two meal treatment groups (M vs. V) in any of the characteristics measured, including age, sex, height, weight, body mass index (BMI), self-reported sensory acuity, COVID-19 history or responses to the VARSEEK questionnaire. It is noteworthy that both meal treatment groups had mean BMI values over 25, classifying them as having overweight or obesity. This is not surprising, as the prevalence of overweight and obesity among adults over the age of 20 in the USA is 73.9% [34].

3.2. Protein and Meal Consumption

Within each meal type, M or V, there were no significant differences in consumption of the entrée or total meal (when expressed as food weight or kcal) between the NS and S versions (Table 2). Likewise, there were no differences in the absolute amount of protein consumed or the amount eaten when expressed per kilogram of body weight for the NS or S versions of both the M and V meals. Energy (kcal) intakes of the entrée when expressed as a fraction of estimated total daily energy expenditure (TDEE, Table 1) were between 15 and 20% for both M and V meals. Values for the total lunch (including crackers and grapes) were between 21 and 26% of TDEE.
Participants served the M meal achieved total protein intakes of 32 ± 1.3 g and 32.7 ± 1.3 g/meal for NS and S versions, respectively (Table 2). Intakes expressed per kilogram of body weight were 0.40 ± 0.01 and 0.41 ± 0.01 g/meal for the NS and S versions, respectively. Seventy-four percent of M meal participants receiving NS achieved a protein intake ≥ 0.36 g/meal (30% of the daily target of 1.2 g/kg body weight/d for older adults), while 80% of those fed the S version ate at least 0.36 g/kg body weight/meal. These were not different between NS and S meals (p = 0.221). When considered against a target intake of 0.4 g/kg body weight/meal, 64% achieved the target for the NS entrée while 68% achieved the target for the S entrée (p = 0.534 for NS vs. S). Moreover, 93 and 94% of subjects served the NS and S meals, respectively, consumed at least 0.24 g/kg body weight/meal (p = 0.770 for S vs. NS), which equates to 30% of the current US RDA of 0.8 g/kg body weight/d (if lunch provided 30% of daily intake).
Protein intake from the V meal was 20 ± 1.4 g for both the NS and S versions (Table 2). Intakes per kilogram body weight were 0.25 ± 0.02 and 0.26 ± 0.02 g for the NS and S meals, respectively. Only 20 and 24% of participants receiving the NS and S V meals, respectively (p = 0.467), achieved a protein intake ≥0.36 g/kg body weight/meal. Compared to a target intake of 0.4 g/kg body weight/meal, 18% and 20% of participants achieved this level of intake for the NS and S meals, respectively (p=0.745). A much greater proportion of subjects, 48% and 60%, respectively, achieved a protein intake of 0.24 g/kg body weight/meal (p = 0.105), a level in line with the current US RDA.
There were no significant entrée by recipe (NS or S) interactions for the amounts/kcals of food consumed (Table 2).

3.3. Liking and Flavor Intensity

Liking scores for the M entrée and total meal ranged between 7.2 and 7.5 on a nine-point Likert scale for both the NS and S recipes (Table 3) and were not significantly different. This was true for the initial evaluation after a few bites of the entrée, for the post-meal evaluation of the entrée and for the entire meal. Liking scores for the V entrée and meal were somewhat lower for both NS and S versions compared to the M meal, ranging between 4.8 and 5.6 for NS and S versions, respectively (Table 3). Spice addition to the V entrée significantly increased liking at the beginning of the meal, at the end of the meal and for the overall meal (Table 3).
Flavor intensity ratings were significantly greater for the S version of both the M and V entrées compared to the NS recipes (Table 3) at both the beginning and end of the meal. Participants also rated the flavor intensity of the S version of the entire V meal significantly higher than that of the NS meal. There were significant entrée by recipe interactions for both liking and flavor intensity across all measures, meaning that the subjective responses to the addition of culinary spices were different between the M and V entrée types, with spices having a greater positive effect on V entrée and meal liking and flavor (Table 3).

3.4. Appetite Measures

There were no statistically significant differences in appetite measures before the meal between lunch types (M or V), nor were there differences in starting values between recipe types (NS or S) for each lunch type (data not shown).
The S version of the M meal was associated with a significantly greater reduction in prospective consumption following the meal compared to the NS meal (p = 0.047, Table 4). No other significant differences were observed for hunger, fullness or desire to eat between M and V meals for either NS or S versions of the entrees. There were no significant entrée by recipe interactions.
More than two-thirds of the participants rated their perception of food flavor and their sense of smell now compared to age 30–35 as being the same or stronger (Table 5), and two thirds rated their sense of smell now as being above average. Between 24 and 32% of participants rated these subjective variables as average or weaker than when they were younger.
Composite VARSEEK scores (range of 1–40, with higher scores indicating more likely to eat unfamiliar or novel items) were not different between the two meal treatment groups, with means of 28.09 ± 4.52 and 30.24 ± 4.72 for M and V groups, respectively (p = 0.176).

3.5. Moderator Effects

There were no significant effects of gender, BMI, sensory acuity, COVID-19 experience or VARSEEK score on the amount of M or V entrée consumed as a function of recipe type, NS or S. However, there were significant effects of gender, BMI, COVID-19 status, and sensory acuity on liking, flavor intensity and appetite scores.
Gender effects: For the M meal, perceived flavor intensity after a few bites was greater for the S version vs. the NS version among females (p = 0.031). In males, flavor intensity was greater for the S version of the M entrée after finishing the entrée and for the complete meal (p = 0.050 and p = 0.026, respectively).
BMI effects: Flavor intensity after a few bites was significantly greater among females with a BMI ≥25 (p < 0.005). Desire to eat and prospective consumption were lower for the S version of the M meal, respectively (p = 0.047 and p = 0.014), among participants with a BMI ≥25.
COVID-19 effects: Flavor intensity was greater for the S version of the M meal after a few bites (p = 0.008) and after finishing the meal (p = 0.039) among participants reporting never having COVID-19 vs. those who had it more than 6 months prior to the study.
Sensory acuity: For participants served the M meal, perceived flavor intensity was greater after a few bites and after finishing the meal (p = 0.028 and p = 0.012, respectively) among those reporting the same or stronger flavor perception now than when they were 30–35 years old. Likewise, reported flavor intensity was greater among those served the S version of the M meal, who rated their sense of smell now as above average or greater than when they were 30–35 years old.
For the V meal, there were a number of significant effects of self-assessed sensory acuity measures on liking and perceived flavor intensity (Table 6).
Participants with above average or better than average perceived smell rated liking and flavor of the S version of the V entrée and meal significantly greater than the NS version. Those with average or below average perceived smell liked the S version significantly more than the NS version for initial liking and flavor intensity and for final flavor intensity. Ratings of liking and flavor intensity were significantly higher for S compared to NS versions of the entrée and meal when participants rated smell the same or stronger now compared to when they were 30–35 years old. Those with perceived weaker smell also rated initial flavor intensity greater for the S version, but other liking and flavor scores were not different between NS and S versions. Perceived flavor sensitivity effects showed a pattern similar to that of smell, with participants reporting the same or stronger flavor sensitivity having significantly greater liking and flavor intensity scores for the S version of the entrée and meal compared to the NS version. Initial entrée flavor intensity and final flavor intensity of the entrée and meal were significantly greater among participants who rated their flavor sensitivity weaker than when they were younger.

4. Discussion

A major objective of this study was to investigate whether protein intake at a single meal could meet or exceed the recommended per meal protein intake for community-dwelling older persons to maintain muscle mass and function. An additional aim was to determine whether the addition of culinary spices could improve liking of high-quality protein foods, potentially increasing their consumption to help meet this protein intake target. A daily protein intake of 1.2–2 g protein/kg body weight/d is recommended to preserve muscle mass and prevent loss of function in older adults [13,18,36,37,38]. This equates to a per meal intake of 25–35 g of high-quality protein, assuming a three meal per day consumption pattern [13,39,40]. Furthermore, when expressed on a body weight basis, an intake of 0.4 g protein/kg body weight/meal or greater is suggested to trigger protein synthesis [41].
In this study, we observed protein intakes for the M meal of > 30 g/meal that met the 25–30 g total protein intake per meal and the 0.4 g/kg body weight/meal targets for roughly two-thirds of participants. In contrast, participants served the V meal consumed a mean of only 20 g of protein, and only one in five met the 0.4 g/kg body weight/meal target. These findings are not surprising given that the protein density of the M meal was nearly three times greater (0.167 g protein/g M entrée vs. 0.062 g protein/g V entrée). Even with the addition of soy protein powder to the V entrée, it was not possible to match the greater protein density of the M entrée. Participants served the V entrée would have had to consume substantially greater volumes in order to achieve the protein intake target, although the portions served were large enough to accomplish this. In the present study, participants fed the V meal actually ate 25% more energy than those fed the M meal, but it was still insufficient to meet the protein intake target. Both treatment groups ate to the same level of satiety (Table 4). The greater protein density and complete indespensible amino acid profile of meat-based vs. vegetable-based sources [42,43,44] is the main reason that it was difficult to match protein and amino acid densities between M and V foods. Furthermore, in an attempt to make the foods easy to chew and swallow, we chose the chicken and bean/rice salad forms, whose recipes had other ingredients that diluted the protein content.
Surprisingly, the addition of culinary spices to either the M or V entrée did not increase consumption beyond that observed with addition of salt and pepper alone. Spice addition significantly improved liking scores for the V entrée, and meal and flavor intensity scores were greater for both M and V entrees and for the overall V meal. Food liking is often correlated with consumption [45,46], although this is not a consistent finding [47,48,49]. Adding seasonings to meals of older adults has been shown to increase flavor intensity perception and intakes of protein, energy and fat [24,25]. This can occur even in the absence of changes in perceived pleasantness [24], providing evidence that increasing liking is not a pre-requisite for increased intake.
In the case of the M entrée, it is possible that the base recipe, with the flavors of chicken, mayonnaise, yogurt, celery and pepper, was sufficiently pleasant to a make further increase in liking less probable. Absolute liking scores for the NS version of the M entrée averaged 7.2–7.4, which is typical for foods that are well liked [50], and these relatively high liking scores leave little room for an increase upon the addition of spices. For the V entrée, absolute liking scores were significantly lower than for the M entrée, averaging 4.8–5.3 for the NS version, and the addition of spices significantly improved both liking and perceived flavor intensity. There was a significant interaction between the effect of spice addition to either the M or V meals, with spices increasing liking for the V meal more than for the M meal. This may not be surprising, as the V meal composed of beans and rice was less flavorful than the M meal, which had the savory flavor of chicken as the foundation.
Furthermore, 24 to 32% of our participants reported having weaker smell and flavor sensitivity than when they were 30–35 years old, while 32% reported average or below average smell acuity at present. These numbers are consistent with other studies of age-related changes in sensory acuity. Rawal and colleagues [32] found that 16% of older women reported below average smell function while the overall occurrence of smell and flavor sensory dysfunction was 30%. Similarly, in other surveys, the incidence of self-reported taste and smell alterations among adults over age 40 was 23% for smell and 19% for taste [51]. In the present study, participants fed the M meal and who reported the same or stronger flavor perception and above average smell than when they were younger scored the S version of the entrée and meal as more intense, while those reporting weaker flavor perception did not. This suggests that the participants with reduced subjective sensory acuity were less able to detect the intensity of the added spices. Even greater differences were found for the V meal (Table 6) showing consistently greater liking and flavor intensity among those with no reported decline in sensory acuity and weaker effects among those reporting sensory decline. This suggests that adding spices to the relatively bland V entrée created a more discernable contrast to the NS version compared to adding spices to the more savory M entrée.
Declining sensory acuity has been suggested to be one factor affecting the loss of appetite, reduced food intake and reduced food liking among older adults [19,21,52,53]. Focus groups and quantitative surveys among older adults suggested that boosting liking and “tastiness” would help promote protein intake [23,54], and adding seasonings is one strategy for accomplishing this [25,55]. Furthermore, food familiarity and other cognitive cues may be helpful in maintaining food intake in the face of declining sensory acuity [56,57]. Our findings support this previous work and indicate that adding spices to high quality protein foods can improve flavor, and sometimes liking, but other factors may drive total food and protein intakes.
Previous COVID-19 status (having the virus more than 6 months prior to study) did not affect the consumption of either the M or V meals. Although sensory losses from COVID-19 are reportedly restored within a few months [58], this may not be true for everyone, with 1 in 10 affected people reporting little improvement after 2 years [59]. In our study, flavor intensity was reportedly greater for the S version of the M meal among those who never reported having COVID-19. It is possible that those who previously had COVID-19 still suffered from some sensory impairment, although not severe enough for them to report it upon screening.
Variety-seeking behavior among individuals is one factor that may affect the selection and intake of foods [30], and the VARSEEK instrument was developed to assess this tendency. We used the VARSEEK instrument to investigate whether variety seeking behavior was associated with the consumption of either NS or S versions of the M and V entrées. The scores of 29–31 obtained for our sample are reflective of medium variety seekers, with scores of below 25 or above 35 indicating either low or high variety seeking, respectively. There was no interaction between these scores and intake suggesting that the foods we served were neither unfamiliar nor particularly novel, which might have affected intake.
This study is encouraging in that it shows it is possible to achieve the proposed protein intake target of 0.4 g protein/kg body weight/meal for older individuals, at least for meat-based entrees. This was accomplished with ingredients that are readily available, affordable (canned chicken breast, etc.) and easily prepared at home. It also points to the challenges of achieving the same high level of protein intake when plant-based protein sources are used. While achieving adequate protein intakes from plant-based diets is possible for younger individuals whose total food and energy intakes are higher than for older adults, the lower protein per kcal density of plant proteins makes it much more difficult for older adults whose protein requirements are greater [44,60,61]. Because most plant proteins do not contain all indispensable amino acids, it is necessary to mix protein sources (e.g., beans and rice) which thus reduces the overall protein density of the food. Even when the V entrée was fortified by adding soy protein powder, it was hard to increase the protein density enough to ensure intakes that would meet the 0.4 g/kg body weight/meal target without negatively affecting mouthfeel. Soy protein powder could be used only in limited amounts due to its astringency and graininess.
Despite the lack of effect of culinary spices on increasing total protein intake in the present study, these substances have bioactive properties beyond their ability to enhance the flavor of foods. Culinary spices such as oregano, rosemary, cumin, parsley and dill used in the present study are rich in polyphenolic compounds [62,63,64] having significant antimicrobial, antioxidant, anti-inflammatory, anti-diabetic and anticancer properties [65]. Thus, the use of culinary spices to boost flavor and improve liking of foods may deliver benefits to adults of all ages.
Study strengths and limitations: Strengths of this study include a relatively large sample size for a randomized and standardized meal test, objective measures of intake and subjective measures of liking, flavor intensity, appetite and subjective sensory acuity. Test foods were selected that were familiar and could easily be prepared at home from readily available and affordable ingredients. The test food formulations also eliminated potential problems related to chewing and swallowing, which are issues more common among older individuals. In effect, the food forms served were optimized for promoting easy consumption, providing a reasonable benchmark for testing the feasibility of achieving the higher protein intake target for older individuals. A significant limitation of this study is that it tested only one meal occasion, lunch, and further studies would be needed to determine whether total daily protein intake could be sustainably increased to meet the 1.2 g protein/kg body weight/day target for older individuals. We did not collect information about the dietary intake of participants either before or after the test lunch, so we could not estimate total daily protein intake. Finally, the particular choice of foods and spices tested is a limitation given the much wider variety of foods consumed as part of a normal diet.

5. Conclusions

This study demonstrates that it is feasible to achieve protein intakes of ≥30 g/meal and 0.4 g/kg body weight/meal in community-dwelling older adults using high-quality meat-based foods that are easy to chew and swallow. Achieving this high level of protein intake from plant sources is more difficult and a selection of foods very high in protein density or fortified with complete protein would be required. Culinary spices can improve flavor intensity and liking of protein-rich foods, but do not necessarily improve protein intake. Further studies are necessary to explore the potential of culinary spices to support greater protein intake among older adults.

Author Contributions

The authors’ responsibilities were as follows—Conceptualization, methodology, investigation, data curation, formal analysis, writing, review and editing, supervision and funding acquisition, J.C.P.; investigation resources, data curation, project administration, review and editing, J.C.P. and J.A.B.; formal analysis, visualization, J.C.P. and Z.P. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

JCP received funding from the McCormick Science Institute for the research reported in this article (grant AWD-213916).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, reviewed and approved by the Colorado Multiple Institutional Review Board of the University of Colorado, Denver (protocol COMIRB #21-3652, documented as an exempt study 20 January 2022).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data described in this article can be requested by any qualified researcher and will be provided upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

We thank the staff of the Clinical Trials unit at the Anschutz Health and Wellness Center for their assistance with participant recruitment, screening and enrollment, meal service, data collection and data entry. We also thank the staff of the Clinical Trials Nutrition Research Core for test meal preparation and food consumption data collection. We are grateful to the culinary staff of the McCormick Company, Inc., who designed and qualified the menus based on the nutrient criteria of the experimental meals defined by the investigators. Study data were collected and managed using REDCap electronic data capture [33] tools hosted at the University of Colorado Denver. 1 REDCap (Research Electronic Data Capture) is a secure, web-based application designed to support data capture for research studies, providing: (1) an intuitive interface for validated data entry; (2) audit trails for tracking data manipulation and export procedures; (3) automated export procedures for seamless data downloads to common statistical packages; and (4) procedures for importing data from external sources. This project and publication are supported by NIH/NCATS, Colorado CTSA Grant Number UL1 TR002535. Its contents are the authors’ sole responsibility and do not necessarily represent official NIH views.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results. The conduct of the study and the content of this paper are solely the responsibility of the authors. This study was registered at ClinTrials.gov, trial NCT05175391.

Appendix A

M and V Entrée Recipes, NS and S Versions
Chicken Salad Entree
No Spice Version (NS) Spiced Version (S)
INGREDIENTSWt. (g)INGREDIENTSWt. (g)
Hellmann’s Mayonnaise80Hellmann’s Mayonnaise80
Fage Greek Yogurt, 2% fat80Fage Greek Yogurt, 2% fat80
Lemon Juice, fresh6.3Lemon Juice, fresh6.3
Black Pepper0.43Black Pepper0.8
Kirkland Canned Chicken, 270 mg sodium/serving454Garlic Powder6
Celery, diced70Onion Powder8
Mustard Seed, ground3.5
Dill1.2
Kirkland Canned Chicken, 270 mg sodium/serving454
Celery, diced70
Chickpea and Rice Salad
No Spice Version (NS) Spiced Version (S)
INGREDIENTSWt (g)INGREDIENTSWt (g)
Hellman’s Mayonnaise80Hellman’s Mayonnaise80
Fage Greek Yogurt, 2% fat80Fage Greek Yogurt, 2% fat80
Lemon Juice, fresh12.8Lemon Juice, fresh13
Water100Water100
Morton Iodized Salt1Morton Iodized Salt1
Black Pepper0.43Black Pepper2
365 Everyday Garbanzo Beans240365 Everyday Garbanzo Beans240
Brown Rice168Brown Rice168
Bob’s Red Mill Soy Protein Powder28Bob’s Red Mill Soy Protein Powder28
Celery, diced70Celery, diced70
Garlic Powder6
Onion Powder6
Parsley0.6
Oregano0.5
Dill0.6
Cumin0.7
Mustard seed, ground0.7
Rosemary, ground0.2

Appendix B

Questionnaires and Visual Analog Appetite Measures
B-1: Self-rated olfactory function
How would you rate your sense of smell right now?
Very PoorPoorBelow AverageAverageAbove AverageGoodExcellent
How would you rate your sense of smell now compared to when you were 30 to 35 years old?
Extremely WeakerMuch WeakerSomewhat WeakerThe SameSomewhat StrongerMuch StrongerExtremely Stronger
How would you rate your ability to sense the flavor of food now compared to when you were 30 to 35 years old?
Extremely WeakerMuch WeakerSomewhat WeakerThe SameSomewhat StrongerMuch StrongerExtremely Stronger
B-2: Food Liking
Thank you for participating in this study. We would like to get your opinion about the meal you have just eaten. Please answer the following questions to best of your ability.
How much do you like or dislike the entrée meal item?
Dislike
extremely
Dislike
very much
Dislike
moderately
Dislike
slightly
Neither
like
nor dislike
Like
slightly
Like
moderately
Like
very much
Like
extremely
How much do you like or dislike this overall meal?
Dislike
extremely
Dislike
very much
Dislike
moderately
Dislike
slightly
Neither
like
nor dislike
Like
slightly
Like
moderately
Like
very much
Like
extremely
B-3: Flavor Intensity
Please rate the flavor intensity of the entrée meal item.
No Intensity at allSlightly IntenseModerately IntenseVery IntenseExtremely Intense
Please rate the flavor intensity of the overall meal.
No Intensity at allSlightly IntenseModerately IntenseVery IntenseExtremely Intense
B-4: Visual Analog Appetite Rating Scales (VAS)
Nutrients 15 01172 i001
B-5: VARSEEK Questionnaire
1.
When I eat out, I like to try the most unusual items, even if I am not sure I would like them.
Strongly
Disagree
DisagreeNeither Disagree or AgreeAgreeStrongly
Agree
2.
When preparing foods or snacks, I like to try out new recipes.
Strongly
Disagree
DisagreeNeither Disagree or AgreeAgreeStrongly
Agree
3.
I think it is fun to try out food items one is not familiar with.
Strongly
Disagree
DisagreeNeither Disagree or AgreeAgreeStrongly
Agree
4.
I am eager to know what kind of foods people from other countries eat.
Strongly
Disagree
DisagreeNeither Disagree or AgreeAgreeStrongly
Agree
5.
I like to eat exotic foods.
Strongly
Disagree
DisagreeNeither Disagree or AgreeAgreeStrongly
Agree
6.
Items on the menu that I am unfamiliar with make me curious.
Strongly
Disagree
DisagreeNeither Disagree or AgreeAgreeStrongly
Agree
7.
I prefer to eat food products I am used to.
Strongly
Disagree
DisagreeNeither Disagree or AgreeAgreeStrongly
Agree
8.
I am curious about food products I am not familiar with.
Strongly
Disagree
DisagreeNeither Disagree or AgreeAgreeStrongly
Agree

References

  1. Volpi, E.; Nazemi, R.; Fujita, S. Muscle tissue changes with aging. Curr. Opin. Clin. Nutr. Metab. Care 2004, 7, 405–410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Holloszy, J.O. The biology of aging. In Mayo Clinic Proceedings; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2000; Volume 75, pp. S3–S9. [Google Scholar]
  3. Melton, L.J., 3rd; Khosla, S.; Crowson, C.S.; O’Connor, M.K.; O’Fallon, W.M.; Riggs, B.L. Epidemiology of sarcopenia. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2000, 48, 625–630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Fulop, T.; Larbi, A.; Khalil, A.; Cohen, A.A.; Witkowski, J.M. Are We Ill Because We Age? Front. Physiol. 2019, 10, 1508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. JafariNasabian, P.; Inglis, J.E.; Reilly, W.; Kelly, O.J.; Ilich, J.Z. Aging human body: Changes in bone, muscle and body fat with consequent changes in nutrient intake. J. Endocrinol. 2017, 234, R37–R51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Anton, S.D.; Hida, A.; Mankowski, R.; Layne, A.; Solberg, L.M.; Mainous, A.G.; Buford, T. Nutrition and Exercise in Sarcopenia. Curr. Protein Pept. Sci. 2018, 19, 649–667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Walston, J.D. Sarcopenia in older adults. Curr. Opin. Rheumatol. 2012, 24, 623–627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  8. Gaytán-González, A.; Ocampo-Alfaro, M.d.J.; Torres-Naranjo, F.; González-Mendoza, R.G.; Gil-Barreiro, M.; Arroniz-Rivera, M.; López-Taylor, J.R. Dietary Protein Intake Patterns and Inadequate Protein Intake in Older Adults from Four Countries. Nutrients 2020, 12, 3156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Shang, X.; Hill, E.; Li, Y.; He, M. Energy and macronutrient intakes at breakfast and cognitive declines in community-dwelling older adults: A 9-year follow-up cohort study. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2021, 113, 1093–1103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Bauer, J.; Biolo, G.; Cederholm, T.; Cesari, M.; Cruz-Jentoft, A.J.; Morley, J.E.; Phillips, S.; Sieber, C.; Stehle, P.; Teta, D.; et al. Evidence-Based Recommendations for Optimal Dietary Protein Intake in Older People: A Position Paper From the PROT-AGE Study Group. J. Am. Med. Dir. Assoc. 2013, 14, 542–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Baum, J.I.; Kim, I.-Y.; Wolfe, R.R. Protein Consumption and the Elderly: What Is the Optimal Level of Intake? Nutrients 2016, 8, 359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Richter, M.; Baerlocher, K.; Bauer, J.M.; Elmadfa, I.; Heseker, H.; Leschik-Bonnet, E.; Stangl, G.; Volkert, D.; Stehle, P. Revised Reference Values for the Intake of Protein. Ann. Nutr. Metab. 2019, 74, 242–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Deutz, N.E.; Bauer, J.M.; Barazzoni, R.; Biolo, G.; Boirie, Y.; Bosy-Westphal, A.; Cederholm, T.; Cruz-Jentoft, A.; Krznariç, Z.; Nair, K.S.; et al. Protein intake and exercise for optimal muscle function with aging: Recommendations from the ESPEN Expert Group. Clin. Nutr. 2014, 33, 929–936. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Nowson, C.; O’Connell, S. Protein Requirements and Recommendations for Older People: A Review. Nutrients 2015, 7, 6874–6899. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Dietary Guidelines for Americans, 2020–2025, 9th ed.; U.S. Department of Agriculture; U.S. Department of Health and Human Services: Washington DC, USA, 2020; pp. 30–36+133. Available online: https://www.dietaryguidelines.gov/sites/default/files/2020-12/Dietary_Guidelines_for_Americans_2020-2025.pdf (accessed on 1 February 2023).
  16. Rafii, M.; Chapman, K.; Elango, R.; Campbell, W.W.; Ball, R.O.; Pencharz, P.B.; Courtney-Martin, G. Dietary Protein Requirement of Men >65 Years Old Determined by the Indicator Amino Acid Oxidation Technique Is Higher than the Current Estimated Average Requirement. J. Nutr. 2015, 146, 681–687. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Rafii, M.; Chapman, K.; Owens, J.; Elango, R.; Campbell, W.W.; Ball, R.O.; Pencharz, P.B.; Courtney-Martin, G. Dietary protein requirement of female adults >65 years determined by the indicator amino acid oxidation technique is higher than current recommendations. J. Nutr. 2015, 145, 18–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Traylor, D.A.; Gorissen, S.H.M.; Phillips, S.M. Perspective: Protein Requirements and Optimal Intakes in Aging: Are We Ready to Recommend More Than the Recommended Daily Allowance? Adv. Nutr. 2018, 9, 171–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Booth, P.; Kohrs, M.B.; Kamath, S. Taste acuity and aging: A review. Nutr. Res. 1982, 2, 95–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Mojet, J.; Christ-Hazelhof, E.; Heidema, J. Taste perception with age: Generic or specific losses in threshold sensitivity to the five basic tastes? Chem. Senses 2001, 26, 845–860. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Chernoff, R. Protein and older adults. J. Am. Coll. Nutr. 2004, 23, 627s–630s. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. van den Heuvel, E.; Murphy, J.L.; Appleton, K.M. The provision of recipes and single-use herb/spice packets to increase egg and protein intake in community-dwelling older adults: A randomised controlled trial. Public Health Nutr. 2021, 21, 2109–2122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Appleton, K.M. Barriers to and Facilitators of the Consumption of Animal-Based Protein-Rich Foods in Older Adults. Nutrients 2016, 8, 187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Appleton, K.M. Increases in energy, protein and fat intake following the addition of sauce to an older person’s meal. Appetite 2009, 52, 161–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Best, R.L.; Appleton, K.M. Comparable increases in energy, protein and fat intakes following the addition of seasonings and sauces to an older person’s meal. Appetite 2011, 56, 179–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Schiffman, S.S. Intensification of Sensory Properties of Foods for the Elderly. J. Nutr. 2000, 130, 927S–930S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Smeuninx, B.; Greig, C.A.; Breen, L. Amount, Source and Pattern of Dietary Protein Intake Across the Adult Lifespan: A Cross-Sectional Study. Front. Nutr. 2020, 7, 25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Verreijen, A.M.; van den Helder, J.; Streppel, M.T.; Rotteveel, I.; Heman, D.; van Dronkelaar, C.; Memelink, R.G.; Engberink, M.F.; Visser, M.; Tieland, M.; et al. A higher protein intake at breakfast and lunch is associated with a higher total daily protein intake in older adults: A post-hoc cross-sectional analysis of four randomised controlled trials. J. Hum. Nutr. Diet. 2021, 34, 384–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Blundell, J.; De Graaf, C.; Hulshof, T.; Jebb, S.; Livingstone, B.; Lluch, A.; Mela, D.; Salah, S.; Schuring, E.; Van Der Knaap, H.; et al. Appetite control: Methodological aspects of the evaluation of foods. Obes. Rev. 2010, 11, 251–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. van Trijp, H.C.M.; Steenkamp, J.-B.E.M. Consumers’ Variety Seeking Tendency with Respect to Foods: Measurement and Managerial Implications. Eur. Rev. Agric. Econ. 1992, 19, 181–195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Harnack, L. Nutrition Data System for Research (NDSR). In Encyclopedia of Behavioral Medicine; Gellman, M.D., Turner, J.R., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 1348–1350. [Google Scholar]
  32. Rawal, S.; Hoffman, H.J.; Chapo, A.K.; Duffy, V.B. Sensitivity and Specificity of Self-Reported Olfactory Function in a Home-Based Study of Independent-Living, Healthy Older Women. Chemosens. Percept. 2014, 7, 108–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Harris, P.A.; Taylor, R.; Thielke, R.; Payne, J.; Gonzalez, N.; Conde, J.G. Research electronic data capture (REDCap)—A metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support. J. Biomed. Inform. 2009, 42, 377–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Cheryl, D.; Fryar, M.S.P.H.; Margaret, D.; Carroll, M.S.P.H.; Joseph Afful, M.S.; Division of Health and Nutrition Examination Surveys. Prevalence of Overweight, Obesity, and Severe Obesity Among Adults Aged 20 and Over: United States, 1960–1962 Through 2017–2018; National Center for Health Statistics: Hyattsville, MD, USA, 2017; pp. 1–7. [Google Scholar]
  35. Mifflin, M.D.; St Jeor, S.T.; Hill, L.A.; Scott, B.J.; Daugherty, S.A.; Koh, Y.O. A new predictive equation for resting energy expenditure in healthy individuals. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1990, 51, 241–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  36. Wolfe, R.R. The role of dietary protein in optimizing muscle mass, function and health outcomes in older individuals. Br. J. Nutr. 2012, 108, S88–S93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. Volpi, E.; Campbell, W.W.; Dwyer, J.T.; Johnson, M.A.; Jensen, G.L.; Morley, J.E.; Wolfe, R.R. Is the optimal level of protein intake for older adults greater than the recommended dietary allowance? J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2013, 68, 677–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Wolfe, R.R.; Miller, S.L.; Miller, K.B. Optimal protein intake in the elderly. Clin. Nutr. 2008, 27, 675–684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Wolfe, R.R. Regulation of muscle protein by amino acids. J. Nutr. 2002, 132, 3219s–3224s. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Loenneke, J.P.; Loprinzi, P.D.; Murphy, C.H.; Phillips, S.M. Per meal dose and frequency of protein consumption is associated with lean mass and muscle performance. Clin. Nutr. 2016, 35, 1506–1511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Moore, D.R.; Churchward-Venne, T.A.; Witard, O.; Breen, L.; Burd, N.A.; Tipton, K.D.; Phillips, S.M. Protein ingestion to stimulate myofibrillar protein synthesis requires greater relative protein intakes in healthy older versus younger men. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2015, 70, 57–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Hoffman, J.R.; Falvo, M.J. Protein—Which is Best? J. Sports Sci. Med. 2004, 3, 118–130. [Google Scholar]
  43. Gorissen, S.H.M.; Crombag, J.J.R.; Senden, J.M.G.; Waterval, W.A.H.; Bierau, J.; Verdijk, L.B.; van Loon, L.J.C. Protein content and amino acid composition of commercially available plant-based protein isolates. Amino Acids 2018, 50, 1685–1695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Gorissen, S.H.M.; Witard, O.C. Characterising the muscle anabolic potential of dairy, meat and plant-based protein sources in older adults. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 2018, 77, 20–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Hellemann, U.; Tuorila, H. Pleasantness ratings and consumption of open sandwiches with varying NaCl and acid contents. Appetite 1991, 17, 229–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Pouyet, V.; Cuvelier, G.; Benattar, L.; Giboreau, A. Influence of flavour enhancement on food liking and consumption in older adults with poor, moderate or high cognitive status. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 44, 119–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Peters, J.C.; Marker, R.; Pan, Z.; Breen, J.A.; Hill, J.O. The Influence of Adding Spices to Reduced Sugar Foods on Overall Liking. J. Food Sci. 2018, 83, 814–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Polsky, S.; Beck, J.; Stark, R.A.; Pan, Z.; Hill, J.O.; Peters, J.C. The influence of herbs, spices, and regular sausage and chicken consumption on liking of reduced fat breakfast and lunch items. J. Food Sci. 2014, 79, S2117–S2126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Vickers, Z.; Mullan, L.; Holton, E. Impact of differences in taste test ratings on the consumption of milk in both a laboratory and a foodservice setting1. J. Sens. Stud. 1999, 14, 249–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Wanich, U.; Sayompark, D.; Riddell, L.; Cicerale, S.; Liem, D.G.; Mohebbi, M.; Macfarlane, S.; Keast, R. Assessing Food Liking: Comparison of Food Liking Questionnaires and Direct Food Tasting in Two Cultures. Nutrients 2018, 10, 1957. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Rawal, S.; Hoffman, H.J.; Honda, M.; Huedo-Medin, T.B.; Duffy, V.B. The Taste and Smell Protocol in the 2011-2014 US National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES): Test-Retest Reliability and Validity Testing. Chemosens. Percept. 2015, 8, 138–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Mojet, J.; Heidema, J.; Christ-Hazelhof, E. Taste perception with age: Generic or specific losses in supra-threshold intensities of five taste qualities? Chem. Senses 2003, 28, 397–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Mojet, J.; Christ-Hazelhof, E.; Heidema, J. Taste perception with age: Pleasantness and its relationships with threshold sensitivity and supra-threshold intensity of five taste qualities. Food Qual. Prefer. 2005, 16, 413–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Best, R.L.; Appleton, K.M. The Consumption of Protein-Rich Foods in Older Adults: An Exploratory Focus Group Study. J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 2013, 45, 751–755. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Mathey, M.F.; Siebelink, E.; de Graaf, C.; Van Staveren, W.A. Flavor enhancement of food improves dietary intake and nutritional status of elderly nursing home residents. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2001, 56, M200–M205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  56. Appleton, K.M.; Smith, E. A Role for Identification in the Gradual Decline in the Pleasantness of Flavors With Age. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2016, 71, 987–994. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  57. Giacalone, D.; Llobell, F.; Jaeger, S.R. “Beyond liking” measures in food-related consumer research supplement hedonic responses and improve ability to predict consumption. Food Qual. Prefer. 2022, 97, 104459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Printza, A.; Katotomichelakis, M.; Valsamidis, K.; Metallidis, S.; Panagopoulos, P.; Panopoulou, M.; Petrakis, V.; Constantinidis, J. Smell and Taste Loss Recovery Time in COVID-19 Patients and Disease Severity. J. Clin. Med. 2021, 10, 966. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. McWilliams, M.P.; Coelho, D.H.; Reiter, E.R.; Costanzo, R.M. Recovery from Covid-19 smell loss: Two-years of follow up. Am. J. Otolaryngol. 2022, 43, 103607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Domić, J.; Grootswagers, P.; van Loon, L.J.C.; de Groot, L.C.P.G.M. Perspective: Vegan Diets for Older Adults? A Perspective On the Potential Impact On Muscle Mass and Strength. Adv. Nutr. 2022, 13, 712–725. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Vieux, F.; Rémond, D.; Peyraud, J.-L.; Darmon, N. Approximately Half of Total Protein Intake by Adults Must be Animal-Based to Meet Nonprotein, Nutrient-Based Recommendations, With Variations Due to Age and Sex. J. Nutr. 2022, 152, 2514–2525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Opara, E.I.; Chohan, M. Culinary herbs and spices: Their bioactive properties, the contribution of polyphenols and the challenges in deducing their true health benefits. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2014, 15, 19183–19202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Ali, A.; Wu, H.; Ponnampalam, E.N.; Cottrell, J.J.; Dunshea, F.R.; Suleria, H.A.R. Comprehensive Profiling of Most Widely Used Spices for Their Phenolic Compounds through LC-ESI-QTOF-MS(2) and Their Antioxidant Potential. Antioxidants 2021, 10, 721. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Singh, N.; Yadav, S.S. A review on health benefits of phenolics derived from dietary spices. Curr. Res. Food Sci. 2022, 5, 1508–1523. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Guldiken, B.; Ozkan, G.; Catalkaya, G.; Ceylan, F.D.; Ekin Yalcinkaya, I.; Capanoglu, E. Phytochemicals of herbs and spices: Health versus toxicological effects. Food Chem. Toxicol. 2018, 119, 37–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. CONSORT Flow Diagram.
Figure 1. CONSORT Flow Diagram.
Nutrients 15 01172 g001
Table 1. Participant Characteristics.
Table 1. Participant Characteristics.
CharacteristicChicken Lunch (n = 50)Bean and Rice Lunch (n = 50)
Sex, n (%)Female 39 (78)
Male 11 (12)
Female 36 (72)
Male 14 (28)
Age, mean ± SD 1 (range)
Age, median
68.7 ± 6.3 (60–81)
68
70.7 ± 7.4 (60–87)
70.5
Ethnicity n, (%)Hispanic/Latino 5 (10)
Non-Hisp./Latino 44 (88)
Other 2 (4)
Hispanic/Latino 6 (12)
Non-Hisp./Latino 41 (82)
Other 3 (6)
Race n, (%)White 48 (96)
Other 2 (4)
White 42 (84)
African American 2 (4)
Asian 1 (2)
Native American 1 (2)
Other 4 (8)
Body Weight (kg), mean ± SD80.4 ± 13.679.1 ± 16.0
2 BMI (kg/m2), mean ± SD30.2 ± 5.128.9 ± 5.1
3 TDEE for light activity level1864.4 ± 274.01866.5 ± 339.6
4 Sense of smell now, mean ± SD5.64 ± 1.445.58 ± 1.3
5 Sense of smell compared to when 30–35 years old, mean ± SD3.84 ± 0.913.78 ± 0.71
5 Sense of flavor now compared to when 30–35 years old, mean ± SD4.00 ± 0.704.00 ± 0.83
Has subject ever had COVID-19? (n, %)No 37 (74.0%)No 34 (68%)
Yes 13 (26.0%)Yes 16 (32%)
If history of COVID-19, did COVID-19 affect subject’s taste, smell or appetite in the last 6 months? (n, %)No 13 (100%)No 15 (94%)
Yes 1 (6%)
If history of COVID-19 affected taste orsmell previously (but not in last 6 months), are effects ongoing? (n, %)No 6 (100%)No 4 (100%)
If history of COVID-19 affected appetite (but not in last 6 months), are appetite
effects ongoing? (n, %)
No 3 (100%)No 4 (100%)
6 Total VARSEEK score, mean ± SD29.98 ± 4.5230.24 ± 4.72
1 SD, Standard deviation; 2 BMI, Body mass index; 3 TDEE, Total Daily Energy Expenditure (as estimated by the Miflin, St Jeor equation, assuming light activity [35]); 4 Score on a 7-point Likert Scale, anchored by “Very Poor” and “Excellent”; 5 Scores on a 7-point Likert Scale, anchored by “Extremely Weaker” to “Extremely Stronger”. 6 The larger the VARSEEK score (range 1–40), the more likely to eat unfamiliar or novel items.
Table 2. Food and protein consumption.
Table 2. Food and protein consumption.
OutcomeType of Entrée ServedLS
Mean
(SEM),
No Spice
LS
Mean
(SEM),
Spice
p for within Entrée Comparison, NS vs. Sp for Entrée
by Recipe
Interaction
Amount entrée eaten (g)Chicken salad181.40
(7.54)
185.36
(7.54)
0.5100.340
Bean/rice salad296.38
(21.57)
282.38
(21.57)
0.432
Water consumed (g)Chicken Salad240.10
(16.18)
240.76
(16.27)
0.9570.268
Bean/rice Salad230.96
(14.41)
252.14
(14.41)
0.133
Kcal entrée eatenChicken Salad278.15
(11.56)
284.22
(11.56)
0.5100.733
Bean/rice Salad369.27
(27.37)
367.09
(27.37)
0.923
Kcal entrée eaten as % of TDEE (from Table 1)Chicken Salad14.87
(0.52)
15.26
(0.52)
0.4100.983
Bean/rice Salad19.66
(1.43)
20.08
(1.43)
0.732
Grams protein
consumed from
entrée
Chicken Salad30.24
(1.26)
30.90
(1.26)
0.5100.661
Bean/rice Salad18.24
(1.36)
18.25
(1.36)
0.995
Grams protein
consumed, total meal
Chicken Salad31.97
(1.29)
32.66
(1.29)
0.4890.655
Bean/rice Salad19.97
(1.38)
19.99
(1.38)
0.982
Total protein consumed, g/kg body weightChicken Salad0.40
(0.01)
0.41
(0.01)
0.5260.997
Bean/rice Salad0.25
(0.02)
0.26
(0.02)
0.585
Table 3. 1 Liking and Flavor Intensity Scores.
Table 3. 1 Liking and Flavor Intensity Scores.
Outcome MeasureType of Entrée
Served
LS Mean
(SEM),
No Spice
LS Mean
(SEM),
Spice
2p for within Entrée Comparison, NS vs. Sp for Entrée
by Recipe
Interaction
Liking Rating of
entree (after eating
a few bites)
Chicken
Salad
7.40
(0.18)
7.46
(0.18)
0.760<0.001
Bean/rice
Salad
5.28
(0.27)
6.56
(0.27)
<0.001
Flavor intensity of
entree (after eating
a few bites)
Chicken
Salad
2.26
(0.12)
2.60
(0.12)
0.0160.016
Bean/rice
Salad
1.56
(0.13)
2.38
(0.13)
<0.001
Liking Rating of
entree (after
finishing lunch
meal)
Chicken
Salad
7.16
(0.20)
7.36
(0.20)
0.4070.004
Bean/rice
Salad
4.78
(0.29)
6.04
(0.29)
<0.001
Liking Rating of
overall meal (after
finishing lunch
meal)
Chicken
Salad
7.22
(0.20)
7.36
(0.20)
0.5330.009
Bean/rice
Salad
5.26
(0.26)
6.28
(0.26)
<0.001
Flavor intensity of
entree (after
finishing lunch
meal)
Chicken
Salad
2.26
(0.13)
2.56
(0.13)
0.0470.017
Bean/rice
Salad
1.54
(0.14)
2.36
(0.14)
<0.001
Flavor intensity of
overall meal (after
finishing lunch
meal)
Chicken
Salad
2.44
(0.12)
2.48
(0.12)
0.7660.003
Bean/rice
Salad
1.74
(0.13)
2.38
(0.13)
<0.001
1 Liking scores are from a 9-point Likert scale anchored by “dislike extremely” to “like extremely”. Flavor intensity scores are from a 5-point Likert scale anchored by “no intensity at all” to “extremely intense”. 2 Values in bold are statistically significant p < 0.05.
Table 4. Appetite measures.
Table 4. Appetite measures.
Visual Analog Question (Scored in mm from 0 to 100 mm)Type of
Entree
Served
LS Mean (mm ± SEM),
No Spice
LS Mean
(mm ± SEM),
Spice
1p for within Entrée Comparison, NS vs. Sp for Entrée
by Recipe
Interaction
How hungry now
rating (after
finishing lunch
meal)
Chicken
Salad
14.56
(2.40)
11.73
(2.40)
0.1760.223
Bean/rice
Salad
14.14
(2.94)
15.58
(2.92)
0.610
How full now rating
(after finishing
lunch meal)
Chicken
Salad
65.58
(3.48)
71.16
(3.48)
0.1450.745
Bean/rice
Salad
69.33
(3.76)
73.16
(3.72)
0.323
How strong desire
to eat rating (after
finishing lunch
meal)
Chicken
Salad
15.84
(2.51)
12.09
(2.51)
0.1610.204
Bean/rice
Salad
13.22
(3.03)
14.95
(3.00)
0.610
How much could
eat rating (after
finishing lunch
meal)
Chicken
Salad
17.86
(2.38)
12.57
(2.38)
0.0470.449
Bean/rice
Salad
15.03
(2.54)
12.64
(2.52)
0.395
1 Values in bold are statistically significant p < 0.05.
Table 5. Subjective Sensory Acuity.
Table 5. Subjective Sensory Acuity.
Sensory Acuity MeasureCategoryCount (Percent)95% Lower CI95% Upper CI
Rating the flavor of food now compared to when you were 30 to 35 years old?Same/stronger38
(76.0%)
61.8%86.9%
Weaker12
(24.0%)
13.1%38.2%
Rating of sense of smell nowAbove average/higher34
(68.0%)
53.3%80.5%
Average/below16
(32.0%)
19.5%46.7%
Rating of sense of smell compared to when 30 to 35
years old
Same/stronger34
(68.0%)
53.3%80.5%
Weaker16
(32.0%)
19.5%46.7%
Table 6. V meal; p values for significant differences between NS and S versions on liking and perceived flavor intensity with sensory acuity within a subgroup. There was no significant interaction (moderation) effect except for flavor of food vs. liking entrée rating. Participants gave higher ratings for S version than for NS for each comparison.
Table 6. V meal; p values for significant differences between NS and S versions on liking and perceived flavor intensity with sensory acuity within a subgroup. There was no significant interaction (moderation) effect except for flavor of food vs. liking entrée rating. Participants gave higher ratings for S version than for NS for each comparison.
Perceived Sensory Acuity RatingInitial Entrée
Liking
Initial Flavor IntensityFinal Entrée LikingOverall Meal LikingFinal Entree Flavor IntensityOverall Meal Flavor Intensity
Rating of sense of smell nowAbove average/higher<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001
Average/below0.0070.010 0.028
Sense of smell compared to when 30 to 35
years old
Same/stronger<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001
Weaker 0.030
Flavor of food compared to when 30 to 35 years oldSame/stronger<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.001<0.0010.002
Weaker 0.005 0.012<0.001
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Peters, J.C.; Breen, J.A.; Pan, Z. Effects of Culinary Spices on Liking and Consumption of Protein Rich Foods in Community-Dwelling Older Adults. Nutrients 2023, 15, 1172. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15051172

AMA Style

Peters JC, Breen JA, Pan Z. Effects of Culinary Spices on Liking and Consumption of Protein Rich Foods in Community-Dwelling Older Adults. Nutrients. 2023; 15(5):1172. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15051172

Chicago/Turabian Style

Peters, John C., Jeanne Anne Breen, and Zhaoxing Pan. 2023. "Effects of Culinary Spices on Liking and Consumption of Protein Rich Foods in Community-Dwelling Older Adults" Nutrients 15, no. 5: 1172. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu15051172

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop