Next Article in Journal
Translation Can Distort the Linguistic Parameters of Source Texts Written in Inflected Language: Multidimensional Mathematical Analysis of “The Betrothed”, a Translation in English of “I Promessi Sposi” by A. Manzoni
Previous Article in Journal
Ricci–Yamabe Solitons on Sasakian Manifolds with the Generalized Tanaka–Webster Connection
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Role of Parenting Styles in Narcissism Development: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis

1
Escola Superior de Tecnologia e Gestão, Instituto Politécnico de Leiria, Campus 2, Morro do Lena-Alto do Vieiro, Apartado 4163, 2411-901 Leiria, Portugal
2
Escola Superior de Saúde, Instituto Politécnico do Porto, Rua Dr. António Bernardino de Almeida 400, 4200-072 Porto, Portugal
3
CEAUL—Centro de Estatística e Aplicações, Faculdade de Ciências, Universidade de Lisboa, 1749-016 Lisboa, Portugal
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
AppliedMath 2025, 5(1), 23; https://doi.org/10.3390/appliedmath5010023
Submission received: 28 December 2024 / Revised: 28 January 2025 / Accepted: 6 February 2025 / Published: 3 March 2025

Abstract

:
There has been considerable debate about whether contemporary Western societies are experiencing an increase in narcissistic tendencies, often referred to as a “narcissism epidemic”. This rise highlights the importance of understanding the origins of narcissism, particularly regarding its potential association with parenting styles. Such insights can inform treatment approaches and contribute to paradigm shifts in developmental psychology. This systematic review and meta-analysis examine how different parenting styles are associated with the development of narcissistic traits, using both partial and zero-order correlations as measures of effect. To ensure a consistent conceptualization of parenting styles, the results were evaluated using Baumrind’s parental styles typology. The review follows PRISMA guidelines and is registered in PROSPERO (CRD42024516395). Studies published in English or Portuguese since 2000 were sourced from PubMed (1039 articles) and Scopus (2120 articles), resulting in a final sample of 53 studies across 38 articles. Data synthesis included assessment of statistical heterogeneity ( I 2 statistic), publication bias (funnel plots, Egger’s test, and the trim and fill method), and methodological quality (adapted Newcastle–Ottawa Scale, NOS). Additionally, sensitivity analyses were conducted to evaluate the effect of excluding studies scoring below eight on the NOS by comparing results from analyses with all studies versus high-quality studies only. Results indicate a significant, albeit weak, association between parenting styles and narcissistic traits, with notable variations between maternal and paternal influences. This analysis provides a comprehensive perspective on the interplay between parenting approaches and the emergence of narcissistic characteristics, underscoring the complexity of factors that contribute to narcissism in contemporary society.

1. Introduction

In the 2000s, research on narcissism gained significant scientific and public attention, largely due to cultural shifts in the new millennium [1]. Jean Twenge’s works, such as “Generation Me” (2006) and “The Narcissism Epidemic” (2009), portrayed a pessimistic view of Western societies becoming increasingly self-focused and entitled, with a decline in adherence to social and moral norms. For instance, the endorsement rate for statements such as “I am an important person” rose from 12% in 1963 to 77–80% in 1992 [2]. This trend permeates various aspects of culture, including the lyrical content of contemporary songs and the thematic focus of popular television shows, which increasingly prioritize individual fame and self-promotion [2].
Jean Twenge and her team investigated the observed increase in narcissistic tendencies by examining generational changes. The study, carried out in 2008, analyzed 85 cohorts of participants who completed the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) scale between 1979 and 2006. The results revealed a 30% increase in narcissism levels among US university students during this period [3]. If this trajectory continues, as many scholars speculate, the path toward heightened narcissism appears inevitable [3].
Later studies have challenged the notion of a narcissism epidemic. For instance, Vater et al. in 2018 found no evidence to support this claim in their comparison of narcissism and self-esteem in East and West Germany [2].
Additionally, a recent cross-temporal meta-analysis of studies involving NPI scores from 1982 to 2023 provides evidence of the opposite trend. The analysis observed a negative cross-temporal change in narcissism, particularly since the start of the millennium. However, NPI scores vary significantly by region and age, with higher scores observed in the USA and among younger individuals [4].
The high levels of narcissism in younger age groups underscores the importance of understanding its origins, namely whether it is influenced by parenting education. Such understanding can inform treatment adaptations and potentially shift paradigms.
The main objective of this work is to verify whether there is a correlation between parental education and the development of narcissistic traits. Hence, this work includes a systematic review and meta-analysis. Section 2 provides a brief introduction to the topics addressed, covering the concept of narcissism, its types and scales, and the various forms of parenting considered. The subsequent section discusses the study methodology that follows Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines. Section 4 presents the results, followed by a discussion in Section 5 and a conclusion in Section 6.

2. Understanding Narcissism: From Personality Traits to Parenting Influences

This section presents a broad review of the literature on narcissistic personality disorder, with a focus on its two main forms: grandiose and vulnerable narcissism. In addition, the section examines the assorted scales used to assess narcissistic traits, as well as the connections between narcissism and parental education.

2.1. Narcissistic Personality Disorder

Speaking of narcissism almost automatically brings us to Narcissus, a figure from Greek mythology known for falling in love with his own reflection in a pond, which ultimately led to his death. The etymology of the word possibly derives from the Greek ν α ρ κ η (narke), meaning “sleep, numbness”. The concept was taken up and refined by psychodynamic theorists, who considered that narcissism functioned as a self-regulatory mechanism as well as a personality disposition (as Jauk and Kanske cited, [1]). In the third edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM), it was included for the first time as a personality disorder, being defined by the American Psychiatric Association as “a pervasive pattern of grandiosity (in fantasy or behavior), need for admiration, and lack of empathy, beginning by early adulthood and present in a variety of contexts” [5].
Individuals with narcissistic personality disorder are highly sensitive to criticism or defeat due to their fragile self-esteem. Even though they may not display it outwardly, criticism can affect them deeply, leaving them feeling humiliated, empty, and degraded. Their reactions may range from disdain and rage to defiant counterattacks. These experiences often result in social withdrawal or a façade of humility, concealing their underlying grandiosity. Their interpersonal relationships suffer significantly due to entitlement issues, a constant need for admiration, and a lack of consideration for others’ feelings [5].
Beneath their superficially smooth and socially adaptive behavior lies deep-seated dysfunction in their internal relations with others. They often oscillate between intense ambitions, grandiose fantasies, and feelings of inferiority, relying heavily on external validation to maintain their self-worth. Despite an outward display of confidence and success, they are plagued by chronic feelings of boredom, emptiness, and dissatisfaction with life. Their constant search for admiration and gratification stems from deep-rooted desires for brilliance, wealth, power, and beauty, often paired with an inability to genuinely love or empathize with others [6].
These individuals struggle with a lack of empathetic understanding, exhibiting exploitative and even ruthless behavior, driven by conscious or unconscious envy. Their persistent dissatisfaction, combined with their envy of others, can result in heightened defenses and further isolation. Although their ambition and confidence may lead to temporary successes, their inability to handle criticism and defeat often undermines their performance, leading to low vocational functioning, depression, and social withdrawal. Periods of grandiosity may be interspersed with hypomanic moods, adding to the instability of their emotional and professional lives [5,6].

2.1.1. Grandiose and Vulnerable Narcissism

As mentioned by Jauk and Kanske [1], emerging consensus suggests that narcissism is multifaceted, with distinct expressions. Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism are recognized as separate yet related manifestations, characterized by either self-assured dominance or self-conscious withdrawal. Despite differences, both entail feelings of self-importance and entitlement.
Narcissism is a multidimensional construct, with grandiose and vulnerable forms distinguished by persistent feelings of importance and grandiosity, alongside a desire for admiration and antagonistic traits. Although the term “narcissism” commonly evokes notions of exaggerated self-worth, superiority, entitlement, and arrogance, this definition closely aligns with the definition of grandiose narcissism. This personality trait encompasses entitlement, extroversion, socially dominant behavior, self-assurance, immodesty, exhibitionism, manipulation, and aggression. While vulnerable narcissism is associated with distrustful, hostile interpersonal styles driven by negative emotionality and problematic attachment that tends towards depressive symptoms and social withdrawal, with less emphasis on grandiose fantasies. Pathological grandiose or vulnerable narcissism may be diagnosed when these traits are pronounced. Both overlap in their use of antagonistic interpersonal strategies but differ in specific traits and behavioral tendencies [1,7,8,9].

2.1.2. Narcissism Scales

With the advancement of studies on narcissism, methods for assessing personality also emerged. The first known assessments were developed by Raskin and Hall, consisting of versions with 80 and 54 items, respectively. The shorter version, Narcissistic Personality Inventory-40 (NPI-40), was subjected to three different studies by the same authors in 1988, and it is the version that is most used and examined in many studies to date [10]. However, short versions were perceived after NPI-40, such as NPI-16 and NPI-34.
The NPI-40 is composed of three subscales, each capturing different facets of narcissism:
  • Entitlement/Exploitativeness: This subscale is often considered the most indicative of narcissistic personality pathology. It is related to lower self-esteem and extraversion, higher mood variability, and neuroticism. Additionally, it is associated with both grandiose and vulnerable narcissism, as well as narcissistic personality disorder [11].
  • Leadership/Authority: This subscale is a more specific marker of grandiose narcissism, associated with higher self-esteem, extraversion, and lower neuroticism. It indicates a tendency to seek and enjoy positions of leadership and authority [11].
  • Grandiose Exhibitionism: Like the Leadership/Authority subscale, this is also a marker of grandiose narcissism. It is associated with higher self-esteem, extraversion, and lower neuroticism. It reflects the need to be the center of attention and to receive admiration from others [11].
Several assessments are commonly used to measure narcissistic personality traits in psychological research. Alongside the widely recognized NPI-40, the Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI), developed by Pincus et al. in 2009, is a 52-item self-report measure that assesses both vulnerable and grandiose narcissism traits. The PNI is divided into four subscales for vulnerable narcissism (Contingent Self-Esteem, Hiding the Self, Devaluing, and Entitlement Rage) and three subscales for grandiose narcissism (Self-Sacrificing Self-Enhancement, Grandiose Fantasies, and Exploitativeness) [11].
The Hypersensitive Narcissism Scale (HSNS), created by Hendin and Cheek in 1997, is a 10-item self-report measure specifically designed to assess vulnerable narcissism [11].
The California Adult Q-Sort (CAQ) is a set of statements used in observer and self-report assessments of personality. It comprises 100 items that have been utilized to assess various personality traits, including narcissism. The CAQ-13 is a measure of narcissism-based CAQ, which consists of 13 items selected to represent it. These items were identified by experts and subjected to factor analysis, resulting in three subscales: Grandiose, Vulnerable, and Autonomy [12,13].
The Dark Triad Dirty Dozen Scale (DTDD) is a 12-item measure used to assess the Dark Triad traits: narcissism, machiavellianism, and psychopathy. Each of these three dimensions is evaluated with four specific items [14,15,16].
The DSM-IV Assessment of Personality Disorders Questionnaire (ADP-IV) is a self-report instrument comprising 94 items, representing the 80 criteria of the 10 DSM-IV personality disorders and the 14 research criteria of the depressive and passive-aggressive personality disorders [17].
The Five-Factor Narcissism Inventory – Short Form (FFNI-SF) is a 60-item self-report questionnaire designed to assess narcissism through the lens of the five-factor model (FFM) of personality. The FFNI-SF assesses both vulnerable and grandiose narcissism across 15 subscales, each representing a maladaptive variant of an FFM trait. Additionally, it considers three dimensions derived from factor analysis: antagonism, neuroticism, and agentic extraversion [18].
The Narcissistic Personality Questionnaire (NPQ) developed by Zhou et al. in 2009 is a 34-item self-report instrument that assesses three dimensions: desire for power, sense of superiority, and self-appreciation [19].
The Personality Diagnostic Questionnaire – 4th Edition Plus (PDQ-4+) is the most recent version of the PDQ. Each version corresponds to the different editions of the DSM since 1980. The PDQ-4+ consists of 99-item true/false questions and assesses ten DSM-IV-TR personality disorders and two provisional personality disorders [20,21,22].
The Short Dark Triad (SD3) and the Short Dark Tetrad (SD4) are 27-item and 28-item, respectively, self-report questionnaires designed to measure individuals’ dark personality traits. The SD4 addresses all four dark personality traits (subclinical narcissism, machiavellianism, subclinical psychopathy, and sadism), whereas the SD3 focuses on only the first three [23,24,25,26].
The Single Item Narcissism Scale (SINS) is a one-item measure that assesses grandiose and vulnerable aspects of non-clinical narcissism. Participants respond on a seven-point scale (1 = “Not very true of me” to 7 = “Very true of me”): “To what extent do you agree with this statement: I am a narcissist. (Note: The word “narcissist” means egotistical, self-focused, and vain.)” [27,28].
The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Personality Disorders (SCID) is a widely used semi-structured interview designed to diagnose personality disorders according to DSM-IV criteria. It includes 94 main yes/no questions that address enduring patterns of inner experience and behavior deviating from cultural expectations, affecting cognition, affectivity, interpersonal functioning, and impulse control [29,30].
The Young Schema Questionnaire – Short Form (YSQ-SF) is a 75-item adaptation of the original 205-item Young Schema Questionnaire. The short form includes five items from each of the 15 original scales, selected based on their strong factor loadings. The 15 subscales assess various schemas, such as abandonment, mistrust/abuse, and emotional deprivation. In the study under analysis, narcissism was specifically assessed using the five-item grandiosity subscale from the YSQ-SF, which measures beliefs of superiority and entitlement to special treatment [31,32].
Until recently, studies on childhood narcissism were scarce. Initial applications of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI) in children revealed it to be unsuitable due to its length (40 items), complex questions not aligned with children’s realities, and poor internal consistency. In response, Ang and Yusof in 2006 developed the Narcissistic Personality Questionnaire for Children (NPQC), a scale with 18 items and four factors, tailored for use with children and adolescents in nonclinical populations [33]. Later, Ang and Raine refined this to the NPQC-R (Narcissistic Personality Questionnaire for Children – Revised), a twelve-item scale with two factors: superiority (six items) and exploitativeness (six items) [34,35].
Additionally, the Childhood Narcissism Scale (CNS) is a unidimensional measure consisting of 10 items designed by Thomas et al. in 2008 to assess narcissistic traits in children [36]. This scale evaluates the degree to which children endorse grandiose and entitled self-perceptions [37,38,39]. These tools address the challenges associated with earlier measures in Children such as the NPI [40].

2.2. Narcissism and Parental Education

Parenting education is widely recognized as an influential factor in the development of a child’s personality traits. As mentioned by Imamoglu and Batigun [41], and Kernberg [6], overly permissive, intrusive, cold, or strict parenting styles—particularly when parents appear functional but are emotionally indifferent or subtly aggressive—can play a significant role in the development of narcissistic traits, including pathological narcissism. Similarly, Young et al. [42] propose that childhood experiences such as loneliness, poor boundaries, manipulation, and conditional approval contribute to the development of a narcissistic personality, often resulting from a lack of genuine love and empathy in these individuals during their early years.
A systematic review revealed that both grandiose and vulnerable narcissism are negatively associated with parental warmth in both genders. Additionally, grandiose narcissism showed a positive relationship with parental overvaluation, while vulnerable narcissism was positively related to parental abuse [43].
Heinz Kohut’s self-psychology model posits that narcissistic psychopathology results from a lack of parental empathy during development, leading to an impaired ability to regulate self-esteem in adulthood. This perspective emphasizes the role of early caregiver interactions in the formation of narcissistic traits [44].
Similarly, Otto Kernberg’s object relations theory suggests that pathological narcissism arises from internalized object relations and serves as a defense mechanism against hostile and neglectful early relationships. This framework highlights the significance of early relational experiences in the development of narcissistic personality structures [45].
These perspectives underscore the complex interplay between parenting styles and the development of grandiose and vulnerable narcissistic traits. They also suggest a potential bidirectional influence, where children’s temperaments and behaviors might shape parental responses, further influencing narcissistic outcomes.
This investigation is based on Baumrind’s four parenting styles: authoritarian, authoritative, permissive, and neglectful [46].
Authoritarian parenting is characterized by shaping and controlling the child’s behavior according to strict standards, emphasizing obedience without explanation, and often relying on punitive measures to enforce compliance [12]. This approach can restrain the child’s development of personal competence, as it discourages the recognition and expression of their thoughts and feelings, leading to a reliance on external approval to maintain a constructed self-image [47,48]. Children raised in authoritarian households may exhibit higher levels of aggression, struggle with decision-making, and have poor self-esteem due to the lack of nurturing and flexibility from their parents. Additionally, strict rules can lead to rebellion or an inflated sense of self-importance if the child is praised for compliance. However, this sense of specialness is fragile, as it depends on the continued admiration of others [12,49].
In contrast, authoritative parenting sets clear behavioral standards but uses reasoning and explanation to guide the child, balancing assertiveness with respect for the child’s perspective and rights. Discipline in this style is more supportive than punitive [12]. This approach is responsive to the child’s needs, fostering autonomy, competent skill-building, and self-regulation [50,51]. By maintaining clear expectations and simultaneously respecting the child’s individuality, authoritative parenting promotes a strong sense of self-confidence and self-esteem, reducing the likelihood of narcissistic traits developing [12].
Permissive parenting is characterized by leniency and affection, often struggling to enforce discipline and not requiring the child to display mature behavior [12]. While this approach meets the child’s needs and affirms their worth, it can foster impulsivity, selfishness, and a lack of self-regulation. Although children raised by permissive parents may develop some level of self-esteem and social skills, they often become demanding and expect their needs to be met without effort. This lack of boundaries may result in compensatory behaviors, where the child inflates their self-image and ignores others who do not satisfy their desires, behaviors commonly associated with narcissism [49,50,52].
On the other hand, neglectful parenting expects the child to manage problems independently, offers little support, and encourages the child to take responsibility for their own life, often neglecting to provide guidance or assistance [12]. Children may develop resilience and self-sufficiency out of necessity, rather than through positive development. This lack of involvement can leave the child feeling incompetent and vulnerable, as they do not receive the support needed to build essential skills and emotional regulation. As a result, they often struggle with controlling their emotions, coping effectively, and facing difficulties in maintaining and nurturing social relationships. Eventually, the child’s ability to develop a strong sense of self is compromised, leading to further challenges in navigating setbacks [49,52].
The different parenting styles can significantly influence the development of narcissistic traits in children. Understanding these dynamics is essential for both researchers and practitioners aiming to address the root causes of narcissism and to promote healthier personality development.

3. Methodology

This systematic review and meta-analysis was performed following the PRISMA guidelines. The study was registered in PROSPERO (an international prospective register for systematic review protocols) under the registration CRD42024516395 (https://www.crd.york.ac.uk/prospero/display_record.php?ID=CRD42024516395, accessed on 2 December 2024), and included a prespecified protocol.
To ensure the integrity of the data extraction process, a systematic approach was employed. Two independent reviewers screened the eligibility of studies based on their titles and abstracts. Articles passing this initial screening underwent full-text review. Extracted data included title, authors, publication year, narcissism scale, information for effect measure computation, and risk of bias assessment.
In line with the systematic selection process, specific inclusion criteria were applied to ensure consistency across the studies analyzed. Eligible studies needed to feature participants who had completed a validated narcissism scale, thus guaranteeing reliable measurement of narcissistic traits. Participants were required to be at least six years old, a threshold informed by developmental psychology theories, such as those proposed by Jean Piaget and Erik Erikson, which suggest that personality traits and self-concept begin to emerge more distinctly around this age. Additionally, studies had to report at least one relevant outcome measure, be published in English or Portuguese, and have a publication date no earlier than 2000, ensuring the inclusion of contemporary research.
A first search for articles was carried out on PubMed and Scopus on 2 January 2024, using Boolean operators and the following search terms: (narcis*) AND (cognitive OR parent* OR educat*).
After screening the titles and abstracts, 2596 articles were excluded. Out of eighty-nine qualified studies, sixteen did not provide quantitative data, thirty-one were outside of scope, the full version of the article could not be retrieved for three, and one was not written in English or Portuguese. A total of 38 studies examining the relationship between parental education and narcissism were included in the final analysis (cf. Figure 1).
Two researchers independently assessed each study, with any discrepancies resolved by a third researcher. In practice, during the initial screening of 2688 titles and abstracts, there were fewer than 10 disagreements between the two reviewers regarding which articles should be reviewed in full, with decisions made in favor of evaluating the full text in cases of uncertainty. No disagreements occurred during the full-text review phase.
Table A1 (Appendix A) summarizes all the studies included in the final analysis [12,14,15,16,18,19,20,21,23,24,27,29,37,38,39,41,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66,67,68,69,70,71,72,73,74]. The studies addressing parental education analyze various characteristics such as overprotection, rejection, and corporal punishment, among others. Consequently, these characteristics were associated with established parenting styles—authoritative, authoritarian, neglectful, and permissive, as detailed in Section 2.2. In situations where multiple characteristics were identified for a single parental style, a 90% confidence interval for the correlation was calculated. If there was an overlap within the confidence intervals, an average was taken; otherwise, the characteristics were excluded.
Additionally, specific terminology was employed to clarify the study. Therefore, the term “parenting” and “parents” refers to the influence of both parents, while “maternal” or “mother” corresponds to the mother’s role, and “paternal” or “father” relates to the father’s influence. The term “overall” was used when discussing narcissism without distinguishing between its subtypes, while “grandiose” and “vulnerable” specifically denote grandiose and vulnerable narcissism, respectively.

3.1. Statistical Analysis

In this meta-analysis, the effect measures considered were partial and zero-order correlations. Partial correlations were preferred over zero-order correlations, as they measure the strength of a relationship between two variables while controlling for the effect of one or more other variables, thereby providing a more accurate representation of the relationships under investigation.
For data synthesis, statistical heterogeneity was assessed using the I 2 statistic, which estimates the fraction of variance that is due to heterogeneity, i.e.,
τ 2 τ 2 + σ 2 ,
where σ 2 represents the average within-study variance, and τ 2 denotes the between-study variance. A value between 25% and 75% indicates moderate heterogeneity and above 75% indicates high heterogeneity. The models were selected based on the I 2 statistic, with a fixed-effects model being used if the value was less than 25%, and a random-effects model being used otherwise [75,76,77]. The fixed-effects model assumes that all studies share a common true effect size ( θ ) and that observed differences are only due to sampling error. An estimator of the common true effect θ is given by
θ ^ = i = 1 k w i θ i ^ i = 1 k w i ,
where θ i ^ is the observed effect size in study i, and w i is the inverse of the variance of the study i, i.e.,
w i = 1 σ i 2 , i = 1 , , k .
In the random-effects model, the weights are the inverse of the sum of the between-study variance τ 2 and the within-study variance σ i 2 , i.e.,
w i = 1 τ 2 + σ i 2 .
In this setting, θ ^ is treated as a random variable that follows a normal distribution with mean θ and variance τ 2 , i.e., θ ^ N ( θ , τ 2 ) . In both models, θ i ^ is assumed to follow a normal distribution. Data analysis was conducted using R statistical software and metacor package [78,79].
Results were presented using forest plots. When a forest plot includes six or more studies, results are grouped by the region of the world where the studies were conducted. The regions considered are based on the Human Development Report 2023-24 (United Nations Development Programme) and include countries from the Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD), as well as developing regions, namely Arab countries, Europe and Central Asia, and South Asia [80]. When at least ten studies were obtained to compute an overall correlation, a meta-regression was performed to evaluate the effect of the mean age of participants on the observed correlation value. Bubble plots were used to provide a graphical representation of this potential association.
During the analysis, confidence intervals (CI) were reported at the 95% level, and hypothesis tests were conducted with a significance level of 5%. The comparison of subgroups was based on the Cochran’s Q test. Publication bias, if present, was assessed using funnel plots, Egger’s test, and the trim and fill method [81,82,83].
It is noteworthy that a minimum criterion has been set for the analysis, as it is necessary to have a minimum number of studies that provide a sufficient basis for reliable statistical analysis and meaningful conclusions. Therefore, in cases where fewer than three studies were available, the analysis was not carried out [75].

3.2. Quality Analysis

The included studies were evaluated for risk of bias using established critical appraisal tools. Hence, the Newcastle–Ottawa Scale (NOS) was used to evaluate methodological quality. A NOS adapted for cross-sectional studies was modified to accommodate the particular circumstances under examination in this analysis. The employed scale and its respective adaptation is detailed in Appendix B, while the individual study scores are presented in Table A1. Typically, the total NOS score ranges from 0 to 9, with higher scores indicating higher study quality. Studies scoring 7–9 are generally considered high-quality (low risk of bias), those scoring 4–6 are moderate-quality (higher risk of bias), and scores of 0–3 indicate low quality (very high risk of bias) [84,85,86]. In our adaptation for Parental Education Studies, the maximum possible score is 10. Hence, using similar thresholds to those set in the original scale, studies scoring at least 8 out of 10 points were considered to be of high quality.
It should be noted that in the NOS evaluation, the issue of non-respondents was particularly noteworthy, as many studies failed to report non-response rates. When rates were provided, they were often quite high. Another critical limitation is the lack of sample representativeness, with some studies providing no description of their sampling strategy. The study with the lowest score (7 points, the only one below the 8-point threshold) revealed some weaknesses, including an unjustifiably high non-response rate (50%) and a failure to control for confounding factors.
To assess the sensitivity of the conclusions, in cases that included studies scored less than eight on the NOS, two analyses were performed—one including all studies regardless of NOS score and another including only those scoring eight or higher—and the results were compared accordingly.

4. Results

This section provides an overview of the findings, organized according to parental education. The statistical analysis and significant patterns observed during the study are presented in detail, forming the basis for the discussion and conclusions that will be presented in the following sections.
A total of eleven, thirteen, nine, and six studies were found relating overall narcissism to authoritative, authoritarian, neglectful, and permissive parenting styles, respectively. Figure 2, Figure 3, Figure 4 and Figure 5 present forest plots summarizing the findings. In all cases, the results revealed significant heterogeneity for the authoritative, authoritarian, and negligent education (authoritative: Q(6530) = 85.37, p < 0.0001; authoritarian: Q(6987) = 73.60, p < 0.0001; negligent: Q(3978) = 82.64, p < 0.0001). Results also showed that the magnitudes of heterogeneity were large for these three parenting styles (authoritative: I 2 = 88.3%; authoritarian: I 2 = 83.7%; negligent: I 2 = 90.3%). Only results for the permissive style showed moderate heterogeneity (Q(2319) = 7.28, p = 0.2005, I 2 = 31.3%).
Given this heterogeneity in all instances, the overall estimates were calculated using a random-effects model. The highest correlations were observed for the permissive (0.15, CI 95% [0.10; 0.21]) and authoritarian (0.15, CI 95% [0.09; 0.21]) styles, followed by neglectful (0.10, CI 95% [0.01; 0.19]) and authoritative (0.08, CI 95% [0.00; 0.15]) styles.
Upon examination of the forest plots, it becomes evident that the confidence intervals intersect, thereby rendering it plausible to suggest that the correlation values are equal. Concerning all parenting styles, a significant, albeit weak, correlation with narcissism is observed, with an estimated range between 0.08 and 0.15.
For all four parenting styles, a significant difference between regions was observed only for the authoritarian style. Studies conducted in South Asia showed a higher overall correlation, while studies conducted in Europe and Central Asia demonstrated a lower overall correlation.
Funnel plots, the trim and fill method, and Egger’s test for funnel plot asymmetry were employed to investigate the potential for publication bias. As illustrated in Appendix C (Figure A1Figure A4), the funnel plots for permissive and authoritarian parenting styles exhibit symmetrical patterns. However, the plots for authoritative and neglectful styles reveal potential missing studies, with two and four studies respectively.
Despite these observations, the p-values from the Egger tests for funnel plot asymmetry are substantially higher than 0.05 (authoritative: p = 0.5533; authoritarian: p = 0.7727; negligent: p = 0.2736; permissive: p = 0.9582), indicating that the results of the meta-analyses are not significantly affected by publication bias.
In the analysis examining the relationship between authoritarian and permissive parenting and overall narcissism, a study was identified with a NOS score below eight. Therefore, a second analysis was performed, excluding this lower-quality study, to assess the robustness of the findings (cf. Figure 6 and Figure 7). In this secondary analysis (authoritarian: 0.15, CI 95% [0.0919, 0.2102]; permissive: 0.15, CI 95% [0.0853, 0.2082]), the conclusions remained consistent with the primary analysis, indicating that the lower-quality study did not significantly influence the overall results. This reinforces the reliability of the conclusions drawn from the full dataset, as the observed results were not contingent on the exclusion of studies with potential methodological limitations.

4.1. Narcissism and Authoritative Education

Focused on maternal and paternal education, a total of 11 studies were identified for each, examining the correlation between the authoritative parenting style and overall narcissism. Figure 8 and Figure 9 present forest plots summarizing the findings. A significant heterogeneity was evident for both maternal and paternal education (mother: Q(4670) = 64.41, p < 0.0001, I 2 = 84.5%; father: Q(4670) = 580.97, p < 0.0001, I 2 = 98.3%).
Consequently, a random-effects model was used to analyze the correlation. Observing the forest plots, the confidence intervals for the correlations between overall narcissism and both maternal and paternal authoritative parenting overlap with zero, suggesting that these correlations are not statistically significant (mother: 0.01, 95% CI [−0.0764; 0.1061]; father: −0.05, 95% CI [−0.2322; 0.1415]).
The two studies conducted in Europe and Central Asia show an overall positive correlation between narcissism and both the mother’s and the father’s authoritative style, whereas the single study conducted in South Asia shows a negative correlation for both relationships.
Although funnel plots identify two missing studies for both maternal and paternal influences (Figure A5 and Figure A6 in Appendix C), the p-values from the regression tests for funnel plot asymmetry remain well above the 0.05 threshold (maternal: p = 0.3003; paternal: p = 0.9498). This suggests that publication bias is not significantly affecting the results of these meta-analyses.
Regarding grandiose narcissism, five studies each were identified examining the correlation between maternal and paternal authoritative parenting (cf. Figure 10 and Figure 11). Upon further analysis, significant heterogeneity was found in these studies. Specifically, the studies focusing on maternal authoritative parenting reveal high heterogeneity (Q(761) = 31.85, p < 0.0001, I 2 = 87.4%), whereas studies focusing on paternal authoritative parenting show moderate heterogeneity (Q(761) = 9.32, p < 0.0001, I 2 = 57.1%).
In the context of vulnerable narcissism, three studies examined overall authoritative parenting, while five studies each focused on maternal and paternal authoritative parenting. Forest plots summarizing the findings are shown in Figure 12, Figure 13 and Figure 14. Once again, the heterogeneity remains significant for both maternal (Q(1076) = 25.04, p < 0.0001, I 2 = 84%) and overall authoritative parenting (Q(1076) = 11.20, p = 0.0037, I 2 = 82.1%), while paternal authoritative parenting shows moderate heterogeneity (Q(1076) = 6.64, p = 0.1564, I 2 = 39.7%).
A closer examination of these specific types of narcissism reveals different patterns in the data. It is noteworthy that while maternal authoritative parenting shows no significant correlation with grandiose narcissism, paternal authoritative parenting exhibits a negative correlation, suggesting the potential for an inverse relationship (mother: −0.06, CI 95% [−0.2510; 0.1329]; father: −0.15, CI 95% [−0.2603; −0.0434]).
For vulnerable narcissism, although the intervals indicate that there is no statistically significant correlation with authoritative parenting (−0.11, CI 95% [−0.2529; 0.0360]), when looking for maternal and paternal influences (mother: −0.28, CI 95% [−0.4046; −0.1521]; father: −0.21, CI 95% [−0.2931; −0.1316]), the negative correlations observed imply that authoritative parenting may be inversely related to vulnerable narcissism.
Funnel plots reveal symmetrical patterns for the analyses of both grandiose (mother and father) and vulnerable narcissism (overall parenting) (cf. Figure A7Figure A9 in Appendix C). However, for maternal and paternal influences on vulnerable narcissism, the plots indicate two missing studies each (cf. Figure A10 and Figure A11 in Appendix C). Despite these findings, there is no evidence of publication bias in the analyses according to the p-values from the Egger tests for funnel plot asymmetry, which were well above 0.05 (grandiose narcissism: mother: p = 0.724, father: p = 0.9242; vulnerable narcissism: parenting: p = 0.4534, mother: p = 0.4423, father: p = 0.7393), suggesting that the meta-analysis results are not significantly affected by publication bias.

4.2. Narcissism and Authoritarian Education

A total of 11 studies were used to examine the relationship between overall narcissism and both authoritarian maternal and paternal parenting styles. Furthermore, five studies each investigated the correlation between vulnerable narcissism and authoritarian parenting styles, including maternal and paternal influences. Figure 15, Figure 16, Figure 17, Figure 18 and Figure 19 present a summary of the findings in the form of forest plots.
Despite the observed heterogeneity in studies analyzing the correlation between authoritarian parenting style and overall narcissism, this heterogeneity is revealed to be not significant when focusing on maternal and paternal education (mother: Q(4779) = 36.18, p < 0.0001; father: Q(4779) = 37.85, p < 0.0001) and in relation to vulnerable narcissism (parents: Q(2263) = 10.61, p = 0.0313; mother: Q(1363) = 3.79, p = 0.4354; father: Q(1363) = 3.93, p = 0.4150).
Results also showed that the magnitudes of heterogeneity were not large when considering overall narcissism and mother education ( I 2 = 72.4%) or father education ( I 2 = 73.6%), and when considering only vulnerable narcissism (parents: I 2 = 62.3%; mother: I 2 = 0%; father: I 2 = 0%). Therefore, a random-effects model was used to analyze the correlation between overall narcissism and mother and father education, as well as vulnerable narcissism and overall parenting. For the correlation between vulnerable narcissism and both mother and father influences, a fixed-effects (common-effects) model was used.
The forest plot analysis clearly indicates a positive correlation between authoritarian parenting style and narcissism, with no difference between maternal and paternal education (mother: 0.08, CI 95% [0.0271; 0.1352]; father: 0.07, CI 95% [0.0107; 0.1247]) (cf. Figure 15 and Figure 16).
The two studies conducted in Europe and Central Asia did not show a significant correlation between narcissism and either the mother’s or the father’s authoritarian style, whereas an overall significant positive correlation was observed in OECD countries.
When comparing overall narcissism with vulnerable narcissism, it is observed that the I 2 value decreases, indicating greater homogeneity of the data that point us to an even stronger correlation (0.22, CI 95% [0.1524; 0.2800]), again with no difference between maternal and paternal education (mother: 0.26, CI 95% [0.2070; 0.3062]; father: 0.26, CI 95% [0.2079; 0.3070]) (cf. Figure 17, Figure 18 and Figure 19).
Although the funnel plots identify missing studies for the analyses of maternal and paternal influences on overall narcissism, and for the analyses of overall parenting with vulnerable narcissism (cf. Appendix C, Figure A12Figure A16), there is no evidence of publication bias with observed regression test p-values substantially higher than 0.05 (mother: p = 0.8892; father: p = 0.6535) for vulnerable narcissism (parents: p = 0.5602; mother: p = 0.8658; father: p = 0.8546).
A meta-regression was conducted for both authoritative and authoritarian parenting styles (see Appendix D, Figure A22Figure A27). For the authoritarian style, the correlation significantly increases with age ( p < 0.001 ). However, this association is not significant for the father’s authoritarian style (p = 0.313) but is significant for the mother’s authoritarian style (p = 0.011). No significant association was found for the authoritative style (parents: p = 0.3919; mother: p = 0.2107; father: p = 0.2039).

4.3. Narcissism and Neglectful Education

In the context of the neglectful parenting style, a total of five studies investigated the relationship between maternal education and overall narcissism, while six studies focused on paternal influences. Additionally, three studies examined the correlation between neglectful parenting and vulnerable narcissism. Forest plots summarizing these findings are presented in Figure 20, Figure 21 and Figure 22.
A more granular analysis distinguishing between maternal and paternal influences reveals persistent heterogeneity in studies focusing on maternal influences. Conversely, studies examining paternal influences display greater homogeneity (mother: Q(1193) = 21.03, p = 0.0003, I 2 = 81%; father: Q(5365) = 12.47, p = 0.0289, I 2 = 59.9%), with no evidence of publication bias (mother: p = 0.4241; father: p = 0.9341). The funnel plots summarizing these findings are presented in Figure A17 and Figure A18 in Appendix C.
Analyzing the correlations, it is evident that for overall narcissism, the confidence intervals of the three estimates—overall parenting, maternal parenting, and paternal parenting—overlap, and the correlation with maternal education crosses zero, indicating a lack of significant correlation (0.05, CI 95% [−0.0901; 0.1980]). In contrast, a significant correlation is observed with paternal education (0.17, CI 95% [0.0910; 0.2400]), as Figure 20 and Figure 21 reveal.
When comparing the studies of negligent parenting style with overall narcissism and with vulnerable narcissism, a notable reduction in the I 2 value is observed (vulnerable narcissism: I 2 = 0%), indicating increased data homogeneity (cf. Figure 22).
The comparison between the correlations of the negligent parenting style with overall narcissism and vulnerable narcissism reveals that the relationship persists, with the correlation for vulnerable narcissism (0.17, CI 95% [0.11; 0.24]) having a higher estimate than that observed with overall narcissism (0.10, CI 95% [0.01; 0.19]). This raises the question of whether the negligent parenting style may not significantly influence grandiose narcissism, which could explain the high heterogeneity and weak correlations observed with overall narcissism.
Similar to the findings for overall narcissism, the funnel plots for vulnerable narcissism, as shown in Figure A19, also identify missing studies. However, the p-value from the Egger’s tests for funnel plot asymmetry is above the 0.05 threshold, indicating no significant evidence of publication bias. This suggests that the correlation between negligent parenting and vulnerable narcissism is stronger than that with overall narcissism.

4.4. Narcissism and Permissive Education

A total of three studies were identified examining the correlation between permissive parenting styles, both maternal and paternal, and overall narcissism. In contrast to other parenting styles, when the analysis of the permissive parenting style is narrowed down to distinguish between maternal and paternal influences, an increase in the I 2 value is observed, indicating greater heterogeneity in studies focusing on paternal influences (mother: Q(752) = 4.57, p = 0.1016, I 2 = 56.3%; father: Q(725) = 16.14, p = 0.0003, I 2 = 87.6%). Due to the high heterogeneity, a random-effects model was applied to better estimate the effect sizes.
Upon examining the correlations, it becomes evident that there is no significant correlation between permissive parenting, whether maternal or paternal, and narcissism (mother: −0.08, CI 95% [−0.2145; 0.0533]; father: −0.04, CI 95% [−0.2764; 0.1886]), as Figure 23 and Figure 24 reveal.
Additionally, there is no evidence of publication bias. Although the funnel plots identified missing studies (cf. Appendix C, Figure A20 and Figure A21), the p-value from Egger’s test for funnel plot asymmetry is above the 0.05 threshold (mother: p = 0.57; father: p = 0.2734), suggesting no significant bias. The results observed in the funnel plots may also be attributed to the small number of studies analyzed, specifically three.

5. Discussion

The results of this meta-analysis provide nuanced insights into the relationship between various parenting styles and the development of narcissistic traits, highlighting both the complexity of these relationships and the differential impacts of maternal and paternal influences.
Although the analysis reveals a significant correlation between overall narcissism and all four parenting styles, these correlations are weak, with estimates ranging between 0.08 and 0.15. At a preliminary analysis of the results between overall narcissism and each one of the parental styles, the overlapping confidence intervals observed in the forest plots indicate that the correlation values among the different parenting styles are comparable, thereby emphasizing the modest influence that parenting styles exert on the development of narcissistic traits [87,88,89].
A previous study revealed a correlation between positive parental practices and grandiose narcissism, while the vulnerable form is associated with negative parenting [53].
A closer examination of authoritative parenting reveals a more complex picture. The confidence intervals for the correlations between overall narcissism and both maternal and paternal authoritative parenting indicate no statistically significant association, suggesting that authoritative parenting, often characterized by a balanced approach of warmth and control, may not have a direct or strong influence on the emergence of narcissistic traits in children. However, when considering specific types of narcissism, such as grandiose and vulnerable narcissism, distinct patterns emerge. Notably, paternal authoritative parenting exhibits a negative correlation with grandiose narcissism, hinting at a potential inverse relationship. In contrast, maternal authoritative parenting shows no significant correlation, which may indicate that fathers may play a unique role in mitigating grandiose narcissistic tendencies through authoritative parenting.
Similarly, for vulnerable narcissism, although the overall correlations with authoritative parenting are not statistically significant, both maternal and paternal authoritative parenting exhibit negative correlations. These findings imply that authoritative parenting, particularly from mothers, may be inversely related to the development of vulnerable narcissism, suggesting a potential protective effect against this subtype of narcissism.
In contrast, the analysis of authoritarian parenting consistently reveals a significant and positive correlation with narcissism. This relationship persists regardless of whether the influence is maternal or paternal, with similar correlation estimates observed. The authoritarian parenting style, characterized by high demandingness and low responsiveness, may also contribute to narcissistic traits. Children raised in such environments might develop narcissistic behaviors as a coping mechanism to gain attention and approval from their parents [6,90]. Furthermore, the correlation between authoritarian parenting and vulnerable narcissism appears even higher, which reinforces the notion that an authoritarian parenting style, characterized by strict discipline and low warmth, is more strongly associated with the development of narcissistic traits, particularly vulnerable narcissism [6,87,91].
The examination of negligent parenting further supports the persistence of the relationship between this style and narcissism, particularly vulnerable narcissism. Neglectful parenting, marked by low responsiveness and low demandingness, can lead to children feeling undervalued and neglected. This lack of attention and care may result in children developing narcissistic traits as a way to compensate for the emotional void left by their parents. The higher correlation estimates for vulnerable narcissism compared to overall narcissism suggest that a neglectful parenting approach may have a more pronounced effect on the development of vulnerable narcissistic traits, potentially due to the lack of emotional support and guidance [88,89].
In contrast, the analysis of permissive parenting reveals no significant correlation with narcissism, whether maternal or paternal. The confidence intervals for maternal and paternal permissive parenting indicate the absence of a meaningful association, which suggests that this style may not significantly contribute to the development of narcissistic traits in children.
Research indicates that permissive parenting, characterized by high responsiveness and low demandingness, can contribute to the development of narcissistic traits in children. A systematic review performed by Longobardi in 2016 found that permissive parenting is a significant predictor of grandiose narcissistic immaturity [90]. This parenting style often involves a lack of structure and boundaries, leading to an inflated sense of self-importance in children. Additionally, a study showed that children raised by permissive parents exhibited more adaptive narcissistic tendencies, such as superiority and grandiosity [92].
This meta-analysis, while providing valuable insights, was subject to some limitations that need to be acknowledged. One of the primary challenges was the heterogeneity observed across studies, coupled with the lack of sufficient data on grandiose and vulnerable narcissism, which highlights the limitations of the current evidence.
The perception of styles across different cultures may account for the significant heterogeneity observed in certain situations, as well as the use of varying scales to measure narcissism.
It would also be valuable to conduct more studies examining maternal and paternal influences to better understand the distinct roles each parent may play in the development of narcissistic traits, as well as the presence of narcissistic traits in the parents themselves.
It should also be noted that most of the studies analyzed in this meta-analysis do not provide information on factors that might influence narcissistic individuals’ reactions to criticism or failure, nor on other variables that could explain variations in behavior among individuals with similar narcissistic traits.
The way individuals perceive their parents’ behavior significantly influences their self-concept. By addressing the root causes of inflated self-esteem—often shaped by perceived parenting—therapists can help individuals develop a more stable and realistic sense of self. This process fosters healthier self-perceptions and improves interpersonal relationships. Therefore, focusing on self-concept within the framework of perceived parenting may be a critical strategy for reducing narcissistic traits and promoting emotional well-being.
In therapeutic interventions for patients with narcissism, it is essential to address key issues such as the impact of an evaluative environment. Even overly positive evaluations can reinforce the false self. Therapists should avoid evaluative attitudes and prioritize fostering authenticity and self-awareness to support lasting emotional growth [43,90].

6. Conclusions

In summary, this meta-analysis reveals a significant, though weak, correlation between parenting styles and narcissistic traits, with notable differences observed between maternal and paternal influences.
Authoritative parenting shows no significant relationship with overall narcissism, but paternal authoritative parenting is negatively correlated with grandiose narcissism, suggesting fathers may help mitigate these traits. In contrast, both maternal and paternal authoritative parenting exhibit negative correlations with vulnerable narcissism, indicating a potential protective effect. Authoritarian and neglectful parenting are more strongly associated with vulnerable narcissism, reinforcing the detrimental impact of low warmth and emotional neglect. Finally, permissive parenting shows no significant correlation with narcissism, indicating that a lack of discipline does not contribute notably to the development of narcissistic traits.
Despite limitations such as heterogeneity and insufficient data, the findings provide valuable insights into the relationship between parenting styles and narcissistic traits. They also offer guidance for future theoretical models and clinical interventions, suggesting that targeting self-concept in individuals with narcissistic traits could be a particularly effective therapeutic approach, especially when accounting for the role of perceived parenting.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.P.M. and R.S.; methodology, J.P.M. and R.S.; software, A.d.R.; validation, A.d.R., J.P.M., and R.S.; formal analysis, A.d.R., J.P.M., and R.S.; investigation, A.d.R.; resources, J.P.M. and R.S.; writing—original draft preparation, A.d.R.; writing—review and editing, J.P.M. and R.S.; visualization, A.d.R.; supervision, J.P.M. and R.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was partially financed by national funds through FCT—Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia under the project UIDB/00006/2020 (DOI:10.54499/UIDB/00006/2020).

Data Availability Statement

All analyzed data were obtained from the PubMed (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/, accessed on 2 January 2024) and Scopus (https://www.scopus.com/, accessed on 2 January 2024) platforms.

Acknowledgments

The authors extend their heartfelt gratitude to clinical psychologists Marta Faustino and Brígida Ribeiro for their invaluable collaboration in this study. Their professional expertise and clinical insights were instrumental in enriching the practical aspects of the research and ensuring its depth and relevance.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Abbreviations

The following abbreviations are used in this manuscript:
ADPAssessment of Personality Disorders Questionnaire
CAQCalifornia Adult Q-Sort
CIConfidence interval
CNSChildhood Narcissism Scale
DTDDDark Triad Dirty Dozen Scale
DSMDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders
FFNI-SFFive-Factor Narcissism Inventory – Short Form
HSNSHypersensitive Narcissism Scale
I 2 I 2 statistic
NOSNewcastle–Ottawa scale
NPINarcissistic Personality Inventory
NPQNarcissistic Personality Questionnaire
NPQC Narcissistic Personality Questionnaire for Children
NPQC-R Narcissistic Personality Questionnaire for Children-Revised
PDQ-4+Personality Diagnostic Questionnaire—4th Edition Plus
PNIPathological Narcissism Inventory
PRISMAPreferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses
PROSPEROProspective register for systematic review protocols
Q ( . ) Heterogeneity statistic Q
SCIDStructured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Personality Disorders
SD3Short Dark Triad
SD4Short Dark Tetrad
SINSSingle Item Narcissism Scale
YSQ-SFYoung Schema Questionnaire—Short Form

Appendix A. Parental Education Studies Characteristics

Table A1. Parental education studies characteristics: Narc. Scale: narcissism scale; G: grandiose narcissism; V: vulnerable narcissism.
Table A1. Parental education studies characteristics: Narc. Scale: narcissism scale; G: grandiose narcissism; V: vulnerable narcissism.
StudyCountrySampleFemaleMeanNarc.NOS
Size%AgeScale
[20]Pakistan10087 PDQ7
[38]Netherlands565549.6CNS10
[54] 33083.921.6PNI8
PNI-V8
[21]USA23154.539.3PDQ 4+8
[39]Italy51952.49.7CNS9
[12]USA8550.623CAQ-139
CAQ-13-V9
[55]USA102 23CAQ8
[37]USA46058.5 CNS10
[56]Cyprus62845.4 NPI-4010
[24] 11158.615.9SD39
[57]UK176100 PNI-52-G8
PNI-52-V8
1520 PNI-52-G8
PNI-52-V8
[15]China55968.221.2DTDD9
[58]USA21459.315.4NPI-409
[59]USA14532,419.6NPI-409
[60]Australian44268.125.6PNI-529
PNI-52-V9
[41]Turkey50853.331.2PNI9
PNI-V9
[61]UK33479.920.3HSNS9
[62]Iran278 NPI-168
[63]China6815915.6SD3-D9
SD3-I9
[19]China117353.714.8NPQ10
[64]China153355.115.3SD39
[65]Taiwan28570.520.1NPI-409
[66]China53082.318.8DTDD8
[67]UAE7010019.7NPI-408
UK78 21
[68]USA59976,522,3PNI-529
[69]USA15566.719.3PNI-288
PNI-28-V8
[53] 30014.316.6PNI8
PNI-V8
[70]USA2639045NPI-408
[29] 38770.822.8SCID9
[71]UK1195028.8NPI-409
HSNS9
[23]China103557.522.5SD410
[18]Turkey42279.620.1FFNI-SF9
FFNI-SF-V9
[72]USA65369.520PNI-5210
[16]Iran26223.222.8DTDD9
[27]China41724816.4SINS9
[73] 38078.920.1PNI9
PNI-V9
[14]Germany106049 DDS9
[74]Israel6897924.6PNI-289
PNI-28-V9

Appendix B. Adapted Newcastle–Ottawa Scale for Parental Education Studies

Selection: (Maximum five stars)
(1)
Representativeness of the sample:
  • Truly representative of the average in the target population. * (all subjects or random sampling)
  • Somewhat representative of the average in the target population. * (non-random sampling)
  • Selected group of users.
  • No description of the sampling strategy.
(2)
Sample size:
  • Justified and satisfactory. *
  • Not justified.
(3)
Non-respondents:
  • Comparability between respondents and non-respondents characteristics is established, and the response rate is satisfactory. *
  • The response rate is unsatisfactory, or the comparability between respondents and non respondents is unsatisfactory.
  • No description of the response rate or the characteristics of the responders and the non-responders.
(4)
Ascertainment of the exposure (risk factor):
  • Validated measurement tool. **
  • Non-validated measurement tool, but the tool is available or described.*
  • No description of the measurement tool.
Comparability: (Maximum two stars)
(1)
The subjects in different outcome groups are comparable, based on the study design or analysis. Confounding factors are controlled.
  • The study controls for two or more factors. **
  • The study control for one factor. *
Outcome: (Maximum three stars)
(1)
Assessment of the outcome:
  • Validated measurement tool. **
  • Non-validated measurement tool, but the tool is available or described.*
  • No description of the measurement tool.
(2)
Statistical test:
  • The statistical test used to analyze the data is clearly described and appropriate, and the measurement of the association is presented, including confidence intervals and the probability level (p-value). *
  • The statistical test is not appropriate, not described or incomplete.
This scale corresponds to a modified version of the Newcastle–Ottawa scale (NOS) adapted for cross-sectional studies presented in 2013 by Herzog et al. [84,93]. It addresses the specificities of the studies included in this meta-analysis.

Appendix C. Funnel Graphics for Parental Education Studies

Figure A1. Authoritative parenting overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.5533, trim and fill method: two missing studies.
Figure A1. Authoritative parenting overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.5533, trim and fill method: two missing studies.
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a1
Figure A2. Authoritarian parenting overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.7727, trim and fill method: no missing studies.
Figure A2. Authoritarian parenting overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.7727, trim and fill method: no missing studies.
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a2
Figure A3. Neglectful parenting overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.2736, trim and fill method: four missing studies.
Figure A3. Neglectful parenting overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.2736, trim and fill method: four missing studies.
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a3
Figure A4. Permissive parenting overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.9582, trim and fill method: no missing studies.
Figure A4. Permissive parenting overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.9582, trim and fill method: no missing studies.
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a4
Figure A5. Authoritative mother overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.3003, trim and fill method: two missing studies.
Figure A5. Authoritative mother overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.3003, trim and fill method: two missing studies.
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a5
Figure A6. Authoritative father overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.7393, trim and fill method: two missing studies.
Figure A6. Authoritative father overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.7393, trim and fill method: two missing studies.
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a6
Figure A7. Authoritative mother grandiose. Egger’s test p-value = 0.7240, trim and fill method: no missing studies.
Figure A7. Authoritative mother grandiose. Egger’s test p-value = 0.7240, trim and fill method: no missing studies.
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a7
Figure A8. Authoritative father grandiose. Egger’s test p-value = 0.9242, trim and fill method: no missing studies.
Figure A8. Authoritative father grandiose. Egger’s test p-value = 0.9242, trim and fill method: no missing studies.
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a8
Figure A9. Authoritative parenting vulnerable. Egger’s test p-value = 0.4534, trim and fill method: no missing studies.
Figure A9. Authoritative parenting vulnerable. Egger’s test p-value = 0.4534, trim and fill method: no missing studies.
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a9
Figure A10. Authoritative mother vulnerable. Egger’s test p-value = 0.4423, trim and fill method: two missing studies.
Figure A10. Authoritative mother vulnerable. Egger’s test p-value = 0.4423, trim and fill method: two missing studies.
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a10
Figure A11. Authoritative father vulnerable. Egger’s test p-value = 0.7393, trim and fill method: two missing studies.
Figure A11. Authoritative father vulnerable. Egger’s test p-value = 0.7393, trim and fill method: two missing studies.
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a11
Figure A12. Authoritarian mother overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.8892, trim and fill method: three missing studies.
Figure A12. Authoritarian mother overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.8892, trim and fill method: three missing studies.
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a12
Figure A13. Authoritarian father overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.6535, trim and fill method: three missing studies.
Figure A13. Authoritarian father overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.6535, trim and fill method: three missing studies.
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a13
Figure A14. Authoritarian parenting vulnerable. Egger’s test p-value = 0.5602, trim and fill method: two missing studies.
Figure A14. Authoritarian parenting vulnerable. Egger’s test p-value = 0.5602, trim and fill method: two missing studies.
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a14
Figure A15. Authoritarian mother vulnerable. Egger’s test p-value = 0.8658, trim and fill method: no missing studies.
Figure A15. Authoritarian mother vulnerable. Egger’s test p-value = 0.8658, trim and fill method: no missing studies.
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a15
Figure A16. Authoritarian father vulnerable. Egger’s test p-value = 0.8546, trim and fill method: no missing studies.
Figure A16. Authoritarian father vulnerable. Egger’s test p-value = 0.8546, trim and fill method: no missing studies.
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a16
Figure A17. Neglectful mother overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.4241, trim and fill method: no missing studies.
Figure A17. Neglectful mother overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.4241, trim and fill method: no missing studies.
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a17
Figure A18. Neglectful father overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.9341, trim and fill method: no missing studies.
Figure A18. Neglectful father overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.9341, trim and fill method: no missing studies.
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a18
Figure A19. Neglectful parenting vulnerable. Egger’s test p-value = 0.8289, trim and fill method: two missing studies.
Figure A19. Neglectful parenting vulnerable. Egger’s test p-value = 0.8289, trim and fill method: two missing studies.
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a19
Figure A20. Permissive mother overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.5700, trim and fill method: two missing studies.
Figure A20. Permissive mother overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.5700, trim and fill method: two missing studies.
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a20
Figure A21. Permissive father overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.2734, trim and fill method: two missing studies.
Figure A21. Permissive father overall. Egger’s test p-value = 0.2734, trim and fill method: two missing studies.
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a21

Appendix D. Changes in Correlations by Mean Age

Figure A22. Authoritative parenting overall. Estimated slope β ^ = 0.008 , CI 95% [−0.025, 0.010], and p-value = 0.3919 for H 0 : β = 0 .
Figure A22. Authoritative parenting overall. Estimated slope β ^ = 0.008 , CI 95% [−0.025, 0.010], and p-value = 0.3919 for H 0 : β = 0 .
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a22
Figure A23. Authoritarian parenting overall. Estimated slope β ^ = 0.020, CI 95% [0.008, 0.031], and p-value = 0.0008 for H 0 : β = 0 .
Figure A23. Authoritarian parenting overall. Estimated slope β ^ = 0.020, CI 95% [0.008, 0.031], and p-value = 0.0008 for H 0 : β = 0 .
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a23
Figure A24. Authoritative mother overall. Estimated slope β ^ = 0.011 , CI 95% [−0.028, 0.006], and p-value = 0.2107 for H 0 : β = 0 .
Figure A24. Authoritative mother overall. Estimated slope β ^ = 0.011 , CI 95% [−0.028, 0.006], and p-value = 0.2107 for H 0 : β = 0 .
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a24
Figure A25. Authoritative father overall. Estimated slope β ^ = 0.023 , CI 95% [−0.059, 0.013], and p-value = 0.2039 for H 0 : β = 0 .
Figure A25. Authoritative father overall. Estimated slope β ^ = 0.023 , CI 95% [−0.059, 0.013], and p-value = 0.2039 for H 0 : β = 0 .
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a25
Figure A26. Authoritarian mother overall. Estimated slope β ^ = 0.008, CI 95% [0.0018, 0.0138], and p-value = 0.0109 for H 0 : β = 0 .
Figure A26. Authoritarian mother overall. Estimated slope β ^ = 0.008, CI 95% [0.0018, 0.0138], and p-value = 0.0109 for H 0 : β = 0 .
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a26
Figure A27. Authoritarian father overall. Estimated slope β ^ = 0.004, CI 95% [−0.004, 0.013], and p-value = 0.3130 for H 0 : β = 0 .
Figure A27. Authoritarian father overall. Estimated slope β ^ = 0.004, CI 95% [−0.004, 0.013], and p-value = 0.3130 for H 0 : β = 0 .
Appliedmath 05 00023 g0a27

References

  1. Jauk, E.; Kanske, P. Can neuroscience help to understand narcissism? A systematic review of an emerging field. Personal. Neurosci. 2021, 4, e3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Vater, A.; Moritz, S.; Roepke, S. Does a narcissism epidemic exist in modern western societies? Comparing narcissism and self-esteem in East and West Germany. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, 171–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Shaw, D.J. Are We Becoming More Narcissistic? Website. 2021. Available online: https://www.sciencefocus.com/news/are-we-becoming-more-narcissistic (accessed on 2 December 2024).
  4. Oberleiter, S.; Stickel, P.; Pietschnig, J. A Farewell to the Narcissism Epidemic? A Cross-Temporal Meta-Analysis of Global NPI Scores (1982–2023). J. Personal. 2024, 0, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013; Available online: https://archive.org/details/APA-DSM-5/page/671/mode/2up (accessed on 2 December 2024).
  6. Kernberg, O. Borderline Conditions and Pathological Narcissism; Rowman and Littlefield Publishers, Inc.: Lanham, MD, USA, 2004; Available online: https://archive.org/details/borderlinecondit00kern (accessed on 2 December 2024).
  7. Clemens, V.; Fegert, J.M.; Allroggen, M. Adverse childhood experiences and grandiose narcissism—Findings from a population-representative sample. Child Abus. Negl. 2022, 127, 105545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Rawn, K.P.; Keller, P.S.; Widige, T.A. Parent Grandiose Narcissism and Child Socio-Emotional Well Being: The Role of Parenting. Sage J. 2023, 0, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Miller, J.; Price, J.; Gentile, B.; Lynam, D.R.; Campbell, W.K. Grandiose and vulnerable narcissism from the perspective of the interpersonal circumplex. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2012, 53, 507–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Statistics Solutions. Narcissistic Personality Inventory-40 (NPI-40). 2024. Available online: https://www.statisticssolutions.com/free-resources/directory-of-survey-instruments/narcissistic-personality-inventory-40-npi-40/ (accessed on 2 December 2024).
  11. Gentile, B.; Miller, J.D.; Hoffman, B.J.; Reidy, D.E.; Zeichner, A.; Campbell, W.K. A Test of Two Brief Measures of Grandiose Narcissism: The Narcissistic Personality Inventory—13 and the Narcissistic Personality Inventory—16. Psychol. Assess. 2013, 25, 1120–1136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Cramer, P. Adolescent Parenting, Identification, and Maladaptive Narcissism. Psychoanal. Psychol. 2015, 32, 559–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Lanning, K.; Sherman, R.A. The California Adult Q-Sort. In Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences; Zeigler-Hill, V., Shackelford, T., Eds.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Yendell, A.; Clemens, V.; Schuler, J.; Decker, O. What makes a violent mind? The interplay of parental rearing, dark triad personality traits and propensity for violence in a sample of German adolescents. PLoS ONE 2022, 17, e0268992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Guo, J.; Zhang, J.; Pang, W. Parental warmth, rejection, and creativity: The mediating roles of openness and dark personality traits. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2021, 168, 110369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Tajmirriyahi, M.; Doerfler, S.M.; Najafi, M.; Hamidizadeh, K.; Ickes, W. Dark Triad traits, recalled and current quality of the parent-child relationship: A non-western replication and extension. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2021, 180, 110949. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Hengartner, M.; Ajdacic-Gross, V.; Rodgers, S.; Müller, M.; Rossler, W. Childhood adversity in association with personality disorder dimensions: New findings in an old debate. Eur. Psychiatry 2013, 28, 476–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Şar, V.; Türk-Kurtça, T. The Vicious Cycle of Traumatic Narcissism and Dissociative Depression Among Young Adults: A Trans-Diagnostic Approach. J. Trauma Dissociation 2021, 22, 502–521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Li, J.; Wang, X.; Wu, X.; Guo, Y. Early material parenting and adolescents’ materialism: The mediating role of overt narcissism. Curr. Psychol. 2023, 42, 10543–10555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Batool, N.; Shehzadi, H.; Riaz, M.N.; Riaz, M.A. Paternal malparenting and offspring personality disorders: Mediating effect of early maladaptive schemas. JPMA J. Pak. Med. Assoc. 2017, 67, 556–560. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  21. Cohen, L.J.; Tanis, T.; Bhattacharjee, R.; Nesci, C.; Halmi, W.; Galynker, I. Are there differential relationships between different types of childhood maltreatment and different types of adult personality pathology? Psychiatry Res. 2014, 215, 192–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. PsycNet, A. Personality Diagnostic Questionnaire—4+. 2024. Available online: https://psycnet.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2Ft07759-000 (accessed on 2 December 2024).
  23. Ren, M.; Zou, S.; Ding, S.; Ding, D. Childhood Environmental Unpredictability and Prosocial Behavior in Adults: The Effect of Life-History Strategy and Dark Personalities. Psychol. Res. Behav. Manag. 2022, 15, 1757–1769. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Ferencz, T.; Láng, A.; Kocsor, F.; Kozma, L.; Babós, A.; Gyuris, P. Sibling relationship quality and parental rearing style infuence the development of Dark Triad traits. Curr. Psychol. 2022, 42, 24764–24781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Babakr, Z.; Fatahi, N. Risk-taking Behaviour: The Role of Dark Triad Traits, Impulsivity, Sensation Seeking and Adverse Childhood Experience. Acta Inform. Medica 2023, 31, 292–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Abilleira, M.P.; Rodicio-García, M.L.; de Deus, M.P.R.; del Hierro, T.A. Adaptation of the Short Dark Triad (SD3) to Spanish Adolescents. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2024, 14, 1585–1596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Wang, P.; Hu, H.; Mo, P.K.H.; Ouyang, M.; Geng, J.; Zeng, P.; Mao, N. How is Father Phubbing Associated with Adolescents’ Social Networking Sites Addiction? Roles of Narcissism, Need to Belong, and Loneliness. J. Psychol. 2022, 156, 331–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. PsycNet, A. Single Item Narcissism Scale. 2024. Available online: https://psycnet.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2Ft46661-000 (accessed on 2 December 2024).
  29. Ochojska, D.; Pasternak, J. The selected psychosocial risk factors in the development of personality disorders in a group of Polish young adults. J. Psychopathol. 2021, 27, 125–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Glasofer, D.R.; Brown, A.J.; Riegel, M. Structured Clinical Interviewfor DSM-IV (SCID). In Encyclopedia of Feeding and Eating Disorders; Springer: Singapore, 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. PsycNet, A. Young Schema Questionnaire–Short Form. 2024. Available online: https://psycnet.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2Ft12644-000 (accessed on 2 December 2024).
  32. Calvete, E.; Orue, I.; Gamez-Guadix, M. Predictors of Child-to-Parent Aggression: A 3-Year Longitudinal Study. Dev. Psychol. 2015, 51, 663–676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Ang, R.P.; Yusof, N. Development and initial validation of the narcissistic personality questionnaire for children: A preliminary investigation using school-based Asian samples. Educ. Psychol. 2006, 26, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Ang, R.P.; Raine, A. Reliability, Validity and Invariance of the Narcissistic Personality Questionnaire for Children-Revised (NPQC-R). J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 2009, 31, 143–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Loke, S.W.; Lowe, P.A. Validation of the Narcissistic Personality Questionnaire for Children—Revised among U.S. students. Psychol. Assess. 2014, 26, 619–627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Thomaes, S.; Stegge, H.; Bushman, B.J.; Olthof, T.; Denissen, J. Development and validation of the childhood narcissism scale. J. Personal. Assess. 2008, 90, 382–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Eberly-Lewis, M.B.; Vera-Hughes, M.; Coetzee, T.M. Parenting and Adolescent Grandiose Narcissism: Mediation through Independent Self-Construal and Need for Positive Approval. J. Genet. Psychol. 2018, 179, 207–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Brummelman, E.; Thomaes, S.; Nelemans, S.A.; de Castro, B.O.; Overbeek, G.; Bushman, B.J. Origins of narcissism in children. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2015, 112, 3659–3662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Coppola, G.; Musso, P.; Buonanno, C.; Semeraro, C.; Iacobellis, B.; Cassibba, R.; Levantini, V.; Masi, G.; Thomaes, S.; Muratori, P. The Apple of Daddy’s Eye: Parental Overvaluation Links the Narcissistic Traits of Father and Child. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 5515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Muratori, P.; Milone, A.; Brovedani, P.; Levantini, V.; Melli, G.; Pisano, S.; Valente, E.; Thomaes, S.; Masi, G. Narcissistic traits and self-esteem in children: Results from a community and a clinical sample of patients with oppositional defiant disorder. J. Affect. Disord. 2018, 241, 275–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Imamoglu, A.H.; Batigun, A.D. The assessment of the relationship between narcissism, perceived parental rearing styles, and defense mechanisms. Dusunen Adam J. Psychiatry Neurol. Sci. 2020, 33, 388–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Young, J.E.; Klosko, J.S.; Weishaar, M.E. Schema Theraphy: A Practitioner’s Guide; The Guilford Press, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 2003; Available online: https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2003-00629-000 (accessed on 2 December 2024).
  43. Kılıçkaya, S.; Uçar, N.; Nazlıgül, M.D. A Systematic Review of the Association between Parenting Styles and Narcissism in Young Adults: From Baumrind’s Perspective. Psychol. Rep. 2023, 126, 620–640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. McLean, J. Psychotherapy with a Narcissistic Patient Using Kohut’s Self Psychology Model. Psychiatry 2007, 4, 40–47. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  45. Schmidt, A. Comparison of Kernberg’s and Kohut’s Theory of Narcissistic Personality Disorder. Turk. J. Psychiatry 2019, 30, 137–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Baumrind, D. Current Patterns of Parental Authority. Dev. Psychol. 1971, 4, 1–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Miller, A. Prisoners of Childhood; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1981; Available online: https://www.alice-miller.com/en/prisoners-of-childhood/ (accessed on 2 December 2024).
  48. Winnicott, D.W. Ego distortion in terms of true and false self. InThe Maturational Processes and the Facilitating Environment: Studies in the Theory of Emotional Development; International Universities Press: Madison, CT, USA, 1965; pp. 140–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Sanvictores, T.; Mendez, M.D. Types of Parenting Styles and Effects on Children; StatPearls Publishing LLC.: Tampa, FL, USA, 2024. Available online: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/33760502/ (accessed on 2 December 2024).
  50. George, C.; Solomon, J. Internal working models of caregiving and security of attachment at age six. Infant Ment. Health J. 1989, 10, 222–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Pomerantz, E.M.; Thompson, R.A. Parent’s role in children’s personality development: The psychology resource principle. In Handbook of Personality: Theory and Research; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1981; Available online: https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2008-11667-013 (accessed on 2 December 2024).
  52. Kernberg, P.F.; Weiner, A.S.; Bardenstein, K.K. Personality Disorders in Children and Adolescents; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 2000; Available online: https://archive.org/details/personalitydisor0000kern (accessed on 2 December 2024).
  53. Mechanic, K.L.; Barry, C.T. Adolescent Grandiose and Vulnerable Narcissism: Associations with Perceived Parenting Practices. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2015, 24, 1510–1518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Cater, T.E.; Zeigler-Hill, V.; Vonk, J. Narcissism and recollections of early life experiences. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2011, 51, 935–939. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Cramer, P. Young adult narcissism: A 20 year longitudinal study of the contribution of parenting styles, preschool precursors of narcissism, and denial. J. Res. Personal. 2011, 45, 19–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Farzand, M.; Cerkez, Y.; Baysen, E. Effects of Self-Concept on Narcissism: Mediational Role of Perceived Parenting. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 674679. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Green, A.; MacLean, R.; Charles, K. Recollections of Parenting Styles in The Development of Narcissism: The Role of Gender. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2020, 167, 110246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Horton, R.S.; Bleau, G.; Drwecki, B. Parenting Narcissus: What Are the Links Between Parenting and Narcissism? J. Personal. 2006, 74, 345–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Horton, R.S.; Tritch, T. Clarifying the Links Between Grandiose Narcissism and Parenting. J. Psychol. 2014, 148, 133–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Huxley, E.; Bizumic, B. Parental Invalidation and the Development of Narcissism. J. Psychol. 2016, 151, 130–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Kealy, D.; Hewitt, P.L.; Cox, D.W.; Laverdière, O. Narcissistic vulnerability and the need for belonging: Moderated mediation from perceived parental responsiveness to depressive symptoms. Curr. Psychol. 2021, 42, 2820–2826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Khorshidtalab, E.; Niknam, M. The Mediating Role of Basic Psychological Need Satisfaction in the Relationship Between Overparenting and Narcissism in Iranian College Students. Iran. J. Psychiatry Clin. Psychol. 2023, 29, 332–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Lan, X. Disengaged and highly harsh? Perceived parenting profiles, narcissism, and loneliness among adolescents from divorced families. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2021, 171, 110466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Li, R.; Yao, M.; Chen, Y.; Liu, H. Parent Autonomy Support and Psychological Control, Dark Triad, and Subjective Well-Being of Chinese Adolescents: Synergy of Variable- and Person-Centered Approaches. J. Early Adolesc. 2020, 40, 966–995. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Lin, C.C. Parental attachment and dispositional gratitude: The mediating role of adaptive narcissism. Curr. Psychol. 2023, 42, 16121–16130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Liu, G.; Meng, Y.; Pan, Y.; Ma, Y.; Zhang, D. Mediating Effect of Dark Triad Personality Traits on the Relationship Between Parental Emotional Warmth and Aggression. J. Interpers. Violence 2021, 36, 21–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Lyons, M.; Morgan, K.; Thomas, J.; Hashmi, A.A. Patterns of Parental Warmth, Attachment, and Narcissism in Young Women in United Arab Emirates and the United Kingdom. Individ. Differ. Res. 2013, 11, 149–158. Available online: https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2014-035 81-003 (accessed on 12 December 2024).
  68. Maxwell, K.; Huprich, S. Retrospective reports of attachment disruptions, parental abuse and neglect mediate the relationship between pathological narcissism and self-esteem. Personal. Ment. Health 2014, 8, 290–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. McGinley, M.; Muzzy, B.M.; Hermann, M.; Thompson, R.A. Secure attachment and social and personality outcomes: The moderating role of emerging adults’ autobiographical memories of parents. Rev. Soc. Dev. 2022, 32, 329–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Nguyen, K.T.; Shaw, L. The aetiology of non-clinical narcissism: Clarifying the role of adverse childhood experiences and parental overvaluation. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2020, 154, 109615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Otway, L.J.; Vignoles, V.L. Narcissism and childhood recollections: A quantitative test of psychoanalytic predictions. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2006, 32, 104–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Segrin, C.; Woszidlo, A.; Givertz, M.; Montgomery, N. Parent and Child Traits Associated with Overparenting. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 2013, 32, 569–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Winner, N.A.; Nicholson, B.C. Overparenting and Narcissism in Young Adults: The Mediating Role of Psychological Control. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2018, 27, 3650–3657. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Zarbiv, B.; Goldner, L. Understanding PTSD Symptoms Resulting from Childhood Emotional Abuse and Boundary Dissolution: The Mediating Role of Narcissistic Pathology. J. Aggress. Maltreat. Trauma 2022, 31, 1279–1298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Valentine, J.C.; Pigott, T.D.; Rothstein, H.R. How Many Studies Do You Need?: A Primer on Statistical Power for Meta-Analysis. J. Educ. Behav. Stat. 2010, 35, 215–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Higgins, J.; Thompson, S. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Stat. Med. 2002, 21, 1539–1558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Higgins, J.P.; Thompson, S.G.; Deeks, J.J.; Altman, D.G. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ (Clin. Res. Ed.) 2003, 327, 557–560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. 2024. Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 2 December 2024).
  79. Laliberté, E. Package ‘Metacor’ Meta-Analysis of Correlation Coefficients. Website. 2019. Available online: http://download.nust.na/pub3/cran/web/packages/metacor/metacor.pdf (accessed on 2 December 2024).
  80. United Nations Development Programme. Human Development Report 2023-24: Breaking the Gridlock: Reimagining Cooperation in a Polarized World; United Nations Development Programme (UNDP): New York, NY, USA, 2024; Available online: https://hdr.undp.org/content/human-development-report-2023-24 (accessed on 2 December 2024).
  81. Brilhante, M. Uma Introdução à Meta-Análise Minicurso do XXIII Congresso; Sociedade Portuguesa de Estatística: Lisboa, Portugal, 2017; Available online: https://www.spestatistica.pt/publicacoes/publicacao/uma-introducao-meta-analise (accessed on 2 December 2024).
  82. Shim, S.R.; Kim, S.J. Intervention meta-analysis: Application and practice using R software. Epidemiol. Health 2019, 41, e2019008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Hansen, C.; Steinmetz, H.; Block, J. How to conduct a meta-analysis in eight steps: A practical guide. Manag. Rev. Q. 2022, 72, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Wells, G.; Shea, B.; O’Connell, D.; Peterson, J.; Welch, V.; Losos, M.; Tugwell, P. The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for Assessing the Quality of Nonrandomised Studies in Meta-Analyses. 2014. Available online: http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_epidemiology/oxford.asp (accessed on 2 December 2024).
  85. Koshy, L.; Jeemon, P.; Ganapathi, S.; Madhavan, M.; Urulangodi, M.; Sharma, M.; Harikrishnan, S. Association of South Asian-specific MYBPC3Δ25bp deletion polymorphism and cardiomyopathy: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Meta Gene 2021, 28, 100883. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Pereira, A.; Pinho, C.; Oliveira, A.; Santos, R.; Felgueiras, M.; Martins, J.P. Vaccination Promotion Strategies in the Elderly: Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Vaccines 2024, 12, 1395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Millon, T.; Grossman, S.; Millon, C.; Meagher, S.; Ramnath, R. Personality Disorders in Modern Life; John Wiley & Sons, Inc.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2004; Available online: https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2004-18756-000 (accessed on 2 December 2024).
  88. Kohut, H. The Analysis of the Self: A Systematic Approach to the Psychoanalytic Treatment of Narcissistic Personality Disorders; International Universities Press: Madison, CT, USA, 1971; Available online: https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2009-16139-000 (accessed on 2 December 2024).
  89. Kohut, H. The Restoration of the Self; International Universities Press: Madison, CT, USA, 1977; Available online: https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2009-16135-000 (accessed on 2 December 2024).
  90. Longobardi, C. Can parenting styles affect the children’s development of narcissism? A systematic review. Open Psychol. J. 2016, 9, 84–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Zeigler-Hill, V.; Clark, C.B.; Pickard, J.D. Narcissistic Subtypes and Contingent Self-Esteem: Do All Narcissists Base Their Self-Esteem on the Same Domains? J. Personal. 2008, 76, 753–774. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  92. Cramer, P. Narcissism through the ages: What happens when narcissists grow older? J. Res. Personal. 2011, 45, 479–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Herzog, R.; Álvarez Pasquin, M.J.; Díaz, C.; Barrio, J.L.D.; Estrada, J.M.; Gil, Á. Are healthcare workers’ intentions to vaccinate related to their knowledge, beliefs and attitudes? a systematic review. BMC Public Health 2013, 13, 154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. PRISMA 2020 flow diagram.
Figure 1. PRISMA 2020 flow diagram.
Appliedmath 05 00023 g001
Figure 2. Forest graphic of authoritative parenting and overall narcissism [15,19,39,53,54,56,58,59,64,66,68].
Figure 2. Forest graphic of authoritative parenting and overall narcissism [15,19,39,53,54,56,58,59,64,66,68].
Appliedmath 05 00023 g002
Figure 3. Forest graphic of authoritarian parenting and overall narcissism [18,19,20,54,56,58,59,60,62,64,72,73,74].
Figure 3. Forest graphic of authoritarian parenting and overall narcissism [18,19,20,54,56,58,59,60,62,64,72,73,74].
Appliedmath 05 00023 g003
Figure 4. Forest graphic of neglectful parenting and overall narcissism [15,21,23,39,53,56,59,60,71].
Figure 4. Forest graphic of neglectful parenting and overall narcissism [15,21,23,39,53,56,59,60,71].
Appliedmath 05 00023 g004
Figure 5. Forest graphic of permissive parenting and overall narcissism [19,20,39,58,70,71].
Figure 5. Forest graphic of permissive parenting and overall narcissism [19,20,39,58,70,71].
Appliedmath 05 00023 g005
Figure 6. Forest graphic of authoritarian parenting and overall narcissism – with NOS score studies higher or equal to 8 [18,19,54,56,58,59,60,62,64,72,73,74].
Figure 6. Forest graphic of authoritarian parenting and overall narcissism – with NOS score studies higher or equal to 8 [18,19,54,56,58,59,60,62,64,72,73,74].
Appliedmath 05 00023 g006
Figure 7. Forest graphic of permissive parenting and overall narcissism – with NOS score studies higher or equal to 8 [19,39,58,70,71].
Figure 7. Forest graphic of permissive parenting and overall narcissism – with NOS score studies higher or equal to 8 [19,39,58,70,71].
Appliedmath 05 00023 g007
Figure 8. Forest of graphic authoritative mother and overall narcissism [12,14,16,24,37,38,41,55,63,69].
Figure 8. Forest of graphic authoritative mother and overall narcissism [12,14,16,24,37,38,41,55,63,69].
Appliedmath 05 00023 g008
Figure 9. Forest graphic of authoritative father and overall narcissism [12,14,16,24,37,38,41,55,63,69].
Figure 9. Forest graphic of authoritative father and overall narcissism [12,14,16,24,37,38,41,55,63,69].
Appliedmath 05 00023 g009
Figure 10. Forest graphic of authoritative mother and grandiose narcissism [57,65,67].
Figure 10. Forest graphic of authoritative mother and grandiose narcissism [57,65,67].
Appliedmath 05 00023 g010
Figure 11. Forest graphic of authoritative father and grandiose narcissism [57,65,67].
Figure 11. Forest graphic of authoritative father and grandiose narcissism [57,65,67].
Appliedmath 05 00023 g011
Figure 12. Forest Graphic Authoritative Parenting Vulnerable Narcissism [53,54,61].
Figure 12. Forest Graphic Authoritative Parenting Vulnerable Narcissism [53,54,61].
Appliedmath 05 00023 g012
Figure 13. Forest graphic of authoritative mother vulnerable narcissism [12,41,57,69].
Figure 13. Forest graphic of authoritative mother vulnerable narcissism [12,41,57,69].
Appliedmath 05 00023 g013
Figure 14. Forest graphic authoritative father vulnerable narcissism [12,41,57,69].
Figure 14. Forest graphic authoritative father vulnerable narcissism [12,41,57,69].
Appliedmath 05 00023 g014
Figure 15. Forest graphic of authoritarian mother and overall marcissism [12,14,16,24,29,37,41,55,60,63].
Figure 15. Forest graphic of authoritarian mother and overall marcissism [12,14,16,24,29,37,41,55,60,63].
Appliedmath 05 00023 g015
Figure 16. Forest graphic of authoritarian father and overall narcissism [12,14,16,24,29,37,41,55,60,63].
Figure 16. Forest graphic of authoritarian father and overall narcissism [12,14,16,24,29,37,41,55,60,63].
Appliedmath 05 00023 g016
Figure 17. Forest graphic of authoritarian parenting and vulnerable narcissism [18,54,60,73,74].
Figure 17. Forest graphic of authoritarian parenting and vulnerable narcissism [18,54,60,73,74].
Appliedmath 05 00023 g017
Figure 18. Forest graphic of authoritarian mother and vulnerable narcissism [12,41,57,60].
Figure 18. Forest graphic of authoritarian mother and vulnerable narcissism [12,41,57,60].
Appliedmath 05 00023 g018
Figure 19. Forest graphic of authoritarian father and vulnerable narcissism [12,41,57,60].
Figure 19. Forest graphic of authoritarian father and vulnerable narcissism [12,41,57,60].
Appliedmath 05 00023 g019
Figure 20. Forest graphic of neglectful mother and overall narcissism [12,24,29,41,55].
Figure 20. Forest graphic of neglectful mother and overall narcissism [12,24,29,41,55].
Appliedmath 05 00023 g020
Figure 21. Forest graphic of neglectful father and overall narcissism [12,24,27,29,41,55].
Figure 21. Forest graphic of neglectful father and overall narcissism [12,24,27,29,41,55].
Appliedmath 05 00023 g021
Figure 22. Forest graphic of neglectful parenting and vulnerable narcissism [53,60,71].
Figure 22. Forest graphic of neglectful parenting and vulnerable narcissism [53,60,71].
Appliedmath 05 00023 g022
Figure 23. Forest Graphic Permissive Mother Overall Narcissism [12,38,55].
Figure 23. Forest Graphic Permissive Mother Overall Narcissism [12,38,55].
Appliedmath 05 00023 g023
Figure 24. Forest graphic of permissive father and overall narcissism [12,38,55].
Figure 24. Forest graphic of permissive father and overall narcissism [12,38,55].
Appliedmath 05 00023 g024
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

dos Reis, A.; Martins, J.P.; Santos, R. The Role of Parenting Styles in Narcissism Development: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. AppliedMath 2025, 5, 23. https://doi.org/10.3390/appliedmath5010023

AMA Style

dos Reis A, Martins JP, Santos R. The Role of Parenting Styles in Narcissism Development: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. AppliedMath. 2025; 5(1):23. https://doi.org/10.3390/appliedmath5010023

Chicago/Turabian Style

dos Reis, Ariana, João Paulo Martins, and Rui Santos. 2025. "The Role of Parenting Styles in Narcissism Development: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis" AppliedMath 5, no. 1: 23. https://doi.org/10.3390/appliedmath5010023

APA Style

dos Reis, A., Martins, J. P., & Santos, R. (2025). The Role of Parenting Styles in Narcissism Development: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. AppliedMath, 5(1), 23. https://doi.org/10.3390/appliedmath5010023

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop