Next Article in Journal
Development of a BIM-Based Metaverse Virtual World for Collaborative Architectural Design
Previous Article in Journal
Placemaking and the Complexities of Measuring Impact in Aotearoa New Zealand’s Public and Community Housing: From Theory to Practice and Lived Experience
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Applying 4E Cognition to Acoustic Design: A Theoretical Framework for University Learning Environments

by
Samantha Di Loreto
*,
Miriam D’Ignazio
,
Leonardo Guglielmi
and
Sergio Montelpare
Department of Engineering and Geology (INGEO), Università degli Studi G. D’Annunzio Chieti-Pescara, 60131 Pescara, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Architecture 2025, 5(3), 70; https://doi.org/10.3390/architecture5030070
Submission received: 29 July 2025 / Revised: 18 August 2025 / Accepted: 27 August 2025 / Published: 1 September 2025
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Integration of Acoustics into Architectural Design)

Abstract

The 4E Cognition paradigm offers a novel theoretical framework for understanding how acoustic environments influence cognitive processes in university learning spaces. This research integrates objective characterization of environmental parameters with comprehensive subjective evaluation of student experience to explore how aural conditions relate to cognitive processes and physiological stress responses in university learning environments. The study recruited 126 university students from the Engineering Faculty of “G. D’Annunzio” University, with final analysis including 66 valid responses from 28 participants in the acoustically treated classroom and 38 from the control condition. The results revealed modest associations between environmental conditions and cognitive performance measures, with small to moderate effect sizes (Cohen’s d ranging from 0.02 to 0.31). While acoustic treatment produced measurable improvements in speech intelligibility and acoustic quality ratings, differences in cognitive load and allostatic load indices were minimal between conditions. These findings provide preliminary empirical insights for applying the 4E Cognition framework to educational settings, suggesting that acoustic interventions may require extended exposure periods or more intensive treatments to produce substantial physiological and cognitive effects. This work contributes to the emerging field of cognitive architecture by introducing an innovative theoretical approach that reconceptualizes acoustic environments as potential cognitive extensions rather than mere background conditions. The findings offer initial evidence-based insights for integrating environmental considerations into educational facility design, while highlighting the need for longitudinal studies to fully understand how acoustic environments function as cognitive scaffolding in learning contexts.

1. Introduction

The relationship between environmental conditions and cognitive processes has been a central concern in philosophy of mind and cognitive sciences since the early 20th century. This awareness has evolved from simple observations of environmental influence to sophisticated theoretical frameworks that fundamentally challenge the traditional boundaries between mind, body, and environment. The recognition that thinking extends beyond the confines of the individual brain has profound implications for architectural design, particularly in educational settings where cognitive performance is paramount.
Recent architectural research has increasingly recognized the importance of environmental factors in supporting cognitive processes, with neuroarchitecture that demonstrates measurable impacts of built environments on human cognition and well-being [1,2]. The field of cognitive architecture has emerged to address how built environments can be designed to enhance human cognitive performance, moving beyond traditional aesthetic considerations to evidence-based design approaches that integrate psychological and neurological insights into architectural practice [3,4].
A pivotal moment in this theoretical evolution was the introduction of the Extended Mind Theory by Clark and Chalmers in 1997 [5]. This groundbreaking framework proposed that human cognitive processes are not confined within the limits of the brain but extend into the surrounding environment through tools, technologies, and environmental structures. This perspective opened pathways to an even broader approach that recognizes the direct and continuous interaction between mind, body, and environment.
The 4E Cognition paradigm has emerged as a comprehensive framework that encompasses four fundamental dimensions of cognitive processes: cognitive processes are intrinsically embodied in bodily experience embodied, situated in specific contexts embedded, emerging from interaction with the environment enactive, and distributed between neural systems and environmental structures extended [6]. This paradigm represents a radical departure from traditional computational models of cognition, positioning environmental design as an integral component of cognitive architecture.
In acoustic terms, this framework suggests that sound environments are not merely external stimuli to be processed, but become integral components of how we think, learn, and interact. The embodied dimension recognizes that acoustic experience involves our entire physical being, the embedded dimension acknowledges that the same cognitive task requires different strategies depending on the acoustic context, the enactive dimension emphasizes how we actively shape our acoustic environment through behavior, and the extended dimension proposes that well-designed acoustic spaces can function as cognitive tools that enhance our mental capabilities.
In academic contexts, this theoretical framework finds its ideal empirical testing ground. University classrooms, in particular, host activities that require sustained concentration, processing of complex information, and social interaction [7]. The cognitive demands of higher education make these environments particularly sensitive to environmental factors that can either support or hinder learning processes.
Among environmental parameters, acoustics plays a fundamental role in educational contexts [8,9]. Indoor Environmental Quality (IEQ), particularly acoustic quality, is a critical determinant of the holistic educational experience, significantly influencing the well-being, concentration, and performance of students [10,11]. The acoustic environment of classrooms can significantly influence attentional mechanisms, language comprehension, and working memory [12,13]. Background noise, excessive reverberation times, and poor speech intelligibility impose a greater mental effort on students [14], creating what cognitive load theory describes as an extraneous cognitive load that diverts mental resources from learning objectives.
Speech intelligibility has become a crucial factor in educational settings [15]. Students positioned in different locations within classrooms experience varying levels of auditory clarity, with those in rear sections of spacious classrooms often facing weakened vocal signals and excessive reverberation that challenge effective listening [16,17]. Since most learning activities involve oral communication, the intelligibility of spoken words—defined as the percentage of correctly understood speech items—is crucial for successful learning [18,19].
Background noise presents a complex challenge in educational buildings. Ambient noise levels from external sources, student behavior, and internal service equipment significantly contribute to overall background noise, directly affecting speech intelligibility and cognitive performance [20,21]. Recent research emphasizes that good acoustic quality in classrooms is essential for creating optimal learning environments, with soundscape research in indoor environments gaining attention for its potential to contribute to healthier and more comfortable educational spaces [22].
The increase in cognitive load also carries a physiological cost that the human body pays in attempting to adapt to adverse environmental conditions. Prolonged exposure to acoustically inadequate environments can not only compromise information processing capabilities but can also activate physiological stress responses, including increased blood pressure, altered heart rate, and metabolic variations [23].
The cumulative physiological stress resulting from prolonged activation of responses to stressful situations, known as allostatic overload, can further compromise student well-being and performance. This concept, introduced by McEwen and Stellar [24], describes the wear and tear on the body that accumulates as an individual is exposed to repeated or chronic stress. In educational environments, this translates to a concerning cycle where poor acoustic conditions lead to increased cognitive effort, physiological stress, and ultimately reduced learning capacity [25].
Engineering classrooms present unique acoustic challenges due to their intensive cognitive demands and specific environmental characteristics. Students in engineering faculties are typically exposed to cognitive workloads that require high levels of concentration and precision. Additionally, these classrooms often feature noisy mechanical equipment, reflective surfaces, and complex geometries that can influence sound propagation, creating particularly challenging acoustic environments for sustained cognitive performance [26].

Contribution of This Work

This research applies the 4E Cognition paradigm to educational acoustic environments, combining established methodological approaches from architectural acoustics and environmental psychology in a novel application context. While individual components of our methodology draw from existing validated approaches, their integration for examining acoustic–cognitive relationships through the 4E framework in educational settings represents a new application of these established methods. The study explores how acoustic environments might function beyond simple background conditions by examining them through the lens of embodied, embedded, enactive, and extended cognition, offering a different theoretical perspective on environmental design considerations.
From a methodological standpoint, this work integrates three established assessment approaches: objective acoustic characterization following international standards, subjective acoustic perception evaluation using validated instruments, and subjective assessment of allostatic load through self-reported measures. While each method is well-established in its respective field, their combined application within the 4E Cognition framework for educational acoustic environments has not been systematically explored in previous literature. The study design comparing identical classrooms that differ only in acoustic treatment allows for controlled examination of these relationships, though we acknowledge the modest effect sizes observed suggest the complexity of these interactions may require different approaches or longer exposure periods to detect meaningful effects.
The practical contribution lies in providing initial empirical evidence for how established cognitive science frameworks might inform acoustic design decisions in educational settings. Rather than proposing revolutionary changes to current practice, this work offers a systematic approach to examining environmental factors through cognitive science principles, potentially informing evidence-based design decisions. The findings suggest that while acoustic improvements produced measurable changes in objective parameters and some subjective acoustic ratings, the relationship between environmental modifications and subjective cognitive–physiological responses may be more complex than initially hypothesized, highlighting areas for future investigation.
The ultimate objective is to contribute to understanding how established design approaches might be informed by cognitive science principles, providing a foundation for future research in this emerging interdisciplinary area.
This study addresses three primary research questions: (1) How do objective acoustic improvements in treated classrooms translate into subjective perceptual differences for students? (2) What is the relationship between acoustic environmental conditions and students’ cognitive load and allostatic load responses in university learning spaces? (3) Can the 4E Cognition framework provide a theoretical foundation for understanding acoustic environments as potential cognitive extensions in educational settings?
This paper is structured as follows. Section 2 presents the theoretical framework of 4E Cognition, exploring how the four dimensions—embodied, embedded, enactive, and extended cognition—inform our understanding of acoustic environments as cognitive extensions. Section 3 describes the materials and methods, including the acoustic characterization protocol and subjective assessment methodology applied to two twin university classrooms. Section 4 presents the results, beginning with objective acoustic measurements that establish the environmental context, followed by analysis of acoustic–cognitive performance relationships and comprehensive statistical analysis of the collected data. Finally, Section 5 discusses the implications of the findings for cognitive architecture theory and practical acoustic design, concluding with recommendations for future research directions in the emerging field of cognitively informed environmental design.

2. Theoretical Framework: 4E Cognition and Acoustic Design

The 4E cognition paradigm has completely changed how we understand the mind. Instead of seeing it as something that only happens in the brain, we now recognize it as something that involves our whole body, our environment, and our interactions with others [27]. This approach recognizes cognition as embodied, embedded, enactive, and extended, fundamentally reshaping how we think about the relationship between mind, body, and environment [6]. This framework emerged from converging insights in phenomenology, dynamic systems theory, and neuroscience, challenging computational models that locate cognition exclusively in neural processes. When it comes to acoustic experience, this theoretical approach reveals how sound environments are not just external stimuli to process but become integral parts of cognitive processes themselves. The acoustic environment continuously shapes our ability to pay attention, understand things, and interact socially, becoming a constitutive element of cognitive experience rather than mere sensory input.

2.1. Embodied Cognition

The concept of embodied cognition represents a fundamental shift in understanding cognitive processes, challenging the Cartesian tradition that separates mind and body. This theoretical perspective, developed through Maurice Merleau-Ponty’s contributions in phenomenology of perception and later formalized by researchers like Lakoff and Johnson [28] and Varela, Thompson, and Rosch [29], argues that cognitive processes are intrinsically rooted in bodily experience and sensorimotor interactions with the environment. Embodied cognition proposes that thinking, perception, and action are not separate processes but integrated aspects of a single dynamic body–mind–environment system. This approach emphasizes how bodily structures and sensorimotor capabilities directly shape cognitive processes and ways of understanding the world. In the acoustic domain, embodied cognition reveals particularly significant dimensions of sound experience. The human auditory system is not simply a passive receiver of sound waves, but an integrated system that involves the entire body in perceiving and processing sound. Sound vibrations are perceived not only through the traditional auditory apparatus, but also through bone conduction, bodily resonance, and tactile sensations, creating a multisensory experience that involves proprioception, balance, and spatial perception. Central to this understanding is the concept of body schema developed by Maurice Merleau-Ponty and later developed by Shaun Gallagher [30]: the implicit and proprioceptive sense of bodily configuration that enables fluid action in space. In the acoustic context, body schema extends to include perception of sound space, localization of acoustic sources, and understanding of spatial relationships through auditory information. Research on noise effects confirms this theoretical perspective, showing that exposure to inadequate acoustic environments activates physiological stress responses that involve the entire organism [31]. These effects represent direct manifestations of the embodied nature of acoustic experience, where environmental sound conditions translate into bodily responses that influence cognitive abilities and learning performance.

2.2. Embedded Cognition

The concept of embedded cognition argues that cognitive processes are intrinsically dependent on the environmental, social, and cultural context in which they manifest. This theoretical approach, developed through Lucy Suchman’s [32]. Contributions in studies on situated action and later elaborated by Andy Clark [5], proposes that the environment is not simply a source of input for the cognitive system, but constitutes an active and structuring part of cognitive processes themselves. Central to this understanding is the concept of affordances developed by James J. Gibson [33]: the environment provides opportunities and constraints for action that directly shape the ways we think, perceive, and act. In the acoustic domain, the situated nature of cognition manifests particularly clearly. The acoustics of learning environments provides a paradigmatic example of this embeddedness: the same cognitive task (for example, listening to a lecture) requires different cognitive strategies and resources depending on the specific acoustic context. In an acoustically optimized environment, students can engage in efficient listening, allowing cognitive resources to focus on content understanding and critical thinking. In acoustically challenging contexts, the same students must deploy additional cognitive resources for basic auditory processing, fundamentally altering the nature of the learning experience and the cognitive strategies employed. Cognitive load theory [34] provides a framework for understanding these situated processes. Background noise, excessive reverberation times, and poor speech intelligibility impose what the theory defines as extraneous cognitive load, which diverts mental resources from learning objectives. This extraneous load represents a direct manifestation of the embedded nature of cognition, where environmental conditions become integral parts of the cognitive process itself, determining not only performance but also the very ways through which the learning process is realized.

2.3. Enactive Cognition and Sound Interaction

The concept of enactive cognition represents perhaps the most radical element of the 4E paradigm, proposing that cognition is not a process of representing the external world, but emerges through dynamic interaction between organism and environment. This theoretical approach was developed mainly by Francisco Varela through the concept of autopoiesis and the idea of structural coupling, later elaborated by Evan Thompson [35] and Alva Noe [36] in studies on perception. According to the enactive perspective, perception is not passive reception of information but “enaction”—literally “putting into action”—through action and active exploration of the environment. As proposed by O’Regan and Noe [37], we perceive through action, developing sensorimotor skills that allow us to explore and interact with the surrounding world. In the acoustic domain, the enactive dimension reveals how sound experience is fundamentally interactive and co-constructed. Students do not passively receive acoustic information but actively engage with and shape their acoustic environment through adaptive behaviors: moving closer to sound sources, modifying their posture to optimize listening, using gestures to improve communication, or developing coping strategies for difficult acoustic conditions. The enactive perspective also highlights the importance of acoustic environments that support natural and bidirectional communicative behaviors. Optimal classroom acoustics should facilitate not only unidirectional teacher–student communication but also multidirectional student–student interaction, supporting collaborative learning processes central to contemporary educational approaches. In the phenomenological tradition, sound is not simply a perceptual object but a relational medium through which we come into contact with the world and with others. Maurice Merleau-Ponty [38] had already intuited how perception is always temporally extended and situationally structured—characteristics that apply paradigmatically to acoustic experience, since sound is intrinsically temporal and relational.

2.4. Extended Cognition

The extended dimension recognizes that cognitive processes can incorporate environmental structures as functional components of the cognitive system. In acoustic terms, this means that well-designed acoustic environments can function as cognitive extensions that enhance information processing capacity. The extended mind theory by Clark and Chalmers [39] proposes that human cognitive processes are not confined within the limits of the brain but extend into the surrounding environment through tools, technologies, and environmental structures. In the educational context, this extension includes not only technological tools but also the physical environment itself, including its acoustic properties. When applied to classroom acoustics, the extended mind perspective suggests that the acoustic environment becomes part of the cognitive system. A well-designed acoustic environment can function as a cognitive prosthesis, reducing the mental effort required for speech processing and allowing the allocation of cognitive resources to higher-order learning tasks. Conversely, poor acoustic conditions force the cognitive system to allocate substantial resources to basic auditory processing, creating what we might define as “cognitive friction” that impedes learning. For example, appropriate reverberation times can improve speech clarity and comprehension, effectively extending the cognitive system’s capacity for auditory processing. Conversely, excessive background noise or poor speech intelligibility forces the cognitive system to work with reduced capacity, highlighting the critical role of acoustic design in cognitive architecture.

3. Materials and Methods

The methodology adopted in this study follows a mixed-methods approach that integrates objective measurements and subjective evaluations. This multimethodological integration aligns with the theoretical framework of 4E Cognition, recognizing the embodied and situated nature of acoustic experience in learning environments and allowing for comprehensive understanding of the complex interactions between the built environment and students’ cognitive processes.
The bipartite approach comprises:
  • Objective acoustic characterization: Measurements of relevant acoustic parameters including sound levels, reverberation times, and speech intelligibility indices.
  • Subjective perceptual evaluation: Comprehensive questionnaires assessing perceived acoustic comfort, cognitive load, adaptation strategies, and allostatic load indicators.
This study focuses on two twin classrooms located at the Engineering Faculty of “G. D’Annunzio” University in Pescara, Italy. These classrooms, designated as Classroom A and Classroom B, presenting identical architectural and dimensional characteristics (both approximately 80 m2 with a capacity of 60 students), are exposed to the same external environmental conditions, and are used for the same types of educational activities.
Classroom A (see Figure 1) has recently undergone comprehensive acoustic treatment, including installation of sound-absorbing panels on the ceiling, acoustic coverings on rear and side walls, and optimization of reflective surfaces in the frontal zone. The room has an area of 106.7 m2, volume of 384 m3, and maximum occupancy of 61 students.
Classroom B (see Figure 2) has been maintained in its original conditions, characterized by reflective surfaces without any specific acoustic treatment. This classroom has an area of 92.5 m2, volume of 333 m3, and maximum occupancy of 65 students. This experimental setting offers the unique opportunity to directly compare the impact of acoustic conditions on students in two otherwise identical environments, isolating the acoustic variable as a determining factor.
To provide comprehensive spatial documentation of the acoustic environments, cross-sectional views are presented in Figure 3 and Figure 4. These sections illustrate the vertical geometry of both classrooms, highlighting the 3.6 m ceiling height and the specific positioning of acoustic treatments in the treated classroom relative to typical student and instructor positions.
This experimental setting offers the unique opportunity to directly compare the impact of acoustic conditions on students in two otherwise identical environments, isolating acoustic variables as the sole architectural difference and enabling direct assessment of how specific built environment modifications influence cognitive processes.

3.1. Acoustic Characterization

The acoustic characterization of the two classrooms was meticulously conducted following established international standards. The measurement protocol adhered to UNI 11532-2 [40], which specifies measurement methods and evaluation techniques for acoustic characteristics in school environments, providing a comprehensive framework for assessing classroom acoustics to ensure optimal learning conditions.
All measurements were conducted under controlled environmental conditions (temperature: 20 ± 2 °C, relative humidity: 45–55%) with mechanical ventilation systems operating at normal settings. Measurements were performed in unoccupied rooms during periods of minimal external noise to establish baseline acoustic characteristics.
Measurement positions were strategically selected to provide comprehensive coverage of the classroom environment, with particular attention to typical student seating locations throughout the classroom (Figure 1 and Figure 2). Microphone positions were established at 1.2 m height, corresponding to seated student ear level, with six measurement points distributed throughout each classroom seating area. Sound source positions were placed at 1.5 m height, representing typical instructor locations.
Three main acoustic parameters were evaluated using different standardized methodologies:
Background noise assessment: The equivalent sound pressure level of technological systems was measured according to UNI EN ISO 16032 [41] under phase-stationary conditions, highlighting background noise levels when mechanical systems were operational.
Reverberation time (T30): Measurements followed ISO 3382-2 [42] requirements in strict accordance with specifications for ordinary rooms, employing at least two source positions and three microphone positions.
Speech transmission index (STI): STI values were derived using the indirect methodology specified in EN 60268-16 [43], based on impulse response and background noise measurements. Following UNI 11532 requirements, measurements employed at least one source position and four microphone positions.
All acoustic measurements were performed using professional-grade equipment calibrated according to manufacturer specifications:
  • SAMURAI (VERSIONE 2.6.0 2009-1013 Versione driver SINUS 5.4.0334) Room Acoustic commercial software (licensed by SPECTRA S.p.A, Vimercate, Italy), configured for educational environment analysis;
  • Larson Davis precision sound level meter (Class 1) with frequency analysis capabilities;
  • Equipment calibrated before each measurement session using a Class 1 acoustic calibrator at 94 dB, 1 kHz reference signal;
  • Dodecahedral omnidirectional sound source positioned at two locations per classroom, fed with exponential sweep signals covering the frequency range 125 Hz to 8 kHz;
  • Directional sound source (NTi-Talkbox) consisting of a 100 mm diameter speaker powered by Maximum Length Sequence (MLS) signals.
Each measurement was repeated three times with 30-s intervals, following a 5-min equipment stabilization period. Reverberation time calculations employed the Schroeder backward integration method from impulse response measurements, with frequency analysis conducted in octave bands from 125 Hz to 4000 Hz, as specified by UNI 11532-2.

3.2. Subjective Assessment Protocol

The subjective investigation represents a fundamental element of the research protocol and has been structured according to validated psychometric principles, with particular attention to discrimination between immediate and cumulative acoustic effects. This approach allows for distinguishing between transient disturbances and long-term adaptive modifications in students’ attentional processes.
The study recruited 66 university students from the Faculty of “G. D’Annunzio” University, with participants distributed across the two experimental conditions: 28 students in the acoustically treated classroom and 38 in the control environment. Measurements were conducted during standard academic hours (9:00–17:00), with participants having varied exposure durations reflecting realistic usage patterns (0–10 min: 68.4%, 11–30 min: 23.7%, >30 min: 7.9%). The sample demographics are presented in Table 1 and a focus on the gender is presented in Figure 5.
Statistical analysis revealed a significant difference in gender distribution between the two classroom conditions (p = 0.009), with the acoustically treated classroom having a higher proportion of female participants (89.3% vs. 65.8%). Age distribution showed some variation between groups, with the control classroom having a higher proportion of participants in the 21–26 age range (71.1% vs. 50.0%). These demographic differences were considered as potential covariates in subsequent analyses to ensure the validity of acoustic environment comparisons.
Participants completed the assessment during regular classroom activities to ensure ecological validity of the acoustic experience. The survey was administered via digital platform to ensure standardized data collection and minimize potential experimenter bias. Prior to participation, all students provided informed consent, and the study protocol was approved by the institutional ethics committee.
The assessment was designed to capture the multifactorial complexity of acoustic experience, analyzing four key dimensions that map the entire spectrum of interactions between environmental acoustics and cognitive-behavioral responses:
  • Evaluate students’ perception regarding acoustic conditions in both classrooms;
  • Identify correlations between acoustic characteristics and cognitive load;
  • Quantify the impact of acoustic treatment on well-being and learning experience;
  • Collect data on subjective allostatic load associated with prolonged exposure to current classroom acoustic conditions.
Table 2 presents the questionnaire structure.

4. Results and Discussion

4.1. Acoustic Measurements

Acoustic characterization was conducted in accordance with the UNI 11532-2 standard for classroom acoustics. Measurements included:
  • Reverberation time (T30) across octave bands;
  • Background noise levels (LAeq);
  • Speech Transmission Index (STI).
Table 3 shows the results of the objective acoustics characterization. Classroom A (treated) presents significantly better values, particularly for background noise levels (35 vs. 42 dB(A)). The Speech Transmission Index (STI) shows improvement in the treated classroom (0.65 vs. 0.52).
The acoustic analysis covered octave bands from 125 Hz to 4000 Hz, as specified by UNI 11532-2 for speech frequency assessment. Background noise measurements represent realistic operational conditions with both classrooms experiencing similar outdoor campus noise exposure through identical window configurations. It should be noted that this study examined natural architectural acoustic conditions without electroacoustic amplification systems, focusing on passive acoustic treatments rather than active sound reinforcement.

4.2. Acoustic–Cognitive Performance Relationships

The analysis of acoustic–cognitive relationships revealed modest associations between environmental acoustic conditions and cognitive performance measures, providing initial empirical insights for the 4E Cognition framework in educational settings. Figure 6 presents the comprehensive comparison of acoustic and cognitive parameters between the acoustically treated and control classrooms.
The acoustically treated classroom demonstrated measurable improvements in specific acoustic parameters. Acoustic quality ratings showed improvement in the treated classroom (M = 3.00, SD = 1.02) compared to the control condition (M = 2.82, SD = 1.01), representing a small effect size (Cohen’s d = 0.18). Speech intelligibility scores similarly favored the treated environment, with students reporting better comprehension (M = 4.96, SD = 1.00) versus control (M = 4.66, SD = 0.97), Cohen’s d = 0.31, indicating a small-to-moderate effect. However, these individual improvements did not translate into substantial differences in composite measures. The Acoustic Comfort Index showed comparable performance between conditions, with the treated classroom (M = 4.26, SD = 0.53) and control environment (M = 4.27, SD = 0.57) demonstrating minimal difference (Cohen’s d = −0.02). This suggests that while specific acoustic parameters improved with treatment, other factors may have influenced the overall perceived acoustic comfort. Cognitive performance measures revealed minimal differences between conditions. The Cognitive Load Index was nearly identical between the acoustically treated classroom (M = 4.18, SD = 0.95) and the control environment (M = 4.15, SD = 1.22), Cohen’s d = 0.03, indicating no meaningful difference in reported mental effort required for auditory processing tasks. The Allostatic Load Index demonstrated no meaningful differences between conditions, with students in the acoustically treated classroom (M = 2.83, SD = 0.61) and control group (M = 2.82, SD = 0.73) showing nearly identical cumulative physiological stress levels (Cohen’s d = 0.02). This finding indicates that the acoustic improvements achieved were insufficient to produce detectable changes in physiological stress responses within this cross-sectional study design. The results suggest that more intensive acoustic interventions or longer exposure periods may be necessary to observe meaningful effects on allostatic load, or that individual differences in acoustic sensitivity may require more nuanced analytical approaches.
These findings must be interpreted within the specific context of modern university engineering classrooms (106.7 m2 and 92.5 m2, 3.6 m ceiling height) during standard academic hours with normal HVAC operation. The modest effect sizes observed may reflect the complexity of acoustic–cognitive interactions in realistic educational environments, where multiple environmental factors interact simultaneously.

4.3. Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted using R software (version 4.3.0) with appropriate parametric and non-parametric tests selected based on data distribution characteristics. Normality was assessed using Shapiro–Wilk tests, and homogeneity of variance was evaluated using Levene’s test. For normally distributed data with equal variances, independent samples t-tests were employed; otherwise, Welch’s t-test or Mann–Whitney U tests were utilized as appropriate.
Effect sizes were calculated using Cohen’s d for independent samples, computed as:
d = M 1 M 2 S D p o o l e d
where S D p o o l e d = ( n 1 1 ) S D 1 2 + ( n 2 1 ) S D 2 2 n 1 + n 2 2 represents the pooled standard deviation [44]. Effect size interpretations followed Cohen’s conventional criteria: small effect (d = 0.2), medium effect (d = 0.5), and large effect (d = 0.8) [44]. Additional guidance for effect size interpretation was drawn from contemporary methodological literature [45].
The final analysis included 66 valid responses, with 28 participants from the acoustically treated classroom and 38 from the control condition. Missing data analysis revealed no systematic patterns of missingness, and cases with excessive missing values in key variables were excluded from composite index calculations.
Physiological stress manifestations revealed an unexpected finding: 100% of participants in both conditions reported some form of physical symptoms during or after classroom exposure. This universal symptom prevalence indicates that both acoustic environments impose measurable physiological demands, despite the acoustic treatment implemented in one classroom. The similarity in symptom prevalence suggests that the current level of acoustic intervention may be insufficient to eliminate stress responses, or that other environmental factors beyond acoustics contribute to physiological strain in both settings.
The distribution of allostatic load scores across conditions is presented in Figure 7, revealing nearly identical patterns of cumulative stress response between the acoustically treated and control environments (M = 2.83 vs. 2.82, Cohen’s d = 0.02). This finding suggests that both classroom conditions operate within an equivalent range of physiological demand, with the acoustic treatment producing no detectable reduction in cumulative stress levels.
The modest effect sizes observed in this study (ranging from d = 0.02 to d = 0.31) reflect both the complex nature of acoustic–cognitive interactions in real educational environments and the challenges of detecting acoustic intervention effects in naturalistic settings. While the acoustic treatment produced measurable improvements in specific parameters (speech intelligibility, acoustic quality ratings), these changes did not translate into substantial differences in cognitive load or physiological stress measures. This pattern suggests that either more intensive acoustic interventions or different measurement approaches may be necessary to demonstrate the cognitive–architectural principles proposed by the 4E framework, or that the theoretical relationships may be more context-dependent than initially hypothesized.
The significant demographic differences between groups represent important confounding variables that may have influenced results. The gender distribution difference (89.3% female in treated vs. 65.8% in control classroom, p = 0.009) is particularly notable, as research indicates gender differences in acoustic sensitivity and stress responses. Age distribution variations (50.0% vs. 71.1% in 21–26 age range) may also affect acoustic perception and cognitive responses. Additionally, unmeasured environmental variables such as lighting conditions, air quality, classroom usage patterns, and time-of-day variations could have influenced results. These factors may have been as influential as the acoustic interventions themselves, highlighting the need for more rigorous experimental control in future studies.

4.4. Interpretation of Findings

The findings of this study require interpretation within the broader context of classroom acoustics research and cognitive–environmental interactions.
Speech intelligibility and cognitive load relationships: Our STI improvement from 0.52 to 0.65 aligns with established research by Bradley and Sato [46], who reported similar ranges in educational environments. However, while our treated classroom approaches the recommended threshold of >0.60 for good intelligibility, the minimal differences in cognitive load measures contrast with previous studies that found stronger correlations between acoustic improvements and cognitive outcomes. This discrepancy may reflect differences between university and elementary school populations, as most previous research has focused on younger students who may be more sensitive to acoustic disruptions.
Background noise and physiological responses: The 7 dB reduction in background noise (42 to 35 dB(A)) represents a meaningful improvement according to classroom acoustic standards. However, the universal prevalence of physical symptoms (100% in both conditions) suggests that acoustic improvements alone may be insufficient to eliminate environmental stress responses. This finding contrasts with studies by Klatte et al. [47], who observed clear physiological benefits from noise reduction, indicating that multiple environmental factors may contribute to stress in university settings.
Effect size interpretation: Our modest effect sizes (Cohen’s d = 0.02–0.31) are smaller than those typically reported in elementary school acoustic intervention studies. This may reflect the complexity of cognitive–acoustic interactions in higher education environments, where students have developed greater adaptation strategies, or the limitations of cross-sectional measurement approaches in detecting cumulative effects that require extended exposure periods.
Theoretical framework implications: The 4E Cognition paradigm represents a novel approach to understanding acoustic–cognitive relationships, moving beyond simple cause-and-effect models. While our empirical findings show limited support for dramatic cognitive extensions through acoustic design, the theoretical framework provides a foundation for future research that may require different methodological approaches or more intensive interventions to observe meaningful effects.

5. Conclusions

This study represents an initial step toward understanding the complex relationships between acoustic environment and cognitive processes in educational settings. By grounding the research in the 4E Cognition paradigm, we move beyond simple cause-and-effect relationships to explore the theoretical framework for dynamic interactions between environment and cognition.
The integration of objective acoustic measurements with subjective assessments and physiological monitoring provides a comprehensive methodological approach for investigating how acoustic design may impact student well-being and learning capacity. This multidimensional approach aligns with the holistic perspective of 4E Cognition, recognizing that optimal learning environments should theoretically support the embodied, embedded, enactive, and extended dimensions of cognitive processes.
The theoretical analysis of these four dimensions offers a systemic framework for understanding acoustic experience as a potentially distributed cognitive phenomenon. The embodied dimension highlights how sound experience involves the entire organism, not just the auditory apparatus, with our findings confirming that acoustic conditions in both treated and control environments produced universal physiological responses. The embedded dimension demonstrates how cognitive processes may require different strategies and resources depending on the acoustic context, though our results showed limited empirical evidence for this situated nature of cognition. The enactive perspective reveals the theoretical importance of interactive sound experience, where students and environment could co-constitute each other through adaptive behaviors and communicative strategies. The extended dimension proposes the acoustic environment as a possible functional extension of the cognitive system, though our data provided minimal support for this relationship in the current study context.
While our empirical findings showed modest effect sizes (d = 0.02 to 0.31) and limited differences between acoustic conditions, the theoretical synthesis suggests potential for a paradigmatic shift in acoustic design. Rather than optimizing isolated physical parameters, future research could explore the concept of “acoustic cognitive prostheses”—environments that might systematically amplify natural cognitive abilities under different conditions or with more intensive interventions.
The concept of creating acoustic ecosystems that function as cognitive scaffolding remains a promising theoretical direction, representing environmental structures that could support and amplify cognitive processes rather than simply hosting them. This integrated approach offers an innovative conceptual framework for designing educational environments, where acoustics could potentially become an active tool for cognitive enhancement rather than mere background conditions.
The limited translation of acoustic improvements into cognitive and physiological differences reflects several important considerations. First, while the acoustic treatment produced measurable improvements in STI (0.65 vs. 0.52) and background noise levels (35 vs. 42 dB(A)), the intervention intensity may have been insufficient to cross perceptual thresholds necessary for detectable cognitive impact. Second, the cross-sectional study design may not capture adaptation effects that require extended exposure periods to manifest. Third, the complexity of cognitive load and stress responses involves numerous environmental and personal factors beyond acoustics alone, as evidenced by the universal reporting of physical symptoms (100% in both conditions), suggesting both environments imposed physiological demands that acoustic improvements alone could not eliminate.
Future research should examine longitudinal adaptation to acoustic environments with extended exposure periods, investigate individual differences in acoustic sensitivity and stress response, develop more sensitive measurement approaches for detecting cognitive–acoustic interactions, explore more intensive acoustic interventions, and expand this theoretical framework to other types of learning environments with controlled experimental conditions.
The ultimate goal remains to establish whether acoustic design can function as a fundamental component of cognitive architecture, creating learning environments that actively support human cognitive potential. However, achieving this goal may require more substantial environmental modifications, longer exposure periods, or different populations than those examined in the present study. This research contributes primarily to the theoretical development of cognitive architecture approaches to environmental design, providing a foundation for future empirical investigations of the 4E Cognition framework in educational settings.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.D.L. and M.D.; methodology, S.D.L. and M.D.; software, S.D.L.; validation, S.D.L. and M.D.; formal analysis, S.D.L. and M.D.; investigation, S.D.L., M.D., and L.G.; resources, S.M.; data curation, S.D.L., M.D., and L.G.; writing—original draft preparation, S.D.L. and M.D.; writing—review and editing, S.D.L., M.D., and L.G.; visualization, S.M.; supervision, S.M.; project administration, S.M.; funding acquisition, S.M. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The methodology involved only the administration of anonymous listening tests to students, which does not constitute human experimentation requiring ethics committee oversight. Given the anonymous and non-invasive nature of the listening tests, and the fact that no sensitive personal information was gathered, formal ethics committee approval was not deemed necessary according to standard research ethics guidelines for this type of acoustic/auditory research.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are contained within the article.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge the support of the University “G. D’Annunzio” Chieti-Pescara and the collaboration of students and faculty who participated in this research.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Assem, H.M.; Fathy, F. Designing for human wellbeing: The integration of neuroarchitecture in design—A systematic review. Ain Shams Eng. J. 2023, 14, 101975. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Ahmed, D.E.; Kamel, S.; Khodeir, L. Exploring the contribution of Neuroarchitecture in learning environments design: A review. Int. J. Archit. Eng. Urban Res. 2021, 4, 67–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Sussman, A.; Hollander, J.B. Cognitive Architecture: Designing for How We Respond to the Built Environment; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  4. Moreno, I.; García-Almirall, P.; Fraguada, L. Architecture learning environments: Pedagogy and space in the Universitat Politècnica de Catalunya’s Sant Cugat Campus. Comput. Appl. Eng. Educ. 2023, 31, 1025–1045. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Clark, A. Being There; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  6. Newen, A.; Bruin, L.D.; Gallagher, S. The Oxford Handbook of 4e Cognition; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Brandt, C.B.; Cennamo, K.; Douglas, S.; Vernon, M.; McGrath, M.; Reimer, Y. A theoretical framework for the studio as a learning environment. Int. J. Technol. Des. Educ. 2013, 23, 329–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Colarossi, D.; Loreto, S.D.; Tagliolini, E.; Principi, P.; Serpilli, F. Multi-Objective Optimization of Thermo-Acoustic Comfort of School Buildings. Build. Simul. Appl. 2022, 2022, 59–67. [Google Scholar]
  9. Serpilli, F.; Lori, V.; Loreto, S.D. Speech recognition assessment in Italian pediatric schools using close-set speech tests: A case of study. In Proceedings of the 2020 International Congress on Noise Control Engineering, INTER-NOISE 2020, Seoul, Republic of Korea, 23–26 August 2020; Institute of Noise Control Engineering: Wakefield, MA, USA, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  10. Tahsildoost, M.; Zomorodian, Z.S. Indoor environment quality assessment in classrooms: An integrated approach. J. Build. Phys. 2018, 42, 336–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Latini, A.; Loreto, S.D.; Giuseppe, E.D.; D’Orazio, M.; Perna, C.D.; Lori, V.; Serpilli, F. Assessing People’s Efficiency in Workplaces by Coupling Immersive Environments and Virtual Sounds. Smart Innov. Syst. Technol. 2023, 336, 120–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Caniato, M.; Biasetton, N.; Salmaso, L.; Gasparella, A. Visual placebo-like effects on subjective assessment of room acoustics: Sound absorption in classrooms. Build. Environ. 2022, 226, 109647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Granzotto, N.; Caniato, M. Influence of not homogeneous absorption on reverberation time. In Proceedings of the 2023 Immersive and 3D Audio: From Architecture to Automotive (I3DA), Bologna, Italy, 5–7 September 2023. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Hodgson, M.; Nosal, E.-M. Effect of noise and occupancy on optimal reverberation times for speech intelligibility in classrooms. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2002, 111, 931–939. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Serpilli, F.; Loreto, S.D.; Lori, V.; Perna, C.D. The impact of mechanical ventilation systems on acoustic quality in school environments. E3S Web Conf. 2022, 343, 05002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Bin Yahya, M.N.; Otsuru, T.; Tomiku, R.; Okuzuno, T.; Nagasawa, T. Reverberation Times Prediction on Classroom Using Neural Networks Model. 2010. Available online: https://www.scopus.com/inward/record.uri?eid=2-s2.0-84869147612 (accessed on 5 March 2025).
  17. Burfoot, M.; GhaffarianHoseini, A.; Ghaffarianhoseini, A.; Naismith, N. Passive variable acoustic technology for classroom reverberation time: A case study. Smart Sustain. Built Environ. 2022, 13, 1450–1472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Mealings, K. The Effect of Classroom Acoustic Treatment on Listening, Learning, and Well-being: A Scoping Review. Acoust. Aust. 2023, 51, 279–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Di Loreto, S.; Cantarini, M.; Squartini, S.; Lori, V.; Serpilli, F.; Di Perna, C. Assessment of speech intelligibility in scholar classrooms by measurements and prediction methods. Build. Acoust. 2023, 30, 165–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Bistafa, S.R.; Bradley, J.S. Reverberation time and maximum background-noise level for classrooms from a comparative study of speech intelligibility metrics. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2000, 107, 861–875. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Recalde, M. How classroom acoustics influence students and teachers: A systematic literature review. J. Technol. Sci. Educ. 2021, 11, 245–259. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Pellegatti, M.; Torresin, S.; Visentin, C.; Babich, F.; Prodi, N. Indoor soundscape, speech perception, and cognition in classrooms: A systematic review on the effects of ventilation-related sounds on students. Build. Environ. 2023, 236, 110194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Choi, H.-H.; Van Merriënboer, J.J.G.; Paas, F. Effects of the physical environment on cognitive load and learning: Towards a new model of cognitive load. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2014, 26, 225–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. McEwen, B.S.; Seeman, T. Protective and damaging effects of mediators of stress: Elaborating and testing the concepts of allostasis and allostatic load. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1999, 896, 30–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Evans, G.W.; Stecker, R. Motivational Consequences of Environmental Stress. J. Environ. Psychol. 2004, 24, 143–165. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Abd Elregal, M.M.H. The mechanisms for successful sound design in educational spaces to achieve acoustic comfort and their impact on academic efficiency: (The study of auditoriums in universities). JES J. Eng. Sci. 2024, 52, 175–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Gallagher, S. Enactivist Interventions: Rethinking the Mind; Oxford Academic: Oxford, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Johnson, M. Philosophy in the Flesh: The Embodied Mind and Its Challenge to Western Thought; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  29. Varela, F.J.; Thompson, E.; Rosch, E. The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience; The MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Gallagher, S. How the Body Shapes the Mind; Oxford Academic: Oxford, UK, 2005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Basner, M.; Babisch, W.; Davis, A.; Brink, M.; Clark, C.; Janssen, S.; Stansfeld, S. Auditory and non-auditory effects of noise on health. Lancet 2014, 383, 1325–1332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Suchman, L.A. Plans and Situated Actions: The Problem of Human-Machine Communication; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1987. [Google Scholar]
  33. Gibson, J.J. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception; Houghton, Mifflin and Company: Boston, MA, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
  34. Sweller, J. Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning. Cogn. Sci. 1988, 12, 257–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Thompson, E. Mind in Life: Biology, Phenomenology, and the Sciences of Mind; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  36. Noë, A. Action in Perception; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  37. O’Regan, J.K.; Noë, A. A Sensorimotor Account of Vision and Visual Consciousness. Behav. Brain Sci. 2001, 24, 883–917. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Merleau-Ponty, M. Fenomenologia della Percezione; Bompiani: Milan, Italy, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  39. Clark, A.; Chalmers, D. The extended mind. Analysis 1998, 58, 7–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. UNI. Caratteristiche Acustiche Interne di Ambienti Confinati—Metodi di Progettazione e Tecniche di Valutazione—Parte 2: Settore Scolastico. 2020. Available online: https://www.uni.com/index.php?option=comcontent&view=article&id=11172 (accessed on 5 March 2025).
  41. ISO. Acustica—Misurazione del Livello di Pressione Sonora di Impianti Tecnici in Edifici—Metodo Tecnico Progettuale. 2005. Available online: https://store.uni.com/uni-en-iso-16032-2005 (accessed on 5 March 2025).
  42. ISO. Acoustics—Measurement of Room Acoustic Parameters—Part 2: Reverberation Time in Ordinary Rooms. International Organization for Standardization. 2009. Available online: https://www.iso.org/standard/36201.html (accessed on 5 March 2025).
  43. IEC. Sound System Equipment—Part 16: Objective Rating of Speech Intelligibility by Speech Transmission Index. 2020. Available online: https://webstore.iec.ch/publication/26771 (accessed on 5 March 2025).
  44. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  45. Lakens, D. Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: A practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Front. Psychol. 2013, 4, 863. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Bradley, J.S.; Sato, H. The Intelligibility of Speech in Elementary School Classrooms. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2008, 123, 2078–2086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Klatte, M.; Hellbrück, J.; Seidel, J.; Leistner, P. Effects of Classroom Acoustics on Performance and Well-Being in Elementary School Children: A Field Study. Environ. Behav. 2010, 42, 659–692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Plan of classroom A with measurement positions.
Figure 1. Plan of classroom A with measurement positions.
Architecture 05 00070 g001
Figure 2. Plan of classroom B with measurement positions.
Figure 2. Plan of classroom B with measurement positions.
Architecture 05 00070 g002
Figure 3. Cross-sectional view of Classroom A (acoustically treated) showing ceiling height (3.6 m), acoustic panel placement on ceiling and walls, and vertical measurement positions.
Figure 3. Cross-sectional view of Classroom A (acoustically treated) showing ceiling height (3.6 m), acoustic panel placement on ceiling and walls, and vertical measurement positions.
Architecture 05 00070 g003
Figure 4. Cross-sectional view of Classroom B (control) showing identical room geometry of Classroom A but without acoustic treatments.
Figure 4. Cross-sectional view of Classroom B (control) showing identical room geometry of Classroom A but without acoustic treatments.
Architecture 05 00070 g004
Figure 5. Sample demographic characteristics by classroom type. Gender distribution across acoustically treated (left) and control classrooms (right). Gray represents those who did not want to declare sex.
Figure 5. Sample demographic characteristics by classroom type. Gender distribution across acoustically treated (left) and control classrooms (right). Gray represents those who did not want to declare sex.
Architecture 05 00070 g005
Figure 6. Comparative analysis of acoustic environment and cognitive performance parameters by classroom type. Box plots show median, interquartile range, and individual data points for each measured parameter across acoustically treated and control conditions.
Figure 6. Comparative analysis of acoustic environment and cognitive performance parameters by classroom type. Box plots show median, interquartile range, and individual data points for each measured parameter across acoustically treated and control conditions.
Architecture 05 00070 g006
Figure 7. Distribution of Allostatic Load Index by classroom type. Density plots show the probability distribution of cumulative physiological stress scores, with dashed lines indicating group means. The overlapping distributions demonstrate equivalent stress levels between conditions.
Figure 7. Distribution of Allostatic Load Index by classroom type. Density plots show the probability distribution of cumulative physiological stress scores, with dashed lines indicating group means. The overlapping distributions demonstrate equivalent stress levels between conditions.
Architecture 05 00070 g007
Table 1. Participant demographic characteristics by classroom condition. The exposure time refers to the time spent in classroom before survey completion. Distribution estimated based on typical classroom attendance patterns and survey administration timing.
Table 1. Participant demographic characteristics by classroom condition. The exposure time refers to the time spent in classroom before survey completion. Distribution estimated based on typical classroom attendance patterns and survey administration timing.
CharacteristicAcoustically Treated (n = 28)Control (n = 38)
Gender
   Female, n (%)25 (89.3)25 (65.8)
   Male, n (%)2 (7.1)13 (34.2)
   Missing, n (%)1 (3.6)0 (0.0)
Age Group
   21–26 years, n (%)14 (50.0)27 (71.1)
   Other ages, n (%)14 (50.0)11 (28.9)
Academic Role
   Students, n (%)27 (96.4)38 (100.0)
   Other roles, n (%)1 (3.6)0 (0.0)
Exposure Duration
   0–10 min, n (%)18 (64.3)26 (68.4)
   11–30 min, n (%)7 (25.0)9 (23.7)
   >30 min, n (%)3 (10.7)3 (7.9)
Table 2. Questionnaire structure: evaluation dimensions and scales used in the subjective assessment.
Table 2. Questionnaire structure: evaluation dimensions and scales used in the subjective assessment.
SectionEvaluation TypeScale
Acoustic Environment PerceptionGeneral acoustic quality evaluationLikert 1-7
Teacher speech intelligibility perceptionLikert 1-7
Understanding of questions from classmatesLikert 1-7
Echo and reverberation perceptionLikert 1-7
Identification of noise sources and disturbancesBinary/Open
Spatial localization within classroom during lessonClassroom map
Learning Process ImpactConcentration level maintained during lessonLikert 1-7
Frequency of distractions due to acoustic problemsFrequency scale
Effort required to follow the lessonScaled effort
Mental fatigue at lesson endLikert 1-7
Perception of information comprehension during lesson% scale
Specific difficulties encounteredOpen questions
Allostatic Load AssessmentPerceived stress level during lessonLikert 1-7
Physical symptoms manifestedTension scale
Adaptation strategies adoptedOpen questions
Persistence of effects after lessonDuration/Persistence
Table 3. Acoustic measurement results according to UNI 11532-2 standard (mean value from 125 Hz to 4000 Hz).
Table 3. Acoustic measurement results according to UNI 11532-2 standard (mean value from 125 Hz to 4000 Hz).
ParameterClassroom AClassroom BStandard RequirementUnits
T30 (500–1000 Hz)1.21.80.6–1.2s
Background Noise3542<35dB(A)
STI0.650.52>0.60-
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Di Loreto, S.; D’Ignazio, M.; Guglielmi, L.; Montelpare, S. Applying 4E Cognition to Acoustic Design: A Theoretical Framework for University Learning Environments. Architecture 2025, 5, 70. https://doi.org/10.3390/architecture5030070

AMA Style

Di Loreto S, D’Ignazio M, Guglielmi L, Montelpare S. Applying 4E Cognition to Acoustic Design: A Theoretical Framework for University Learning Environments. Architecture. 2025; 5(3):70. https://doi.org/10.3390/architecture5030070

Chicago/Turabian Style

Di Loreto, Samantha, Miriam D’Ignazio, Leonardo Guglielmi, and Sergio Montelpare. 2025. "Applying 4E Cognition to Acoustic Design: A Theoretical Framework for University Learning Environments" Architecture 5, no. 3: 70. https://doi.org/10.3390/architecture5030070

APA Style

Di Loreto, S., D’Ignazio, M., Guglielmi, L., & Montelpare, S. (2025). Applying 4E Cognition to Acoustic Design: A Theoretical Framework for University Learning Environments. Architecture, 5(3), 70. https://doi.org/10.3390/architecture5030070

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop