1. Introduction
Screen-based advertising to children has exploded over the past decade, with child audiences routinely exposed to branded content across platforms including YouTube, TikTok, social media, educational websites, and mobile apps [
1,
2]. For instance, in 2024, according to a Pew Research Center survey, 90% of American teenagers use YouTube, and roughly 60% use TikTok on a frequent basis [
3,
4]. These sites generally insert branded communications or advertisements—e.g., advergames—into games and videos, employing persuader design elements like algorithmic personalization, gamification, narrative storytelling, and emotional appeals to capture attention and influence behavior. Thus, online advertising has become a ubiquitous aspect of children’s daily media lives. Studies show that children are exposed to a high volume of advertisements in a year and that many online platforms have a number of advertisements in a single video [
5,
6]. This ubiquitous integration ensures that advertising is no longer relegated to the sole preserve of conventional commercial breaks but is integrated into the digital spaces that children use on a daily basis. In such a case, recognizable online environments and characters tend to erode the boundaries between entertainment and advertising. Marketers today speak of the “digital marketing ecosystem” as a networked, data-driven space within which children are simultaneously targets and influencers, wielding purchase power and informing peer and parent purchases. This has developed at a pace that has outstripped conventional regulatory approaches, fueling anxiety regarding children’s exposure to influence technologies and algorithmic targeting [
5,
6]. Developmental psychology emphasizes that children in middle school have a limited ability to understand and counter persuasive intent. Advertising literacy, including the ability to identify advertisements, understand their sponsorship, and understand their selling intent, is acquired gradually throughout childhood. Young children cannot differentiate between content and embedded advertisements, and even older children might not fully grasp advertisers’ tactics. A systematic review identified that persuasion awareness and advertising literacy are not yet well developed until adolescence. This phase of development means that children do not have the critical thinking ability and executive function to knowingly undo marketing attempts. Even in older children who are able to recognize content as advertising, they might still be vulnerable to appealing content. Additionally, impulse control and self-control are still developing in children, so emotionally appealing or gamified advertising is especially persuasive. Traditional developmental theories indicate that it is only at 8–12 years that children start to comprehend advertising intent, with many of the subtleties not being grasped until older ages [
7,
8,
9]. These are issues that are reflected in more current research; for instance, it is found that children between the ages of 9 and 11 can very likely identify food advertising in videos but still react positively since they like the content. This research emphasizes that awareness of an advert does not necessarily entail resistance [
2,
7]. With this context in mind, the current study is an examination of how middle school students view neuromarketing methods and television advertising within their online contexts. Particular focus is placed on how knowledge, interest, and ethical issues related to neuromarketing relate to advertising literacy, understood as persuasion knowledge and critical thinking, and how these, in turn, impact students’ behavioral intentions. By incorporating insights from developmental psychology, media literacy, and neuromarketing, the research seeks to offer empirical insights into children’s cognitive and emotional attitudes toward algorithmically driven persuasion.
The moral implications of this susceptibility are profound. Children’s developing competencies have particular safeguards necessary in marketing contexts. Children comprise a legally and ethically susceptible group, with a right to privacy and protection against exploitation [
1,
8]. In practice, nevertheless, the online marketing context habitually exploits children’s personal information and cognitive inexperience. Research suggests that most children’s websites and apps gather and disclose personal data—names, video, location, and use—without express consent. In one examination of child-targeted websites, for example, most gathered personal data, and a large percentage of those sites passed it along to third parties. This extensive data collection raises concerns about manipulative profiling and targeted persuasion [
2,
9].
Acknowledging these concerns, regulators have taken steps: in the United States, the Children’s Online Privacy Protection Act (COPPA) limits data collection from children under 13 without parental consent, and the EU’s General Data Protection Regulation (GDPR) provides protection up to the age of 16, requiring child-friendly notices and additional restrictions, including a ban on profiling children [
10]. Loopholes do exist, however, especially for teenagers, and some self-regulatory codes still allow data use with little supervision. Overall, online marketing to children is riddled with difficult ethical issues in manipulation, privacy, and fairness that require thoughtful attention.
Further complicating this environment is the advent of neuromarketing—the use of neuroscience instrumentation to investigate consumer decision-making processes [
11]. Neuromarketing uses methods like functional MRI, EEG, eye-tracking, and biometric feedback to gauge consumers’ neural and physiological reactions to ads and brands. By quantifying stimuli that attract subconscious attention or emotional connection, marketers can craft content to be most effective [
8,
12,
13]. Internet-based neuromarketing is formulated in terms of algorithmic personalization and targeted advertisement designs drawing from a knowledge base of what activates primal reward processes. Advertisements, for instance, can be tested for their ability to generate emotional arousal or brand association at the neural level. While supporters of neuromarketing argue that it broadens the scope of consumer behavior knowledge, these alternatives have ethical connotations when directed toward vulnerable populations [
14,
15].
Children’s distinctive neurocognitive profiles add to these worries. Although empirical neuromarketing research has largely been attentive to adults, preliminary evidence indicates that children’s brains are extremely sensitive to branding [
14,
15]. For example, in an fMRI study of children in elementary school, it was discovered that exposure to known food logos stimulated reward-associated brain areas, and it was a sign of the excessive influence of branding on the child brain. In spite of these findings, systematic research on children’s perception or resistance of sophisticated marketing techniques is limited. Scholars have urged more research on children’s neural and psychological reactions to marketing, considering its ubiquity. In brief, the development of neuromarketing techniques provides advertisers with unprecedented control, yet children have minimal defenses, so empirical insight into how school-aged children perceive or react to these unnoticed digital prompts to persuasion is essential [
11,
14,
16].
Collectively, the literature reveals an evident gap. Although there is evidence to indicate that children are exposed to a deluge of screen-based advertisements and have emergent, and often limited, persuasion knowledge, there has been minimal research into middle school students’ understandings of contemporary persuasive strategies. In particular, children’s understanding of personalized or game-based advertisements presented on digital devices, and the degree to which they are aware of the marketing tactics used, is unknown. Moreover, most of the studies have been carried out in home or clinical environments, and little has been conducted in school environments [
1,
2,
8]. In a school environment, educational website adverts or ads inserted into free online computer games can pose some specific challenges. As yet, there is no empirical research investigating children’s attitudes towards neuromarketing or the effects of exposure to screen advertising in a school environment on their enjoyment of persuasion. This literature gap means that important questions regarding children’s perceptions and media literacy development in the current digital landscape remain unanswered [
1,
2,
8].
Building on this research gap, the present study investigates how middle school children perceive and react to AVPs based on the use of neuroadvertising and neuromarketing strategies in the educational context. More specifically, this study examines the connections between students’ advertising awareness (i.e., advertising recognition and understanding), interest in digital marketing content, and self-reported exposure to screen-based advertising and levels of their persuasion knowledge, advertising skepticism, and purchase intentions. This research also seeks to establish whether learners with more exposure to, or greater interest in, digital advertisements have greater advertising literacy and better critical evaluation skills, and, subsequently, are more desirous of the advertised items.
Accordingly, this study is guided by the following overarching research questions:
RQ1: How do adolescents perceive and respond to neuromarketing-based and screen-based persuasive strategies in digital media environments?
RQ2: What cognitive (e.g., persuasion knowledge) and attitudinal (e.g., advertising skepticism) factors mediate the effects of digital advertising exposure on purchase intentions?
These questions are examined through a structural equation model, operationalized by the specific hypotheses developed in
Section 2.
The findings supported the majority of the postulated associations. Knowledge and Awareness of Neuromarketing (NAK) and Exposure to Screen-Based Advertising (SBA) were significant predictors of Purchase Intentions (PIs), and Interest in Neuromarketing (NEI) had no immediate effect. Persuasion Knowledge (PK) and Advertising Skepticism (AS) were both positive drivers of PIs. Mediational analyses indicated that PK and AS partially mediated NAK and SBA effects on PIs, and NEI only had an indirect effect on PIs through AS. Multi-group analyses confirmed strong age-based, past neuromarketing exposure-based, ad-click-based, and advertising literacy education-based moderating effects. Young adolescents were more cognitively resistant (e.g., NAK on PK), but older adolescents were more behaviorally responsive to online exposure (e.g., SBA on PI). The non-educated and unaware students were more susceptible to influence, but the students with prior media literacy training were better equipped with persuasion knowledge and skepticism. These results validate the intricate interaction between cognitive, emotional, and developmental processes employed in adolescents’ use of persuasive digital media.
The rest of the paper proceeds as follows:
Section 2 outlines neuromarketing and advertising literacy research.
Section 3 introduces the conceptual model and hypotheses.
Section 4 describes the methodology and data collection as well as analysis techniques.
Section 5 displays SEM results, including direct, mediating, and group-specific effects.
Section 6 presents practical implications.
Section 7 concludes with the main findings and suggestions for future work.
5. Discussion
This study investigated how middle school students’ exposure to screen-based advertising and neuromarketing-related experiences influence their purchase intentions through cognitive and attitudinal mechanisms [
43,
70,
71]. The model integrated direct and mediated effects of Neuromarketing Awareness and Knowledge (NAK), Screen-Based Advertising Exposure (SBA), and Interest in Neuromarketing (NEI) on Purchase Intentions (PIs), with Persuasion Knowledge (PK) and Advertising Skepticism (AS) serving as mediators. Multi-group analyses further explored how age, digital behavior, educational exposure, and prior awareness moderated these relationships.
5.1. Direct Effect Results
The most significant direct impact was from Neuromarketing Awareness and Knowledge (NAK) to Purchase Intentions (PIs), reinforcing H1. This result expresses a paradoxical relationship in persuasion literacy: even though increased awareness of marketing strategies is intended to encourage resistance, it also improves perceived message relevance, particularly when adolescents receive the same messages as individualized or sophisticated. This would be consistent with earlier research showing that conceptual advertising literacy, or the ability to decode the motivation and strategy of advertising, will not, in itself, be followed by a resistance to arguments [
4,
22,
28,
45]. Especially in the case of digital and algorithmic personalization, students might perceive messages influenced by neuromarketing as credible, targeted, and relevant. That is, the detection of persuasive intent will not induce resistance but instead create a feeling of being personalized that further adds to the appeal and persuasiveness of the message [
41,
44,
70,
72].
Likewise, Screen-Based Advertising Exposure (SBA) had a positive influence on Purchase Intentions, although the effect size was smaller (H2 supported). This result accords with the mere exposure effect [
72], since repeated exposure to a stimulus leads to more intense liking and familiarity independent of conscious awareness. For teenagers deeply engaged in screen-based cultures, repeated exposure to digital advertisements, be it on YouTube, video games, or social media, has the potential to legitimize and normalize consumer messages. Past research by [
9,
43,
45] has found that the repeated viewing of branded content as part of programming leads to recalling and liking the brand name, particularly in the absence of or failure of disclosures. Therefore, although SBA is a less active mode of interaction than NAK, it also has an effect on buying behavior, possibly by way of heuristic processing and conditioning with repetition.
Conversely, Interest and Engagement with Neuromarketing (NEI) did not strongly directly impact PIs; H3 was not confirmed. This result indicates that basic curiosity or interest in neuromarketing—without the co-present conceptual knowledge—cannot be considered to be equivalent to behavioral intention. This would make it possible to have a dual-processing account of advertising effects: while elaboration cognition (e.g., NAK) has the potential to increase message receptivity by perceived relevance, affective involvement without thinking (e.g., NEI) might be insufficient on the basis of knowledge to affect intention [
8,
12,
13,
15,
17]. It also emphasizes that advertising literacy is not homogeneous; affective involvement itself might not neutralize or augment persuasion unless it has more understanding of it. In addition, the conclusions of the present study are consistent with recent consumer behavior studies highlighting that youth purchase intentions are not only driven by message exposure but also by emotional responses, product perceived quality, and social influence. For example, current research shows that while product features, price, and reputation largely drive smartphone buying intent at uncertain times such as the COVID-19 pandemic, social influence does not necessarily appear as an overriding force [
73]. In the same way, emotionally engaging content in electronic word-of-mouth (eWOM) contexts enhances perceived quality and lowers purchase risk, consequently strengthening purchase intentions—though cognitive involvement (e.g., brand trust) is less significant [
74]. Online opinion ratings and influencer credibility, as opposed to appeal, were stronger predictors of purchase intentions in beauty product contexts among young consumers [
75]. These findings address our research by further confirming that the consumption reactions of adolescents are not merely cognitive processes but also experienced as a function of relevance, affective congruence, and trust of context, especially in virtual spaces where advertising surrounds them and is interactive.
The structural model also showed that Persuasion Knowledge (PK) and Advertising Skepticism (AS) both positively influenced Purchase Intentions (supporting H4a and H4b). Although existing literature routinely assumes PK and AS to be cognitive defenses [
41,
44,
70], the direction of influences in the current study suggests a more involved development process. Instead of being obstacles to persuasion, PK and AS can be favorable to the persuasive effect when ads are understood by teens as normative, funny, or socially attractive. Teens with higher persuasion knowledge perhaps consider themselves more intelligent consumers and might utilize their judgment to respond selectively to advertising. Likewise, skeptics will still be purchased if the message is found to be entertaining or normative among peers, an age-dependent balance of resistance and acceptance [
11,
33,
34].
Cumulatively, these results demonstrate that knowledge of persuasive techniques (NAK) and familiarity with Internet-based advertisement (SBA) enhance students’ sensitivity in behavior, as long as interest accompanied by knowledge (NEI) is lacking. Also, both PK and AS are not only defensive barriers, but also intellectual filters shaping message interpretation in complex ways, at times complementing buying intentions according to situation and developmental stage.
5.2. Mediation Analysis Results
The mediation analysis yielded new evidence for the psychological processes by which students’ knowledge, interest, and exposure to persuasive content affect their purchase intentions. The findings validated that Persuasion Knowledge (PK) and Advertising Skepticism (AS) are important mediators that validate most of the indirect hypotheses and fill some gaps in our knowledge regarding adolescents’ processing of online advertisements.
Specifically, both AS and PK moderately mediated the influence of Neuromarketing Awareness and Knowledge (NAK) on Purchase Intentions (PIs) (H5a and H5b confirmed). That is, awareness of manipulative strategies directly influences the intention of students to act but also triggers cognitive and attitudinal filters to influence the treatment of adverts. Such findings extrapolate the Persuasion Knowledge Model (PKM) in recognizing that advertising literacy is not merely a passive awareness of tactics but an active process that drives downstream effects [
6,
26,
42]. When they acquire neuromarketing competencies, they not only become open to influence messages (if interpreted as personally relevant or high-tech), but also take evaluative positions—accepting or rejecting being persuaded on the basis of inferred purpose and consonance with personal values.
Additionally, Exposure to Screen-Based Advertising (SBA) revealed partial mediation via both PK and AS (H6a and H6b supported). This is in line with the assumption that online exposure to it provokes more than habituation; it demands thinking answers, whereby children not only learn about types of persuasiveness but also through emotional resistance. Such cross-path mediation effects are consistent with dual-process theories, which hold that exposure to ads triggers both heuristic and systematic processing, where PK and AS are the respective routes [
44,
46,
47]. What is more, this also highlights that passive exposure provides latitude for learning and resistance, particularly in online environments where content is pervasive and highly personalized.
A rich and intriguing result was obtained in the Interest and Engagement with Neuromarketing case, where although it did not directly affect PIs, nor was suggested to indirectly affect significantly via PK (H7a not supported), it did have full mediation via AS (H7b supported). This implies a unique pathway: more engaged students with persuasive strategies are more critical, and this reduces their purchasing intentions. In contrast to NAK, which calls for both knowledge and involvement, NEI calls for more attitudinally defensive positioning, with interest in persuasion technologies perhaps leading to critical assessment and counteraction, rather than free spontaneity. This differential illustrates that knowledge without involvement does not necessarily mean purchasing behavior but rather can construct affective barriers against persuasion [
41,
47,
70].
Together, these mediation results paint a more subtle picture of how teen consumers react to online persuasion. The partial mediation paths through NAK and SBA indicate that students do not react in the same way to persuasive material; their reactions are mediated by what they know and also by what they feel about advertising practices. In contrast, the full mediation of NEI by AS portrays a developmental setting in which interest generates skepticism, at least under conditions of a lack of critical conceptual knowledge.
5.3. Multi-Group Analysis
The multi-group analysis (MGA) indicated significant age, previous advertising and neuromarketing experience, and education-based moderating effects, supporting the contention that responses of adolescents to electronic persuasion are not homogeneous but heterogeneous and influenced by experience as well as development. Subgroup differences are informative about how persuasive operations are internalized and responded to according to varying cognitive and experiential profiles.
Age also functioned as a major moderator on some of the most significant paths. Adolescents aged between 12 and 13 years had much stronger effects of Neuromarketing Awareness and Knowledge (NAK) and Screen-Based Advertising Exposure (SBA) on Persuasion Knowledge (PK), and PK to Purchase Intentions (PIs). Such trends lend evidence to cognitive developmental models, whereby younger children are more mentally susceptible to the communication of advertising literacy and more likely to apply the principles learned, such as PK, into behavioral reasoning. By comparison, the older adolescents (14−15 years) showed more direct effects of SBA to PIs in keeping with transition from cognitive processing to increased experiential and affect-guided decision-making—a finding consistent with earlier work on adolescent media responsiveness [
11,
33,
34].
Students’ prior instructional experience in advertisement literacy also strongly mediated persuasion effects. Those students who were previously formally trained in ad literacy classes had higher PK and lower susceptibility to persuasive messages, confirming findings that media literacy training develops concept awareness and affective resistance [
4,
22,
28]. The relationship between NAK and PK was significantly more robust with formally trained students in ad literacy, which indicates that systematized knowledge is associated with more intensive processing and use.
Advance knowledge of neuromarketing also affected various model paths. Consumers who were familiar with some neuromarketing terms or principles were both more attentive to persuasive content, but more skeptical (AS) and more dependent on PK. This dual reaction displays that awareness can at the same time heighten attention to persuasive stimuli but also enhance evaluative criticism, an indicator of a developing persuasion literacy that negotiates engagement with critical distance.
One of the most interesting results was for past ad-clicking history. Students who had never clicked on an ad exhibited stronger NAK on AS and SBA on PI effects, with greater latent resistance or increased sensitivity to persuasion attempts. Students who indicated clicking on ads exhibited weaker engagement of cognitive and attitudinal defenses, indicating that past ad-clicking can make persuasive exposure normal, making reflective processing less likely [
4,
22,
28].
Together, these moderation effects point to the influence of online experience, media education, and age on how adolescents process and react to persuasive messages. They emphasize the need for developmental and experience-informed, age-relevant content and pedagogy media literacy interventions for students’ digital and developmental selves. Any future initiatives need to move away from one-size-fits-all solutions, recognizing that resistance to digital persuasion is dynamically crafted at the knowledge–experience–self-regulation nexus [
11,
33,
34].
7. Conclusions, Limitations, and Future Research
This research sheds light on children’s interaction with the digital advertising environment by uncovering that ad literacy, screen-based exposure, and neuromarketing awareness play a crucial role in their cognitive and behavior outcomes [
11,
33,
34]. The results are consistent with a dual-pathway theory of persuasion and resistance, age- and experience-modulated, and highlight the key role of education in enabling young consumers to decode, resist, or comply with persuasive messages. The results have important implications for digital ethics, child protection, and learning innovation in an algorithmic advertising era [
11,
33,
34].
Though its strength, this research has some limitations. As an extension of this research, subsequent studies can continue constructing persuasion literacy with richer, multi-method designs [
43,
70,
71]. For example, longitudinal designs would be particularly appropriate in following how advertisement literacy develops over time, given that students are being exposed to greater levels of algorithmically filtered content. These designs can assist in untangling the temporal relationships of awareness, resistance, and behavior change—a topic that is developmentally ripe for study [
43,
70,
71]. In addition, the inclusion of behavioral tasks or the simultaneous presentation of media in future research would offer findings with more ecological validity. The combination of self-report with observational evidence or experimental manipulation can uncover inconsistencies between what students report and actually do in response to influence attempts in context [
9,
43,
45]. Extending this line of inquiry to modern-day educational and cultural environments is a complementary avenue. Given varying media environments and advertising regulations worldwide, comparative cross-cultural research may challenge the global generalizability of current evidence as much as discover culturally unique patterns of resistance or openness to persuasion. In relation, it would be fascinating to explore how various curricula of digital literacy in various sociocultural environments affect the acquisition of critical advertising skills [
6,
26,
42]. Subsequent research could also consider the influence of emerging forms of persuasory technology—i.e., AI-based personalization, influencer marketing, and gamification advertising—on adolescent decision-making. The novel formats introduce novel challenges and are apt to engage with cognitive defenses such as PK and AS differently. The combination of psychophysiological or neuromarketing methods (e.g., eye-tracking, pupillometry, EEG) may further clarify the insidious, frequently unconscious dynamics within which online persuasion is exerted [
6,
26,
42]. Lastly, there is the possibility to extend the existing model by including psychosocial and motivational variables such as peer influence, emotional control, materialistic orientations, and digital well-being. These variables may interact with persuasion knowledge to strengthen or buffer advertisement impacts, particularly on highly engaging media channels like social media or video-sharing applications [
6,
26].
Overall, although this research further extends our knowledge about youth reactions to neuromarketing and screen promotion, future studies are desired, utilizing a broader range of methods, populations, and frameworks, which will be useful to inform educational interventions as well as codes of ethics for influence throughout the digital communication age.