Next Article in Journal
Newspaper Coverage of Government Contracting in the United States: Fueling a Narrative of Distrust and Incompetence
Next Article in Special Issue
Mapping Feminist Politics on Tik Tok during the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Content Analysis of the Hashtags #Feminismo and #Antifeminismo
Previous Article in Journal
TikTok Practices among Teenagers in Portugal: A Uses & Gratifications Approach
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Effects of the COVID-19 “Infodemic” on Journalistic Content and News Feed in Online and Offline Communication Spaces
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Emotions in Crisis Coverage: How UK News Media Used Fear Appeals to Report on the Coronavirus Crisis

by
Valerie Hase
1,* and
Katherine M. Engelke
2
1
Department of Media and Communication, LMU Munich, 80799 Munich, Germany
2
Department of Communication, University of Münster, 48143 Münster, Germany
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Journal. Media 2022, 3(4), 633-649; https://doi.org/10.3390/journalmedia3040042
Submission received: 31 August 2022 / Revised: 22 September 2022 / Accepted: 26 September 2022 / Published: 10 October 2022

Abstract

:
During crises, journalists rely on emotional appeals to alert the public. This includes fear appeals, i.e., journalistic depictions of threats and measures against them. Focusing on the coronavirus crisis, this study analyzes the prevalence of fear appeals in journalistic news, differences between outlets, and changes over time. It employs a manual content analysis of UK online news between January and May 2020 (N = 1048). Results indicate that, during the early phases of the coronavirus pandemic, journalists relied heavily on fear-inducing messages by emphasizing threats related to COVID-19 and, though to a lesser degree, measures against these threats. Besides differences between tabloids and quality outlets, we find that fear-inducing content decreased before the UK itself became most affected, indicating that coverage served a warning function rather than mirroring national affectedness. Overall, the study illustrates that fear appeals are common in coverage of crises, where they enable journalists to take on the role of public mobilizers and facilitators of crises response strategies, for instance by governments.

1. Introduction

Crises such as terrorist attacks, climate change, and the outbreak of diseases are extraordinary, often unexpected events that challenge journalistic routines (Olsson et al. 2015). Throughout crises, journalists play a pivotal role: They disseminate up-to-date information and help audience members react to emerging threats, for example by mobilizing self-protective behavior (Klemm et al. 2019). To alert and engage the public, journalists often rely on emotional appeals (Pantti 2018), including fear-inducing messages (Goodall et al. 2012; Hart and Feldman 2014).
This study is interested in how journalists used fear-inducing messages during the early stages of an unprecedented health crisis: the outbreak of the coronavirus disease (COVID-19). Qualitative insights by Wahl-Jorgensen (2020b) as well as Sowden et al. (2021) indicate that fear-inducing content may have been central to coverage of the coronavirus crisis. Moreover, frequent media use was associated with fear (Li 2021). Correspondingly, scholars have identified the role of emotions, including fear appeals, as a central point of debates in research on the coronavirus crisis (Quandt and Wahl-Jorgensen 2022; Wagner and Reifegerste 2022). However, research on the prevalence of fear-inducing content itself is largely absent. Existing scholarship on the coverage of COVID-19 has mostly focused on actors, topics, or sentiment (Aslam et al. 2020; Eisenegger et al. 2020; Quandt et al. 2020).
Based on a manual content analysis of UK online news (N = 1048, January to May 2020), our study fills this gap in research. It analyzes the prevalence of fear-inducing content during the first wave of the COVID-19 outbreak, including differences between outlets and changes over time. The study contributes to research in three ways: First, it adapts the concept of fear appeals to the news. Second, it analyzes the prevalence of fear-inducing content for a specific crisis, here the coronavirus pandemic. Third, it illustrates how coverage of COVID-19 compares to previous crises and what we can learn by studying this specific case. As such, our study contributes to research in journalism studies, especially journalistic crisis communication and the role of emotions in and for journalism.

1.1. The Role of Emotions in and for Journalism

To understand emotional appeals in the news, we have to first understand emotions: brief affective experiences—i.e., feelings—rooted in appraisals—i.e., subjective judgments—about objects (Keltner and Lerner 2010). Emotions influence individual information processing and decision-making, especially in the face of risk (Loewenstein et al. 2001). As such, they fundamentally shape how we perceive and behave in crises, including disease outbreaks (van Bavel et al. 2020). Importantly, emotions may be elicited through media use and these emotions, subsequently, may mediate audience reactions (Nabi and Prestin 2016).
In journalism studies, emotions have for far too long been viewed with suspicion. Wahl-Jorgensen (2020a) notes that scholars have understood emotions to stand contrary to the ideal of objectivity by criticizing emotional coverage as sensationalized or commercialized. By doing so, however, research ignores that emotions have always been part of the news, especially in crises (Eisele et al. 2022; Pantti 2018)—which is why their role has been reconsidered in an “emotional turn” (Wahl-Jorgensen 2020a, p. 175) in journalism studies: Recent work on emotional labor illustrates how journalists manage their emotions during terrorist attacks (Kotišová 2017) or disease outbreaks (Perreault and Perreault 2021). Studies on emotional audience reactions underline the ambivalent effects of emotion-inducing news. While emotional appeals can foster public engagement with crises, for instance information acquisition and seeking (Bas and Grabe 2015) or compliance with recommended behavior (Harper et al. 2020), it may also invoke maladaptive responses such as information avoidance (Goodall and Reed 2013). As such, emotional appeals play a pivotal role for how audiences react to crises. Finally, and as a prerequisite for such effects, research on emotion-inducing content illustrates journalistic notions on and use of emotional appeals as part of editorial routines (Pantti 2010). During crises, journalists detach from being objective disseminators of information and more strongly take on the role of public mobilizers: To foster crisis management, they cooperate with governments and motivate recipients to engage in self-protective behavior (Klemm et al. 2019). This shift in journalistic roles includes heavier reliance on emotional appeals as journalists consider emotion-inducing news content “the most effective way to mobilize public opinion and affect government action” (Kogen 2019, p. 10). At the same time, journalists can use emotions to mirror people’s experiences of newsworthy events and make news more relatable to the audience (Wahl-Jorgensen 2020a). Overall, emotions have thus long been central to journalistic routines (Wahl-Jorgensen 2020a), even more so during the coronavirus crisis (Quandt and Wahl-Jorgensen 2022). One type of emotional appeals, especially in crisis coverage, are fear appeals (Goodall et al. 2012; Hart and Feldman 2014; Wagner and Reifegerste 2022).

1.2. Fear Appeals: Adaptation for the Context of News

Fear, an emotion rooted in the appraisal of threats (Keltner and Lerner 2010; Witte 1992), has been of particular interest to understanding individual perceptions of and behaviors towards crises. To shed light on the emergence of fear, a large strand of research has focused on fear appeals as “persuasive messages that attempt to arouse fear by emphasizing the potential danger and harm that will befall individuals if they do not adopt the messages’ recommendations” (Tannenbaum et al. 2015, p. 1178). Fear appeal messages aim to change recipients’ attitudes, intentions, or behaviors in line with depicted recommendations and broadly include a threat (threat component) and recommended responses (efficacy component) (Mongeau 2013; Tannenbaum et al. 2015; Witte 1992; Witte and Allen 2000).
Given that journalism scholars have reconsidered the role of emotions for journalistic routines (Wahl-Jorgensen 2020a), the concept of fear appeals—otherwise mostly discussed regarding health or political campaigns (Tannenbaum et al. 2015)—is increasingly transferred to the news. Within newsrooms, fear appeals may serve two functions: Journalists rely on emotional appeals to attract audiences’ attention so that recipients can take protective action against emerging threats. They thus use fear in a functional way (Wagner and Reifegerste 2022) and act as public mobilizers (Klemm et al. 2019; Perreault and Perreault 2021). However, journalists may also employ fear appeals—for example as clickbait—to attract audiences’ attention for more commercial goals, thus partly exaggerating threats in a more dysfunctional way (Wagner and Reifegerste 2022).
In this study, we analyze the degree to which fear appeals are used, independently of journalistic reasons for doing so. We thereby follow studies that similarly aim to understand how journalists emphasize the threat or the efficacy component in coverage of crises (Evensen and Clarke 2012; Goodall et al. 2012; Hart and Feldman 2014) and beyond (Hase et al. 2020). This relates our study to work on other types of emotion-inducing news content, including risk communication, sensationalism, or personalization (Ihekweazu 2017; Jerit et al. 2019; Ophir 2018; Peter and Zerback 2020; Uribe and Gunter 2004; Vasterman and Ruigrok 2013). Drawing on previous research, especially work on fear appeals by Witte (1992), we here define fear appeals in the context of journalistic coverage as news content that depicts (serious and likely) threats (threat component) as well as (individual and societal) measures on how to deal with these threats (efficacy component). Both components are elaborated on in the following sections.

1.2.1. Defining the Threat Component in Crisis Coverage

A threat is “an environmental characteristic that represents something that portends negative consequences for the individual.” (Mongeau 2013, p. 185) The threat component of fear appeal messages thus includes descriptions of both a threat’s severity (i.e., how serious it is) and susceptibility to it (i.e., how likely the individual is to experience it) (Mongeau 2013; Tannenbaum et al. 2015; Witte 1992; Witte and Allen 2000). In the context of news, we know that journalists often depict threats in coverage, both in the context of crises (Hart and Feldman 2014; Houston et al. 2012) and beyond (Hase et al. 2020). Importantly, news can depict past, present, or future threats (Hart and Feldman 2014). Fear appeals research argues that the seriousness of these threats can be conveyed via the use of vivid and intense language (Mongeau 2013; Witte 1992; Witte and Allen 2000). In journalistic news, this may entail reliance on fear-inducing language, another common element of crisis coverage (Ihekweazu 2017; Jerit et al. 2019; Vasterman and Ruigrok 2013). According to fear appeals literature, the susceptibility to threats is further conveyed by directly addressing individuals (Witte 1992; Witte and Allen 2000). As such practices are not commonly found in the news (Andersen et al. 2019), we diverge from this understanding and, in the context of news, argue that susceptibility can be indicated through two elements: first, by journalists illustrating who amongst the audience is likely to experience threats, for example by emphasizing the location of a threat’s impact (Goodall et al. 2012; Hart and Feldman 2014); second, by using a personal angle, meaning at least one person—understood as ordinary citizens, public figures, or professional experts—reports their private experiences and/or opinions (Peter and Zerback 2020; Uribe and Gunter 2004). Personal angles are important as “audiences are more likely to be emotionally engaged, recall information and take action when news stories are relatable” (Wahl-Jorgensen 2020a, p. 189).

1.2.2. Defining the Efficacy Component in Crisis Coverage

Fear appeals theory understands the efficacy component to depict response efficacy, i.e., how effective recommended responses are, and self-efficacy, i.e., recipients’ ability to perform recommended responses (Witte 1992; Witte and Allen 2000). However, studies on fear appeals in the news have also captured which response measures are mentioned in the first place, both on an individual and a societal level (Evensen and Clarke 2012; Goodall et al. 2012; Hart and Feldman 2014). Since news coverage often does not depict response efficacy (Goodall et al. 2012) or self-efficacy (Hart and Feldman 2014), we solely focus on the depiction of measures. Although our understanding therefore diverges from Witte (1992) in that we do not include response efficacy, we nevertheless capture self-efficacy in that depicting measures may imply actors’ ability to perform them in the first place. If journalists use the efficacy component, they thus describe measures on how to deal with the threat both on the individual level (i.e., measures taken by individual actors) and on the societal level (i.e., measures taken by societal actors). These responses can include past, present, or future measures (Goodall et al. 2012; Hart and Feldman 2014). Since measures are helpful for encouraging self-protective behavior, their inclusion closely aligns with journalists acting as public mobilizers during crises (Klemm et al. 2019). Studies on health crises (Goodall et al. 2012; Ophir 2018; You et al. 2017) show that the depiction of individual measures may include journalists emphasizing social distancing or getting vaccinated as responses taken by citizens. In turn, measures by societal actors such as the government may include lockdowns or contact tracing.

1.3. Prevalence of Fear Appeals in Coronavirus Coverage

We now turn towards the prevalence of fear appeals in coronavirus coverage.

1.3.1. Prevalence of the Threat Component

Existing studies mostly analyze overarching topics in coronavirus coverage, not threats of which the coronavirus was the direct or the indirect origin. Still, these studies indicate that threats were covered frequently. Coverage of the COVID-19 pandemic included infection with and death due to the virus, economic downturn, or social distancing threatening citizens’ lifestyles (Eisenegger et al. 2020; Nerlich and Jaspal 2021; Quandt et al. 2020; Sowden et al. 2021). As such, it shares two key similarities with news on previous health crises (Lewison 2008; Ophir 2018; You et al. 2017) and other types of crises (Hart and Feldman 2014; Houston et al. 2012): In the face of crises, journalists clearly focus on threats. Moreover, these threats are depicted to concern vastly different parts of society: in the case of the coronavirus crisis, coverage emphasized health threats (e.g., death due to the virus), political threats (e.g., restrictions of public life,), economic threats (e.g., economic uncertainty), and other threats (e.g., hoarding and shortages of goods) (Bhatti et al. 2022; Eisenegger et al. 2020; Fox 2021; Nerlich and Jaspal 2021; Quandt et al. 2020; Sowden et al. 2021). We ask:
  • RQ1: How prevalent are depictions of threats in coronavirus coverage?
Related to the severity of threats, negative or fear-inducing language seemed to have also been present in coverage (Aslam et al. 2020; Fox 2021; Quandt et al. 2020; Sowden et al. 2021). The coronavirus was described as a “killer virus” (Wahl-Jorgensen 2020b) and society as being “at war” with it (Nerlich and Jaspal 2021), while journalists depicted apocalyptic scenarios (Eisenegger et al. 2020). We ask:
  • RQ2: How prevalent are depictions of severity, i.e., fear-inducing language, in coronavirus coverage?
Turning to susceptibility, studies come to different results in terms of whether crises are “domesticated”, meaning that impacts on the nation instead of the international community are emphasized. For crises considered to be originating in an outlet’s home country—e.g., the swine flu in the US in 2009 (Goodall et al. 2012) or natural disasters (Houston et al. 2012)—, coverage focused on national impacts. For crises of global impact, however, a “domestication” seems less likely: when analyzing coverage of the climate crisis, Hart and Feldman (2014) find that only around a third of US articles described national impacts. Moreover, journalists say that they often “attempt to avoid localizing their stories […] by bringing the story’s focus back to the locales where people suffer most” (Kim 2020, p. 181). Thus far, studies indicate that the COVID-19 crisis was domesticated rather than being portrayed via a global angle (Bhatti et al. 2022).
Considering personal angles as the second indicator of susceptibility, we expect journalists to depict private experiences with COVID-19 since they aim to “give voice to the victims” (Kim 2020, p. 181) of crises. There is, indeed, evidence that personal angles are a consistent element in coverage of health crises (Figenschou et al. 2021; Ihekweazu 2017) or natural disasters (Houston et al. 2012). Journalists may have also employed personal angles when covering COVID-19: Quandt et al. (2020, p. 11) find that news at least sometimes portrayed “people who either suffer particularly from the pandemic […] or people who start initiatives against its social effects”, similar to Sowden et al. (2021). Furthermore, citizens were quoted far more often than, for example, politicians or academics (Hubner 2021). We ask:
  • RQ3: How prevalent are depictions of susceptibility, i.e., national impacts and a personal angle, in coronavirus coverage?

1.3.2. Prevalence of the Efficacy Component

During crises, news media usually focus more on threats than clearly communicate response measures (Hart and Feldman 2014). If they mention response measures, however, journalists usually depict societal over individual actions (Evensen and Clarke 2012; Ihekweazu 2017; Ophir 2018; You et al. 2017): For example, journalists more often report on research on vaccines than communicate how recipients themselves can take action to avoid infections. Turning to the coronavirus crisis, studies indicate that journalists rarely depicted individual response measures. In a qualitative study, Nerlich and Jaspal (2021) find that early UK coverage of the pandemic failed to adequately inform readers about how to follow social distancing recommendations. Similarly, Sowden et al. (2021) argue that coverage seldom included practical steps for readers to overcome their fear of COVID-19, something supported by quantitative studies (Bhatti et al. 2022; Fox 2021). To extend these insights, we ask:
  • RQ4: How prevalent were depictions of the efficacy component, i.e., individual and societal measures, in coronavirus coverage?

1.3.3. Differences between Tabloids and Quality Outlets

In the UK, tabloids such as The Sun and The Mirror are frequently associated with soft news (Esser 1999; Uribe and Gunter 2004), often in contrast to quality outlets such as The Times or The Telegraph. Soft news is—among other indicators—characterized by personal or emotional reporting styles (Otto et al. 2017; Reinemann et al. 2012). We know that UK tabloids often rely on, for example, personalization (Esser 1999; Uribe and Gunter 2004) and that journalists working for tabloids prefer to report on unexpected and dramatic events (Skovsgaard 2014). Given that tabloids may be more likely to publish emotion-inducing content, they may also more often rely on fear appeals. Few analyses on fear appeals have included and compared different types of media outlets. However, studies by Feldman et al. (2017) as well as Brookes and Baker (2021) indicate that if news media cover crises, here climate change or health risks, tabloids more often emphasize threats or focus on national impacts, i.e., the threat component, than quality outlets. However, their findings do not indicate consistent differences concerning the efficacy component. We ask:
  • RQ5: How does the prevalence of fear appeals in coronavirus coverage differ between tabloids and quality outlets?

1.3.4. Differences over Time

Furthermore, journalistic use of fear-inducing content may shift over time due to national affectedness by COVID-19. For the case of our study—the UK during the first wave of the COVID-19 pandemic—two scenarios seem likely:
On the one hand, journalists need to warn their audience of emerging threats. Thus, they may have relied on fear appeals once COVID-19 emerged but before the virus reached the UK. As such, we would expect the highest prevalence of fear-inducing content at the beginning of the crisis. This scenario would align with journalists’ role as public mobilizers (Klemm et al. 2019): To motivate protective behavior, journalists use fear-inducing content before their own country is affected. Moreover, journalists more heavily rely on and cooperate with governmental sources and public health officials during the early stages of crises. These actors often promote response measures, i.e., elements of the efficacy component, which is why cooperation may add to journalistic reliance on fear appeals.
On the other hand, the prevalence of fear appeals may have increased throughout the crisis once COVID-19 affected the UK. When covering crises, journalists often say that they aim for “fact-based, data-driven reporting” (Kim 2020, p. 183). Correspondingly, coverage of diseases partly corresponds with rising numbers of hospitalizations (Vasterman and Ruigrok 2013). While the first wave started with UK citizens being affected at the end of January 2020, infections and deaths in the UK only rapidly increased from March 2020 onwards (Dong et al. 2020). Similarly, societal response measures, for instance the first lockdown (Nerlich and Jaspal 2021), were only implemented throughout March. Thus, if coverage mirrors national developments—e.g., infections, deaths, or implementations of restrictive measures—fear-inducing content may have only become prevalent later.
So far, studies support the first over the second scenario, meaning crisis coverage is often characterized by a decrease in fear-inducing content over time (Vasterman and Ruigrok 2013) and, thus, a shift from “alarm” to “reassurance” (Ungar 1998) independent of actual affectedness. While studies rarely analyze these temporal patterns for the coronavirus crisis, Eisenegger et al. (2020) show that, during the first wave of COVID-19 in Switzerland, threats were mentioned less over time, which the authors attribute to declining cases in the country. Similarly, Eisele et al. (2022) illustrate an increase in emotional reactions in public communication before infections peaked. Thus, we pose:
H1: 
The prevalence of fear appeals in coronavirus coverage decreased over time.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. The Case: The UK during the First Wave of COVID-19

This study analyzes coronavirus coverage in the UK from 22 January to 31 May 2020. We chose the UK as a country highly affected by COVID-19 (Dong et al. 2020). Between January and May, the first wave of the virus occurred, including first deaths and infections related to COVID-19, the first lockdown (Nerlich and Jaspal 2021), and a slow decrease in cases throughout May (Dong et al. 2020). January 22 was determined as the exact start date as almost no articles were published in the UK beforehand and COVID-related cases were only registered from this time onwards. We consider the UK and the first wave an important case for two reasons: first, the UK is a country where we may expect journalists to rely on emotional appeals—not only because of the UK’s affectedness by COVID-19 but also due to its established tabloid market (Esser 1999; Uribe and Gunter 2004). Second, uncertainty concerning COVID-19 may have led to stronger reliance on emotional appeals during the first wave, which makes this time period an interesting case to study. However, our focus on a country hit particularly hard by COVID-19 and the first wave as a period of high uncertainty limits the generalizability of our findings.
We sampled news from the online presence of The Times, The Telegraph, The Sun, and The Mirror. We chose digital newspapers due to the fact that citizens often relied on news organizations and digital media during the first wave (Nielsen et al. 2020), leading to an unprecedented rise in readership for many of these outlets (Mayhew 2020). Our sample furthermore includes both quality outlets (The Times, The Telegraph) and tabloids (The Sun, The Mirror), which are often associated with different degrees of a soft news orientation (Esser 1999; Uribe and Gunter 2004).
We retrieved articles mentioning the search terms “coronavirus! OR covid-19! OR sars-cov!” at least twice, similar to previous studies (Eisenegger et al. 2020; Quandt et al. 2020). As non-validated search terms may introduce measurement error (Mahl et al. 2022), the validity of our sampling approach was tested to reassure that we only retrieved articles that dealt with the virus as their main topic (see Supplementary Material, Element A1, F1 score = 0.96). Based on a stratified sampling strategy, we randomly retrieved two articles from each outlet for every day between 22 January and 31 May 2020, leading to N = 1048 articles.

2.2. Operationalization

To measure fear-inducing content in the news, two researchers coded seven variables. The variable Threat measures whether an article included a coronavirus-related threat portending negative consequences. For operationalization, we relied on and inductively extended a list of threats developed by Hase et al. (2020). Threat includes the subcategories Health threats (e.g., infection), Political threats (e.g., restrictions of public life), Economic threats (e.g., economic uncertainty), and Other threats (e.g., hoarding). Coders chose one out of 18 different threats or decided that no threat was present (see Supplementary Material, Element A2). Related to the severity of threats, Fear-inducing language (headline) and Fear-inducing language (body) capture fear-inducing language. Language was measured separately for articles’ headlines and bodies as fear-inducing headlines can have an influence on their own, for example on news consumption (Ng and Zhao 2020). Coders received a list of words deduced from previous studies on the coverage of health crises (e.g., Jerit et al. 2019; Vasterman and Ruigrok 2013) and exemplary quotes to decide whether fear-inducing language was present (0 = No, 1 = Yes). The list included words such as ‘catastrophe’, ‘deadly’, ‘inferno’, and ‘panic’. Regarding susceptibility, National impacts captures whether the UK as a whole or a UK-specific entity (e.g., UK citizens or businesses) were depicted as negatively impacted by the threat (0 = No, 1 = Yes). Personal angle describes whether a person reports their private experiences with and/or opinions on the coronavirus (0 = No, 1 = Yes).
Turning to the efficacy component, Individual measures entails whether an article mentions individual responses to deal with the threat, for example getting vaccinated or social distancing (0 = No, 1 = Yes). Societal measures describes responses by societal actors (e.g., government, businesses), for example enforcing lockdowns or contact tracing (0 = No, 1 = Yes). Measures were only coded if at least one threat was mentioned as, theoretically, responses are only then deemed necessary.
Intercoder reliability reached satisfactory values (N = 100, αmin = 0.71, see Supplementary Material, Element A3), except for the dichotomous variable Societal measures, where reliability was at the lower end of the acceptable threshold (α = 0.69). Since Krippendorff’s α often is too conservative for binary variables and the Holsti coefficient reached 0.89, we concluded that reliability was sufficient.

3. Results

3.1. The Prevalence of Fear Appeals in Coronavirus Coverage

Table 1 illustrates the prevalence of fear appeals in UK news coverage. Concerning threats (RQ1), 91.1% of all articles emphasized at least one threat. As illustrated by Table 2, Health threats were most prevalent (50.9%), followed by Economic threats (15.9%), Political threats (15.7%), and Other threats (8.6%). The category Health threats includes the most prevalent threat: being infected with the virus (36.6%). For instance, The Sun emphasized how easily transmittable the virus is by exemplifying how a man caught the virus “just 15 SECONDS after standing next to [an] infected woman” (Coyle 2020, para. 1). Journalists also described death (10.6%) or insufficient medical treatment (2.2%) due to the NHS being overwhelmed. The category Economic threats includes the second most prevalent threat: economic uncertainty (13.6%). For instance, The Telegraph described how “the coronavirus crisis shifts from being a human health story to an economic one” (Stevenson 2020, para. 4). Journalists also mentioned job loss (1.9%) or price spikes (0.4%), such as increasing food prices.
Political threats entail restrictions of public life (9.4%), specifically the first lockdown in March, as “the biggest restriction of civil liberties in peacetime” (Donnelly et al. 2020, para. 1). Another political threat was the lack of political governance (5.6%) and, although rarely mentioned, the rise of Chinese soft power (0.8%). Lastly, news described Other threats including unique, openly coded threats (4.7%), such as hoarding of goods (1.9%) and racism (0.8%), often towards the Asian community.
Regarding the severity of threats (RQ2), Fear-inducing language was used in 38.6% of articles’ headlines and 55.5% of articles’ bodies. Articles described the virus as a “killer bug” or the “devil” and the ongoing crisis as a “plague panic” or “fresh hell”. Journalists also used metaphors of war: The Times, for example, explained how “Wuhan is the site of the decisive battle, and we must concentrate our powers for a battle to annihilate it” (Tang 2020, para. 10). The virus was often personalized, with The Sun condemning it as the “public enemy number one” (Mullin and McDermott 2020, para. 1).
Concerning susceptibility (RQ3), 69.6% of articles described national impacts of the virus. Personal angles were prevalent in 36.5% of articles. For example, The Mirror illustrated how a midwife died of coronavirus only two weeks after her father had passed away (Fricker 2020) or how Boris Johnson went jogging after his infection (Mills 2020).
Regarding the efficacy component (RQ4), 40.4% of all articles underlined individual measures (e.g., washing hands, supporting the NHS by clapping hands, or donating) and 70.1% societal measures (e.g., governmental lockdown or ramping up testing capabilities).

3.2. Differences between Tabloids and Quality Outlets

Turning to RQ5, Table 3 illustrates differences between quality outlets (The Times, The Telegraph) and tabloids (The Mirror, The Sun). Results tentatively indicate that tabloids more often used fear appeals: Related to the threat component, they more often relied on fear-inducing language in headlines (45.8% tabloids, 31.5% quality outlets). According to a Chi-squared test, this difference is consistent: χ2(1, N = 1048) = 22.04, p < .001. Tabloids also more often used personal angles (45.4% tabloids, 27.5% quality outlets), χ2(1, N = 1048) = 35.63, p < .001. However, there were no consistent differences between tabloids’ and quality outlets’ depiction of threats (90.3% tabloids, 92% quality outlets), fear-inducing language in articles’ bodies (58% tabloids, 53% quality outlets), or their focus on national impacts (71% tabloids, 68.1% quality outlets).
Turning to the efficacy component, there were consistent differences for one of the two elements: While tabloids and quality outlets reported on societal measures to almost the exact same degree (70.2% tabloids, 70% quality outlets), tabloids more often emphasized individual measures (46.4% tabloids, 34.4% quality outlets), χ2(1, N = 1048) = 15.42, p < .001.

3.3. Differences over Time

To understand changes over time (H1), Figure 1 depicts the prevalence of fear appeals as 7-day rolling averages. For comparison, it also displays infections and deaths related to COVID-19 in the UK according to the COVID-19 Data Repository by the Center for Systems Science and Engineering (CSSE) at Johns Hopkins University (Dong et al. 2020). The figure indicates that fear appeals became somewhat less prevalent over time, independent of COVID-related infections and deaths in the UK. Fear-inducing news was most prevalent long before March 2020, when infections and deaths rose and the first lockdown occurred. To test for monotonic trends, i.e., increases or decreases of fear appeals over time, we used the non-parametric Mann–Kendall test. We relied on an adjusted approach that corrects for autocorrelation (Hamed and Ramachandra Rao 1998).
Over time, three out of five elements of the threat component decreased: Throughout the first wave, UK news outlets less often reported on threats (z = −7.52, p < .001) and less often used fear-inducing language in articles’ headlines (z = −7.72, p < .001) and bodies (z = −4.87, p < .001). However, there was no consistent trend in how often journalists reported on national impacts of the coronavirus crisis (z = 1.82, p = .07) or relied on personal angles (z = −1.95, p = .05). Turning to the efficacy component, journalists less often mentioned societal measures (z = −3.55, p < .001), while there was no consistent trend for individual measures (z = −1.59, p = .11). H1 is partly supported.

4. Discussion

Based on a manual content analysis (N = 1048), this study analyzed fear-inducing content in UK coverage of the coronavirus crisis during the first wave of the pandemic. Results illustrate that journalists frequently relied on emotion-inducing fear appeals, especially immediately after the virus emerged.

4.1. Did Journalists Rely on Fear-Inducing Content When Covering COVID-19?

UK coverage of the coronavirus crisis was characterized by fear-inducing content: Related to RQ1, more than 90% of articles underlined coronavirus-related threats such as infection with or death due to the virus, impending economic uncertainty, or restrictions of public life, as indicated by previous studies (Bhatti et al. 2022; Eisenegger et al. 2020; Nerlich and Jaspal 2021; Quandt et al. 2020; Sowden et al. 2021). Concerning RQ2, news resorted to fear-inducing language in more than half of the articles’ bodies and more than a third of the articles’ headlines in line with existing work (Aslam et al. 2020; Nerlich and Jaspal 2021; Quandt et al. 2020; Sowden et al. 2021). In addition, almost 70% of articles domesticated the crisis by emphasizing the UK’s affectedness. More than a third conveyed the audience’s susceptibility through personal angles, for instance by covering infected persons (RQ3). As societal measures were mentioned in 70.1% and individual measures in 40.4% of coverage (RQ4), news focused less on measures to react towards COVID-19 and more on threats related to it. Journalists also more often pointed out what the government or other societal actors did or should do to battle the virus than depict measures individuals could take (see similarly Bhatti et al. 2022). However, we found differences between outlets and over time: concerning RQ5, the tabloids The Sun and The Mirror, often associated with emotion-inducing content (Esser 1999; Uribe and Gunter 2004), were somewhat more likely to use fear appeals. Our study thus not only answers recent calls for studies focusing explicitly on differences between quality and tabloid outlets in crisis communication (Eisele et al. 2022), but also shows that it is important and worthwhile to differentiate between these different types of outlets. Related to H1, fear-inducing content tentatively decreased over time, indicating a shift from alarming to reassuring coverage, similar to other crises (Ungar 1998; Vasterman and Ruigrok 2013). Overall, this indicates that reliance on emotional appeals is a core element of journalistic routines during crises, as indicated by prior research (Pantti 2018; Wahl-Jorgensen 2020a).

4.2. How Does Coverage of the Coronavirus Crisis Compare to Other Crises?

The COVID-19 pandemic represents a crisis unprecedented in impact. Thus, by studying it, what can we learn about patterns in crisis coverage—and where is the coronavirus crisis unique in nature? When contextualizing our findings, we find that coverage shares four similarities with previous crises: journalistic reliance on emotional appeals, an emphasis on crises’ impacts across society, a focus on threats rather than how to handle them, and a shift from alarm to reassurance.
First, this study supports assertions that emotional appeals, including fear-inducing content, are a common element in coverage of crises (Pantti 2018). Journalists themselves are often vulnerable to and affected by crises, as work on emotional labor has highlighted (Kotišová 2017). During the coronavirus coverage, they faced economic pressure, changing routines, and the risk of being infected themselves, which increased journalistic belief in the importance of communicating risks to readers (Perreault and Perreault 2021). By relying on emotion, journalists may thus not only aim to mobilize and protect (Klemm et al. 2019) as well as to make the crisis in question more relatable to the public (Wahl-Jorgensen 2020a) but also display their own vulnerabilities to crises.
Second, our results indicate another common pattern in crises, including the coronavirus pandemic: Crises are depicted as impacting society as a whole. Independent of whether they are primarily connected to health threats, journalists often underline a range of political, economic, and societal threats (Houston et al. 2012; Lewison 2008; Ophir 2018; You et al. 2017). In a way, journalists constitute crises—and thus contribute to the societal amplification of risk (Kasperson et al. 1988)—by presenting them as a threat to different parts of society.
Third, our results support assertions that crisis coverage often concentrates more on what is wrong, i.e., the threat component, than on how recipients can deal with threats, i.e., the efficacy component (Hart and Feldman 2014), something often criticized for the coronavirus crisis (Nerlich and Jaspal 2021; Sowden et al. 2021). Even when journalists include response measures during crises, they more often mention actions by societal actors than actions citizens can take to protect themselves (Ophir 2018; You et al. 2017).
Fourth and last, we find support for temporal patterns in crisis coverage (Lewison 2008; Ungar 1998; Vasterman and Ruigrok 2013): Coverage shifts from an “alarm” mode where threats are exaggerated to more reassuring coverage, something that holds for the coronavirus crisis. Thus, crises often become “old”, even if their risks persist (Kitzinger 1999), potentially because, at some point, audiences feel fatigue (Groot Kormelink and Klein Gunnewiek 2021).
Overall, we conclude that even during crises as unprecedented as the coronavirus pandemic, journalists rely on similar routines. Coverage of the coronavirus was not entirely “new”—instead journalists, in many aspects, reported on the pandemic as could have been expected based on previous crises. However, readers should also note differences in how journalists covered the coronavirus pandemic compared to previous crises: We found a stronger emphasis both on threats in general (Hart and Feldman 2014) and on non-health threats than research indicated for previous outbreaks of diseases (Lewison 2008; You et al. 2017). In the UK, the crisis was also more strongly “domesticated” than expected given other global crises (Hart and Feldman 2014). We think these differences may be due to the unprecedented nature of the COVID-19 pandemic, which heavily impacted global routines, including those in newsrooms, and our focus on the UK, a country hit hard by COVID-19.

4.3. Perils and Pitfalls of Fear-Inducing Coverage in Crises

Across disciplines, authors have discussed how institutions, including the news media, should communicate crises and risks (Kitzinger 1999; Sorribes and Rovira 2011; Vasterman et al. 2008). This extends to the coronavirus crisis (Stolow et al. 2020; Wagner and Reifegerste 2022). Turning to the implications of our results, what are the potential effects of fear-inducing coverage and which functions did journalists fulfill (or neglect) when covering COVID-19?
On the one hand, emotion-inducing content may have had positive effects on managing the crisis: Negative emotional reactions such as fear, which was partly induced by coverage, correlated with information seeking about COVID-19 (Li 2021), belief in accurate information (Freiling et al. 2021), and compliance with recommended behavior (Harper et al. 2020). Thus, fear appeals may have enabled journalists to fulfill their role as public mobilizers who warn, educate, and mobilize citizens during crises, including the coronavirus crisis (Klemm et al. 2019; Perreault and Perreault 2021). Moreover, the fact that both societal and individual measures were mentioned, for instance via news on governmental recommendations concerning social distancing, indicates that, to some extent, journalists also acted as facilitators: They cooperated with authorities to facilitate crisis management. Some recommend for journalists to fulfill exactly these functions (Sorribes and Rovira 2011). Thus, fear-inducing content does not necessarily indicate systemic dysfunctionalities.
On the other hand, a large strand of research has underlined a lack of effectiveness or adverse consequences of fear appeals (Kok et al. 2018), also for the coronavirus pandemic (Stolow et al. 2020). Negative emotional reactions, including fear, may have amplified the perceived risk of COVID-19 (Breakwell and Jaspal 2020), belief in misinformation (Freiling et al. 2021), or out-group prejudice (van Bavel et al. 2020). Citizens sometimes perceived news media to negatively impact how they coped with the crisis, with some avoiding news in a “corona-fatigue” (Groot Kormelink and Klein Gunnewiek 2021). Thus, fear-inducing content may also have led the audience not to being aware and alerted but to being in a state of shock, which could hamper the implementations of protective measures. Without efficacy information, for example, fear appeals may engage people to do exactly the opposite of what is advocated (Witte 1992). As such, the high prevalence of fear-inducing content, especially journalists’ focus on threats over efficacy and their reliance on fear-inducing language, also indicates that journalists did not adhere to key recommendations for how to cover crises: Media should avoid dramatic coverage (Sorribes and Rovira 2011), for instance by not only mentioning threats but also recommendations on how individuals can cope with these to not contribute to the amplification of risk (Vasterman et al. 2008). Ophir (2019, p. 552) states “that the journalistic practice most damaging to crisis and risk communication efforts is communicating health risks without providing efficacy information”. Journalists should also not capitalize on an increased reliance on news during crises, for instance by using emotional appeals as clickbait. By relying on fear over facts, journalists thus also partly neglected their role as neutral disseminator of information.
Overall, our study underlines that emotions play a pivotal role in crisis communication, including news coverage. However, emotional appeals—including fear-inducing news content—may emerge as a double-edged sword in that they both support and hinder how journalists fulfill key functions during crises (Wagner and Reifegerste 2022): While they enable journalists to mobilize the public and assist in governmental crisis management, they also decrease journalistic focus on facts, which can have detrimental effects on audience reactions. We thus agree with Ribeiro and Schwarzenegger (2022, p. 13) that “media and fear can sometimes be highly functional and beneficial, sometimes disastrous and toxic, but it is always and certainly an ongoing relationship.” As such, journalists and authorities alike must carefully consider the degree to which emotional appeals can and should be implemented in crisis communication.

4.4. Limitations and the Road Ahead

Our results should be considered in light of several limitations. While we measured journalistic content assumed to elicit emotions, we did not analyze audience reactions induced by such content. Empirical research on the effectiveness of fear appeals has been inconclusive at best (Kok et al. 2018). While we assume that content we defined as fear-inducing may have elicited emotional reactions, we can neither confirm that this was the case nor which emotional reactions were elicited. Moreover, analyzing fear-inducing content during crises presented somewhat of a challenge: Fear appeals may signal both accurate journalistic representations of emerging threats as well as journalistic exaggerations of such threats for commercial goals. Our operationalization includes variables that may capture both (e.g., a focus on threats as a depiction of reality; a focus on fear-inducing language as exaggeration), limiting the degree to which we can evaluate whether journalists used fear in a functional or dysfunctional way. In addition, our sample is limited given that we did not include more left-leaning, highly used news outlets, which were evaluated positively during the crisis (Fletcher et al. 2020). Our results are therefore not generalizable beyond the outlets, the country, and the time we analyzed. Future studies should amplify existing research on emotion-inducing news (Nabi and Prestin 2016) to better understand the actual effects of fear-inducing news. Moreover, they could extend the observation period to understand how coverage changed with subsequent waves of the pandemic.
Despite these limitations, this study contributes to research on journalistic crisis communication and the role of emotional news content not only by providing insights into a specific crisis but also by discussing how this specific case resembles and differs from previous crises and what related coverage can mean for audiences. It thus helps researchers to gain a better understanding of journalistic routines during crises and can serve as a starting point for further research both on the coronavirus pandemic in particular and journalistic crisis communication more broadly.

Supplementary Materials

Supporting information can be downloaded at: https://osf.io/fr36x/?view_only=84f59f81975a4e9dbbb1774f9cf250a8.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, V.H. and K.M.E.; methodology, V.H. and K.M.E.; software, V.H.; validation, V.H.; formal analysis, V.H.; investigation, V.H. and K.M.E.; resources, V.H. and K.M.E.; data curation, V.H. and K.M.E.; writing—original draft preparation, V.H. and K.M.E.; writing—review and editing, V.H. and K.M.E.; visualization, V.H. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Data sharing not applicable (Newspaper data used for the content analysis cannot be shared due to copyright issues).

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Andersen, Elisabeth Muth, Anette Grønning, Maiju Hietaketo, and Marjut Johansson. 2019. Direct Reader Address in Health-related Online News Articles: Imposing Problems and Projecting Desires for Action and Change onto Readers. Journalism Studies 20: 2478–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Aslam, Faheem, Tahir Mumtaz Awan, Jabir Hussain Syed, Aisha Kashif, and Mahwish Parveen. 2020. Sentiments and emotions evoked by news headlines of coronavirus disease (COVID-19) outbreak. Humanities and Social Sciences Communications 7: 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Bas, Ozen, and Maria Elizabeth Grabe. 2015. Emotion-Provoking Personalization of News: Informing Citizens and Closing the Knowledge Gap? Communication Research 42: 159–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Bhatti, Shumaila J., Paul P. Billinson, Lauren A. Cornell, Ashmita Das, Courtney Gammon, Lauren O. Kelly, Jeongwon Yang, and Silje Kristiansen. 2022. A Country Comparative Analysis of International Print Media’s Framing of the COVID-19 Pandemic. International Journal of Communication 16: 1282–308. [Google Scholar]
  5. Breakwell, Glynis M., and Rusi Jaspal. 2020. Identity change, uncertainty and mistrust in relation to fear and risk of COVID-19. Journal of Risk Research 24: 335–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Brookes, Gavin, and Paul Baker. 2021. Fear and responsibility: Discourses of obesity and risk in the UK press. Journal of Risk Research 25: 363–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Coyle, Matt. 2020. SWIFT AND DEADLY Chinese Man, 56, Catches Coronavirus in just 15 SECONDS after Standing Next to Infected Woman at Market. The Sun. Available online: https://www.thesun.co.uk/news/10911341/coronavirus-chinese-man-caught-virus-15-seconds (accessed on 31 August 2022).
  8. Dong, Ensheng, Hongru Du, and Lauren Gardner. 2020. An interactive web-based dashboard to track COVID-19 in real time. The Lancet Infectious Diseases 20: 533–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Donnelly, Laura, Gordon Rayner, and Henry Bodkin. 2020. Coronavirus Puts Life on Hold as Boris Johnson Urges Public to Avoid All “Non-Essential Contact”. The Telegraph. Available online: https://www.telegraph.co.uk/news/2020/03/16/coronavirus-puts-life-hold-boris-johnson-urges-public-avoid (accessed on 31 August 2022).
  10. Eisele, Olga, Olga Litvyak, Verena K. Brändle, Paul Balluff, Andreas Fischeneder, Catherine Sotirakou, Pamina Syed Ali, and Hajo G. Boomgaarden. 2022. An Emotional Rally: Exploring Commenters’ Responses to Online News Coverage of the COVID-19 Crisis in Austria. Digital Journalism 10: 952–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Eisenegger, Mark, Franziska Oehmer, Linards Udris, and Daniel Vogler. 2020. Qualität der Medien Studie 1/2020. Die Qualität der Medienberichterstattung zur Corona-Pandemie [New Media Quality Study 1/2020. On the Quality of News Coverage of the Coronavirus Pandemic]. Available online: https://www.foeg.uzh.ch/dam/jcr:b87084ac-5b5b-4f76-aba7-2e6fe2703e81/200731_Studie%20Leitmedien%20Corona.pdf (accessed on 31 August 2022).
  12. Esser, Frank. 1999. ‘Tabloidization’ of News: A Comparative Analysis of Anglo-American and German Press Journalism. European Journal of Communication 14: 291–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Evensen, Darrick T., and Christopher E. Clarke. 2012. Efficacy Information in Media Coverage of Infectious Disease Risks: An Ill Predicament? Science Communication 34: 392–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Feldman, Lauren, P. Sol Hart, and Tijana Milosevic. 2017. Polarizing news? Representations of threat and efficacy in leading US newspapers’ coverage of climate change. Public Understanding of Science 26: 481–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Figenschou, Tine Ustad, Kjersti Thorbjørnsrud, and Daniel C. Hallin. 2021. Whose stories are told and who is made responsible? Human-interest framing in health journalism in Norway, Spain, the U.K. and the U.S. Journalism. Online first publication. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Fletcher, Richard, Antonis Kalogeropoulos, and Rasmus Kleis Nielsen. 2020. News Media Broadly Trusted, Views of UK Government Response to COVID-19 Highly Polarised. Available online: https://reutersinstitute.politics.ox.ac.uk/news-media-broadly-trusted-views-uk-government-response-covid-19-highly-polarised (accessed on 31 August 2022).
  17. Fox, Colm A. 2021. Media in a Time of Crisis: Newspaper Coverage of COVID-19 in East Asia. Journalism Studies 22: 1853–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Freiling, Isabelle, Nicole M. Krause, Dietram A. Scheufele, and Dominique Brossard. 2021. Believing and sharing misinformation, fact-checks, and accurate information on social media: The role of anxiety during COVID-19. New Media & Society. Online first publication. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Fricker, Martin. 2020. “Special” Midwife, 30, Dies of Coronavirus Two Weeks after the Death of Her Dad. The Mirror. Available online: https://www.mirror.co.uk/news/uk-news/special-midwife-30-dies-coronavirus-22031687 (accessed on 31 August 2022).
  20. Goodall, Catherine E., and Phillip Reed. 2013. Threat and Efficacy Uncertainty in News Coverage About Bed Bugs as Unique Predictors of Information Seeking and Avoidance: An Extension of the EPPM. Health Communication 28: 63–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Goodall, Catherine, Jason Sabo, Rebecca Cline, and Nichole Egbert. 2012. Threat, Efficacy, and Uncertainty in the First 5 Months of National Print and Electronic News Coverage of the H1N1 Virus. Journal of Health Communication 17: 338–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Groot Kormelink, Tim, and Anne Klein Gunnewiek. 2021. From “Far Away” to “Shock” to “Fatigue” to “Back to Normal”: How Young People Experienced News During the First Wave of the COVID-19 Pandemic. Journalism Studies 23: 669–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Hamed, Khaled H., and A. Ramachandra Rao. 1998. A modified Mann-Kendall trend test for autocorrelated data. Journal of Hydrology 204: 182–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Harper, Craig A., Liam P. Satchell, Dean Fido, and Robert D. Latzman. 2020. Functional Fear Predicts Public Health Compliance in the COVID-19 Pandemic. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction 19: 1875–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Hart, P. Sol, and Lauren Feldman. 2014. Threat without Efficacy? Climate Change on U.S. Network News. Science Communication 36: 325–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Hase, Valerie, Katherine M. Engelke, and Kimon Kieslich. 2020. The Things We Fear. Combining Automated and Manual Content Analysis to Uncover Themes, Topics and Threats in Fear-Related News. Journalism Studies 21: 1384–1402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Houston, J. Brian, Betty Pfefferbaum, and Cathy Ellen Rosenholtz. 2012. Disaster News: Framing and Frame Changing in Coverage of Major U.S. Natural Disasters, 2000–2010. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly 89: 606–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Hubner, Austin. 2021. How did we get here? A framing and source analysis of early COVID-19 media coverage. Communication Research Reports 38: 112–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Ihekweazu, Chioma. 2017. Ebola in Prime Time: A Content Analysis of Sensationalism and Efficacy Information in U.S. Nightly News Coverage of the Ebola Outbreaks. Health Communication 32: 741–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Jerit, Jennifer, Yangzi Zhao, Megan Tan, and Munifa Wheeler. 2019. Differences between National and Local Media in News Coverage of the Zika Virus. Health Communication 34: 1816–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Kasperson, Roger E., Ortwin Renn, Paul Slovic, Halina S. Brown, Jacque Emel, Robert Goble, Jeanne X. Kasperson, and Samuel Ratick. 1988. The Social Amplification of Risk: A Conceptual Framework. Risk Analysis 8: 177–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Keltner, Dacher, and Jennifer S. Lerner. 2010. Emotion. In Handbook of Social Psychology. Edited by Susan T. Fiske, Daniel T. Gilbert and Gardner Lindzey. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., pp. 317–352. [Google Scholar]
  33. Kim, Youngrim. 2020. Outbreak news production as a site of tension: Journalists’ news-making of global infectious disease. Journalism 23: 171–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Kitzinger, Jenny. 1999. Researching risk and the media. Health, Risk & Society 1: 55–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Klemm, Celine, Enny Das, and Tilo Hartmann. 2019. Changed priorities ahead: Journalists’ shifting role perceptions when covering public health crises. Journalism 20: 1223–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  36. Kogen, Lauren. 2019. News You Can Use or News That Moves? Journalists’ rationales for coverage of distant suffering. Journalism Practice 13: 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Kok, Gerjo, Gjalt-Jorn Y. Peters, Loes T. E. Kessels, Gill A. ten Hoor, and Robert A. C. Ruiter. 2018. Ignoring theory and misinterpreting evidence: The false belief in fear appeals. Health Psychology Review 12: 111–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Kotišová, Johana. 2017. Cynicism ex machina: The emotionality of reporting the ‘refugee crisis’ and Paris terrorist attacks in Czech Television. European Journal of Communication 32: 242–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Lewison, Grant. 2008. The reporting of the risks from severe acute respiratory syndrome (SARS) in the news media, 2003–2004. Health, Risk & Society 10: 241–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Li, Ruobing. 2021. Fear of COVID-19: What Causes Fear and How Individuals Cope with It. Health Communication. Online first publication. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Loewenstein, George F., Elke U. Weber, Christopher K. Hsee, and Ned Welch. 2001. Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin 127: 267–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Mahl, Daniela, Gerret von Nordheim, and Lars Guenther. 2022. Noise pollution: A multi-step approach to assessing the consequences of (not) validating search terms on automated content analyses. Digital Journalism. Online first publication. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Mayhew, Freddy. 2020. COVID-19 Prompts Record Digital Audience for UK National Press with 6.6m Extra Daily Readers. Available online: https://www.pressgazette.co.uk/covid-19-prompts-record-digital-audience-for-uk-national-press-with-6-6m-extra-daily-readers (accessed on 31 August 2022).
  44. Mills, Kelly-Ann. 2020. Queen gives Boris Johnson permission to jog in Buckingham Palace gardens. The Mirror. Available online: https://www.mirror.co.uk/news/uk-news/queen-gives-boris-johnson-permission-22094341 (accessed on 31 August 2022).
  45. Mongeau, Paul A. 2013. Fear Appeals. In The SAGE Handbook of Persuasion: Developments in Theory and Practice. Edited by J. P. Dillard and L. Shen. Los Angeles: SAGE Publications, pp. 184–99. [Google Scholar]
  46. Mullin, Gemma, and Nick McDermott. 2020. PUBLIC ENEMY Coronavirus COVID-19 Could Kill 45 Million—And “Poses Bigger Threat Than Terrorism”. The Sun. Available online: https://www.thesun.co.uk/news/10945744/coronavirus-covid-19-kill-45-million-terrorism (accessed on 31 August 2022).
  47. Nabi, Robin L., and Abby Prestin. 2016. Unrealistic Hope and Unnecessary Fear: Exploring How Sensationalistic News Stories Influence Health Behavior Motivation. Health Communication 31: 1115–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Nerlich, Brigitte, and Rusi Jaspal. 2021. Social representations of ‘social distancing’ in response to COVID-19 in the UK media. Current Sociology 69: 566–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Ng, Yu-Leung, and Xinshu Zhao. 2020. The Human Alarm System for Sensational News, Online News Headlines, and Associated Generic Digital Footprints: A Uses and Gratifications Approach. Communication Research 47: 251–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Nielsen, Rasmus Kleis, Richard Fletcher, Nic Newman, J. Scott Brennen, and Philip N. Howard. 2020. Navigating the ‘Infodemic’: How People in Six Countries Access and Rate News and Information about Coronavirus. Available online: https://reutersinstitute.politics.ox.ac.uk/sites/default/files/2020-04/Navigating%20the%20Coronavirus%20Infodemic%20FINAL.pdf (accessed on 31 August 2022).
  51. Olsson, Eva-Karin, Lars W. Nord, and Jesper Falkheimer. 2015. Media Coverage Crisis Exploitation Characteristics: A Case Comparison Study. Journal of Public Relations Research 27: 158–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Ophir, Yotam. 2018. Coverage of Epidemics in American Newspapers Through the Lens of the Crisis and Emergency Risk Communication Framework. Health Security 16: 147–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Ophir, Yotam. 2019. The Effects of News Coverage of Epidemics on Public Support for and Compliance with the CDC—An Experimental Study. Journal of Health Communication 24: 547–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Otto, Lukas, Isabella Glogger, and Mark Boukes. 2017. The Softening of Journalistic Political Communication: A Comprehensive Framework Model of Sensationalism, Soft News, Infotainment, and Tabloidization: Softening of Journalistic Political Communication. Communication Theory 27: 136–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  55. Pantti, Mervi. 2010. The value of emotion: An examination of television journalists’ notions on emotionality. European Journal of Communication 25: 168–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Pantti, Mervi. 2018. Crisis and Disaster Coverage. In The International Encyclopedia of Journalism Studies. Edited by In Tim P. Vos, Folker Hanusch, Dimitra Dimitrakopoulou, Margaretha Geertsema-Sligh and Annika Sehl. Hoboken: Wiley-Blackwell. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Perreault, Mildred F., and Gregory P. Perreault. 2021. Journalists on COVID-19 Journalism: Communication Ecology of Pandemic Reporting. American Behavioral Scientist 65: 976–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Peter, Christina, and Thomas Zerback. 2020. Ordinary Citizens in the News: A Conceptual Framework. Journalism Studies 21: 1003–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Quandt, Thorsten, and Karin Wahl-Jorgensen. 2022. The Coronavirus Pandemic and the Transformation of (Digital) Journalism. Digital Journalism 10: 923–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Quandt, Thorsten, Svenja Boberg, Tim Schatto-Eckrodt, and Lena Frischlich. 2020. Pandemic News: Facebook Pages of Mainstream News Media and the Coronavirus Crisis—A Computational Content Analysis. arXiv arXiv:2005.13290 [Cs]. Available online: http://arxiv.org/abs/2005.13290 (accessed on 31 August 2022).
  61. Reinemann, Carsten, James Stanyer, Sebastian Scherr, and Guido Legnante. 2012. Hard and soft news: A review of concepts, operationalizations and key findings. Journalism 13: 221–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  62. Ribeiro, Nelson, and Christian Schwarzenegger. 2022. Introduction: Media and Fear—Diachronic, Intermedia, and Transcultural Perspectives on a Toxic and Functional Relationship during Pandemics, Wars, and Political Crises. In Media and the Dissemination of Fear. Edited by Nelson Ribeiro and Christian Schwarzenegger. Berlin: Springer International Publishing, pp. 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Skovsgaard, Morten. 2014. A tabloid mind? Professional values and organizational pressures as explanations of tabloid journalism. Media, Culture & Society 36: 200–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Sorribes, Carles Pont, and Sergi Cortñinas Rovira. 2011. Journalistic practice in risk and crisis situations: Significant examples from Spain. Journalism 12: 1052–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Sowden, Ryann, Erica Borgstrom, and Lucy E. Selman. 2021. ‘It’s like being in a war with an invisible enemy’: A document analysis of bereavement due to COVID-19 in UK newspapers. PLoS ONE 16: e0247904. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Stevenson, Tom. 2020. The Stock Market May Catch a Cold from Coronavirus. The Telegraph. Available online: https://www.telegraph.co.uk/business/2020/02/23/stock-market-may-catch-cold-coronavirus (accessed on 31 August 2022).
  67. Stolow, Jeni A., Lina M. Moses, Alyssa M. Lederer, and Rebecca Carter. 2020. How Fear Appeal Approaches in COVID-19 Health Communication May Be Harming the Global Community. Health Education & Behavior 47: 531–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Tang, Didi. 2020. Coronavirus: High-Ranking Wuhan Doctor Dead from COVID-19. The Times. Available online: https://www.thetimes.co.uk/article/coronavirus-high-ranking-wuhan-doctor-feared-dead-from-covid-19-5r2b0hvf0 (accessed on 31 August 2022).
  69. Tannenbaum, Melanie B., Justin Hepler, Rick S. Zimmerman, Lindsey Saul, Samantha Jacobs, Kristina Wilson, and Dolores Albarracín. 2015. Appealing to fear: A meta-analysis of fear appeal effectiveness and theories. Psychological Bulletin 141: 1178–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Ungar, Sheldon. 1998. Hot Crises and Media Reassurance: A Comparison of Emerging Diseases and Ebola Zaire. The British Journal of Sociology 49: 36–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Uribe, Rodrigo, and Barrie Gunter. 2004. Research Note: The Tabloidization of British Tabloids. European Journal of Communication 19: 387–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. van Bavel, Jay J., Katherine Baicker, Paulo S. Boggio, Valerio Capraro, Aleksandra Cichocka, Mina Cikara, Molly J. Crockett, Alia J. Crum, Jamil Zaki, Sean R. Zion, and et al. 2020. Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nature Human Behaviour 4: 460–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Vasterman, Peter L., and Nel Ruigrok. 2013. Pandemic alarm in the Dutch media: Media coverage of the 2009 influenza A (H1N1) pandemic and the role of the expert sources. European Journal of Communication 28: 436–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Vasterman, Peter, Otto Scholten, and Nel Ruigrok. 2008. A Model for Evaluating Risk Reporting: The Case of UMTS and Fine Particles. European Journal of Communication 23: 319–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Wagner, Anna, and Doreen Reifegerste. 2022. From Black Death to COVID-19: The Mediated Dissemination of Fear in Pandemic Times. In Media and the Dissemination of Fear. Edited by Nelson Ribeiro and Christian Schwarzenegger. Cham: Springer International Publishing, pp. 19–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Wahl-Jorgensen, Karin. 2020a. An Emotional Turn in Journalism Studies? Digital Journalism 8: 175–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Wahl-Jorgensen, Karin. 2020b. Coronavirus: How Media Coverage of Epidemics Often Stokes Fear and Panic. Available online: https://theconversation.com/coronavirus-how-media-coverage-of-epidemics-often-stokes-fear-and-panic-131844 (accessed on 31 August 2022).
  78. Witte, Kim. 1992. Putting the fear back into fear appeals: The extended parallel process model. Communication Monographs 59: 329–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Witte, Kim, and Mike Allen. 2000. A Meta-Analysis of Fear Appeals: Implications for Effective Public Health Campaigns. Health Education & Behavior 27: 591–615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. You, Myoungsoon, Jungmin Joo, Esuri Park, Ghee-Young Noh, and Youngkee Ju. 2017. Emerging Infectious Disease Content in Newspaper Editorials: Public Health Concern or Leadership Issue? Science Communication 39: 313–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Differences over Time.
Figure 1. Differences over Time.
Journalmedia 03 00042 g001
Table 1. Fear Appeals in Coronavirus Coverage.
Table 1. Fear Appeals in Coronavirus Coverage.
Threat ComponentEfficacy Component
OutletThreatFear-Inducing
Language
National ImpactsPersonal
Angle
Individual MeasuresSocietal Measures
HeadlineBody
Times90.5%26%51.9%68.7%27.5%29.8%69.1%
Telegraph93.5%37%54.2%67.6%27.5%38.9%71%
Mirror90.1%46.6%53.4%74%48.1%40.5%69.8%
Sun90.5%45%62.6%67.9%42.7%52.3%70.6%
Overall91.1%38.6%55.5%69.6%36.5%40.4%70.1%
Table 2. Overview of Different Threats.
Table 2. Overview of Different Threats.
CategoryPrevalenceThree Most Common Threats within Category
(%, Descending)
Health threats50.9%Infection (36.6%), Death (10.6%), Insufficient medical treatment (2.2%)
Economic threats15.9%Economic uncertainty (13.6%), Job loss (1.9%), Price spike (0.4%)
Political threats15.7%Restriction of public life (9.4%), Lack of political governance (5.6%), Rise of Chinese soft power (0.8%)
Other threats8.6%Other, openly coded threat (4.7%), Hoarding (1.9%), Racism (0.8%)
No threat identified8.9%
Table 3. Differences between Tabloids and Quality Outlets.
Table 3. Differences between Tabloids and Quality Outlets.
VariableTabloidsQualityChi-Square Test
Threat Component
Threat 90.3%92%χ2(1, N = 1048) = 0.76, p = .38
Fear-ind. language (head.)45.8%31.5%χ2(1, N = 1048) = 22.04, p < .001 ***
Fear-ind. language (body)58%53%χ2(1, N = 1048) = 2.42, p = .12
National impacts71%68.1%χ2(1, N = 1048) = 0.88, p = .35
Personal angle45.4%27.5%χ2(1, N = 1048) = 35.63, p < .001 ***
Efficacy Component
Individual measures46.4%34.4%χ2(1, N = 1048) = 15.24, p < .001 ***
Societal measures70.2%70%χ2(1, N = 1048) = 0, p = .99
Note. *** p < .001.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Hase, V.; Engelke, K.M. Emotions in Crisis Coverage: How UK News Media Used Fear Appeals to Report on the Coronavirus Crisis. Journal. Media 2022, 3, 633-649. https://doi.org/10.3390/journalmedia3040042

AMA Style

Hase V, Engelke KM. Emotions in Crisis Coverage: How UK News Media Used Fear Appeals to Report on the Coronavirus Crisis. Journalism and Media. 2022; 3(4):633-649. https://doi.org/10.3390/journalmedia3040042

Chicago/Turabian Style

Hase, Valerie, and Katherine M. Engelke. 2022. "Emotions in Crisis Coverage: How UK News Media Used Fear Appeals to Report on the Coronavirus Crisis" Journalism and Media 3, no. 4: 633-649. https://doi.org/10.3390/journalmedia3040042

APA Style

Hase, V., & Engelke, K. M. (2022). Emotions in Crisis Coverage: How UK News Media Used Fear Appeals to Report on the Coronavirus Crisis. Journalism and Media, 3(4), 633-649. https://doi.org/10.3390/journalmedia3040042

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop