Next Article in Journal
Carbon Stock Assessment in Gypsum-Bearing Soils: The Role of Subsurface Soil Horizons
Previous Article in Journal
Optimization of Pollutant Discharge Permits, Using the Trading Ratio System: A Case Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Impact of Perceptions of Air Pollution and Noise on Subjective Well-Being and Health

by
Carolina Herrera
1,2,* and
Pablo Cabrera-Barona
1
1
Facultad Latinoamericana de Ciencias Sociales—FLACSO, Quito 170135, Ecuador
2
Centro de Investigación para la Salud en América Latina, Pontificia Universidad Católica del Ecuador, Quito 170143, Ecuador
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Earth 2022, 3(3), 825-838; https://doi.org/10.3390/earth3030047
Submission received: 10 May 2022 / Revised: 7 July 2022 / Accepted: 8 July 2022 / Published: 13 July 2022

Abstract

:
With a growing interest in the study of urban life and health, evidence indicates that the quality of the environment in which we live can have implications for our subjective well-being and health. This study assesses the potential impacts of perceptions of visual air pollution, olfactory air pollution, and noise pollution on self-perceived health, self-perceived happiness, and satisfaction with life, through the calculation of ordinal logistic regressions, using the information of an online survey carried out in Quito, Ecuador. We found that perceptions of unpleasant odors and noise pollution influence self-perceived health, self-perceived happiness, and satisfaction with life. The obtained results may support the incorporation of citizens’ perspectives to better understand environmental pollution and to enrich local planning for urban sustainability.

1. Introduction

Mental disorders materialize their effects on people’s quality of life reducing their opportunities (e.g., obtaining a job or maintaining a normal life) [1], and reducing levels of happiness and satisfaction with life. A lack of economic support, absence of specialized personnel in health units, and little or no investment in prevention are some of the conditions that make the world a hostile place for people with mental health issues. Worldwide, mental disorders constitute the second cause of disability [2]. This situation is aggravated in developing countries, where investment in health promotion is limited. An important percentage of disability-adjusted life years at the national level corresponding to noncommunicable diseases, is related to mental and neurological issues [2,3].
Mental disorders influenced by pollution can disrupt human well-being and quality of life. Well-being and quality of life are multidimensional concepts. However, these concepts can be interpreted in terms of health, satisfaction, and happiness [4]. Works in environmental epidemiology show evidence that cities act as triggers for the aggravation of health issues [5,6,7]. In the field of urban studies, research indicates that there is a strong relationship between the environmental conditions of cities and the possible improvement or deterioration in the health conditions of urban inhabitants [8,9,10]. Additionally, areas with a large number of inhabitants per square meter, usually correspond to areas or neighborhoods with limited resources [6].
Whether through manifestations in physical or mental health, variables such as green areas, security, provision of health services, air quality, and poverty levels are closely linked to urban human well-being [11,12,13]. For instance, areas with a greater presence of industries or high-traffic highways show worse results in quality of life indices [14]. Even when the pollution effects on well-being and health are of high concern, the quantification of these effects considering subjective measures is still a challenge and a priority to better understand psychophysical evaluations of urban pollution [15]. Furthermore, the empirical research on this topic in Latin America is very limited [16]. The present investigation is an exploratory study that contributes to partially closing this research gap.

1.1. Air Pollution and Human Well-Being

Considered one of the great killers of our time, air pollution is associated with around 8.79 million deaths a year worldwide [17]. There is a growing concern about the effects of polluted air on human health since exposure is no longer confined to the home (burning wood/coal for cooking and heating) but extends throughout the environment, especially in cities, where large amounts of the population are concentrated, and, therefore, the number of vehicles and industries, and the amount of waste increase [7], turning cities into places with a high concentration of pollutants [18]. Other factors such as temperature and population density also aggravate the quality of urban life [19]. Some of the most palpable effects of air pollution can be seen in difficulties in the respiratory system, heart, and even in the brain [20,21]. These last effects have implications for our mental health due to inflammatory reactions in the brain [22]. Experiments in mice show how exposure to fine particulate matter, nitrogen dioxide, sulfur dioxide, carbon monoxide (CO), and photochemical oxidants produces neurological responses similar to those in episodes of depression and anxiety [23]. All the pollutants mentioned are present in urban environments originating from different sources such as vehicles, dust, ashes, or soot [24]. Studies have found significant relationships between higher levels of environmental pollution and an increase in the number of visits to hospital emergency services due to suicide attempts [25,26].
The particulates of air pollution could cause neuroinflammation [27] and are linked to structural brain changes [28], which may be associated with illnesses such as depression, anxiety, and stress [29]. One of the mechanisms through which air pollution affects mental health and well-being is the alteration in cytokine production that changes the neural connections of mood [30]. Furthermore, in recent times mental health has been markedly affected by pollution in the context of the pandemic and climate change [31]. Recent research has found associations that indicate that exposure to pollutants could not only produce a higher incidence of mental illness but also generate dependence on antidepressants and benzodiazepines [32]. This dependency has long-term implications as it perpetuates the cycle of a poor quality of life among people.

1.2. Noise and Human Well-Being

Although more conclusive scientific evidence is still needed to validate the effects of noise on health [33], noise pollution can be considered the second-biggest threat to people’s health, just behind air pollution [34].
City dwellers are especially vulnerable to the effects of disturbing noises, which is a type of environmental pollution originating from vehicular traffic, air traffic, car sirens, commerce, industries, entertainment venues, and stadiums, among others [35]. The presence of noise in the urban environment increases in densely populated areas, where the population is affected by negative externalities caused by the excessive concentration of people [36,37]. Even though some of the better-known effects of noise on health are hearing loss and tinnitus (ringing in the ears) [38,39], those are not the only possible consequences. Regarding physical health, exposure to noise pollution has been linked to heart and metabolic diseases, high blood pressure, low birth weight, diabetes, and sleep problems [40,41,42,43]. This last one is the most commented on since as a product of bad rest, there are symptoms of discomfort, a lack of concentration in daily activities, or stress [44,45,46].
A lack of concentration, the inability to adequately perform regular activities, and generalized annoyance are symptoms that have alerted environmental epidemiologists about the possible relationships between noise and well-being [46]. Those adults who report higher levels of annoyance with environmental noise also report worse states of mental health [47]. The consequences of environmental pollution are not limited to the adult population. Pregnant mothers and their babies can be highly affected by excessive noise levels. This exposure at such an early age affects the proper development of the fetus and the long-term implications remain undetermined, although anxiety disorders, impaired spatial memory, and impaired hippocampal plasticity are already being discussed as possible consequences in postnatal life [48].
Despite the amount we have learned in recent decades about the adverse effects of pollution on health, there is still much to be discovered in the field of human health and subjective well-being. Additionally, understanding perceptions regarding urban pollution is an open field to be explored for better bottom-up urban and local planning. In Latin American cities, important advances are evident in the monitoring of environmental pollution [49]. However, it is necessary to take new steps and examine the implications of this pollution on people. Through an evaluation of perceptions of city dwellers, this study aims to assess the impact of perceptions of air and noise pollution on subjective well-being while simultaneously contributing to the development of a baseline for this matter in Latin America. The objective of this study is not to apply a psychological assessment to urban residents but rather to explore possible associations between perceptions of the urban environment and perceptions of health, happiness, and satisfaction with life. The selection of our study area, Quito (Ecuador), is justified since in addition to having environmental monitoring for more than a decade, the geographical conditions make the city a place more likely to encapsulate environmental pollution [50], and there is limited information about pollution perceptions, although this city is a region where there is exposure to pollution and noise for its inhabitants.

2. Materials and Methods

An online survey was designed with 17 questions encompassing the topics of socioeconomic conditions and demographics, air pollution, noise pollution, and well-being-related variables. The survey was carried out in Quito (Figure 1), in January 2020. We applied an online survey in the context of the social distancing applied in Quito due to the COVID-19 pandemic. Using a snowball strategy via E-mail and social networks, we obtained 269 valid responses.
The survey included an informative section where the participant’s consent was requested, a socioeconomic and demographic information section that includes variables of sex, age, education, marital status, and income, a section related to visual/olfactory air pollution and noise pollution, perceived by people in their immediate surroundings (the block surrounding their households), and a section dedicated to the variables of self-perceived health, self-perceived happiness, and satisfaction with life. Socioeconomic and demographic variables were selected based on a literature review of the associations of these variables with mental health and well-being [51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58]. These kinds of variables can be considered significant covariates of urban residents’ subjective well-being and health [59].
All the variables of air and noise pollution used a 4-category Likert scale, where a value of 1 was assigned to the “lower” category and the value of 4 was assigned to the “higher” category. For instance, the categories of the variable of visual air pollution received the following values, Not at all polluted = 1, Slightly polluted = 2, Moderately polluted = 3, and Very polluted = 4, while the categories of the variable of noise pollution (noise affecting well-being), Never, Rarely, Sometimes, and Always, received the values of 1, 2, 3, and 4, respectively.
The variable sex was coded with Female = 1, Male = 2, age and income were coded using a 5-point Likert scale (e.g., 18–21 = 1, >54 = 5; USD 0–USD 400 = 1, USD 2001 or more = 5), education was defined with a 4-point Likert scale (e.g., Basic = 1, Postgraduate studies = 4), and marital status was defined as a nominal variable of 4 categories (Single = 1, Married = 2, Divorced = 3, Widowed = 4).
The dependent variables, self-perceived health, self-perceived happiness, and satisfaction with life used a 4-category Likert scale, where a value of 1 was assigned to the “lower” category and the value of 4 was assigned to the “higher” category. The categories of the variable of self-perceived health received the following values: Not being healthy = 1, Having poor health = 2, Being healthy = 3, and Being very healthy = 4. The categories of the variable of self-perceived happiness received the following values: Not being happy = 1, Having low levels of happiness = 2, Being happy = 3, and Being very happy = 4. The categories of satisfaction with life received the following values: Not being satisfied with life = 1, Having low satisfaction with life = 2, Being satisfied with life = 3, and Being very satisfied with life = 4.
We calculated three ordinal logistic regression models considering the dependent variables of self-perceived health, self-perceived happiness, and satisfaction with life. This investigation uses these variables as proxies to represent people’s well-being and (mental) health. The independent variables used for the models were the variables of perceptions of air pollution and noise pollution, and the socioeconomic and demographic (confounders) variables of sex, age, education, marital status, and income. A variable of perception of the quality of health services was also considered in the models, to control the regressions in terms of healthcare accessibility.
An ordinal logistic regression can be expressed with the following equation
l o g i t ( P ( Y j ) ) = β j 0 + β j 1 X 1 + β j 2 X 2 + + β j p X p
where j = 1 ,   ,   J 1   and p are the predictors X of Y .
Multicollinearity between independent variables was evaluated by applying correlation analyses, before the application of the ordinal logistic regression models. None of the correlations were higher than 0.8.

3. Results

Table 1 summarizes in percentages the socioeconomic and demographic information of the interviewees. In total, 70.60% of the interviewees were female and 29.40% were male. Of the responses received, 43.50% corresponded to the age range of 18 to 24 years, followed by 31.60% in the range of 25 to 34 years old, 8.60% between 35 and 44, 7.10% between 45 and 54, and 9.3% over 54. Regarding the education variable, 15.20% had graduated from high school, 66.50% of the respondents were studying or had completed their higher education, and 16.4% had completed postgraduate studies. In total, 74.30% were single, 21.20% were married, 3.70% were divorced, and 0.70% widowed. Regarding the monthly income of the interviewees, 49.40% reported receiving between USD 0 and USD 400 per month, 24.90% were earning between USD 401 and USD 800, and 10.40% had an income between USD 801 and USD1200.
Table 2 indicates the percentages of responses of each category of the dependent variables and independent variables of perceived pollutions. Most of the interviewees reported being healthy (72.20%), being happy (53.90%), and being satisfied with life (52.80%). Being very happy and being very satisfied with life also had notable percentages, 29.70% and 26.80%, respectively. Regarding visual air pollution, most of the interviewees reported that the air is slightly polluted (51.70%) or moderately polluted (34.20%). In total, 46.50% of people mentioned that they rarely perceive unpleasant odors in their neighborhoods and 37.90% reported that they sometimes perceive unpleasant noises.
Table 3 shows the results of the model with self-perceived health as the dependent variable. Overall, the model explained 27% of the variability of the dependent variable (Nagelkerke R2 = 0.27).
Regarding the olfactory air pollution categories, “Never” was found to be significant at 90% confidence to explain self-perceived health. The odds ratio (OR) suggests that perceiving good health was 4.00 times greater for respondents who reported never perceiving unpleasant odors than for those who perceive unpleasant odors.
For the noise pollution variable, the experience of discomfort “Sometimes”, could explain self-perceived health at 95% confidence. The relative odds of perceiving good health were 2.38 times greater for respondents who reported sometimes perceiving disturbing noises than for those who continuously perceive unpleasant odors. The healthcare services’ quality was also found to be significant.
Being in the age range between 18 and 44, and having a high school/higher education influenced self-perceived health. Having an income between USD 1601 and USD 2000 was also found to be significant at 90% confidence.
Table 4 shows the results of the regression model with the dependent variable of self-perceived happiness. The Nagelkerke coefficient R2 was 0.313; that is, the categories found to be significant explained the variability in self-perceived happiness up to 31%.
The level of self-perceived happiness could be explained by the perception of air pollution (unpleasant odors) in its categories of “Never” (99% of confidence) and “Sometimes” (95% of confidence), with positive estimates of 2.12 and 1.17, respectively. The OR shows that perceiving happiness was 8.36 and 3.23 times greater for those who never and sometimes (respectively) perceive unpleasant odors than for those who always perceive unpleasant odors.
Regarding the noise pollution variable, the category “Never” was highly significant in explaining self-perceived happiness. The relative odds of perceiving happiness were 6.62 times greater for respondents reporting never perceiving noises that could affect their well-being than for those perceiving disturbing noises. The healthcare services’ quality was also found significant.
Basic education and higher education could explain self-perceived happiness at 90% confidence. Marital status was a significant variable to explain self-perceived happiness.
Table 5 shows the results of the regression model with the dependent variable of satisfaction with life. The Nagelkerke coefficient R2 was 0.292.
“Never” and “Sometimes” having perceived unpleasant odors in their neighborhoods could affect the variable of satisfaction with life at 95% confidence. The relative odds of being satisfied with life were 5.49 and 3.13 times greater for respondents who reported never and sometimes perceiving unpleasant odors, respectively, than for those who reported always perceiving unpleasant odors.
“Never” having perceived disturbing noises in the neighborhood influenced satisfaction with life; that is, the relative odds of being satisfied with life were 5.46 times greater for those who never perceived a disturbing noise than for those who reported the perception of disturbing noise.
Age between 25 and 34, having a basic education, being single, being divorced, and healthcare services’ quality could also influence satisfaction with life.

4. Discussion

This study evaluates the possible influence of perceptions of air pollution/noise on perceptions of health, satisfaction with life, and happiness. The study also uses socioeconomic and demographic determinants to be assessed in the performed models.
We found influences of air pollution (unpleasant odors) and noise on the dependent variables: never perceiving unpleasant odors in the air significantly influences self-perceived health, self-perceived happiness, and satisfaction with life; never perceiving disturbing noises impacts self-perceived happiness and satisfaction with life; sometimes perceiving disturbing noises may influence self-perceived health. The results of the present study are in line with previous research that found significant influences of odor and noise annoyances on subjective well-being and health [60]. These findings have practical implications regarding the quality of life and public health for urban residents. For instance, people perceiving unpleasant odors more often move out of their homes [61], and odor pollution can trigger symptoms of poor health [62], while noise pollution affects mental well-being causing symptoms such as anxiety and stress in urban dwellers [59]. The level of recognition of the damage caused by noise, although still in development, is much lower than what we know about the effects of other types of environmental pollution [63]. However, our results are consistent in terms of having low or null levels of noise associated with being satisfied with life and being happy.
We did not find an influence of visual air pollution on the dependent variables. According to the air quality reports from the Office of Environment of the Municipality of Quito, the quality of air in Quito remained within the “Desirable” categories (0–50) and “Acceptable” (50–100) in the IQCA (local air quality index). The IQCA [64] is a reference index based on international recommendations and can be compared to the AQI (Air Quality Index). This finding is in accordance with previous research [18], which states that polluted air is not a perceivable problem until it exceeds 140 AQI and does not cause discomfort/discomfort with the environment until it exceeds 150 in the AQI. Considering that the values of air pollution in Quito have not exceeded these limits, this result could also support why air quality does not appear as the main problem to be addressed in Quito, as indicated by the “Citizen Perception Survey” of the project “Quito cómo vamos” [65]. Along the same lines, the consistency in all the models of significant values for the variable of “Never” experiencing unpleasant odors contrasts with the relatively low pollution rates recorded in the city. However, this situation does not rule out the potential health effects of air pollution, especially among the most vulnerable people [8,66].
The quality of healthcare services was found to be significant for explaining the dependent variables. Access to healthcare services of quality is an element of people’s well-being. Previous research has found that access to healthcare services of quality is associated with well-being perceptions, such as healthcare satisfaction and self-reported health [67,68]. By not considering healthcare as an efficient service, individuals can be affected in such a way that their perceptions of health, happiness, and satisfaction with life are impaired, which would ultimately lead to a greater degree of dissatisfaction and lower mental health.
Age ranging between 18 and 44 may influence self-perceived health and satisfaction with life. As indicated by previous research, generations between these age ranges correspond to those who in recent years have shown greater concern for their comprehensive health and greater interest in receiving psychological therapy [69]. Basic education may explain self-perceived happiness and satisfaction with life, and higher education could influence self-perceived happiness. Previous research has found associations between education and satisfaction with life [70], as well as other studies that have shown that education creates conditions that positively relate to happiness [71], both results closely related to the ones of this study. In the city of Quito, there is a disproportion in the levels of attention that women and men give to environmental pollution [72]. This author indicates that women have a greater concern for the environment. However, in the case of our investigation, none of the models produced results that indicated something similar: the sex of the participants remained a non-significant variable.

Limitations of the Study

The present research has some limitations, which, nevertheless, are opportunities for future investigations. The used survey is a convenience survey, carried out during the COVID-19 pandemic social distancing measures. Thus, although the findings of this investigation are in accordance with previous research, these results should not be extended to interpret the reality of the entire population of the study area, and the significance levels of the independent variables should be interpreted with prudence. This suggestion goes in line with the possibility of misinterpreting the effects of exposure variables (e.g., unpleasant noise) due to the presence of confounders in the models (e.g., sex, age) [73], and with the presence of large confidence intervals in some independent variables, which indicates a sample size that does not represent the entire population. We suggest that future researches related to this study apply different survey strategies to represent the entire population and experiment with different models with additional confounders, and without any confounders.
The considered dependent variables are useful measures of subjective well-being and health. However, these measures cannot be considered a standard of assessment of mental health. Even when the aim of this study was not to medically diagnose any mental disorder, we believe that other measures can be used to represent subjective well-being and health. For instance, future research can use the DASS scale (Depression Anxiety Stress Scales), developed to measure negative emotional states [74]. We did not consider any biomedical factor as an independent variable, such as medication use or diagnosis of mental health. Although our study is not biomedical, we believe that future investigations may incorporate additional independent variables representing the physical and medical condition of interviewees. The present study did not consider any pollutant measure as an independent variable due to the aim of our research being centered on perceptions. The calculated models are applied at an individual level, and, usually, data on pollutants are obtained at the area level. However, we believe that the regression models can be enriched by incorporating pollutant measures as independent variables, and we consider that succeeding investigations can incorporate these kinds of measures to assess mental health and well-being.

5. Conclusions

The results of this investigation show the influence of olfactory pollution and noise pollution on self-perceived health, self-perceived happiness, and satisfaction with life. The obtained results also show the importance of socioeconomic and demographic determinants to assess subjective well-being. A better quality of the environment can determine better subjective well-being that, consequently, may become a barrier against poor mental health. Our study reveals the importance of considering people’s perceptions when assessing environmental and health/well-being conditions of urban environments. We argue that environmental quality should not only be interpreted in terms of physical measures but also in terms of qualitative measures obtained from urban inhabitants. Furthermore, our study is part of a shift in the way how urban social–environmental links are measured, a change that is oriented towards applying more social and pluralistic approaches, beyond the use of traditional physical–environmental variables. We expect that the results of the present research can be considered by local stakeholders and decision makers to incorporate citizens’ perspectives in urban environmental planning and pollution scrutiny, to construct a more inclusive sustainable future for the city.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.H. and P.C.-B.; methodology, C.H. and P.C.-B.; software, C.H.; validation, C.H. and P.C.-B.; formal analysis, C.H.; investigation, C.H.; resources, C.H.; data curation, C.H.; writing—original draft preparation, C.H.; writing—review and editing, P.C.-B.; supervision, P.C.-B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Ethical review and approval were waived for this study due to the applied sampling strategy and the design of the survey considering privacy issues.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Corrigan, P.W.; Watson, A.C. Understanding the Impact of Stigma on People with Mental Illness. World Psychiatry 2002, 1, 16–20. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  2. OPS. La Carga de Los Trastornos Mentales En La Región de Las Américas; Organización Panamericana de la Salud: Washington, WA, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  3. Kassebaum, N.J.; Arora, M.; Barber, R.M.; Brown, J.; Carter, A.; Casey, D.C.; Charlson, F.J.; Coates, M.M.; Coggeshall, M.; Cornaby, L.; et al. Global, Regional, and National Disability-Adjusted Life-Years (DALYs) for 315 Diseases and Injuries and Healthy Life Expectancy (HALE), 1990–2015: A Systematic Analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2015. Lancet 2016, 388, 1603–1658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Veenhoven, R. The Four Qualities of Life. J. Happiness Stud. 2000, 1, 1–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Vassos, E.; Pedersen, C.B.; Murray, R.M.; Collier, D.A.; Lewis, C.M. Meta-Analysis of the Association of Urbanicity with Schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 2012, 38, 1118–1123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Heinz, A.; Deserno, L.; Reininghaus, U. Urbanicity, Social Adversity and Psychosis. World Psychiatry 2013, 12, 187–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  7. Landrigan, P.J. Air Pollution and Health. Lancet Public Health 2017, 2, e4–e5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Galea, S.; Ahern, J.; Nandi, A.; Tracy, M.; Beard, J.; Vlahov, D. Urban Neighborhood Poverty and the Incidence of Depression in a Population-Based Cohort Study. Ann. Epidemiol. 2007, 17, 171–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Galea, S.; Freudenberg, N.; Vlahov, D. Cities and Population Health. Soc. Sci. Med. 2005, 60, 1017–1033. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Gruebner, O.; Rapp, M.A.; Adli, M.; Kluge, U.; Galea, S.; Heinz, A. Cities and Mental Health. Dtsch. Arztebl. Int. 2017, 114, 121–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Caracci, G. General Concepts of the Relationship between Urban Areas and Mental Health. Curr. Opin. Psychiatry 2008, 21, 385–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Marangoni, C.; Hernandez, M.; Faedda, G.L. The Role of Environmental Exposures as Risk Factors for Bipolar Disorder: A Systematic Review of Longitudinal Studies. J. Affect. Disord. 2016, 193, 165–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Bortolato, B.; Köhler, C.A.; Evangelou, E.; León-Caballero, J.; Solmi, M.; Stubbs, B.; Belbasis, L.; Pacchiarotti, I.; Kessing, L.V.; Berk, M.; et al. Systematic Assessment of Environmental Risk Factors for Bipolar Disorder: An Umbrella Review of Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses. Bipolar Disord. 2017, 19, 84–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Srinivasan, S.; O’Fallon, L.R.; Dearry, A. Creating Healthy Communities, Healthy Homes, Healthy People: Initiating a Research Agenda on the Built Environment and Public Health. Am. J. Public Health 2003, 93, 1446–1450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Li, Y.; Guan, D.; Tao, S.; Wang, X.; He, K. A Review of Air Pollution Impact on Subjective Well-Being: Survey versus Visual Psychophysics. J. Clean. Prod. 2018, 184, 959–968. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Ahumada, G.; Iturra, V. If the Air Was Cleaner, Would We Be Happier? An Economic Assessment of the Effects of Air Pollution on Individual Subjective Well-Being in Chile. J. Clean. Prod. 2021, 289, 125152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Lelieveld, J.; Klingmüller, K.; Pozzer, A.; Pöschl, U.; Fnais, M.; Daiber, A.; Münzel, T. Cardiovascular Disease Burden from Ambient Air Pollution in Europe Reassessed Using Novel Hazard Ratio Functions. Eur. Heart J. 2019, 40, 1590–1596. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Li, Y.; Guan, D.; Yu, Y.; Westland, S.; Wang, D.; Meng, J.; Wang, X.; He, K.; Tao, S. A Psychophysical Measurement on Subjective Well-Being and Air Pollution. Nat. Commun. 2019, 10, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Shaposhnikov, D.; Revich, B.; Bellander, T.; Bedada, G.B.; Bottai, M.; Kharkova, T.; Kvasha, E.; Lezina, E.; Lind, T.; Semutnikova, E.; et al. Mortality Related to Air Pollution with the Moscow Heat Wave and Wildfire of 2010. Epidemiology 2014, 25, 359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Block, M.L.; Calderón-Garcidueñas, L. Air Pollution: Mechanisms of Neuroinflammation and CNS Disease. Trends Neurosci. 2009, 32, 506–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Mills, N.L.; Donaldson, K.; Hadoke, P.W.; Boon, N.A.; MacNee, W.; Cassee, F.R.; Sandström, T.; Blomberg, A.; Newby, D.E. Adverse Cardiovascular Effects of Air Pollution. Nat. Clin. Pract. Cardiovasc. Med. 2009, 6, 36–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Khan, A.; Plana-Ripoll, O.; Antonsen, S.; Brandt, J.; Geels, C.; Landecker, H.; Sullivan, P.F.; Pedersen, C.B.; Rzhetsky, A. Environmental Pollution Is Associated with Increased Risk of Psychiatric Disorders in the US and Denmark. PLoS Biol. 2019, 17, e3000353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Braithwaite, I.; Zhang, S.; Kirkbride, J.B.; Osborn, D.P.J.; Hayes, J.F. Air Pollution (Particulate Matter) Exposure and Associations with Depression, Anxiety, Bipolar, Psychosis and Suicide Risk: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Environ. Health Perspect. 2020, 127, 126002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Linares Gil, C. Las PM 2.5 y Su Impacto Sobre La Salud. El Caso de La Ciudad de Madrid. Ecosostenible 2007, 35, 32–37. [Google Scholar]
  25. Oudin, A.; Carlsen, H.; Åström, D.; Asplund, P.; Steingrimsson, S.; Szabo, Z. The Association between Daily Concentrations of Air Pollution and Visits to a Psychiatric Emergency Unit: A Case-Crossover Study. Environ. Health 2018, 17, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  26. Szyszkowicz, M.; Willey, J.B.; Grafstein, E.; Rowe, B.H.; Colman, I. Air Pollution and Emergency Department Visits for Suicide Attempts in Vancouver, Canada. Environ. Health Insights 2010, 4, EHI-S5662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Yang, Z.; Song, Q.; Li, J.; Zhang, Y.; Yuan, X.C.; Wang, W.; Yu, Q. Air Pollution and Mental Health: The Moderator Effect of Health Behaviors. Environ. Res. Lett. 2021, 16, 044005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Sørensen, M.; Daneshvar, B.; Hansen, M.; Dragsted, L.O.; Hertel, O.; Knudsen, L.; Loft, S. Personal PM2.5 Exposure and Markers of Oxidative Stress in Blood. Environ. Health Perspect. 2003, 111, 161–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Salim, S.; Chugh, G.; Asghar, M. Chapter One—Inflammation in Anxiety. In Advances in Protein Chemistry and Structural Biology; Donev, R., Ed.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2012; Volume 88, pp. 1–25. ISBN 1876-1623. [Google Scholar]
  30. MohanKumar, S.M.J.; Campbell, A.; Block, M.; Veronesi, B. Particulate Matter, Oxidative Stress and Neurotoxicity. Neurotoxicology 2008, 29, 479–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Marazziti, D.; Cianconi, P.; Mucci, F.; Foresi, L.; Chiarantini, I.; Della Vecchia, A. Climate Change, Environment Pollution, COVID-19 Pandemic and Mental Health. Sci. Total Environ. 2021, 773, 145182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Vert, C.; Sánchez-Benavides, G.; Martínez, D.; Gotsens, X.; Gramunt, N.; Cirach, M.; Molinuevo, J.L.; Sunyer, J.; Nieuwenhuijsen, M.J.; Crous-Bou, M.; et al. Effect of Long-Term Exposure to Air Pollution on Anxiety and Depression in Adults: A Cross-Sectional Study. Int. J. Hyg. Environ. Health 2017, 220, 1074–1080. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. WHO Regional Office for Europe. Noise Guidelines for the European Region; WHO: Geneva, Switzerland, 2018; ISBN 9789289053563. [Google Scholar]
  34. World Health Organization. Burden of Disease from Environmental Noise: Quantification of Healthy Life Years Lost in Europe; WHO: Copenhagen, Denmark, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  35. Sampath, S.; Das, S.M.; Kumar, V.S. Ambient Noise Levels in Major Cities in Kerala. J. Ind. Geophys. Union 2004, 8, 293–298. [Google Scholar]
  36. Yuan, M.; Yin, C.; Sun, Y.; Chen, W. Examining the Associations between Urban Built Environment and Noise Pollution in High-Density High-Rise Urban Areas: A Case Study in Wuhan, China. Sustain. Cities Soc. 2019, 50, 101678. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Carozzi, F.; Roth, S. Dirty Density: Air Quality and the Density of American Cities. SSRN 2020, 13191, 1–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Centros Para el Control y la Prevención de Enfermedades CDC. Tipos de Pérdida Auditiva (Sordera). Available online: https://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/spanish/hearingloss/types.html (accessed on 9 May 2022).
  39. MedlinePlus. Tinnitus. Available online: https://medlineplus.gov/spanish/tinnitus.html (accessed on 9 May 2022).
  40. Gan, W.Q.; Davies, H.W.; Koehoorn, M.; Brauer, M. Association of Long-Term Exposure to Community Noise and Traffic-Related Air Pollution With Coronary Heart Disease Mortality. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2012, 175, 898–906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Münzel, T.; Sørensen, M.; Gori, T.; Schmidt, F.P.; Rao, X.; Brook, J.; Chen, L.C.; Brook, R.D.; Rajagopalan, S. Environmental Stressors and Cardio-Metabolic Disease: Part I–Epidemiologic Evidence Supporting a Role for Noise and Air Pollution and Effects of Mitigation Strategies. Eur. Heart J. 2017, 38, 550–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Cui, B.; Gai, Z.; She, X.; Wang, R.; Xi, Z. Effects of Chronic Noise on Glucose Metabolism and Gut Microbiota–Host Inflammatory Homeostasis in Rats. Sci. Rep. 2016, 6, 36693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Halperin, D. Environmental Noise and Sleep Disturbances: A Threat to Health? Sleep Sci. 2014, 7, 209–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Langdon, F.J.; Buller, I.B. Road Traffic Noise and Disturbance to Sleep. J. Sound Vib. 1977, 50, 13–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Perron, S.; Plante, C.; Ragettli, M.; Kaiser, D.; Goudreau, S.; Smargiassi, A. Sleep Disturbance from Road Traffic, Railways, Airplanes and from Total Environmental Noise Levels in Montreal. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13, 809. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Eriksson, C.; Bodin, T.; Selander, J. Burden of Disease from Road Traffic and Railway Noise—A Quantification of Healthy Life Years Lost in Sweden. Scand. J. Work. Environ. Health 2017, 43, 519–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Hammersen, F.; Niemann, H.; Hoebel, J. Environmental Noise Annoyance and Mental Health in Adults: Findings from the Cross-Sectional German Health Update (GEDA) Study 2012. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2016, 13, 954. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Barzegar, M.; Sajjadi, F.S.; Talaei, S.A.; Hamidi, G.; Salami, M. Prenatal Exposure to Noise Stress: Anxiety, Impaired Spatial Memory, and Deteriorated Hippocampal Plasticity in Postnatal Life. Hippocampus 2015, 25, 187–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Riojas-Rodríguez, H.; Soares Da Silva, A.; Texcalac-Sangrador, J.L.; Moreno-Banda, G.L. Air Pollution Management and Control in Latin America and the Caribbean: Implications for Climate Change. Rev. Panam. Salud Pública 2016, 40, 150–159. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  50. Páez, C. Gestión de La Contaminación Atmosférica Urbana: El Caso de Quito; FlacsoAndes: Quito, Ecuador, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  51. Judd, F.; Armstrong, S.; Kulkarni, J. Gender-Sensitive Mental Health Care. Australas. Psychiatry 2009, 17, 105–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Inglehart, R. Gender, Aging, and Subjective Well-Being. Int. J. Comp. Sociol. 2002, 43, 391–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Diener, E.; Suh, M.E. Subjective Well-Being and Age: An International Analysis. Annu. Rev. Gerontol. Geriatr. 1998, 17, 304–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Golberstein, E. The Effects of Income on Mental Health: Evidence from the Social Security Notch. J. Ment. Health Policy Econ. 2015, 18, 27–37. [Google Scholar]
  55. Gardner, J.; Oswald, A.J. Money and Mental Wellbeing: A Longitudinal Study of Medium-Sized Lottery Wins. J. Health Econ. 2007, 26, 49–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Zepke, N. Lifelong Education for Subjective Well-Being: How Do Engagement and Active Citizenship Contribute? Int. J. Lifelong Educ. 2013, 32, 639–651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Kristoffersen, I. Great Expectations: Education and Subjective Wellbeing. J. Econ. Psychol. 2018, 66, 64–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Bailey, T.C.; Snyder, C.R. Satisfaction with Life and Hope: A Look at Age and Marital Status. Psychol. Rec. 2007, 57, 233–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Ma, J.; Li, C.; Kwan, M.-P.; Chai, Y. A Multilevel Analysis of Perceived Noise Pollution, Geographic Contexts and Mental Health in Beijing. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 1479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  60. Oiamo, T.H.; Luginaah, I.N.; Baxter, J. Cumulative Effects of Noise and Odour Annoyances on Environmental and Health Related Quality of Life. Soc. Sci. Med. 2015, 146, 191–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Wojnarowska, M.; Plichta, G.; Sagan, A.; Plichta, J.; Stobiecka, J.; Sołtysik, M. Odour Nuisance and Urban Residents’ Quality of Life: A Case Study in Kraków’s in Plaszow District. Urban Clim. 2020, 34, 100704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Shusterman, D. The Health Significance of Environmental Odour Pollution: Revisited. J. Environ. Med. 1999, 1, 249–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Shroff, F.M.; Jung, D. Here’s to Sound Action on Global Hearing Health through Public Health Approaches. Int. J. Health Gov. 2020, 25, 235–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Secretaría de Ambiente AQI|Municipio Del Distrito Metropolitano de Quito. Available online: http://www.quitoambiente.gob.ec/index.php/indice-de-calidad-del-aire (accessed on 9 May 2022).
  65. Quito Cómo Vamos. Encuesta de Percepción Ciudadana de Quito; Quito Cómo Vamos: Quito, Ecuador, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  66. Makri, A.; Stilianakis, N.I. Vulnerability to Air Pollution Health Effects. Int. J. Hyg. Environ. Health 2008, 211, 326–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Cabrera-Barona, P.; Blaschke, T.; Gaona, G. Deprivation, Healthcare Accessibility and Satisfaction: Geographical Context and Scale Implications. Appl. Spat. Anal. Policy 2018, 11, 313–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Cabrera-Barona, P. Influence of Urban Multi-Criteria Deprivation and Spatial Accessibility to Healthcare on Self-Reported Health. Urban Sci. 2017, 1, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. American Psychological Association. Stress in AmericaTM Generation Z; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, WA, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  70. Tobias, M.I.; Cheung, J. Monitoring Health Inequalities: Life Expectancy and Small Area Deprivation in New Zealand. Popul. Health Metr. 2003, 1, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  71. Chen, W. How Education Enhances Happiness: Comparison of Mediating Factors in Four East Asian Countries. Soc. Indic. Res. 2012, 106, 117–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Contreras, J. El Impacto de La Contaminación Del Aire En La Vida y En Las Percepciones de Género En La Ciudad de Quito; FLACSO Sede Ecuador: Quito, Ecuador, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  73. Westreich, D.; Greenland, S. The Table 2 Fallacy: Presenting and Interpreting Confounder and Modifier Coefficients. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2013, 177, 292–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  74. Psychology Foundation of Australia Depression Anxiety Stress Scales—DASS. Available online: http://www2.psy.unsw.edu.au/groups/dass/ (accessed on 9 May 2022).
Figure 1. Study area.
Figure 1. Study area.
Earth 03 00047 g001
Table 1. Summary of socioeconomic and demographic variables.
Table 1. Summary of socioeconomic and demographic variables.
VariableCategory Percentage
SexFemale70.60
Male29.40
Age18–2443.50
25–3431.60
35–448.60
45–547.10
>549.30
EducationBasic1.90
High school15.20
Higher education66.50
Postgraduate studies16.40
Marital statusSingle74.30
Married21.20
Divorced3.70
Widow0.70
IncomeUSD 0–USD 40049.40
USD 401–USD 80024.90
USD 801–USD 120010.40
USD 1201–USD 16005.60
USD 1601–USD 20004.80
USD 2001 or more4.80
Table 2. Summary of dependent variables and independent variables of perceived pollutions.
Table 2. Summary of dependent variables and independent variables of perceived pollutions.
VariableCategory Percentage
Self-perceived healthNot being healthy0.70
Having poor health14.50
Being healthy72.20
Being very healthy 12.60
Self-perceived happinessNot being happy3.00
Having low levels of happiness13.40
Being happy 53.90
Being very happy29.70
Satisfaction with lifeNot being satisfied with life3.30
Having low satisfaction with life17.10
Being satisfied with life52.80
Being very satisfied with life26.80
Air pollution (visual)Not at all polluted7.80
Slightly polluted51.70
Moderately polluted34.20
Very polluted6.30
Air pollution (unpleasant odors)Never9.70
Rarely46.50
Sometimes36.80
Always7.10
Noises affecting well-beingNever7.10
Rarely29.70
Sometimes37.90
Always25.30
Table 3. Results of the regression model with the dependent variable of self-perceived health.
Table 3. Results of the regression model with the dependent variable of self-perceived health.
VariableCategoryEstimatep-ValueOdds Ratio95% Confidence Intervals
LowerUpper
Air pollution (visual)Not at all polluted0.320.701.380.277.13
Slightly polluted0.430.511.530.445.39
Moderately polluted0.410.531.510.435.27
Very polluted0-1--
Air pollution (unpleasant odors)Never1.390.08 *4.000.8319.42
Rarely0.440.481.550.455.27
Sometimes0.520.401.680.495.67
Always0-1--
Noises affecting well-beingNever0.890.192.430.649.17
Rarely0.390.361.470.633.42
Sometimes0.870.02 **2.381.115.09
Always0-1--
Healthcare services qualityNot at all efficient−2.120.01 **0.120.020.59
A little efficient−2.870.00 ***0.060.010.22
Moderately efficient−2.120.00 ***0.120.030.45
Very efficient0-1--
SexFemale−0.570.100.570.291.13
Male0-1--
Age18–241.440.04 **4.231.0816.56
25–341.260.06 *3.520.9712.75
35–441.9210.01 **6.831.5929.24
45–540.300.681.360.325.69
>540-1--
EducationBasic−0.070.950.940.099.54
High school−1.2090.06 *0.290.091.03
Higher education−0.820.09 *0.440.171.13
Postgraduate studies0-1--
Marital statusSingle−0.800.660.450.0113.51
Married−0.590.750.550.0217.58
Divorced1.340.493.830.09159.81
Widow0-1--
IncomeUSD 0–USD 4000.180.841.190.216.61
USD 401–USD 8000.370.651.440.287.49
USD 801–USD 12001.360.123.890.6722.44
USD 1201–USD 1600−1.010.250.360.072.02
USD 1601–USD 20001.580.09 *4.870.7431.89
USD 2001 or more0-1--
Levels of significance: *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1.
Table 4. Results of the regression model with the dependent variable of self-perceived happiness.
Table 4. Results of the regression model with the dependent variable of self-perceived happiness.
VariableCategoryEstimatep-ValueOdds Ratio95% Confidence Intervals
LowerUpper
Air pollution (visual)Not at all polluted−0.320.680.720.163.34
Slightly polluted−0.870.150.420.131.34
Moderately polluted−0.440.460.640.202.04
Very polluted0-1--
Air pollution (unpleasant odors)Never2.120.00 ***8.361.8936.88
Rarely0.470.401.590.544.74
Sometimes1.170.03 **3.231.099.57
Always0-1--
Noises affecting well-beingNever1.890.00 ***6.621.9422.56
Rarely0.470.221.590.763.35
Sometimes0.490.151.630.843.16
Always0-1--
Healthcare services qualityNot at all efficient−2.840.00 ***0.060.010.32
A little efficient−2.080.01 **0.120.030.58
Moderately efficient−1.430.06 *0.240.051.10
Very efficient0-1--
SexFemale0.070.821.070.591.94
Male0-1--
Age18–24−0.290.660.750.212.67
25–34−0.780.220.460.131.56
35–440.310.651.360.365.17
45–54−0.410.590.670.162.78
>540-1--
EducationBasic2.620.05 *13.771.06179.29
High school0.210.701.230.423.67
Higher education−0.800.06 *0.450.191.03
Postgraduate studies0-1--
Marital statusSingle2.960.04 **19.231.26293.09
Married3.120.03 **22.681.39370.72
Divorced3.390.03 **29.851.39638.41
Widow0-1--
IncomeUSD 0–USD 400−0.890.270.410.091.91
USD 401–USD 800−0.360.640.690.163.07
USD 801–USD 12000.820.322.260.4710.95
USD 1201–USD 1600−0.620.450.540.112.57
USD 1601–USD 2000−0.150.870.860.154.82
USD 2001 or more0-1--
Levels of significance: *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1.
Table 5. Results of the regression model with the dependent variable of satisfaction with life.
Table 5. Results of the regression model with the dependent variable of satisfaction with life.
VariableCategoryEstimatep-ValueOdds
Ratio
95% Confidence Intervals
LowerUpper
Air pollution (visual)Not at all polluted−0.380.610.690.162.95
Slightly polluted−0.560.320.570.181.74
Moderately polluted0.220.691.250.413.82
Very polluted0-1--
Air pollution (unpleasant odors)Never1.700.02 **5.491.3422.46
Rarely0.390.471.490.524.25
Sometimes1.140.04 **3.131.098.97
Always0a-1--
Noises affecting well-beingNever1.690.00 ***5.461.6518.07
Rarely0.240.521.270.602.65
Sometimes0.160.631.170.612.23
Always0-1--
Healthcare services qualityNot at all efficient−1.780.02 **0.170.040.74
A little efficient−1.690.01 **0.180.050.66
Moderately efficient−0.780.240.460.131.62
Very efficient0-1--
SexFemale0.040.891.040.591.85
Male0-1--
Age18–240.270.661.320.404.34
25–34−0.990.09 *0.370.121.16
35–440.510.431.670.475.92
45–540.460.511.580.416.02
>540-1--
EducationBasic3.870.00 ***47.943.44668.73
High school0.200.711.220.433.45
Higher education−0.190.640.830.381.81
Postgraduate studies0-1--
Marital statusSingle2.690.06 *14.700.92233.69
Married2.330.1110.280.617171.46
Divorced2.890.07 *17.930.844380.84
Widow0-1--
IncomeUSD 0–USD 400−1.190.120.300.071.28
USD 401–USD 800−0.780.290.4580.111.86
USD 801–USD 12000.770.332.160.499.43
USD 1201–USD 1600−0.710.370.4890.1102.168
USD 1601–USD 2000−0.240.770.790.163.95
USD 2001 or more0-1--
Levels of significance: *** p < 0.01, ** p < 0.05, * p < 0.1.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Herrera, C.; Cabrera-Barona, P. Impact of Perceptions of Air Pollution and Noise on Subjective Well-Being and Health. Earth 2022, 3, 825-838. https://doi.org/10.3390/earth3030047

AMA Style

Herrera C, Cabrera-Barona P. Impact of Perceptions of Air Pollution and Noise on Subjective Well-Being and Health. Earth. 2022; 3(3):825-838. https://doi.org/10.3390/earth3030047

Chicago/Turabian Style

Herrera, Carolina, and Pablo Cabrera-Barona. 2022. "Impact of Perceptions of Air Pollution and Noise on Subjective Well-Being and Health" Earth 3, no. 3: 825-838. https://doi.org/10.3390/earth3030047

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop