Next Article in Journal
Post-Mortem 7.0-Tesla Magnetic Resonance Imaging of the Hippocampus in Progressive Supranuclear Palsy with and without Cerebral Amyloid Angiopathy
Previous Article in Journal
Systems Biology in Neuroscience: the Paramount Importance of Data Sharing and Citation
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Blockade of Catecholamine Reuptake in the Prelimbic Cortex Decreases Top-Down Attentional Control in Response to Novel, but not Familiar Appetitive Distracters, within a Timing Paradigm

Interdisciplinary Program in Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, USTAR BioInnovations Center, Utah State University, Logan, UT 84322, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
NeuroSci 2020, 1(2), 99-114; https://doi.org/10.3390/neurosci1020010
Submission received: 27 September 2020 / Revised: 2 November 2020 / Accepted: 4 December 2020 / Published: 8 December 2020

Abstract

:
Emotionally charged distracters delay timing behavior. Increasing catecholamine levels within the prelimbic cortex has beneficial effects on timing by decreasing the delay after aversive distracters. Here we examined whether increasing catecholamine levels within the prelimbic cortex also protects against the deleterious timing delays caused by novel distracters or by familiar appetitive distracters. Rats were trained in a peak-interval procedure and tested in trials with either a novel (unreinforced) distracter, a familiar appetitive (food-reinforced) distracter, or no distracter after being locally infused with catecholamine reuptake blocker nomifensine within the prelimbic cortex. Prelimbic infusion of nomifensine did not alter timing accuracy and precision. However, it increased the delay caused by novel distracters in an inverted-U dose-dependent manner, while being ineffective for appetitive distracters. Together with previous data, these results suggest that catecholaminergic modulation of prelimbic top-down attentional control of interval timing varies with distracter’s valence: prelimbic catecholamines increase attentional control when presented with familiar aversive distracters, have no effect on familiar neutral or familiar appetitive distracters, and decrease it when presented with novel distracters. These findings detail complex interactions between catecholaminergic modulation of attention to timing and nontemporal properties of stimuli, which should be considered when developing therapeutic methods for attentional or affective disorders.

1. Introduction

Time estimation is critical for human and animal survival. For instance, the anticipation of an approaching car allows a pedestrian or an animal to know when it is safe to cross. Interval timing—perception of time within the seconds-to-minutes range [1]—is thought to underlie adaptive behaviors such as action, decision making, and rate calculation [2,3,4]. Diverting attention away distorts timing and impairs cognitive processing and timed responses [5,6,7].
In interval timing tasks, such as the peak-interval procedure, subjects are trained to respond to a particular time criterion when reward is expected. In these procedures, timing can be interrupted by task-irrelevant distracters, such as auditory stimuli, which results in a delay in responding [8,9,10]. The Relative Time-Sharing (RTS) model [6,9,11] proposes that a limited pool of attentional resources is shared by all cognitive processes including timing. When distracters are presented, attentional resources are diverted away from the primary timing task and reallocated towards the processing of the distracter, which results in a delay in timed responses. Depending on the salience of the distracter [8], the subject may ignore the distracter (“run”, no delay), may interrupt timing during the distracter (“stop”, delay equal to the distracter), may restart timing immediately after the distracter (“reset”) [6], or may wait much longer after the distracter before restarting timing (“over-reset”) [5,7,12,13,14].
The medial prefrontal cortex is thought to play an important role in timing [15,16,17,18,19], as well as in other processes such as inhibitory control, working memory, and decision-making [20,21,22]. The prelimbic cortex (PrL) has been identified as a brain region involved in time discrimination [23] and as a mediator of top-down attentional control during a timing task [7,18]. Presentation of familiar aversive distracters, such as stimuli previously paired with aversive consequences, results in large delays in timing (over-reset) [5], which can be in part alleviated by increasing catecholamine levels within PrL [7]. Individuals with depression show impairments both in time perception [24] and attentional processing [25], both dependent on prefrontal cortex (PFC) [2,26,27], but these impairments are less studies and understood.
Timing is sensitive to a variety of pharmacological manipulations including catecholaminergic drugs [7,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36]. Stimulants like methamphetamine and cocaine inhibit reuptake of catecholamines and have been shown to alter both timing [28] and attention to time [29]. Some catecholamine reuptake inhibitors, such as nomifensine [37], are effective antidepressants. For example, nomifensine has been shown to reduce immobility both in the forced swim test [38,39] and in the tail suspension test [40] and was marketed as antidepressant in the 1970s and 1980s in the United States under the names Merital/Alival. Although the effects of some antidepressants on timing have been previously examined [7,41,42,43], further elucidation in regard to their effects on attention to time, particularly in paradigms involving distracters, is warranted.
Here we sought to investigate the effects of blocking catecholamine reuptake within the PrL using nomifensine on the delaying effects of auditory stimuli previously paired with appetitive reinforcers, or presented for the first time during the testing phase, using procedures similar to [14]. Based on previous studies [7], we expected that (a) blockade of catecholamine reuptake would have no effect on timing behavior in the absence of distracters, and that (b) blockade of catecholamine reuptake would enhance top-down attentional control and decrease the timing delays after familiar appetitive or novel distracters, as it does for aversive distracters [7]. The first prediction was confirmed, but the latter was not.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Subjects

The subjects were 26 male Sprague Dawley rats (Harlan Laboratories, Inc., Indianapolis, IN, USA), approximately 3 months old (300–350 g) at the start of the experiment. Subjects were randomly assigned to two groups: Appetitive and Novel and trained in procedures described below. After removing subjects due to misplaced injector tips, poor timing accuracy after surgery, unreliable timing functions, or failure to respond consistently on the nontiming lever, the final number of subjects used in analyses was 17 (appetitive n = 10, novel n = 7). Rats were maintained at approximately 85% of their ad libitum weight by restricting food access in their home cages. Rats were housed in a temperature and humidity-controlled room with a 12/12 light-dark cycle. Water was freely available in the home cage. All experimental procedures were conducted in accordance with the National Institutes of Health’s Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals [44] and were approved by Utah State University IACUC Committee (protocol 2254).

2.2. Apparatus

The apparatus consisted of 16 standard rat operant chambers (Med Associates, St. Albans, VT, USA), housed in sound attenuating cubicles. All chambers were equipped with two levers situated on either side of a pellet receptacle: the left lever was the timing lever for all rats. Rats received 45-mg pellets (Bio-Serv, Flemington, NJ, USA) as rewards. A 28-V 100-mA house light was used as a to-be-timed stimulus. An 80-dB white noise stimulus was used as a distracter stimulus for all rats.

2.3. Behavioral Procedures

2.3.1. Interval Timing–Left Lever

Rats were initially reinforced on a fixed-ratio (FR)-1 schedule with food pellets for pressing on the timing (left) lever. Rats were then trained on a reversed fixed-interval (RFI)-40 schedule [6,45], where the to-be-timed visual stimulus for timing behavior was the absence of the house light. During RFI trials, the first lever response on the timing lever, 40 s into the trial, resulted in food delivery and turned on the house light for the duration of the intertrial interval (ITI). Afterwards, RFI trials were randomly intermixed with reversed peak-interval (RPI) trials, which were unreinforced probe trials during which the to-be-timed stimuli lasted three times the criterion time (120 s) irrespective of responding. Responding on the nontiming (right) lever during this stage of training was inconsequential. Trials were separated by variable 60 ± 30 s ITIs, during which the house light was illuminated.

2.3.2. Appetitive Noise Training–Right Lever

Rats were randomly assigned to two groups. Group Novel was not exposed to the noise until testing and was never rewarded for pressing on the nontiming (right) lever. Group Appetitive was (sometimes) reinforced with food pellets for pressing the right lever during the noise, as follows: In separate sessions, Appetitive rats were occasionally rewarded for lever pressing during the noise on the nontiming (right) lever in three 3-hr random-interval (RI)-16 sessions (16 s noise) with probability 1.0, averaging about 130 trials/session or until rats earned 200 rewards, followed by ten 3-hr RI8 sessions (8 s noise) with probability gradually decreasing from 1.0, to 0.65, and finally to 0.5; the latter RI8 sessions included 150 trials/session or until rats earned 200 rewards. In all, Appetitive rats received about 1700 noise presentations prior to testing. One rat was eliminated from the study because it failed to respond consistently on the nontiming (right) lever in the presence of the noise.

2.4. Surgery

Rats underwent aseptic surgical procedures under isoflurane anesthesia. Bilateral cannula guides (PlasticsOne, Roanoke, VA) were implanted into the PrL at bregma AP: + 2.50 mm, ML: ± 0.60 mm, DV: −3.50 mm [46] and permanently affixed to the skull. Rats were allowed to recover at least one week before behavioral testing resumed.

2.5. Drugs

Catecholamine reuptake blocker Nomifensine maleate (NOM) (Sigma Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA) was dissolved in vehicle: 45% methyl-beta-cyclodextrin (Sigma Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA) in sterile saline. Drugs were prepared fresh prior to all testing procedures.

2.6. Local Infusions

Before testing, rats received 0.5 µL infusions of vehicle, NOM 0.4 µg, or NOM 4 µg bilaterally into the PrL. Rats were tested in a Latin-square design to prevent testing biases. Infusions occurred at a rate of 0.25 µL/min for two minutes. Injector cannulae were left in place for two minutes following the infusion.

2.7. Testing with and without Noise Distracters

Following drug infusions, rats in both groups were placed in operant boxes and received a 3-hr test session where rats were randomly presented RFI trials, RPI trials, and reversed peak-interval trials with noise distracter (RPI + N; for Novel rats these were the first trials with a noise presentation). In RPI + N trials, an 8-s noise was presented 8 s into the trial. RPI + N trials were unreinforced, response-independent probe trials lasting 120 s. Appetitive rats received noise retraining sessions in between drug testing sessions.

2.8. Histology

Rats were transcardially perfused with 10% formalin (Fisher Scientific, Pittsburgh, PA, USA). Brains were sectioned at 60 µm on a vibratome (VT 1200S, Leica, Wetzlar, Germany). Injector tip placements were identified (see Figure 1). Two rats were removed from the final analyses due to injector tip placements outside the PrL.

2.9. Data Collection and Analysis

Lever response data was collected using MED-PC-IV software (Med Associates, St Albans, VT, USA). Timing (left) lever responses in RPI and RPI + N trials in the testing phase were analyzed to determine timing accuracy (estimated peak time, or estimated time of maximal lever responding, thought to be an individual estimate of the criterion duration) [47], and timing precision (estimated width of the response function), using two methods. First, using a Curve Fitting (CF) method, individual response curves were fit to a Gaussian [45,48], using the Marquardt–Levenberg algorithm [49] over an 80-s window of analysis: 0–80 s in RPI and 16–96 s in RPI + N trials. The parameters of curve fitting algorithm directly provided estimates for timing accuracy (peak time) and precision (width of the timing function). Second, using the Inter-Quartile (IQR) method, responses were divided into 4 quartiles; the average of the 25% and 75% quartiles was taken as an estimate of timing accuracy (peak time), and the time difference between the 25% and 75% quartile was taken as an estimate of timing precision (width of the response function) [6]. We opted for these two methods because they are very different in complexity and assumptions: The CF method assumes a Gaussian-shaped response function, is relatively complex, and it is not intuitive. In contrast, the IQR method is intuitive, easy to understand, and has no assumptions relative to the shape of the response function. Despite being very different in assumptions and complexity, when applied to previous data sets, these methods provided roughly similar estimates for timing accuracy (peak time) and precision (width of the response function) [6,50]. Using each of the two methods, an individual time delay in RPI + N trials relative to RPI trials was calculated as the difference between the estimated peak times in RPI + N and RPI trials. Five rats were removed for poor timing accuracy after surgery (estimated peak time more than 20% off from the criterion time). One rat had very noisy timing functions (coefficient of determination r2 = 0.42) and was removed from further analyses (see Figure 1; final counts: Appetitive n = 10, Novel n = 7).
The number of nontiming (right) lever responses, the number of rewards earned in the noise conditioning sessions, and the number of nontiming lever responses during the RPI and RPI + N sessions were collected and analyzed. A response elevation ratio for the nontiming (right) lever was calculated as the ratio between the number of nontiming (right) lever presses made during the 8 s noise presentation period during RPI + N trials and the sum of the responses during the 8-s noise and during the 8-s interval preceding the noise.

2.10. Statistical Analyses

Estimated measures of peak time, time delay, response function widths, coefficients of determination (r2), timing lever and nontiming lever response rates, number of responses on the nontiming lever during noise presentations, number of rewards earned during noise conditioning sessions, and elevation ratios for the nontiming lever, were analyzed using mixed ANOVAs with NOM dose (0, 0.4, 4 µg) as the repeated measure and group (Appetitive, Novel) as the between-subjects variable, followed by planned comparisons and Fisher LSD post-hoc analyses. The alpha level for analyses was set at p = 0.05. Analyses were conducted using Statistica (StatSoft, Inc., Tulsa, OK, USA).

3. Results

3.1. Appetitive Rats Conditioned to the Noise Stimuli

To confirm that Appetitive rats conditioned to the noise stimulus, the response rates and number of rewards received during the appetitive noise training conditioning were analyzed (Figure 2). As the appetitive noise conditioning progressed, the response rate on the nontiming (right) lever during the noise significantly increased from 0.8 ± 0.3 resp/min in the first conditioning session to 10.4 ± 0.6 resp/min in the last conditioning session (Figure 2A) (F(1,9) = 246.70, p < 0.0001). Additionally, the number of rewards earned significantly increased from 72.3 ± 25.7 in the first conditioning session to 154.8 ± 12.3 in the last conditioning session (Figure 2B) (F(1,9) = 16.28, p = 0.0030). Taken together, these results indicate that Appetitive rats acquired the secondary task (occasionally earning rewards by pressing on the right lever during the noise).

3.2. PrL Blockade of Catecholamine Reuptake Did Not Alter Timing Accuracy in Trials without Distracters

Figure 3 (solid lines) shows normalized left lever timing functions in RPI trials (without distracters). Irrespective of group or drug dose the response functions in trials without distracters (RPI, solid curves) peaked about the criterion interval (40 s) suggesting that rats acquired the timing task and were not affected by the drug regimen. These suggestions were supported by both analyses using the Curve Fitting (CF) method and the Interquartile Range (IQR) method.
The CF method provided the following statistics for trials without distracters (RPI, solid curves in Figure 3): Under vehicle, timing functions peaked at 40.1 ± 0.8 s for Appetitive rats and at 41.5 ± 1.0 s for Novel rats, indicating that rats acquired the timing task. Appetitive rats peaked at 42.0 ± 1.8 s under the NOM 0.4µg, and at 41.2 ± 1.8 s under NOM 4 µg, while Novel rats peaked at 41.2 ± 1.2 s under NOM 0.4 µg, and 41.6 ± 0.6 s under NOM 4 µg. A mixed ANOVA of estimated peak times in RPI trials failed to reveal any significant differences between groups, drug conditions, or group x drug interaction, all Fs < 0.32, all ps > 0.73. These results indicate that in peak trials without distracters, the drug regimen did not affect timing accuracy irrespective of the group or the dose.
Very similar statistics were provided for RPI trials using the IQR method. Under vehicle, timing functions peaked at 40.2 ± 1.0 s for Appetitive rats and at 41.3 ± 1.0 s for Novel rats, indicating that rats acquired the timing task. Appetitive rats peaked at 41.5 ± 1.4 s under the NOM 0.4 µg, and at 42.4 ± 1.1 s under NOM 4 µg, while Novel rats peaked at 42.1 ± 1.0 s under NOM 0.4 µg, and 42.3 ± 0.6 s under NOM 4 µg. A mixed ANOVA of estimated peak times in RPI trials failed to reveal any significant differences between groups, drug conditions, or group x drug interaction, all Fs < 1.81, all ps > 0.18. Taken together, these results indicate that, according to both the CF and IQR methods, in RPI trials rats peaked about the criterion interval irrespective of the group or drug dose.

3.3. PrL Blockade of Catecholamine Reuptake Significantly Modulated Responding after Novel Distracters in an Inverted-U, Dose-Dependent Manner but Not after Familiar Appetitive Distracters

In RPI + N trials (Figure 3, dotted lines), the 8-s noise distracter was presented 8 s into the trial (indicated by the black rectangle), resulting in delayed timing behavior irrespective of group or drug dose. Figure 3 also indicates that the delay was larger in Novel rats at the 0.4 µg NOM dose than in any other group/condition. These suggestions were supported by both analyses using the CF and IQR methods. The time delay in RPI + N trials relative to RPI trials was compared with the behavioral responses of “run” (0-s delay), “stop” (8-s delay), and “reset” (16-s delay).
The left panel of Figure 4 shows the estimated delay using the CF method. Appetitive rats delayed 15.9 ± 3.8 s under vehicle, 14.8 ± 2.8 s under NOM 0.4 µg, and 22.2 ± 3.6 s under NOM 4 µg (Figure 4, left panel, closed squares), not significantly different from resetting: Vehicle, t(9) = 0.037, p > 0.9715, NOM 0.4 µg, t(9) = 0.433, p > 0.6749, and NOM 4 µg, t(9) = 1.735, p > 0.1168. Novel rats delayed 13.7 ± 3.2 s under vehicle, 25.2 ± 3.9 s under NOM 0.4 µg, and 15.8 ± 3.5 s under NOM 4 µg (Figure 4, left panel, open circles). Novel rats reset timing under the vehicle, t(6) = 0.710, p > 0.5043, and under NOM 4 µg conditions, t(6) = 0.053, p > 0.9597, however, they trended towards over-resetting timing under NOM 0.4 µg, t(6) = 2.380, p = 0.0548. A mixed ANOVA of time delay indicated a significant drug x group effect, F(2,30) = 5.396, p = 0.0010, but it failed to indicate significant effects of group, F(1,15) = 0.025, p = 0.877, or drug F(2,30) = 2.174, p = 0.131. A Fisher LSD post-hoc analysis indicated that under NOM 0.4 µg, the Novel group delayed significantly more than both under vehicle and under NOM 4 µg, ps < 0.05, and significantly more than Appetitive rats at the same dose, p < 0.05 (Figure 3, left panel). Unlike Appetitive rats, whose delay was not affected by the drug, Novel rats modulated their delay in an inverted-U dose-dependent manner: they delayed significantly more at the NOM 0.4 µg drug dose than under the vehicle and NOM 4 µg conditions.
Similar conclusions were reached using the IQR analysis method (Figure 4, right panel). Using the IQR method, we estimated that Appetitive rats delayed 16.1 ± 3.4 s under vehicle, 14.5 ± 2.4 s under NOM 0.4 µg, and 18.6 ± 2.0 s under NOM 4 µg (Figure 4, right panel, closed squares) not significantly different from resetting: Vehicle, t(9) = 0.044, p > 0.9656, NOM 0.4 µg, t(9) = 0.628, p > 0.5452, and NOM 4 µg, t(9) = 1.285, p > 0.2308. Novel rats delayed 12.4 ± 2.4 s under vehicle, 20.0 ± 2.7 s under NOM 0.4 µg, and 10.7 ± 3.7 s under NOM 4 µg (Figure 4, right panel, open circles), not significant from stopping for vehicle, t(6) = 1.823, p > 0.1180 and NOM 4 µg, t(6) = 0.737, p = 0.4888, and not significantly different from resetting for NOM 0.4 µg, t(6) = 0.148, p > 0.1876. A Fisher LSD post-hoc analysis indicated that while Appetitive rats’ delay was not significantly different under all drug doses (ps > 0.05), Novel rats delayed significantly more at the NOM 0.4 µg dose than both under vehicle and under NOM 4 µg, ps < 0.05. In summary, both the CF and the IQR analysis methods indicated that the drug did not affect the delay in Appetitive rats but modulated the delay in Novel rats in an inverted-U dose-dependent manner: Novel rats delayed significantly more at the NOM 0.4 µg dose than both under vehicle and under NOM 4 µg in an inverted-U fashion.

3.4. PrL Blockade of Catecholamine Reuptake did not Significantly Change the Width of the Response Functions

Timing precision was estimated using both the CF and IQR methods: Using the CF method, timing precision (the estimated width of the response function) is one of the parameters of the Gaussian curve fitting; using the IQR method, timing precision can be estimated as the difference between the 75% quartile and the 25% quartile. Because of the inherent differences between the two methods, the two estimates are expected to differ; however, we expect that both estimates will be similarly affected by the group, drug dose, and trial type variables.
A mixed ANOVA of the estimated width of the response function using the CF method [45] failed to indicate a significant main effect of either group, F(1,15) = 0.753, p > 0.3992, or drug dose, F(2,30) = 0.259, p > 0.7733, although analyses indicated a significant main effect of trial type, F(1,15) = 5.277, p = 0.0364, suggesting that the width was significantly larger in RPI + N trials than in RPI trials; analyses failed to indicate any significant interactions, Fs < 1.460, ps > 0.313. Similar results were found using the IQR method [45]: A mixed ANOVA of the estimated width of the response function failed to indicate a significant main effect of either group, F(1,15) = 0.700, p > 0.4158, or drug dose, F(2,30) = 1.256, p > 0.2993, although analyses indicated a significant main effect of trial type, F(1,15) = 9.783, p = 0.0069, suggesting that the width was significantly larger in RPI + N trials than in RPI trials; analyses failed to indicate any significant interactions, Fs < 1.509, ps > 0.2374. Taken together, although timing precision was significantly lower (width of the timing function was significantly larger) in RPI + N trials relative to RPI trials, both methods indicated that timing precision was not affected by neither drug nor group. Thus, the differences in timing delay under the drug between groups cannot be explained by differences in timing precision.

3.5. PrL Blockade of Catecholamine Reuptake did not Significantly Change the Coefficients of Determination (r2) of the Response Functions

Using the CF method, one estimates timing accuracy (peak time) and timing precision (width of the response function) by fitting a Gaussian curve onto the individual response curve in each condition. The goodness of fit is estimated by the coefficient of determination (r2). A high coefficient of determination indicates a good fit with a Gaussian curve, while a low coefficient of determination indicates that the individual rat’s response curve is noisy, and it is poorly fit by a Gaussian. As detailed in Methods, one rat had very noisy timing functions (r2 = 0.42) and was removed from analyses.
The r2 values for the Appetitive group were 0.97 ± 0.01 in RPI trials and 0.85 ± 0.04 in RPI + N trials under vehicle, 0.96 ± 0.02 in RPI trials and 0.82 ± 0.04 in RPI + N trials under NOM 0.4 µg, and 0.94 ± 0.02 in RPI trials and 0.84 ± 0.03 in RPI + N trials under NOM 4 µg. Goodness of fit values for the Novel group were 0.98 ± 0.01 in RPI trials and 0.86 ± 0.03 in RPI + N trials under vehicle, 0.93 ± 0.01 in RPI trials and 0.70 ± 0.08 in RPI + N trials under NOM 0.4 µg, and 0.94 ± 0.02 in RPI trials and 0.88 ± 0.04 in RPI + N trials under NOM 4 µg. A mixed ANOVA analysis of the r2 values failed to indicate a significant main effect of either group, F(1,15) = 0.437, p > 0.5186, drug dose, F(2,30) = 2.831, p = 0.0748, or any interactions, all Fs < 2.857, ps > 0.0732. There was however a significant main effect of trial type, F(1,15) = 44.068, p < 0.0001, suggesting rats’ response functions in RPI + N trials were significantly noisier (r2 = 0.84 ± 0.02) than in RPI trials (r2 = 0.95 ± 0.01). In summary, although the goodness of fit (coefficient of determination, r2) was slightly, but significantly, lower in RPI + N trials relative to RPI trials, it was not affected by neither drug dose nor group. Therefore, the differences in timing delay under the drug between groups cannot be explained by differences in goodness of fit.

3.6. PrL Blockade of Catecholamine Reuptake did not Affect Lever Pressing on either the Timing or Nontiming Levers

3.6.1. Timing Lever

Mixed ANOVAs of timing (left) lever response rates in RPI and RPI + N trials failed to indicate drug, group or drug-group interactions within either RPI (all Fs < 0.21, ps > 0.70) or RPI + N trials (all Fs < 2.33, ps > 0.11). Analyses also indicated a significant effect of trial type on response rate, F(1,15) = 66.97, p < 0.0001, but they failed to indicate any trial x group effects, trial x drug effects, or drug x trial x group effects, all Fs < 1.19, ps > 0.32, indicating that noise presentations significantly decreased the response rate on the timing (left) lever in RPI + N trials relative to RPI trials under all drug or group conditions. However, the rate of responding on the timing lever was independent of drug manipulation.

3.6.2. Nontiming Lever

A mixed ANOVA of the number of responses on the nontiming (right) lever during RPI trials (without noise distracter) failed to indicate significant effects of group, drug, or interactions, all Fs < 2.52, p > 0.096, suggesting that, in trials where the noise was not presented, both Appetitive and Novel rats responded similarly (at very low levels) on the nontiming lever under all drug conditions. However, in trials with noise presentations (RPI + N), analyses of nontiming lever responding indicated a significant effect of group, F(1,15) = 11.67, p = 0.0038, with no reliable drug or drug x group interaction effects, all Fs > 0.30, p > 0.74, suggesting that Appetitive rats responded significantly more than Novel rats irrespective of drug dose and that the level of responding (high in Appetitive and low in Novel) was not affected by the drug dose (Figure 5A).
To further investigate possible drug-related changes in the rate of nontiming lever responding in RPI + N trials, a response elevation ratio was computed by dividing the number of responses during the 8-s noise by the number of right lever responses during the 8-s noise and the preceding 8-s interval (Figure 5B). Figure 5B shows that Appetitive, but not Novel rats, significantly elevated their nontiming lever rate of response compared to chance level (elevation ratio 0.5). A mixed ANOVA of the elevation ratio indicated a significant main effect of group, F(1,15) = 14.798, p = 0.0016 but no effect of drug or drug x group interaction (all Fs < 0.33, p > 0.71). Planned comparisons between the Appetitive and Novel groups’ elevation ratios indicated significant differences under vehicle, F(1,15) = 11.246, p = 0.0044, under NOM 0.4 µg, F(1,15) = 5.340, p = 0.0355, and under NOM 4 µg, F(1,15) = 4.856, p = 0.0436. Overall, Appetitive rats significantly increased responding on the nontiming lever relative to pre-noise levels when the noise was presented during RPI + N trials, while Novel rats responded on the nontiming lever at low levels both before and during the noise. Most importantly, despite group and trials differences, the response rates on either the timing (left) lever or the nontiming (right) lever were not significantly affected by PrL catecholamine manipulations. Therefore, the differential effect of the drug on time delay in Novel rats at the NOM 0.4 µg dose (Figure 4) cannot be simply attributed to changes in the responding on either lever in either group, to changes in timing precision, or to changes in goodness of fit, but is more compatible with a change in top-down attentional control to timing vs. processing the distracter.

4. Discussion

We adapted a peak-interval timing paradigm with distracters to explore the effects of local catecholamine reuptake blockade in the PrL on timing accuracy and on the top-down attentional control of timing when presented with familiar appetitively-conditioned or novel noise distracters. Based on a previous study which investigated the effect of PrL catecholamine reuptake blockade on timing with aversive distracters [7], we expected that (a) PrL infusion of catecholamine reuptake blocker nomifensine (NOM) would not affect timing accuracy (in trials without distracters), and that (b) PrL infusion of NOM would reduce the delaying effect of appetitive or novel noise distracters on timing (in trials with distracters). While the first prediction was confirmed, the latter was not.
Overall, our study revealed a double-dissociation of the effect of PrL catecholamine reuptake blockade on timing between trials with or without distracters and between familiar appetitively-conditioned distracters (Appetitive group) or novel distracters (Novel group). In trials without distracters, PrL infusion of NOM had no effect on timing accuracy, timing precision, or response rate on either lever in either group. Although NOM has been shown to increase dopamine (DA) accumulation in the frontal cortices, brain areas related to timing [23,51,52], and although DAergic drugs alter timing [2], we found no effect of NOM on timing accuracy and timing precision in trials without distracters in either Novel or Appetitive rats. These findings can be explained by the fact that although nomifensine acts as an indirect DA agonist, binding and blocking DAT and NET similar to amphetamine, it does not have amphetamine’s stimulatory effects on DA release [53], as NOM does not reverse the DA pump direction [54,55]. Therefore, while amphetamine [28,33,56] affects timing accuracy, NOM failed to show such an effect in both the present study and in previous studies involving either rats [7] or healthy human participants [57].
Instead, in trials with distracters, not only did PrL infusion of NOM fail to reduce the timing delay after these distracters as predicted by [7] but also significantly increased the timing delay after novel distracters in a dose dependent manner, while failing to alter the time delay following familiar appetitive distracters (Figure 4). Indeed, Appetitive rats reset (restarted) their timing immediately following the noise irrespective of NOM dose; instead, under NOM Novel rats delayed their timing in an inverted-U manner: They reset (restarted) their timing following the novel noise at 0 and 4 µg NOM dose but significantly delayed their responding at the 0.4 µg NOM dose (Figure 4). These results cannot be explained by changes in lever pressing on the nontiming lever, since NOM failed to alter responding on this lever irrespective of dose (Figure 5A,B). Moreover, these results cannot be explained by changes in timing precision (width of response function) or in goodness of fit (coefficient of determination, r2). Taken together, these results suggest that during distracter presentations PrL blockade of catecholamine reuptake did not affect top-down attentional control in Appetitive rats, but decreased top-down attentional control of timing in Novel rats in an inverted-U fashion, without having any effects on the general timing ability of the subject.
The nonlinear inverted-U dose-response curve is a signature of activation of frontal cortex in tasks requiring attentional control or working memory in humans [58], monkeys [59], and rodents [60]. NOM blocks catecholamine reuptake and indirectly increases activation of both DA and norepinephrine (NE) receptors. Therefore, NOM’s inverted-U dose-response curve in Novel rats may be due to differential activation of receptors on one or both neurotransmitter systems (e.g., differential affinity towards DAT and NET [61]), activation of nonlinear signaling cascades (e.g., D1 receptors and their cAMP intracellular signaling [59]), regulation of transporter function and surface expression [62], or activation of autoreceptors and reduced neurotransmitter release (NOM has affinity towards alpha-2 adrenergic receptors [63,64,65,66]). Our study cannot differentiate between these possibilities, but it provides yet another example in which prefrontal cortex DA and/or NE show a nonlinear cognitive control, generating a dynamic balance between focused attention and flexible attention [67]: Too little or too much PFC activation may have Appetitive and Novel rats “reset” (restart) timing immediately after the distracter in an “automatic” manner, without processing of the distracter. Instead, at an intermediate “optimal” PFC activation level provided by NOM administration, Novel rats may “flexibly” process the distracter and fail to immediately return their attentional resources to timing, thus further delaying timing beyond a reset.
Alternatively, Novel rats may have experienced the novel noise distracter as an orienting stimulus [68], which may have prompted them to investigate the novel stimulus, its location, and/or its possible consequences or significance. The Relative Time-Sharing (RTS) model [6,9,11] proposes that a limited pool of attentional resources is shared by all cognitive processes including timing. When distracters are presented, attentional resources are diverted away from the primary timing task and are reallocated towards the processing of the distracter [9,11]. In Novel rats, PrL blockade of catecholamine reuptake may have exacerbated the attentional resources allocated to the novel distracter (orienting stimulus), its location, or its possible consequences even after the distracter [69], such that attentional resources were reallocated back to timing with a significant delay after the distracter, which resulted in time delays beyond a reset (Figure 4). Because in our study NOM was only effective in Novel rats, it suggests that PrL catecholamine blockade may have affected exploration following novel stimuli [69] or may have resulted in Novel rats being apprehensive (anxious) of the novel noise [70]. Indeed, NOM administration has been previously shown to result in anxiety-like behaviors, such as an increase in novelty-induced hypophagia when food is presented in the middle of a novel open field area [71].
When contrasting the results of the present study with those reported in a previous study in our lab [7], it may be puzzling to note that (a) in the present study rats delayed timing after the neutral distracter while they failed to do so in [7] and (b) in the present study NOM further delayed timing after the neutral distracter while it failed to do so in [7]. Both discrepancies are easily accounted for by the fact that in the present study rats were not presented with the neutral distracter before the test, and as such during the test the neutral distracter was a novel stimulus of unknown consequence, a stimulus that attracts attention [72]. Such a stimulus is expected to delay timing because its processing requires attentional resources which are taken away from processing time [9], as well as be affected by NOM because it engages attentional control circuits in PFC responsible for resource allocation. In contrast, in [7] rats were exposed to the neutral distracter before the test (pre-exposed); by being pre-exposed, the neutral distracter became a stimulus-of-no-consequence, to-be-ignored (latent inhibition [73]). The presentation of such a stimulus is neither expected to affect behavior (e.g., delay timing) nor to be affected by NOM because it does not engage attentional control circuits in PFC.
It is interesting to note that in the present study, the “novel” distracter was repeatedly presented during testing, yet its time-delaying properties failed to diminish. This may be due to its small number of presentations during testing, as well to the relatively unknown relationship between “novelty” and “time-delaying properties”. While there is strong evidence that the time delay produced by an interrupting event increases with discriminability (an element of “novelty”) [8,74,75,76], only one experimental investigation of the relationship between time delay and the number of presentations of the event exists to date. To some surprise, we found that under specific conditions distracters may retain their time-delaying properties despite considerable numbers of presentations of the distracter [50], a finding that parallels the fact that under specific experimental conditions repetition enhances rather than diminishes the “novelty” of a stimulus (reviewed in [77]). The latter finding is supported by computer simulations showing that “novelty” does not simply decrease with repetitions but that it may increase rather than decrease with repeated presentation of a stimulus (or under specific physiological conditions) [78]. Taken together, discrepancies between this study and previous studies are likely to be accounted for by details of the procedure, possibly by the neutral stimulus in this study being “novel” and engaging attentional processing, while in [7] the neutral stimulus was “pre-exposed”, thus failing to engage attentional processing and delay timing.
In summary, the selective impairment in top-down attentional control of timing after novel, but not appetitive distracters, suggests that PrL catecholamine blockade does not affect timing by altering timing or reward processes but rather processes related to novelty processing [79], increased exploration following novel stimuli [69], or possible defensive or anxiety-related processes activated by novel, unexpected stimuli [80].

5. Conclusions

Here we document that prelimbic catecholaminergic top-down attentional control of timing depends upon the valence of distracters. In this study, PrL blockade of catecholamine reuptake by NOM neither altered timing accuracy in trials without distracters nor affected attentional control in rats presented with an appetitive distracter. However, contrary to its beneficial effects on attentional control of timing when presented with aversive distracters [7], PrL blockade of catecholamine reuptake impaired top-down attentional control of timing when presented with novel distracters in an inverted-U, dose-dependent manner. It stands to reason that the valence of attentional distracters, drug dosage, and the specific medications used to alleviate their effects must be carefully considered when prescribing medications to patients with attentional or affective disorders. Future research must be performed to clarify valence-related aspects of attentional disorders and the medications commonly prescribed to lessen their effects.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.V.B.; data curation, C.V.B.; formal analysis, A.R.M. and C.V.B.; funding acquisition, C.V.B.; investigation, A.R.M.; methodology, C.V.B.; project administration, C.V.B.; resources, C.V.B. and M.B.; software, C.B.; supervision, C.V.B.; validation, C.V.B.; visualization, A.R.M. and C.V.B.; writing—original draft preparation, A.R.M.; writing—review and editing, A.R.M., M.B. and C.V.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was funded by National Institutes of Health grants MH065561 and MH073057 to C.B.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Daniel Leakehe for his help with independently conducting the IQR analysis. We would also like to thank two anonymous reviewers for thoughtful comments on an early version of the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Buhusi, C.V.; Meck, W.H. (Eds.) Timing Behavior; Springer: Berlin, Germany, 2010; pp. 1319–1323. [Google Scholar]
  2. Buhusi, C.V.; Meck, W.H. What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2005, 6, 755–765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Buhusi, C.V.; Oprisan, S.A.; Buhusi, M. Biological and Cognitive Frameworks for a Mental Timeline. Front. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Buhusi, C.V.; Oprisan, S.A.; Buhusi, M. Clocks within Clocks: Timing by Coincidence Detection. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2016, 8, 207–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Brown, B.L.; Richer, P.; Doyere, V. The effect of an intruded event on peak-interval timing in rats: Isolation of a postcue effect. Behav. Process. 2007, 74, 300–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Buhusi, C.V.; Meck, W.H. Time sharing in rats: A peak-interval procedure with gaps and distracters. Behav. Process. 2006, 71, 107–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Matthews, A.R.; He, O.H.; Buhusi, M.; Buhusi, C.V. Dissociation of the role of the prelimbic cortex in interval timing and resource allocation: Beneficial effect of norepinephrine and dopamine reuptake inhibitor nomifensine on anxiety-inducing distraction. Front Integr. Neurosci. 2012, 6, 111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  8. Buhusi, C.V. Time-sharing in rats: Effect of distracter intensity and discriminability. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 2012, 38, 30–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Buhusi, C.V.; Meck, W.H. Relative time sharing: New findings and an extension of the resource allocation model of temporal processing. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 2009, 364, 1875–1885. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Buhusi, C.V.; Meck, W.H. Interval timing with gaps and distracters: Evaluation of the ambiguity, switch, and time-sharing hypotheses. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 2006, 32, 329–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Buhusi, C.V. Dopaminergic Mechanisms of Interval Timing and Attention. In Functional and Neural Mechanisms of Interval Timing; Meck, W.H., Ed.; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2003; pp. 317–338. [Google Scholar]
  12. Buhusi, M.; Bartlett, M.J.; Buhusi, C.V. Sex differences in interval timing and attention to time in C57Bl/6J mice. Behav. Brain Res. 2017, 324, 96–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Oprisan, S.A.; Dix, S.; Buhusi, C.V. Phase resetting and its implications for interval timing with intruders. Behav. Process. 2014, 101, 146–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Aum, S.W.; Brown, B.L.; Hemmes, N.S. The effects of concurrent task and gap events on peak time in the peak procedure. Behav. Process. 2004, 65, 43–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Meck, W.H. Frontal cortex lesions eliminate the clock speed effect of dopaminergic drugs on interval timing. Brain Res. 2006, 1108, 157–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Coull, J.T.; Cheng, R.K.; Meck, W.H. Neuroanatomical and neurochemical substrates of timing. Neuropsychopharmacology 2011, 36, 3–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Merchant, H.; Harrington, D.L.; Meck, W.H. Neural basis of the perception and estimation of time. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2013, 36, 313–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Buhusi, C.V.; Reyes, M.B.; Gathers, C.A.; Oprisan, S.A.; Buhusi, M. Inactivation of the Medial-Prefrontal Cortex Impairs Interval Timing Precision, but Not Timing Accuracy or Scalar Timing in a Peak-Interval Procedure in Rats. Front Integr. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Tucci, V.; Buhusi, C.V.; Gallistel, R.; Meck, W.H. Towards an integrated understanding of the biology of timing. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 2014, 369, 20120470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Diamond, A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Euston, D.R.; Gruber, A.J.; McNaughton, B.L. The role of medial prefrontal cortex in memory and decision making. Neuron 2012, 76, 1057–1070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Buhusi, M.; Olsen, K.; Buhusi, C.V. Increased temporal discounting after chronic stress in CHL1-deficient mice is reversed by 5-HT2C agonist Ro 60-0175. Neuroscience 2017, 357, 110–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Kim, J.; Jung, A.H.; Byun, J.; Jo, S.; Jung, M.W. Inactivation of medial prefrontal cortex impairs time interval discrimination in rats. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2009, 3, 38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Gil, S.; Droit-Volet, S. Time perception, depression and sadness. Behav. Process. 2009, 80, 169–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Keller, A.S.; Leikauf, J.E.; Holt-Gosselin, B.; Staveland, B.R.; Williams, L.M. Paying attention to attention in depression. Transl. Psychiatry 2019, 9, 279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Koenigs, M.; Grafman, J. The functional neuroanatomy of depression: Distinct roles for ventromedial and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Behav. Brain Res. 2009, 201, 239–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Clark, K.L.; Noudoost, B. The role of prefrontal catecholamines in attention and working memory. Front. Neural Circuits 2014, 8, 33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Maricq, A.V.; Roberts, S.; Church, R.M. Methamphetamine and time estimation. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 1981, 7, 18–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Buhusi, C.V.; Meck, W.H. Differential effects of methamphetamine and haloperidol on the control of an internal clock. Behav. Neurosci. 2002, 116, 291–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Cheng, R.K.; MacDonald, C.J.; Meck, W.H. Differential effects of cocaine and ketamine on time estimation: Implications for neurobiological models of interval timing. Pharm. Biochem. Behav. 2006, 85, 114–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Lau, C.E.; Ma, F.; Foster, D.M.; Falk, J.L. Pharmacokinetic-pharmacodynamic modeling of the psychomotor stimulant effect of cocaine after intravenous administration: Timing performance deficits. J. Pharm. Exp. 1999, 288, 535–543. [Google Scholar]
  32. Matell, M.S.; King, G.R.; Meck, W.H. Differential modulation of clock speed by the administration of intermittent versus continuous cocaine. Behav. Neurosci. 2004, 118, 150–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Meck, W.H. Selective adjustment of the speed of internal clock and memory processes. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 1983, 9, 171–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Meck, W.H. Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 1996, 3, 227–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Matell, M.S.; Bateson, M.; Meck, W.H. Single-trials analyses demonstrate that increases in clock speed contribute to the methamphetamine-induced horizontal shifts in peak-interval timing functions. Psychopharmacology 2006, 188, 201–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Oprisan, S.A.; Buhusi, C.V. Modeling pharmacological clock and memory patterns of interval timing in a striatal beat-frequency model with realistic, noisy neurons. Front Integr. Neurosci. 2011, 5, 52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Katz, N.S.; Guiard, B.P.; El Mansari, M.; Blier, P. Effects of acute and sustained administration of the catecholamine reuptake inhibitor nomifensine on the firing activity of monoaminergic neurons. J. Psychopharmacol. 2010, 24, 1223–1235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Basso, A.M.; Gallagher, K.B.; Bratcher, N.A.; Brioni, J.D.; Moreland, R.B.; Hsieh, G.C.; Drescher, K.; Fox, G.B.; Decker, M.W.; Rueter, L.E. Antidepressant-like effect of D(2/3) receptor-, but not D(4) receptor-activation in the rat forced swim test. Neuropsychopharmacol. Off. Publ. Am. Coll. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2005, 30, 1257–1268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Porsolt, R.D.; Bertin, A.; Blavet, N.; Deniel, M.; Jalfre, M. Immobility induced by forced swimming in rats: Effects of agents which modify central catecholamine and serotonin activity. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 1979, 57, 201–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Steru, L.; Chermat, R.; Thierry, B.; Simon, P. The tail suspension test: A new method for screening antidepressants in mice. Psychopharmacology 1985, 85, 367–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Bayley, P.J.; Bentley, G.D.; Dawson, G.R. The effects of selected antidepressant drugs on timing behaviour in rats. Psychopharmacology 1998, 136, 114–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Ho, M.Y.; al-Zahrani, S.S.; Velazquez Martinez, D.N.; Lopez Cabrera, M.; Bradshaw, C.M.; Szabadi, E. Effects of desipramine and fluvoxamine on timing behavior investigated with the fixed-interval peak procedure and the interval bisection task. Psychopharmacology 1996, 125, 274–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Heilbronner, S.R.; Meck, W.H. Dissociations between interval timing and intertemporal choice following administration of fluoxetine, cocaine, or methamphetamine. Behav. Process. 2014, 101, 123–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. National Research Council. Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals, 8th ed.; National Academies Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2011. [CrossRef]
  45. Buhusi, C.V.; Meck, W.H. Timing for the absence of a stimulus: The gap paradigm reversed. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 2000, 26, 305–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Paxinos, G.; Watson, C. (Eds.) The Rat Brain, 4th ed.; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
  47. Roberts, S. Isolation of an internal clock. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 1981, 7, 242–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Swearingen, J.E.; Buhusi, C.V. The pattern of responding in the peak-interval procedure with gaps: An individual-trials analysis. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 2010, 36, 443–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Marquardt, D.W. An algorithm for least-squares estimation of nonlinear parameters. J. Soc. Ind. Appl. Math. 1963, 11, 431–441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Buhusi, C.V.; Matthews, A.R. Effect of distracter preexposure on the reset of an internal clock. Behav. Process. 2014, 101, 72–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  51. Carboni, E.; Imperato, A.; Perezzani, L.; Di Chiara, G. Amphetamine, cocaine, phencyclidine and nomifensine increase extracellular dopamine concentrations preferentially in the nucleus accumbens of freely moving rats. Neuroscience 1989, 28, 653–661. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Cass, W.A.; Gerhardt, G.A. In vivo assessment of dopamine uptake in rat medial prefrontal cortex: Comparison with dorsal striatum and nucleus accumbens. J. Neurochem. 1995, 65, 201–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Hunt, P.; Raynaud, J.P.; Leven, M.; Schacht, U. Dopamine uptake inhibitors and releasing agents differentiated by the use of synaptosomes and field-stimulated brain slices in vitro. Biochem. Pharm. 1979, 28, 2011–2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Hoffmann, I. Pharmacology of nomifensine. Int. Pharm. 1982, 17, 4–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Schacht, U.; Heptner, W. Effect of nomifensine (HOE 984), a new antidepressant, on uptake of noradrenaline and serotonin and on release of noradrenaline in rat brain synaptosomes. Biochem. Pharm. 1974, 23, 3413–3422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Maricq, A.V.; Church, R.M. The differential effects of haloperidol and methamphetamine on time estimation in the rat. Psychopharmacology 1983, 79, 10–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Wittenborn, J.R.; Flaherty, C.F., Jr.; McGough, W.E.; Bossange, K.A.; Nash, R.J. A comparison of the effect of imipramine, nomifensine, and placebo on the psychomotor performance of normal males. Psychopharmacology 1976, 51, 85–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Blasi, G.; Taurisano, P.; Papazacharias, A.; Caforio, G.; Romano, R.; Lobianco, L.; Fazio, L.; Di Giorgio, A.; Latorre, V.; Sambataro, F.; et al. Nonlinear response of the anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia as a function of variable attentional control. Cereb. Cortex 2010, 20, 837–845. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Vijayraghavan, S.; Wang, M.; Birnbaum, S.G.; Williams, G.V.; Arnsten, A.F. Inverted-U dopamine D1 receptor actions on prefrontal neurons engaged in working memory. Nat. Neurosci. 2007, 10, 376–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Zahrt, J.; Taylor, J.R.; Mathew, R.G.; Arnsten, A.F. Supranormal stimulation of D1 dopamine receptors in the rodent prefrontal cortex impairs spatial working memory performance. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci. 1997, 17, 8528–8535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Tatsumi, M.; Groshan, K.; Blakely, R.D.; Richelson, E. Pharmacological profile of antidepressants and related compounds at human monoamine transporters. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 1997, 340, 249–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Blakely, R.D.; Bauman, A.L. Biogenic amine transporters: Regulation in flux. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2000, 10, 328–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Richelson, E. The newer antidepressants: Structures, pharmacokinetics, pharmacodynamics, and proposed mechanisms of action. Psychopharmacol. Bull. 1984, 20, 213–223. [Google Scholar]
  64. Richelson, E.; Nelson, A. Antagonism by antidepressants of neurotransmitter receptors of normal human brain in vitro. J. Pharm. Exp. 1984, 230, 94–102. [Google Scholar]
  65. Borsini, F.; Bendotti, C.; Velkov, V.; Rech, R.; Samanin, R. Immobility test: Effects of 5-hydroxytryptaminergic drugs and role of catecholamines in the activity of some antidepressants. J. Pharm. Pharm. 1981, 33, 33–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Racke, K.; Sommer, M.; Burns, F.; Hering, B. Differential effects of electrical stimulation, blockade of neuronal amine uptake and activation of alpha 2-adrenoceptors on the release of endogenous noradrenaline and 5-hydroxytryptamine from the isolated rat pineal gland. Naunyn. Schmiedebergs. Arch. Pharm. 1991, 343, 337–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Berridge, C.W.; Spencer, R.C. Differential cognitive actions of norepinephrine a2 and a1 receptor signaling in the prefrontal cortex. Brain Res. 2016, 1641, 189–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  68. Sokolov, E.N. Higher nervous functions; the orienting reflex. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 1963, 25, 545–580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Sargolini, F.; Roullet, P.; Oliverio, A.; Mele, A. Effects of lesions to the glutamatergic afferents to the nucleus accumbens in the modulation of reactivity to spatial and non-spatial novelty in mice. Neuroscience 1999, 93, 855–867. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Kety, S.S. The biogenic amines in the central nervous system: Their possible roles in arousal, emotion and learning. In The Neurosciences Second Study Program; Schmitt, F.O., Ed.; Rockefeller University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1970; pp. 324–335. [Google Scholar]
  71. Bodnoff, S.R.; Suranyi-Cadotte, B.; Quirion, R.; Meaney, M.J. A comparison of the effects of diazepam versus several typical and atypical anti-depressant drugs in an animal model of anxiety. Psychopharmacology 1989, 97, 277–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Lubow, R.E.; Gewirtz, J.C. Latent inhibition in humans: Data, theory, and implications for schizophrenia. Psychol. Bull. 1995, 117, 87–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Lubow, R.E. Latent inhibition: Effects of frequency of nonreinforced preexposure of the CS. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 1965, 60, 454–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Buhusi, C.V.; Paskalis, J.P.; Cerutti, D.T. Time-sharing in pigeons: Independent effects of gap duration, position and discriminability from the timed signal. Behav. Process. 2006, 71, 116–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Buhusi, C.V.; Sasaki, A.; Meck, W.H. Temporal integration as a function of signal and gap intensity in rats (Rattus norvegicus) and pigeons (Columba livia). J. Comp. Psychol. 2002, 116, 381–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Buhusi, C.V.; Perera, D.; Meck, W.H. Memory for timing visual and auditory signals in albino and pigmented rats. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 2005, 31, 18–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Lubow, R.E. Latent Inhibition and Conditioned Attention Theory; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  78. Buhusi, C.V.; Gray, J.A.; Schmajuk, N.A. Perplexing effects of hippocampal lesions on latent inhibition: A neural network solution. Behav. Neurosci. 1998, 112, 316–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Dias, R.; Honey, R.C. Involvement of the rat medial prefrontal cortex in novelty detection. Behav. Neurosci. 2002, 116, 498–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Aston-Jones, G.; Chiang, C.; Alexinsky, T. Discharge of noradrenergic locus coeruleus neurons in behaving rats and monkeys suggests a role in vigilance. Prog. Brain Res. 1991, 88, 501–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Locations of injector tips targeting the PrL. Brain region outlines are reproduced from [46]. PrL = Prelimbic Cortex. Appetitive = closed circles (left), Novel = open circles (right). (Appetitive n = 10, Novel n = 7).
Figure 1. Locations of injector tips targeting the PrL. Brain region outlines are reproduced from [46]. PrL = Prelimbic Cortex. Appetitive = closed circles (left), Novel = open circles (right). (Appetitive n = 10, Novel n = 7).
Neurosci 01 00010 g001
Figure 2. Average response rate (±SEM) and average rewards earned (±SEM) during appetitive noise conditioning sessions. As conditioning progressed from first to last session, the average response rate on the right lever (A) and the number of rewards earned (B) significantly increased. Open circles indicate individual data points. ** p < 0.01.
Figure 2. Average response rate (±SEM) and average rewards earned (±SEM) during appetitive noise conditioning sessions. As conditioning progressed from first to last session, the average response rate on the right lever (A) and the number of rewards earned (B) significantly increased. Open circles indicate individual data points. ** p < 0.01.
Neurosci 01 00010 g002
Figure 3. Timing response functions in the Appetitive and Novel groups under nomifensine (NOM). Timing in RPI (solid lines) and RPI + N trials (dotted lines) in the Appetitive (top) and the Novel groups (bottom) under the effect of vehicle (left), NOM 0.4 µg (middle), and NOM 4 µg (right). Noise presentations (black rectangles) resulted in timing functions being delayed (shifted rightwards) to a later time.
Figure 3. Timing response functions in the Appetitive and Novel groups under nomifensine (NOM). Timing in RPI (solid lines) and RPI + N trials (dotted lines) in the Appetitive (top) and the Novel groups (bottom) under the effect of vehicle (left), NOM 0.4 µg (middle), and NOM 4 µg (right). Noise presentations (black rectangles) resulted in timing functions being delayed (shifted rightwards) to a later time.
Neurosci 01 00010 g003
Figure 4. Average time delay (± SEM) under PrL infusion of nomifensine (NOM) estimated by the Curve Fitting method (left) and the Inter-Quartile method (right). Both methods indicated that the time delay in RPI + N trials relative to RPI trials were significantly increased for Novel rats (open circles) under the NOM 0.4 µg condition compared to the vehicle and NOM 0.4 µg conditions. Appetitive rats (closed squares) reliably reset under all drug conditions. Dashed lines indicate behavioral responses “run” (0-s delay), “stop” (8-s delay), and “reset” (16-s delay). * p < 0.05.
Figure 4. Average time delay (± SEM) under PrL infusion of nomifensine (NOM) estimated by the Curve Fitting method (left) and the Inter-Quartile method (right). Both methods indicated that the time delay in RPI + N trials relative to RPI trials were significantly increased for Novel rats (open circles) under the NOM 0.4 µg condition compared to the vehicle and NOM 0.4 µg conditions. Appetitive rats (closed squares) reliably reset under all drug conditions. Dashed lines indicate behavioral responses “run” (0-s delay), “stop” (8-s delay), and “reset” (16-s delay). * p < 0.05.
Neurosci 01 00010 g004
Figure 5. Nontiming lever pressing in RPI + N trials was not affected by NOM. (A) Average number of responses (± SEM) on nontiming (right) lever during RPI + N trials in Appetitive (closed squares) and Novel rats (open circles). (B) Response elevation ratio (± SEM) in Appetitive (closed squares) and Novel rats (open circles). Appetitive rats made significantly more responses on the nontiming lever during the noise than Novel rats (A) and increased their responding during the noise relative to prenoise levels (B). Novel rats did not lever press reliably during the noise (A) and responded at chance levels (ratio 0.5, dashed line) during the noise relative to pre-noise levels (B). The rate of responding was not affected by NOM dose in either group. ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05.
Figure 5. Nontiming lever pressing in RPI + N trials was not affected by NOM. (A) Average number of responses (± SEM) on nontiming (right) lever during RPI + N trials in Appetitive (closed squares) and Novel rats (open circles). (B) Response elevation ratio (± SEM) in Appetitive (closed squares) and Novel rats (open circles). Appetitive rats made significantly more responses on the nontiming lever during the noise than Novel rats (A) and increased their responding during the noise relative to prenoise levels (B). Novel rats did not lever press reliably during the noise (A) and responded at chance levels (ratio 0.5, dashed line) during the noise relative to pre-noise levels (B). The rate of responding was not affected by NOM dose in either group. ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05.
Neurosci 01 00010 g005
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Matthews, A.R.; Buhusi, M.; Buhusi, C.V. Blockade of Catecholamine Reuptake in the Prelimbic Cortex Decreases Top-Down Attentional Control in Response to Novel, but not Familiar Appetitive Distracters, within a Timing Paradigm. NeuroSci 2020, 1, 99-114. https://doi.org/10.3390/neurosci1020010

AMA Style

Matthews AR, Buhusi M, Buhusi CV. Blockade of Catecholamine Reuptake in the Prelimbic Cortex Decreases Top-Down Attentional Control in Response to Novel, but not Familiar Appetitive Distracters, within a Timing Paradigm. NeuroSci. 2020; 1(2):99-114. https://doi.org/10.3390/neurosci1020010

Chicago/Turabian Style

Matthews, Alexander R., Mona Buhusi, and Catalin V. Buhusi. 2020. "Blockade of Catecholamine Reuptake in the Prelimbic Cortex Decreases Top-Down Attentional Control in Response to Novel, but not Familiar Appetitive Distracters, within a Timing Paradigm" NeuroSci 1, no. 2: 99-114. https://doi.org/10.3390/neurosci1020010

APA Style

Matthews, A. R., Buhusi, M., & Buhusi, C. V. (2020). Blockade of Catecholamine Reuptake in the Prelimbic Cortex Decreases Top-Down Attentional Control in Response to Novel, but not Familiar Appetitive Distracters, within a Timing Paradigm. NeuroSci, 1(2), 99-114. https://doi.org/10.3390/neurosci1020010

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop