Next Article in Journal
Oecumene: Repositioning Ourselves in Our Habitat
Previous Article in Journal
Exploring Justice in the Process of Redesigning Local Development Strategies for LEADER: Representation, Distribution, and Recognition
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

A Gender Study of Food Stress and Implications for International Students Acculturation

1
Civil, Commercial and Economic Law School, China University of Political Science and Law, Beijing 100088, China
2
Centre for Interdisciplinary Social Research, Phenikaa University, Yen Nghia Ward, Ha Dong District, Hanoi 100803, Vietnam
3
Sciences Po Paris, 75007 Paris, France
4
A.I. for Social Data Lab (AISDL), Vuong & Associates, Hanoi 100000, Vietnam
5
Centre for Crop Systems Analysis, Wageningen University & Research, Droevendaalsesteeg 4, 6708 PB Wageningen, The Netherlands
6
Vietnam National University of Agriculture, Trau Quy, Gia Lam, Hanoi 100000, Vietnam
7
Monash Business School, Monash University, Victoria 3145, Australia
8
Graduate School of Asia Pacific Studies, Ritsumeikan Asia Pacific University, Beppu 874-8577, Japan
*
Authors to whom correspondence should be addressed.
World 2023, 4(1), 80-94; https://doi.org/10.3390/world4010006
Submission received: 17 November 2022 / Revised: 15 January 2023 / Accepted: 17 January 2023 / Published: 30 January 2023

Abstract

:
Acculturative stress can be a big problem for international students. Among the adaptation difficulties they may face, adjusting to new foods in a new environment is crucial to their well-being. Existing studies related to dietary acculturation point to gender differences, mostly on objective health impacts. Using the information processing approach, this study aims to examine the subjective perception of dietary acculturation difficulties, with a focus on the influence of social connectedness. Using the Bayesian inference approach with the Hamiltonian Markov Chain Monte Carlo (MCMC) technique on a sample of 268 students from a Japanese international university, we found that female students are more likely to have perceived difficulties in the process of adjusting to new foods, but social connectedness lessens this effect. We also found no significant differences between domestic and international students regarding perceived difficulties of food adjustment in this study site, likely due to its highly multicultural environment. We suggest international universities provide better information about the food situations on campuses, especially for female students, and organize more cultural exchange events and food-related social activities to help students overcome barriers of food stress.

1. Introduction

Our world is becoming increasingly interconnected. With the rapid advancement in transportation and information technology, globalization now affects various aspects of human society, such as the economy, education, and culture. People coming to a new environment will need to go through psychological adjustments related to their perceptions of new cultural values, which is the process of acculturation [1]. During the acculturative process, one receives and evaluates new values (as well as re-evaluates priorly integrated old values) to decide what to accept and what to reject as a natural mechanism to update one’s mindset [2]. This process is often not easy and may cause acculturative stress to certain degrees [3,4,5]. Acculturative stress was found to be positively associated with depression [6,7,8], notably among international students [9,10]. Understanding the difficulties people may face during the acculturative process can help create better support strategies to minimize negative effects.
Among the obstacles in the adaptation process, food stress is a serious issue. Food is an essential part of life, not only from the standpoint of nutrition and physical well-being but also from the perspective that cuisine is a major aspect of human culture. Each culture has different perceptions of the social, spiritual, and emotional values of food [11]. Coming to a new environment, people need to adjust to new food items and set up their nutrition plans accordingly. Sufficient helpful information is required in making such adjustments. In cases of international students, this can be provided through social channels such as digital social media platforms, which serve as important sources of information and psychological support during the acculturation process [12,13]. Without proper social support and good information, young adults can develop bad eating habits from dietary acculturation that negatively affect their health [14,15]. Overall, dietary acculturation is dynamic and complex, depending on various personal, cultural, and environmental factors [16].
The process of dietary acculturation can be difficult due to the influence of many different internal and external obstacles. These can include nutrition health awareness, personal preferences, the availability of traditional foods, as well as changes in methods of food preparation [17,18]. This points to the importance of social support to help people familiarize themselves with foreign cuisine cultures [19]. It is suggested that over time, dietary acculturation may lead to a shift in new eating habits different from one’s original diet, which, in some studied cases, is influenced by the host country’s dominant diet characteristics [20,21]. For international students, incorporating new foods without completely discarding the original diet creates a hybrid diet, with eating habits oscillating between the new and traditional dietary cultures [22]. Regardless of adaptation strategies, the changes in perceptions and behaviors toward cultural values within a multicultural environment are under the phenomenon of cultural additivity on both individual and social levels [23]. Thus, examining the social factors affecting dietary acculturation using an information processing framework for mindset shifting can provide helpful insights.
Regarding gender differences in food perception, studies have found that women are often more concerned about the health impacts and weight control aspects of food choice [24]. Additionally, women also tend to care more about the “virtuous” qualities of their food across different dimensions such as health, ethical issues, and body image [25]. Upon migration, women may worry about unfamiliarity with new food items, uncertainty about meal formats and food preparation, or religious food prohibitions, and thus tend to stick to those perceived as familiar or safe [26]. Such dietary concerns put a lot of pressure on women. Studies on international students in Japan found that females are at higher risk of depression [10,27]. However, having a higher level of consideration for food choices may help women achieve better health conditions. A study on food patterns of Korean immigrants in the United States found that women are less likely to be acculturated and less likely to be overweight than men [28]. Among Latinos coming to the United States, women also develop better diets compared to men [29]. In Filipino Americans adapting to Western diets, females were consistently found to have a lower body mass index (BMI) and waist–hip ratio [30]. For Chinese, Korean, and Vietnamese living in the United States, it was found that the association between acculturation and increased BMI is stronger among men compared to women [31].
While existing studies on dietary acculturation tend to focus on objective health impacts, the aspect of subjective perceptions in relation to social information exchange has not been well explored. In this present investigation, we examine possible gender differences in the perceived difficulties of dietary acculturation. As social connectedness is a predictor of the acculturative stress level [32], its influence in terms of information processing can help better explain the adjustment process. Thus, we conducted the present study on students from an international university with a highly multicultural campus environment (see the Study Site and Participants subsection for more details). This also helped investigate the food adjustment issues among both domestic and international students. To examine the possible psychological mechanism underlying food stress perception in terms of information processing, the Bayesian Mindsponge Framework was employed (see the Model Construction and Statistical Analysis subsections for detailed rationale). Additionally, it is important to note that the data used in this current study were collected in 2018—before the COVID-19 pandemic—and all activities on the campus were normal at the time.
The study has the following research questions (RQs):
RQ1: Do international students experience more food stress compared to domestic students in a highly multicultural campus?
RQ2: Do female students experience more food stress due to dietary acculturation compared to male students?
RQ3: Does social connectedness influence the above relationship between gender and food stress?
In the Material and Methods section, we present the information-processing-based rationale for model construction, information about the study site and participants, and descriptions of the variables to be used in the statistical analysis. Technical validation and the results of the analysis are presented in the Results section. In the Discussion section, we present our arguments on the psychosocial factors and information processes related to food stress due to acculturation, as well as implications for policymaking.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Model Construction

We used the Bayesian Mindsponge Framework (BMF) to construct conceptual models and conduct statistical analysis on the acculturative process of adjusting to new food [33]. In detail, the Mindsponge mechanism [2] was used as the theoretical foundation for model construction, while Bayesian inference was utilized to fit the model.
The Mindsponge mechanism is an information processing framework of how the human mind receives, filters, and accepts (or rejects) new values [34], which can be used to explain acculturative processes effectively. In a new (and multicultural) environment, students need to absorb and evaluate new values to adjust their mindset accordingly, which is necessary for them to adapt, regardless of chosen strategies [35]. This framework was used to provide insights into how acculturative stress and social connectedness may affect university students’ depression levels [10]. In the Mindsponge mechanism, there are two basic conditions for the mind to incorporate a new value: information accessibility (the mind can receive the information that carries values) and favorable evaluation (subjective cost–benefit judgments based on existing trusted values in the mindset resulting in a positive assessment of that value) [36,37]. The mindset updated with the newly trusted (accepted) values will affect the subsequent evaluation of similar values in the respective direction. This updating manner is what creates adaptation over time.
In the current investigation, the new values being examined are new foods, and the adjustment to them in this context is an acculturative process. Many factors with complex interactions affect how a student adjusts to new food during their university years in the new environment. Such factors can be personal preferences, food price, food availability, preparation methods, and health concerns, as well as many other social factors such as curiosity, eating with friends, cultural exchange, etc. In this study, we want to know if there is a difference between domestic and international students regarding perceived food adjustment difficulties. It should be noted that the study site is a highly multicultural environment (see the Study Site subsection below). Next, we focus on examining the gender-based difference, but we also want to see the moderating effect of social connectedness in terms of information processing. As presented in the literature review above, women tend to have a higher level of consideration regarding food choices, which may increase the perceived cost of adapting to new foods. Social connectedness has been shown to function as a “priority pass” that facilitates the processes of receiving and filtering new information in the Mindsponge mechanism [36,38]. Having access to a lot of helpful and trusted information can help ease the perceived difficulties during the process of the food adjustment process in a new environment. On the basis of the reasoning above, we derive the following model:
F o o d P r o b l e m   ~   α + I n t e r _ D o m + G e n d e r + G e n d e r S o c i a l C o n n e c t e d n e s s

2.2. Study Site and Participants

This study employed a dataset from the survey on students’ mental health and social connectedness conducted at a multicultural international university. The data collection was conducted at Ritsumeikan Asia Pacific University (APU), an international university in Oita Prefecture, Japan, from October to December 2018. Regarding the campus environment, the students were from 86 countries and regions, and faculty members were from 22 countries and regions at the time of survey collection. International students accounted for around 50.1% of the whole university’s students on campus during the survey period. The data have been employed and validated in several research papers [10,36,39,40].
The survey procedure strictly conformed to the World Medical Association (WMA) Declaration of Helsinki and was permitted by APU & Ethical Committee Board Approval Number 2018-03. The questionnaire was created and distributed using the web-based platform Google Forms. The questionnaire was available in both English and Japanese. The Japanese version was validated by an APU senior researcher in public health who was fluent in both languages. All international students studying at APU were required to be fluent in either language (the majority of them used English). Two of the authors gave presentations in the classrooms where the survey was distributed online. Before participants filled in the questionnaire, the survey’s purpose, content, confidentiality, and the right to refuse to participate in the survey were clearly explained. Participants could quit the survey at any time by either choosing “Not agree” to the informed consent or not submitting the survey. The data were anonymized and processed using Microsoft Excel and later stored as a comma-separated values (CSV) file.
There were 201 international students (75%) and 67 domestic ones (25%) in the total sample of 268 students. The mean age of participants was 20.9. The percentages of female and male students were 63% (n = 170) and 37% (n = 98), respectively. A total of 92% were undergraduate students and 8% were graduate students. A total of 43% were first-year students, 45% had been staying in Japan for 2 or 3 years, and 12% had been staying for 4 years or more. The origins of international students were diverse, including East Asia, South Asia, Southeast Asia, America, and others. Among all regions, the majority of students were from Southeast Asian countries (including Indonesia, Vietnam, Thailand, etc.), accounting for 45.5% of the total participants (n = 122). International students from East Asian countries (e.g., China, South Korea, etc.) accounted for 17.9% of all participants (n = 48), whereas the percentage for South Asian countries (e.g., India, Bangladesh, etc.) was 6.7% (n = 18). A complete description and validation of the dataset can be viewed in the article “A Dataset of Students’ Mental Health and Help-Seeking Behaviors in a Multicultural Environment” [39]. The dataset is openly accessible online, which helps with transparent and cost-effective scientific cross-checking or reproduction [41].

2.3. Variable Description

Within the dataset, we purposely chose variables that could help explain the acculturative process regarding adjusting to new food among international and domestic students. In total, we employed four variables. A description of each variable is presented in Table 1.
We generated an additional variable Gender_SocialConnectedness besides the above variables (Table 1) from the dataset. The variable Gender_SocialConnectedness is the interaction between two variables Gender and SocialConnectedness, which is used to examine their influence on FoodProblem. The outcome variable FoodProblem was based on the answers to the single question “I feel uncomfortable to adjust to new foods”. Respondents chose one of five response options: “Strongly disagree”, “Disagree”, “Not sure”, “Agree”, and “Strongly agree”. These responses were coded as 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5, respectively. The question was based on the Acculturative Stress Scale for International Students proposed by Sandhu and Asrabadi [43].
Students’ social connectedness is measured by asking eight questions about feelings of connection to others in their social environment, which is demonstrated by the SocialConnectedness variable. The social connectedness level was rated based on a 6-point Social Connectedness Scale ranging from 1 (“Strongly disagree”) to 6 (“Strongly agree”), developed by Lee and Robins [42]. Examples of the questions in the Social Connectedness Scale are “I feel so distant from people”, “I have no sense of togetherness with my peers”, “I don’t feel related to anyone”, etc. [42]. Regarding internal consistency, Cronbach’s alpha for this scale in this dataset is 0.95 [10]. The two demographic variables Gender and Inter_Dom are determined by asking respondents, “What is your gender?” and “Are you a domestic student or an international student?”, respectively.

2.4. Statistical Analysis

For assessing the food acculturative issue in students, the data were analyzed using the Bayesian analysis method with the Hamiltonian MCMC (Markov Chain Monte Carlo) technique. There are three main reasons for employing the Bayesian method in this study. First, since the evidence in the current study is novel, the Bayesian’s updating capability can benefit future similar studies [44,45]. Second, since the Bayesian inference approach treats all the properties probabilistically, including unknown parameters, it aids the construction of parsimonious models [44]. By incorporating the power of the MCMC technique, Bayesian methods can fit a wide variety of models, such as multi-level correlation structures and non-linear regression frameworks, making flexibility a major advantage [46]. Finally, since the sample size in the dataset was small, the Bayesian MCMC simulation technique also helped enhance the estimation’s precision [47]. Given these features, the Bayesian MCMC approach is highly compatible with the Mindsponge framework, and thus, the BMF was employed.
The data were analyzed using R software (version 4.0.2) with the bayesvl package [48,49]. The package was chosen due to its good visualization power and user-friendly, transparent operation. The analysis was run using 5000 iterations (including 2000 warm-up iterations), four Markov chains, and four cores. To check the model’s goodness-of-fit, we employed the Pareto smoothed importance-sampling leave-one-out cross-validation (PSIS-LOO-CV) approach [50]. We checked the Markov property with effective sample sizes, Gelman factors, and several visualized plots (see Results section). Because this is the first exploratory study of its scope and method, all estimations’ priors were set as uninformative [45], which is equivalent to a normal distribution with mean = 0 and standard deviation = 10. We also performed the “prior-tweaking” technique to check the model’s robustness. To do this, we reran the analysis using prior distributions demonstrating two cases of belief and disbelief toward the acquired results of a parameter. If the differences in the simulated results are small and negligible, the posterior results can be considered robust.

3. Results

To test the hypothesis, we examined the model as presented in the Model Construction subsection. The model examined the effects of students’ gender and its interaction with social connectedness, and the type of student (international or domestic student) on the perceived difficulty of adjusting to new food. The model’s logical network is shown in Figure 1.
The PSIS diagnostic plot shows that all k values are below 0.5, implying that the model has a high goodness-of-fit with the data (see Figure 2).
All parameters show a good convergence since the effective sample sizes (n_eff) are greater than 1000, and Gelman shrink factors (Rhat) all equal one (see Table 2). Furthermore, the convergence was also confirmed visually using trace plots, Gelman plots, and autocorrelation plots (see Figure 3, Figure 4 and Figure 5).
The trace plots in Figure 3 show the Markov chains fluctuating around a central equilibrium (posterior values on the y-axis over iterations on the x-axis), indicating that the chains converge well.
As shown in Figure 4, the shrink factors rapidly drop to one during the warm-up phase, meaning that the average variance between chains and the average variance within chains are similar, which indicates good convergence [51]. Additionally, we can observe in Figure 5 that the autocorrelation for each parameter is quickly eliminated, which again confirms the healthy Markov property of the chains.
Regarding gender, females were found to be positively associated with a higher level of perceived difficulties in adjusting to new food ( μ G e n d e r = 1.03 and σ G e n d e r = 0.30). However, the interaction with social connectedness produced a negative value, suggesting social connectedness lessens the effect of gender on food problems ( μ G e n d e r _ S o c i a l C o n n e c t e d n e s s = −0.19 and σ G e n d e r _ S o c i a l C o n n e c t e d n e s s = 0.06). The impact of the type of student (domestic or international student) was found to be not significant toward food acculturative problems ( μ I n t e r _ D o m = −0.04 and σ I n t e r _ D o m   = 0.13). The posterior distributions of Gender and Gender_SocialConnectedness fell entirely on the positive and negative, respectively, indicating the high reliability of the results (see Figure 6). We can also see that the majority of simulated samples are located on the positive side for Gender (x-axis) and the negative side for Gender_SocialConnectedness (y-axis) in the pairwise distribution graph (see Figure 7). We can also confirm the result’s reliability through the demonstration of the 95% highest posterior density intervals (HPDIs), as shown in Figure 8.
To check the robustness of the model, we performed the prior-tweaking technique on Gender_SocialConnectedness using two prior distributions: belief on effect and disbelief on effect. The prior demonstrating our belief that the moderation effect of Gender_SocialConnectedness is positive was set using a normal distribution with a mean of −0.5 and a standard deviation of 0.3. The prior demonstrating the disbelief about the moderation effect was set using a normal distribution with a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 0.3. The simulated results using informative priors are almost similar to the generated results employing uninformative priors (see Table 2), which indicates the model’s high resistance to initial changes in priors. In other words, the model can be deemed robust.

4. Discussion

Our analysis result shows no significant difference between domestic and international students regarding the perceived difficulties in adjusting to new food. This result should be interpreted carefully since it is heavily dependent on context. While some former studies only focused on examining the acculturative effects on international students or immigrants coming to the host countries [19,20], our study investigates both domestic and international students in a highly multicultural environment. To elaborate, at the time of data collection, about half of the total student population on campus were international students (see the Methodology section). Asian dietary culture in general can be quite different from that of the West where high-calorie foods and dairy products have a relatively higher consumption frequency [52]. Even within the East Asia sphere, the dietary cultures of different countries vary to certain degrees [53]. Cultural values are reinforced collective values in specific infospheres (societies). As these values in one’s mindset influence the evaluation of related information, higher degrees of value deviation will likely face a stricter filtering process [33]. Thus, students who are sensitive regarding dietary changes are likely more affected by differences in dietary cultures and food items.
APU regularly held social events (such as Multicultural Weeks) co-organized by students to introduce and exchange the cultural values in every country or region [54]. Foods from various cultures are accessible to all students not only during these events but also through the school cafeteria’s diverse menu, as well as many independent food vendors on the campus. There are also usual informal shared cooking activities among domestic and international students in the university’s dormitory, which follows the university’s cultural exchange system, especially for first-year students. Additionally, the local government has been supporting cultural diversity, and a wide range of foreign food items are available in non-Japanese restaurants and supermarkets across the town. In this highly multicultural environment, all students, domestic and international, have access to food items that are unfamiliar to them. Here, not only international students can incorporate the host country’s foods into their new diets [22] but domestic students can (and are socially encouraged to) incorporate foreign foods into their hybrid diets too. Thus, in this particular environment, the acculturative process regarding food can be applied to all students regardless of their origins.
Regarding gender differences, we found that female students are more likely to perceive difficulties in the process of dietary acculturation; however, a higher level of social connectedness helps lessen this effect. Several characteristics may help explain why female students may find it harder to adjust to new food. Women are relatively more selective regarding food choices due to a higher level of concern about nutrition, body image, and ethical implications [24,25]. As women may exhibit stronger reactions to stressors [55], their concerns about food choices can increase perceived difficulties in adapting to diet changes. Additionally, women may be more risk-averse compared to men [56], which can make them hesitant to try new foods and nutrition plans [26]. Another study suggested that this risk-aversion attitude is self-assessed [57], but subjective assessments would still affect the ideation of perceived risks. Humans decide their behaviors based on intentions [58,59], which in turn are determined by the trust-based subjective cost–benefit judgments of the action’s perceived value [60]. Adjusting to new diets takes a lot of effort [17,18], and these psychological characteristics may add more to the perceived cost of such decisions and will be expressed as uncomfortable feelings. In other words, their intentions and behaviors of adapting to new foods are more likely to cause cognitive dissonance.
How does social connectedness help female students better cope with food adjustment? Women may perceive a lower level of social support than men during the acculturative process [61]. However, they may rely more on collectivistic approaches in dealing with acculturative stress [62]. This suggests that a stronger feeling of being connected to other people can help female students find the adaptation process less stressful. In terms of the information processing mechanism, a sense of connectedness was shown to have the role of a “priority pass” that speeds up the process of receiving and filtering information, increasing the probability of external information absorption and the acceptance of values into one’s mindset [36]. Adaptation requires a lot of information from many sources as well as rigorous processes of filtering such information [34]. In this case, they are food-related information such as taste, nutritional properties, availability, price, preparation methods, attached cultural, religious, and ethical values, etc. Adjusting to new diets in a new living environment is not as simple as trying the items to see if one likes them or not but rather is a complex assessment based on related perceived values in various aspects (a Mindsponge process of adaptation). Therefore, a higher level of social connectedness will make it easier to gather information from social groups or ask for advice from other people about food. The willingness to connect socially also increases trust toward the information sources [38], which helps students accept and integrate the values of new foods and dietary changes easier.
These results have several implications for policy on acculturation and international education. In a highly multicultural environment, everyone is exposed to new cultural values. As the world is becoming more globalized, managing international environments for education, business, or living requires careful consideration of the effects of acculturation on all population groups. For international university campuses, besides a sufficient supply of diverse food items from many cultures, more specific information about food situations (variety, availability, etc.) in the living environment (campus and local places) should be provided to students in advance. Additionally, because gender inequality issues are still a big concern in the globalization context [63], our study shows that there needs to be more support for female students regarding acculturation-related difficulties. Here, providing professional advice about the nutritional values, health impacts, diet structures, and ethical implications of new foods can help female students assess their related concerns in a faster and healthier manner. Social activities such as cultural exchange events or cooking and eating together can increase the level of social connectedness among students in the new environment, helping them provide each other with information as well as practical support.
The current study’s limitations are presented here for transparency. The sample we use for analysis is small and has a higher female-to-male ratio. However, we employed the Bayesian MCMC approach to help increase the simulated results’ precision. Additionally, the number of APU international students with Asian origins is higher than those from other regions; thus, this environment may not represent all cultures equally and sufficiently.

5. Conclusions

Dietary acculturation is an important issue in our increasingly globalized world. Many studies in this field focus on the objective health impacts of changes in eating habits. Our study provides a different perspective on the issue by employing the information processing approach to examine the psychological aspects. Our findings show that female students tend to have more perceived difficulties in adjusting to new foods, but the moderating effect of social connectedness helps lessen this association. Helpful, trusted information from social channels can support female students in overcoming food stress due to their relatively higher dietary concerns. Interestingly, we also found that in a highly multicultural environment where foreign foods are widely available, domestic students also find it equally difficult to adjust to new foods. On the basis of the findings, we suggest that educational institutions and migration policymakers should pay more attention to the subjective aspects of dietary acculturation and provide more cultural exchange opportunities.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, R.J. and T.-T.L.; methodology, M.-H.N. and Q.-H.V.; software, Q.-H.V.; validation, T.-T.L. and Q.-H.V.; formal analysis, T.-T.L. and M.-H.N.; investigation, R.J. and T.-T.L.; resources, T.-T.L. and M.-H.N.; data curation, M.-H.N.; writing—original draft preparation, R.J., T.-T.L., and T.-T.V.; writing—review and editing, T.-P.N. and G.H.; visualization, T.-T.L. and M.-H.N.; supervision, Q.-H.V.; project administration, R.J. and T.-T.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by APU & Ethical Committee Board (Approval Number 2018-03).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The dataset is available in the open-access article “A Dataset of Students’ Mental Health and Help-Seeking Behaviors in a Multicultural Environment” (DOI: 10.3390/data4030124).

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank our friends and other members at Vuong & Associates (Hanoi, Vietnam). We would like to thank Ritsumeikan Asia Pacific University for supporting this research during the data collection.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Berry, J.W. Acculturation: Living successfully in two cultures. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 2005, 29, 697–712. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Vuong, Q.H.; Napier, N.K. Acculturation and global mindsponge: An emerging market perspective. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 2015, 49, 354–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Berry, J.W. Acculturative stress. In Handbook of Multicultural Perspectives on Stress and Coping; Wong, P.T.P., Wong, L.C.J., Eds.; Springer: Boston, MA, USA, 2006; pp. 287–298. [Google Scholar]
  4. Berry, J.W.; Annis, R.C. Acculturative stress: The role of ecology, culture and differentiation. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 1974, 5, 382–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Berry, J.W.; Kim, U.; Minde, T.; Mok, D. Comparative studies of acculturative stress. Int. Migr. Rev. 1987, 21, 491–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Tummala-Narra, P.; Alegria, M.; Chen, C.-N. Perceived discrimination, acculturative stress, and depression among South Asians: Mixed findings. Asian Am. J. Psychol. 2012, 3, 3–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Revollo, H.-W.; Qureshi, A.; Collazos, F.; Valero, S.; Casas, M. Acculturative stress as a risk factor of depression and anxiety in the Latin American immigrant population. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 2011, 23, 84–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Park, H.-S.; Rubin, A. The mediating role of acculturative stress in the relationship between acculturation level and depression among Korean immigrants in the U.S. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 2012, 36, 611–623. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Constantine, M.G.; Okazaki, S.; Utsey, S.O. Self-Concealment, Social Self-Efficacy, Acculturative Stress, and Depression in African, Asian, and Latin American International College Students. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 2004, 74, 230–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Nguyen, M.H.; Le, T.T.; Meirmanov, S. Depression, Acculturative Stress, and Social Connectedness among International University Students in Japan: A Statistical Investigation. Sustainability 2019, 11, 878. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Ares, G.; Giménez, A.; Vidal, L.; Zhou, Y.; Krystallis, A.; Tsalis, G.; Symoneaux, R.; Cunha, L.M.; de Moura, A.P.; Claret, A.; et al. Do we all perceive food-related wellbeing in the same way? Results from an exploratory cross-cultural study. Food Qual. Prefer. 2016, 52, 62–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Pang, H.; Wang, J. Promoting or prohibiting: Understanding the influence of social media on international students’ acculturation process, coping strategies, and psychological consequences. Telemat. Inform. 2020, 54, 101454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Pang, H. Is active social media involvement associated with cross-culture adaption and academic integration among boundary-crossing students? Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 2020, 79, 71–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Kremmyda, L.-S.; Papadaki, A.; Hondros, G.; Kapsokefalou, M.; Scott, J.A. Differentiating between the effect of rapid dietary acculturation and the effect of living away from home for the first time, on the diets of Greek students studying in Glasgow. Appetite 2008, 50, 455–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Almohanna, A.; Conforti, F.; Eigel, W.; Barbeau, W. Impact of Dietary Acculturation on the Food Habits, Weight, Blood Pressure, and Fasting Blood Glucose Levels of International College Students. J. Am. Coll. Health 2015, 63, 307–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Satia-abouta, J.; Patterson, R.E.; Neuhouser, M.L.; Elder, J. Dietary acculturation: Applications to nutrition research and dietetics. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2002, 102, 1105–1118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Varghese, S.; Moore-Orr, R. Dietary acculturation and health-related issues of Indian immigrant families in Newfoundland. Can. J. Diet. Pract. Res. 2002, 63, 72–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Hassan, D.A.E.; Hekmat, S. Dietary Acculturation of Arab Immigrants: In the Greater Toronto Area. Can. J. Diet. Pract. Res. 2012, 73, 143–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Hartwell, H.J.; Edwards, J.S.A.; Brown, L. Acculturation and food habits: Lessons to be learned. Br. Food J. 2011, 113, 1393–1405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Batis, C.; Hernandez-Barrera, L.; Barquera, S.; Rivera, J.A.; Popkin, B.M. Food Acculturation Drives Dietary Differences among Mexicans, Mexican Americans, and Non-Hispanic Whites. J. Nutr. 2011, 141, 1898–1906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Cuy Castellanos, D. Dietary Acculturation in Latinos/Hispanics in the United States. Am. J. Lifestyle Med. 2015, 9, 31–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Shi, Y.; Lukomskyj, N.; Allman-Farinelli, M. Food access, dietary acculturation, and food insecurity among international tertiary education students: A scoping review. Nutrition 2021, 85, 111100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Vuong, Q.-H.; Bui, Q.-K.; La, V.-P.; Vuong, T.-T.; Nguyen, V.-H.T.; Ho, M.-T.; Nguyen, H.-K.T.; Ho, M.-T. Cultural additivity: Behavioural insights from the interaction of Confucianism, Buddhism and Taoism in folktales. Palgrave Commun. 2018, 4, 143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Westenhoefer, J. Age and Gender Dependent Profile of Food Choice. Forum Nutr. 2005, 57, 44–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Beardsworth, A.; Bryman, A.; Keil, T.; Goode, J.; Haslam, C.; Lancashire, E. Women, men and food: The significance of gender for nutritional attitudes and choices. Br. Food J. 2002, 104, 470–491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Terragni, L.; Garnweidner, L.M.; Pettersen, K.S.; Mosdøl, A. Migration as a Turning Point in Food Habits: The Early Phase of Dietary Acculturation among Women from South Asian, African, and Middle Eastern Countries Living in Norway. Ecol. Food Nutr. 2014, 53, 273–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Eskanadrieh, S.; Liu, Y.; Yamashina, H.; Kono, K.; Arai, A.; Lee, R.B.; Tamashiro, H. Depressive symptoms among international university students in northern Japan: Prevalence and associated factors. Kokusai Hoken Iryo (J. Int. Health) 2012, 27, 165–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Jasti, S.; Lee, C.H.; Doak, C. Gender, Acculturation, Food Patterns, and Overweight in Korean Immigrants. Am. J. Health Behav. 2011, 35, 734–745. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Ramírez, A.S.; Wilson, M.D.; Soederberg Miller, L.M. Segmented assimilation as a mechanism to explain the dietary acculturation paradox. Appetite 2022, 169, 105820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Serafica, R.C.; Lane, S.H.; Ceria-Ulep, C.D. Dietary Acculturation and Predictors of Anthropometric Indicators Among Filipino Americans. SAGE Open 2013, 3, 2158244013495543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Chen, L.; Juon, H.-S.; Lee, S. Acculturation and BMI Among Chinese, Korean and Vietnamese Adults. J. Community Health 2012, 37, 539–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Yeh, C.J.; Inose, M. International students’ reported English fluency, social support satisfaction, and social connectedness as predictors of acculturative stress. Couns. Psychol. Q. 2003, 16, 15–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Vuong, Q.-H.; Nguyen, M.-H.; La, V.-P. The Mindsponge and BMF Analytics for Innovative Thinking in Social Sciences and Humanities; De Gruyter: Berlin, Germany, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  34. Vuong, Q.-H.; Le, T.-T.; La, V.-P.; Nguyen, H.T.T.; Ho, M.-T.; Van Khuc, Q.; Nguyen, M.-H. COVID-19 vaccines production and societal immunization under the serendipity-mindsponge-3D knowledge management theory and conceptual framework. Hum. Soc. Sci. Commun. 2022, 9, 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Jin, R.; Wang, X. “Somewhere I belong?” A study on transnational identity shifts caused by “double stigmatization” among Chinese international student returnees during COVID-19 through the lens of mindsponge mechanism. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 1018843. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Nguyen, M.-H.; Le, T.-T.; Nguyen, H.-K.T.; Ho, M.-T.; Nguyen, H.T.T.; Vuong, Q.-H. Alice in Suicideland: Exploring the Suicidal Ideation Mechanism through the Sense of Connectedness and Help-Seeking Behaviors. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 3681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Vuong, Q.-H.; Nguyen, M.-H.; Le, T.-T. Home Scholarly Culture, Book Selection Reason, and Academic Performance: Pathways to Book Reading Interest among Secondary School Students. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2021, 11, 468–495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Vuong, Q.-H.; Nguyen, M.-H.; Le, T.-T. A Mindsponge-Based Investigation into the Psycho-Religious Mechanism Behind Suicide Attacks; De Gruyter: Berlin, Germany, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  39. Nguyen, M.-H.; Ho, M.-T.; Nguyen, Q.-Y.T.; Vuong, Q.-H. A Dataset of Students’ Mental Health and Help-Seeking Behaviors in a Multicultural Environment. Data 2019, 4, 124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Nguyen, M.-H.; Serik, M.; Vuong, T.-T.; Ho, M.-T. Internationalization and Its Discontents: Help-Seeking Behaviors of Students in a Multicultural Environment Regarding Acculturative Stress and Depression. Sustainability 2019, 11, 1865. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Vuong, Q.-H. The (ir)rational consideration of the cost of science in transition economies. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2018, 2, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Lee, R.M.; Robbins, S.B. Measuring belongingness: The social connectedness and the social assurance scales. J. Couns. Psychol. 1995, 42, 232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Sandhu, D.S.; Asrabadi, B.R. Development of an acculturative stress scale for international students: Preliminary findings. Psychol. Rep. 1994, 75, 435–448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Wagenmakers, E.-J.; Marsman, M.; Jamil, T.; Ly, A.; Verhagen, J.; Love, J.; Selker, R.; Gronau, Q.F.; Šmíra, M.; Epskamp, S. Bayesian inference for psychology. Part I: Theoretical advantages and practical ramifications. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2018, 25, 35–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. McElreath, R. Statistical Rethinking: A Bayesian Course with Examples in R and Stan; Chapman and Hall/CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA; London, UK; New York, NY, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  46. Dunson, D.B. Commentary: Practical Advantages of Bayesian Analysis of Epidemiologic Data. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2001, 153, 1222–1226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Uusitalo, L. Advantages and challenges of Bayesian networks in environmental modelling. Ecol. Model. 2007, 203, 312–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. La, V.-P.; Vuong, Q.-H. Package ‘Bayesvl’: Visually Learning the Graphical Structure of Bayesian Networks and Performing MCMC with ‘Stan’; The Comprehensive R Archive Network (CRAN): Windhoek, Namibia, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  49. Vuong, Q.-H.; La, V.-P.; Nguyen, M.-H.; Ho, M.-T.; Tran, T.; Ho, M.-T. Bayesian analysis for social data: A step-by-step protocol and interpretation. MethodsX 2020, 7, 100924. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Vehtari, A.; Gelman, A.; Gabry, J. Practical Bayesian model evaluation using leave-one-out cross-validation and WAIC. Stat. Comput. 2017, 27, 1413–1432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Gelman, A.; Rubin, D.B. Inference from Iterative Simulation Using Multiple Sequences. Stat. Sci. 1992, 7, 457–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Tseng, M.; Wright, D.J.; Fang, C.Y. Acculturation and Dietary Change Among Chinese Immigrant Women in the United States. J Immigr. Minor. Health 2015, 17, 400–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Wahlqvist, M.L.; Lee, M.-S. Regional food culture and development. Asia Pac. J. Clin. Nutr. 2007, 16 (Suppl. 1), 2–7. [Google Scholar]
  54. APU. Multicultural Weeks—Ritsumeikan Asia Pacific University. Available online: https://en.apu.ac.jp/home/life/content25/ (accessed on 15 September 2021).
  55. Misra, R.; Crist, M.; Burant, C.J. Relationships Among Life Stress, Social Support, Academic Stressors, and Reactions to Stressors of International Students in the United States. Int. J. Stress Manag. 2003, 10, 137–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Charness, G.; Gneezy, U. Strong Evidence for Gender Differences in Risk Taking. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 2012, 83, 50–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Brighetti, G.; Lucarelli, C. Gender differences in attitudes towards risk and ambiguity: When psycho-physiological measurements contradict sex-based stereotypes. IJESB 2015, 24, 62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Ajzen, I. From Intentions to Actions: A Theory of Planned Behavior. In Action Control; Kuhl, J., Beckmann, J., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1985; pp. 11–39. [Google Scholar]
  59. Ajzen, I. The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1991, 50, 179–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Vuong, Q.-H.; Le, T.-T.; La, V.-P.; Nguyen, M.-H. The psychological mechanism of Internet information processing for post-treatment evaluation. Heliyon 2022, 8, e09351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Allen, M.W.; Amason, P.; Holmes, S. Social support, Hispanic emotional acculturative stress and gender. Commun. Stud. 1998, 49, 139–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Lee, D.S.; Padilla, A.M. Acculturative Stress and Coping: Gender Differences Among Korean and Korean American University Students. J. Coll. Stud. Dev. 2014, 55, 243–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Kanbur, R. Globalization and Inequality. In Handbook of Income Distribution; Atkinson, A.B., Bourguignon, F., Eds.; Handbook of Income Distribution; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2015; Volume 2, pp. 1845–1881. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. The model’s logical network.
Figure 1. The model’s logical network.
World 04 00006 g001
Figure 2. The model’s PSIS diagnostic plot.
Figure 2. The model’s PSIS diagnostic plot.
World 04 00006 g002
Figure 3. Trace plots for the model’s posterior coefficients.
Figure 3. Trace plots for the model’s posterior coefficients.
World 04 00006 g003
Figure 4. Gelman plots for the model’s posterior coefficients.
Figure 4. Gelman plots for the model’s posterior coefficients.
World 04 00006 g004
Figure 5. Autocorrelation plots for the model’s posterior coefficients.
Figure 5. Autocorrelation plots for the model’s posterior coefficients.
World 04 00006 g005
Figure 6. Interval distributions of the model’s posterior coefficients.
Figure 6. Interval distributions of the model’s posterior coefficients.
World 04 00006 g006
Figure 7. Pairwise distribution plot for Gender and Gender_SocialConnectedness.
Figure 7. Pairwise distribution plot for Gender and Gender_SocialConnectedness.
World 04 00006 g007
Figure 8. Distributions of the model’s coefficients with HPDI 95%.
Figure 8. Distributions of the model’s coefficients with HPDI 95%.
World 04 00006 g008
Table 1. Variable description.
Table 1. Variable description.
NameVariableType of VariableDescription
Feeling uncomfortable in adjusting to new foodsFoodProblemContinuousThe score of having stress when encountering new foods. The data range from 1 (low) to 5 (high).
Social connectednessSocialConnectednessContinuousThe total score of social connectedness (8 items) was measured by the Social Connectedness Scale developed by Lee and Robins [42]. The original values range from 8 to 48, which were then divided by the total item number to better fit the model.
Gender of studentGenderBinominalGender of students: Male or female. “Female student” is coded as 1, and “Male student” is coded as 0.
Type of studentInter_DomBinominalType of student: International student (Inter) or domestic one (Dom). “International student” is coded as 1, and “Domestic student” is coded as 0.
Table 2. The model’s simulated posteriors.
Table 2. The model’s simulated posteriors.
ParametersUninformative PriorPrior-Tweaking (Belief on Effect)Prior-Tweaking (Disbelief on Effect)N_eff *Rhat *
MeanSDMeanSDMeanSD
FoodProblem1.750.141.760.141.750.1459591
Inter_Dom−0.040.13−0.040.13−0.040.1363471
Gender1.030.301.070.300.990.3057301
Gender_SocialConnectedness−0.190.06−0.200.06−0.180.0657931
* n_eff and Rhat values presented in the tables are the effective sample sizes and Gelman values taken from the simulated results using uninformative prior.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Jin, R.; Le, T.-T.; Vuong, T.-T.; Nguyen, T.-P.; Hoang, G.; Nguyen, M.-H.; Vuong, Q.-H. A Gender Study of Food Stress and Implications for International Students Acculturation. World 2023, 4, 80-94. https://doi.org/10.3390/world4010006

AMA Style

Jin R, Le T-T, Vuong T-T, Nguyen T-P, Hoang G, Nguyen M-H, Vuong Q-H. A Gender Study of Food Stress and Implications for International Students Acculturation. World. 2023; 4(1):80-94. https://doi.org/10.3390/world4010006

Chicago/Turabian Style

Jin, Ruining, Tam-Tri Le, Thu-Trang Vuong, Thi-Phuong Nguyen, Giang Hoang, Minh-Hoang Nguyen, and Quan-Hoang Vuong. 2023. "A Gender Study of Food Stress and Implications for International Students Acculturation" World 4, no. 1: 80-94. https://doi.org/10.3390/world4010006

APA Style

Jin, R., Le, T. -T., Vuong, T. -T., Nguyen, T. -P., Hoang, G., Nguyen, M. -H., & Vuong, Q. -H. (2023). A Gender Study of Food Stress and Implications for International Students Acculturation. World, 4(1), 80-94. https://doi.org/10.3390/world4010006

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop